Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 21 May 2015
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 7 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00087

Anterior cingulate cortex mediates the relationship between O3PUFAs and executive functions in APOE e4 carriers

  • 1Decision Neuroscience Laboratory, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL, USA
  • 2Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL, USA
  • 3Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois, Champaign, IL, USA
  • 4Carle Neuroscience Institute, Carle Foundation Hospital, Urbana, IL, USA
  • 5Department of Speech and Hearing Science, University of Illinois, Champaign, IL, USA
  • 6Department of Internal Medicine, University of Illinois, Champaign, IL, USA
  • 7Institute for Genomic Biology, University of Illinois, Champaign, IL, USA
  • 8Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, Champaign, IL, USA

Introduction: Although diet has a substantial influence on the aging brain, the relationship between biomarkers of diet and aspects of brain health remains unclear. This study examines the neural mechanisms that mediate the relationship between omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids (O3PUFAs) and executive functions in at-risk (APOE e4 carriers), cognitively intact older adults. We hypothesized that higher levels of O3PUFAs are associated with better performance in a particular component of the executive functions, namely cognitive flexibility, and that this relationship is mediated by gray matter volume of a specific region thought to be important for cognitive flexibility, the anterior cingulate cortex.

Methods: We examined 40 cognitively intact adults between the ages of 65 and 75 with the APOE e4 polymorphism to investigate the relationship between biomarkers of O3PUFAs, tests of cognitive flexibility (measured by the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Trail Making Test), and gray matter volume within regions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC).

Results: A mediation analysis revealed that gray matter volume within the left rostral anterior cingulate cortex partially mediates the relationship between O3PUFA biomarkers and cognitive flexibility.

Conclusion: These results suggest that the anterior cingulate cortex acts as a mediator of the relationship between O3PUFAs and cognitive flexibility in cognitively intact adults thought to be at risk for cognitive decline. Through their link to executive functions and neuronal measures of PFC volume, O3PUFAs show potential as a nutritional therapy to prevent dysfunction in the aging brain.

Introduction

As the aged population expands, the economic burden of care and treatment of those with age-related health disorders also increases. The current and projected prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease, for example, suggests an increase in the United States from 5.1 to 13.2 million by 2050 with healthcare expenditures related to this growth expected to surpass 1 trillion dollars (Alzheimer’s Association, 2013). Therefore a successful strategy to promote healthy brain aging is of great interest to public health efforts and the United States economy. Diet and the many bioactive substances present in food represent a novel target for intervention that may promote healthy brain aging. Defining the precise mechanisms through which diet may impact brain health is a first step to developing successful dietary strategies against brain aging.

Accumulating evidence indicates that omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids (O3PUFAs) have a beneficial effect on cognitive aging. These long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids serve as structural components of neuronal membranes and may have neuroprotective properties through anti-inflammatory, antioxidant, and energy metabolism pathways (Cunnane et al., 2009). However, the core brain regions that O3PUFAs may act upon are unknown. This study aims to investigate specific and sensitive neural mechanisms that mediate the beneficial effect of O3PUFAs on cognitive aging, and in particular, the brain regions that underlie the relationship between O3PUFAs and cognitive flexibility, a component of the executive functions. This investigation focuses on carriers of the APOE e4 allele to evaluate these relationships in cognitively intact aging individuals thought to be at risk for cognitive decline (Jorm et al., 2007; Kozauer et al., 2008; Wisdom et al., 2011; Bell et al., 2012; Schiepers et al., 2012; Davies et al., 2014).

O3PUFAs have been to linked to superior cognitive performance, primarily in tasks that require executive functions (Beydoun et al., 2007; Witte et al., 2013; Bauer et al., 2014). Executive functions have been defined in many ways, but traditionally consist of planning and execution of goal-directed behaviors, abstract reasoning, and judgment (Stuss and Alexander, 2000). More recently, executive functions have been defined as the efficiency with which an individual applies his or her knowledge to cope with everyday life (Princiotta and Devries, 2014). The presence of executive dysfunction without measureable deficits in general cognition may represent the continuum of normal aging or a preclinical stage of dementia (Johnson et al., 2007). In particular, O3PUFAs have been directly linked to better performance on tasks that entail a particular component of the executive functions, namely cognitive flexibility (Bowman et al., 2013; Johnston et al., 2013). Cognitive flexibility refers to the ability to adjust to new demands or rules, and can be measured using task switching paradigms (Diamond, 2013).

Executive functions are implemented in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), and specific aspects of executive functions may be localized to particular sub-regions within the PFC (Barbey et al., 2012, 2013a,b,c, 2014a,b). In general, higher gray matter volume in the PFC has been associated with better performance on tasks that elicit executive functions (Kochunov et al., 2009; Burzynska et al., 2012; Tu et al., 2012). More specifically, and of interest in this study, one of the anatomically distinct sub-regions of the ventromedial PFC known as the anterior cingulate is thought to underlie aspects of cognition known as attention, cognitive control, working memory, set maintenance, and goal directed behavior (Barbey et al., 2011; Nee et al., 2011). In fact, higher gray matter volumes in the anterior cingulate cortex have been related to the cognitive flexibility component of the executive functions, as evidenced by performance on measures of task switching (Huster et al., 2009; Nee et al., 2011).

Given that O3PUFAs impact cognitive flexibility and that cognitive flexibility seems reliant upon the anterior cingulate cortex, we examined the role of regions within the PFC in mediating the relationship between O3PUFAs, as measured by blood biomarkers, and this particular component of the executive functions in cognitively intact aging individuals at risk for cognitive decline. In light of existing evidence, we predict that gray matter volume of the anterior cingulate would mediate the beneficial effect of O3PUFAs on cognitive flexibility.

Materials and Methods

Participants

This cross-sectional study enrolled 95 elderly adults from Carle Foundation Hospital, a local and readily available cohort of well-characterized elderly adults. No participants were cognitively impaired, as defined by a score of lower than 26 on the Mini-Mental State Examination. Participants with a diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment, dementia, psychiatric illness within the last 3 years, stroke within the past 12 months, and cancer within the last 3 years were excluded. Participants were also excluded for current chemotherapy or radiation, an inability to complete study activities, prior involvement in cognitive training or dietary intervention studies, and contraindications for MRI. Of these 95 participants, only a subset (n = 52) underwent genotyping of APOE alleles. Of this subset, 40 participants had the APOE e4 allele whereas 12 participants did not carry the APOE e4 allele. Our hypothesis focuses on APOE e4 carriers who may be at higher risk for cognitive decline and our sample does not include many APOE e4 non-carriers, hence, only e4 allele carriers were included in the following analyses.

Standard Protocol Approval and Participant Consent

This study was approved by the University of Illinois Institutional Review Board and, in accordance with the stated guidelines, all participants read and signed informed consent documents.

Nutrient Biomarker Acquisition and Analysis

Fasting plasma was collected between 7:00 am and 12:00 noon Central Standard Time from January 2013 to May 2014. Gas chromatography equipped with a flame ionization detector quantified plasma fatty acid concentrations. Plasma lipids were measured with standard enzymatic methods (Bowman et al., 2011). The two fatty acids assessed in the current study include docosahexaeonic acid (DHA, 22:6n-3) and eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA, 20:5n-3), both of which are ω-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids. Participants were divided into low vs. high DHA and EPA levels separately for each nutrient according to a median split. Low/high group assignment for DHA and EPA was the same for 39 of the 40 participants; therefore we computed a composite O3PUFA score by taking the average of DHA and EPA and then divided participants into low vs. high O3PUFA levels based on the composite O3PUFA values. (Bowman et al., 2013; Lotrich et al., 2013; McNamara et al., 2013).

APOE Genotyping

APOE genotyping was determined using PCR (e4 carrier, y/n). The presence of the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium was tested using a chi-square goodness-of-fit test (Hardy, 1908).

Neuropsychological Tests

Executive function was measured by the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) Trail Making Test (Delis et al., 2006). This assessment yields a measure of executive function that can be isolated from underlying skills, including visual scanning, number sequencing, letter sequencing, and motor speed. In this task, participants alternate between multiple task goals (either number or letter sequencing), which elicits a specific type of executive function known as cognitive flexibility. The reported results from the D-KEFS Trail Making Test assess cognitive flexibility while controlling for motor speed and therefore provide a measure of cognitive flexibility that is not confounded by motor skill, which may be an important consideration in an elderly cohort.

Volumetric Brain MRI

Volumetric analysis was performed on data from a 3D high-resolution (0.9 mm isotropic) T1-weighted scan using MPRAGE acquisition. Cortical reconstruction was performed with the Freesurfer image analysis suite, which is documented and freely available for download online.1 The technical details of these procedures are described in prior publications (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999a,b, 2001, 2002, 2004a,b; Fischl and Dale, 2000; Ségonne et al., 2004; Han et al., 2006; Jovicich et al., 2006; Reuter et al., 2010, 2012). All cortical reconstructions were manually checked for accuracy, as recommended by the software developers. This analysis focused on gray matter volumes in the PFC provided by freesurfer parcellation. These regions included the superior frontal cortex, rostral middle frontal cortex, the caudal middle frontal cortex, pars opercularis, pars triangularis, pars orbitalis, lateral orbitofrontal cortex, medial orbitofrontal cortex, precentral gyrus, paracentral gyrus, frontal pole, rostral anterior cingulate cortex, and caudal anterior cingulate cortex. In particular, the analysis sought to assess the specific and sensitive role of the rostral anterior cingulate, and exclude the involvement of other regions within the PFC.

Covariates

Covariates were included according to previous association with cognitive decline. These included age (continuous), gender (man/woman), education (nominal, year, fixed levels), income (nominal, five fixed levels), body mass index (BMI), and depression status (y/n). Although all participants had received a diagnosis of no depression at enrollment, the SF-36 Health Survey (Ware et al., 1993) revealed two participants with symptoms consistent with depression and so, in accordance with other studies, this was considered in the analysis as a covariate. PFC gray matter volume (continuous) was also included as a covariate in mediation analyses to assess the relationship between specific regions within the PFC, O3PUFA levels, and cognitive flexibility. These covariates were included in each of the four steps of the mediation analysis.

Statistical Analyses

All statistics were performed in MATLAB v8.3.0.532 software. The variables of interest included the following: O3PUFA levels in blood, gray matter volumes of regions within the PFC provided by freesurfer parcellation, and cognitive flexibility.

A formal mediation analysis was used in an effort to better understand the relationship between O3PUFA levels, gray matter volume of regions within the PFC, and cognitive flexibility using a four-step framework (Baron and Kenny, 1986). In each step of the mediation, a regression model was estimated and adjusted for all covariates listed above. The first step examined the relationship between O3PUFA levels and gray matter volumes in the PFC (path a). The second step examined the relationship between O3PUFA levels and cognitive flexibility (path c). The third step tested the direct pathway of mediation by including both gray matter volumes in the PFC and O3PUFA levels as independent variables, and cognitive flexibility as the dependent variable (paths b and c considered together). The fourth step tested the indirect pathway of mediation, namely the interaction between paths a and b, using the Sobel z-test (path c’). A perfect mediation was indicated if the first three steps showed statistically significant relationships. A partial mediation was indicated if the first two steps showed statistically significant relationships, the third step showed non-significant relationships, and the Sobel z-test indicated a significant interaction between paths a and b.

Results

Participant Characteristics

Participants had a mean age of 68.80 years and 72.5 percent of participants were females. Mean O3PUFA level in the low O3PUFA group was 102.30 nmol/mL and mean O3PUFA level in the high O3PUFA group was 216.00 nmol/mL. The mean MMSE score was 28.83 and the D-KEFS Trail Making Test cognitive flexibility score was 10.30 (Table 1). APOE genotype distribution did not deviate from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (χ2 = 2.93, p > 0.05).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of study population.

Plasma O3PUFA and Cognitive Flexibility, Mediated by Rostral Anterior Cingulate Structure

The mediation analyses indicated that out of all regions within the PFC, gray matter volume of only the left rostral anterior cingulate partially mediates the relationship between O3PUFA blood levels and cognitive flexibility. Each relationship within the mediation is described below in a stepwise fashion.

First, O3PUFA levels associated with higher volume of the left rostral anterior cingulate cortex (β = 428.6, p = 0.001), with O3PUFA levels explaining 68.7% of the variation in cortical volume (a). Second, O3PUFA levels marginally associated with better cognitive flexibility (β = 1.968, p = 0.056), with O3PUFA levels explaining 53.4% of the variance in cognitive flexibility (c). Third, when adding both O3PUFA levels and left rostral anterior cingulate volume as independent variables, neither independent variable was a significant predictor of cognitive flexibility (β = 0.002, p = 0.120 for left rostral anterior cingulate volume; β = 1.433, p = 0.209 for O3PUFA levels), indicating the lack of a perfect mediation. Finally, the Sobel z-test showed a significant interaction between paths a and b (z = 2.711, p = 0.007), indicating that the volume of this brain region is a partial mediator of the relationship between O3PUFA levels and cognitive flexibility (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Gray matter volume of the left rostral anterior cingulate partially mediates the relationship between O3PUFA blood levels and cognitive flexibility. Paths A, B, C, and C’ refer to the four pathways tested in the mediation analysis.

Discussion

This study found that volume of the left rostral anterior cingulate mediates the relationship between O3PUFA levels and cognitive flexibility. This is the first report of a specific and sensitive volumetric mediation between O3PUFA levels and a particular component of the executive functions, and is shown in an at-risk population of cognitively intact older adults. The individual relationships reported within the mediation, including those between O3PUFA levels and cognitive flexibility, between O3PUFA levels and left rostral anterior cingulate, and between left rostral anterior cingulate and cognitive flexibility, are substantiated by prior findings described below.

The first relationship demonstrated a positive association between higher O3PUFA blood levels and larger volume in the rostral anterior cingulate cortex of the left hemisphere (Figure 1, path A). Past studies have also linked higher O3PUFA blood levels to superior anterior cingulate structure and function (Conklin et al., 2007; McNamara et al., 2013; Bauer et al., 2014). The second relationship revealed higher O3PUFA levels were associated with better cognitive flexibility (Figure 1, path C). In past work, higher O3PUFA intake has been linked to better performance on tasks of cognitive flexibility (Beydoun et al., 2007; Bowman et al., 2013; Witte et al., 2013). The indirect pathway of mediation indicated a mediatory effect of left rostral anterior cingulate gray matter volume on the relationship between O3PUFA blood levels and cognitive flexibility (Figure 1, path C). Higher gray matter volumes in the anterior cingulate cortex have been linked to higher O3PUFA intake as well as superior cognitive flexibility, as evidenced by measures of task switching (Conklin et al., 2007; Huster et al., 2009; Nee et al., 2011; Witte et al., 2013; Raji et al., 2014). The unilateral nature of this mediator is substantiated by prior work implicating the vulnerability of regions within the left hemisphere to Alzheimer’s disease-related degeneration and associated cognitive impairments (Chételat et al., 2005; Querbes et al., 2009; Risacher et al., 2010; Mosconi et al., 2014).

Cognitive aging literature indicates that there are two major postulated routes through which O3PUFAs might impact executive functions, namely the vascular and non-vascular hypotheses. In reporting a volumetric mediation between O3PUFA levels and cognitive flexibility, our study is the first to provide evidence for the non-vascular hypothesis within a formal mediation framework. While the vascular hypothesis suggests that reducing blood pressure, lowering risk of thrombosis, reducing inflammation, and lowering serum triglyceride levels can prevent deterioration of the brain (Keli et al., 1994; Calder, 2004; Kelley et al., 2008), the non-vascular hypothesis states that cortical and subcortical volumes show reduction in healthy aging due to mechanisms not directly related to changes in vasculature. These effects are especially robust in the frontal cortex, and prevention or reduction of this atrophy can prevent cognitive decline (Salat et al., 2004). The non-uniform deterioration of both executive functions and PFC structure associated with aging support the notion that O3PUFAs may slow age effects on specific aspects of the executive functions and in particular regions within the PFC (MacPherson et al., 2002). Possible non-vascular mechanisms that may underlie this volumetric mediation include enhanced membrane and membrane-bound protein function, reduced amyloid-β production, reduced neuroinflammation and oxidative damage, increased levels of brain derived neurotrophic factor, and reduced excitotoxic omega-6 levels (Cole and Frautschy, 2010).

The partial nature of this mediation is supported by the specificity of this analysis, which honed in on one component of the executive functions and one region within the PFC. Brain aging is a dynamic, heterogeneous, and multi-faceted process (Koepsell and Monsell, 2012), and it is unlikely that the volume of a specific region within the PFC would completely and exclusively mediate the relationship between this vital nutrient for healthy aging and such an important cognitive function in aging. Rather than claiming that the rostral anterior cingulate is the sole mediator of the relationship between O3PUFAs and cognitive flexibility, this study is one of the first to begin defining specific and sensitive mediatory factors between nutrition and cognition.

The strength of this study includes the use of blood biomarkers to measure nutrition, which provided a more reliable assessment of dietary intake than that of food frequency questionnaires. The potential limitations of this study include the cross-sectional design, relatively small sample size, and inability to assess this relationship in both carriers and non-carriers of the APOE e4 allele. Future longitudinal studies with larger samples size are needed to confirm these results.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Joachim Operskalski and Kelsey Campbell for their invaluable help with the testing of participants and organization of this study as well as Gene L. Bowman for the blood biomarker assays. This work was supported by a grant from Abbott Nutrition through the Center for Nutrition, Learning, and Memory at the University of Illinois (ANGC1205).

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/

References

Alzheimer’s Association. (2013). 2013 Alzheimer’s Disease Facts and Figures. Available online at: https://www.alz.org/downloads/facts_figures_2013.pdf

Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., and Grafman, J. (2013a). Architecture of cognitive flexibility revealed by lesion mapping. Neuroimage 82, 547–554. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., and Grafman, J. (2013b). Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human intelligence. Neuropsychologia 51, 1361–1369. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., Paul, E. J., Chau, A., Solomon, J., and Grafman, J. H. (2014a). Lesion mapping of social problem solving. Brain 137, 2823–2833. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., Paul, E. J., and Grafman, J. (2014b). Architecture of fluid intelligence and working memory revealed by lesion mapping. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 485–494. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0512-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., Solomon, J., Krueger, F., Forbes, C., and Grafman, J. (2012). An integrative architecture for general intelligence and executive function revealed by lesion mapping. Brain 135, 1154–1164. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbey, A. K., Koenigs, M., and Grafman, J. (2011). Orbitofrontal contributions to human working memory. Cereb. Cortex 21, 789–795. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbey, A. K., Koenigs, M., and Grafman, J. (2013c). Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex 29, 1195–1205. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, R. M., and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, I., Hughes, M., Rowsell, R., Cockerell, R., Pipingas, A., Crewther, S., et al. (2014). Omega-3 supplementation improves cognition and modifies brain activation in young adults. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 29, 133–144. doi: 10.1002/hup.2379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, R. D., Winkler, E. A., Singh, I., Sagare, A. P., Deane, R., Wu, Z., et al. (2012). Apolipoprotein E controls cerebrovascular integrity via cyclophilin A. Nature 485, 512–516. doi: 10.1038/nature11087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beydoun, M. A., Kaufman, J. S., Satia, J. A., Rosamond, W., and Folsom, A. R. (2007). Plasma n-3 fatty acids and the risk of cognitive decline in older adults: the Atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 85, 1103–1111. doi: 10.1017/S1368980007000080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowman, G. L., Dodge, H. H., Mattek, N., Barbey, A. K., Silbert, L. C., Shinto, L., et al. (2013). Plasma omega-3 PUFA and white matter mediated executive decline in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:92. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowman, G. L., Shannon, J., Ho, E., Traber, M. G., Frei, B., Oken, S., et al. (2011). Reliability and validity of food frequency questionnaire and nutrient biomarkers in elders with and without mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 25, 49–57. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181f333d6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burzynska, A. Z., Nagel, I. E., Preuschhof, C., Gluth, S., Bäckman, L., Li, S.-C., et al. (2012). Cortical thickness is linked to executive functioning in adulthood and aging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 1607–1620. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calder, P. C. (2004). n–3 fatty acids and cardiovascular disease: evidence explained and mechanisms explored. Clin. Sci. (Lond.) 107, 1–11. doi: 10.1042/cs20040119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chételat, G., Landeau, B., Eustache, F., Mézenge, F., Viader, F., de la Sayette, V., et al. (2005). Using voxel-based morphometry to map the structural changes associated with rapid conversion in MCI: a longitudinal MRI study. Neuroimage 27, 934–946. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, G. M., and Frautschy, S. A. (2010). DHA may prevent age-related dementia. J. Nutr. 140, 869–874. doi: 10.3945/jn.109.113910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conklin, S. M., Gianaros, P. J., Brown, S. M., Yao, J. K., Hariri, A. R., Manuck, S. B., et al. (2007). Long-chain omega-3 fatty acid intake is associated positively with corticolimbic gray matter volume in healthy adults. Neurosci. Lett. 421, 209–212. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.04.086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunnane, S. C., Plourde, M., Pifferi, F., Bégin, M., Féart, C., and Barberger-Gateau, P. (2009). Fish, docosahexaenoic acid and Alzheimer’s disease. Prog. Lipid Res. 48, 239–256. doi: 10.1016/j.plipres.2009.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., and Sereno, M. I. (1993). Improved localization of cortical activity by combining EEG and MEG with MRI cortical surface reconstruction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 162–176. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.2.162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, G., Harris, S. E., Reynolds, C. A., Payton, A., Knight, H. M., Liewald, D. C., et al. (2014). A genome-wide association study implicates the APOE locus in nonpathological cognitive ageing. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 76–87. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H. (2006). TEST REVIEW: Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS). Appl. Neuropsychol. 13, 275–279. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1304_9

CrossRef Full Text

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., and Dale, A. M. (2000). Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 97, 11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Liu, A., and Dale, A. M. (2001). Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 20, 70–80. doi: 10.1109/42.906426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., et al. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: neurotechnique automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341–355. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., van der Kouwe, A. J. W., Makris, N., Ségonne, F., Quinn, B. T., et al. (2004a). Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage 23(Suppl. 1), S69–S84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., and Dale, A. M. (1999a). Cortical surface-based analysis. Neuroimage 9, 195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., Tootell, R. B. H., and Dale, A. M. (1999b). High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 272–284. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0193(1999)8:4<272::aid-hbm10>3.0.co;2-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., van der Kouwe, A., Destrieux, C., Halgren, E., Ségonne, F., Salat, D. H., et al. (2004b). Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 14, 11–22. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, X., Jovicich, J., Salat, D., van der Kouwe, A., Quinn, B., Czanner, S., et al. (2006). Reliability of MRI-derived measurements of human cerebral cortical thickness: the effects of field strength, scanner upgrade and manufacturer. Neuroimage 32, 180–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, G. H. (1908). Mendelian proportions in a mixed population. Science 28, 49–50. doi: 10.1126/science.28.706.49

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huster, R. J., Wolters, C., Wollbrink, A., Schweiger, E., Wittling, W., Pantev, C., et al. (2009). Effects of anterior cingulate fissurization on cognitive control during stroop interference. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 1279–1289. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J. K., Lui, L.-Y., and Yaffe, K. (2007). Executive function, more than global cognition, predicts functional decline and mortality in elderly women. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 62, 1134–1141. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.10.1134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, D. T., Deuster, P. A., Harris, W. S., Macrae, H., and Dretsch, M. N. (2013). Red blood cell omega-3 fatty acid levels and neurocognitive performance in deployed U.S. Servicemembers. Nutr. Neurosci. 16, 30–38. doi: 10.1179/1476830512y.0000000025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorm, A. F., Mather, K. A., Butterworth, P., Anstey, K. J., Christensen, H., and Easteal, S. (2007). APOE genotype and cognitive functioning in a large age-stratified population sample. Neuropsychology 21, 1–8. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jovicich, J., Czanner, S., Greve, D., Haley, E., van der Kouwe, A., Gollub, R., et al. (2006). Reliability in multi-site structural MRI studies: effects of gradient non-linearity correction on phantom and human data. Neuroimage 30, 436–443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keli, S. O., Feskens, E. J., and Kromhout, D. (1994). Fish consumption and risk of stroke. The Zutphen study. Stroke 25, 328–332. doi: 10.1161/01.str.25.2.328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelley, D. S., Siegel, D., Vemuri, M., Chung, G. H., and Mackey, B. E. (2008). Docosahexaenoic acid supplementation decreases remnant-like particle-cholesterol and increases the (n-3) index in hypertriglyceridemic men. J. Nutr. 138, 30–35.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kochunov, P., Robin, D. A., Royall, D. R., Coyle, T., Lancaster, J., Kochunov, V., et al. (2009). Can structural MRI indices of cerebral integrity track cognitive trends in executive control function during normal maturation and adulthood? Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2581–2594. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koepsell, T. D., and Monsell, S. E. (2012). Reversion from mild cognitive impairment to normal or near-normal cognition: risk factors and prognosis. Neurology 79, 1591–1598. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31826e26b7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozauer, N. A., Mielke, M. M., Chan, G. K. C., Rebok, G. W., and Lyketsos, C. G. (2008). Apolipoprotein E genotype and lifetime cognitive decline. Int. Psychogeriatr. 20, 109–123. doi: 10.1017/s104161020700587x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lotrich, F. E., Sears, B., and McNamara, R. K. (2013). Elevated ratio of arachidonic acid to long-chain omega-3 fatty acids predicts depression development following interferon-alpha treatment: relationship with interleukin-6. Brain Behav. Immun. 31, 48–53. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., and Della Sala, S. (2002). Age, executive function and social decision making: a dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychol. Aging 17, 598–609. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.17.4.598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNamara, R. K., Jandacek, R., Tso, P., Weber, W., Chu, W.-J., Strakowski, S. M., et al. (2013). Low docosahexaenoic acid status is associated with reduced indices in cortical integrity in the anterior cingulate of healthy male children: a 1H MRS study. Nutr. Neurosci. 16, 183–190. doi: 10.1179/1476830512Y.0000000045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mosconi, L., Murray, J., Davies, M., Williams, S., Pirraglia, E., Spector, N., et al. (2014). Nutrient intake and brain biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease in at-risk cognitively normal individuals: a cross-sectional neuroimaging pilot study. BMJ Open 4:e004850. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-004850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nee, D. E., Kastner, S., and Brown, J. W. (2011). Functional heterogeneity of conflict, error, task-switching and unexpectedness effects within medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 54, 528–540. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Princiotta, D., and Devries, M. (2014). “Executive functioning as a mediator of age-related cognitive decline in adults,” in Handbook of Executive Functioning, eds S. Goldstein and J. A. Naglieri (New York, NY: Springer New York), 143–155.

Google Scholar

Querbes, O., Aubry, F., Pariente, J., Lotterie, J.-A., Démonet, J.-F., Duret, V., et al. (2009). Early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease using cortical thickness: impact of cognitive reserve. Brain 132(Pt. 8), 2036–2047. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raji, C. A., Erickson, K. I., Lopez, O. L., Kuller, L. H., Gach, H. M., Thompson, P. M., et al. (2014). Regular fish consumption and age-related brain gray matter loss. Am. J. Prev. Med. 47, 444–451. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2014.05.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter, M., Rosas, H. D., and Fischl, B. (2010). Highly accurate inverse consistent registration: a robust approach. Neuroimage 53, 1181–1196. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter, M., Schmansky, N. J., Rosas, H. D., and Fischl, B. (2012). Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage 61, 1402–1418. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Risacher, S. L., Shen, L., West, J. D., Kim, S., McDonald, B. C., Beckett, L. A., et al. (2010). Longitudinal MRI atrophy biomarkers: relationship to conversion in the ADNI cohort. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 1401–1418. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.04.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salat, D. H., Buckner, R. L., Snyder, A. Z., Greve, D. N., Desikan, R. S. R., Busa, E., et al. (2004). Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cereb. Cortex 14, 721–730. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiepers, O. J., Harris, S. E., Gow, A. J., Pattie, A., Brett, C. E., Starr, J. M., et al. (2012). APOE E4 status predicts age-related cognitive decline in the ninth decade: longitudinal follow-up of the Lothian birth cohort 1921. Mol. Psychiatry 17, 315–324. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ségonne, F., Dale, A. M., Busa, E., Glessner, M., Salat, D., Hahn, H. K., et al. (2004). A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. Neuroimage 22, 1060–1075. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(04)00188-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuss, D. T., and Alexander, M. P. (2000). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view. Psychol. Res. 63, 289–298. doi: 10.1007/s004269900007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tu, P. C., Chen, L. F., Hsieh, J. C., Bai, Y. M., Li, C. T., and Su, T. P. (2012). Regional cortical thinning in patients with major depressive disorder: a surface-based morphometry study. Psychiatry Res. 202, 206–213. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ware, J. E., Snow, K., Kosinski, M., and Gandek, B. (1993). Manual and Interpretation Guide. Boston: The Health Institute, New England Medical Center.

Wisdom, N. M., Callahan, J. L., and Hawkins, K. A. (2011). The effects of apolipoprotein E on non-impaired cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 63–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witte, A. V., Kerti, L., Hermannstädter, H. M., Fiebach, J. B., Schreiber, S. J., Schuchardt, J. P., et al. (2013). Long-chain omega-3 fatty acids improve brain function and structure in older adults. Cereb. Cortex 24, 3059– 3068. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: omega-3 fatty acids, anterior cingulate cortex, executive function, cognitive aging, nutritional neuroscience

Citation: Zamroziewicz MK, Paul EJ, Rubin RD and Barbey AK (2015) Anterior cingulate cortex mediates the relationship between O3PUFAs and executive functions in APOE e4 carriers. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:87. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00087

Received: 20 March 2015; Accepted: 01 May 2015;
Published online: 21 May 2015.

Edited by:

P. Hemachandra Reddy, Texas Tech University, USA

Reviewed by:

Ramesh Kandimalla, Texas Tech University, USA
Latha Ramalingam, Texas Tech University, USA

Copyright © 2015 Zamroziewicz, Paul, Rubin and Barbey. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Aron K. Barbey, Decision Neuroscience Laboratory, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 405 North Mathews Avenue, Urbana, IL 61801, USA, Barbey@Illinois.edu;
www.DecisionNeuroscienceLab.org

Download