Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 31 August 2016
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 8 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00207

Anatomical Correlates of Non-Verbal Perception in Dementia Patients

Pin-Hsuan Lin1 Hsiu-Hui Chen2 Nai-Ching Chen3 Wen-Neng Chang3 Chi-Wei Huang3 Ya-Ting Chang3 Shih-Wei Hsu4 Che-Wei Hsu3 Chiung-Chih Chang3*
  • 1Department of Health and Beauty, Shu-Zen Junior College of Medicine and Management, Kaohsiung, Taiwan
  • 2Department of Physical Education, National Kaohsiung University of Applied Science, Kaohsiung, Taiwan
  • 3Department of Neurology, Cognition and Aging Center, Kaohsiung Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Chang Gung University College of Medicine, Kaohsiung, Taiwan
  • 4Department of Radiology, Kaohsiung Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Chang Gung University College of Medicine, Kaohsiung, Taiwan

Purpose: Patients with dementia who have dissociations in verbal and non-verbal sound processing may offer insights into the anatomic basis for highly related auditory modes.

Methods: To determine the neuronal networks on non-verbal perception, 16 patients with Alzheimer’s dementia (AD), 15 with behavior variant fronto-temporal dementia (bv-FTD), 14 with semantic dementia (SD) were evaluated and compared with 15 age-matched controls. Neuropsychological and auditory perceptive tasks were included to test the ability to compare pitch changes, scale-violated melody and for naming and associating with environmental sound. The brain 3D T1 images were acquired and voxel-based morphometry (VBM) was used to compare and correlated the volumetric measures with task scores.

Results: The SD group scored the lowest among 3 groups in pitch or scale-violated melody tasks. In the environmental sound test, the SD group also showed impairment in naming and also in associating sound with pictures. The AD and bv-FTD groups, compared with the controls, showed no differences in all tests. VBM with task score correlation showed that atrophy in the right supra-marginal and superior temporal gyri was strongly related to deficits in detecting violated scales, while atrophy in the bilateral anterior temporal poles and left medial temporal structures was related to deficits in environmental sound recognition.

Conclusions: Auditory perception of pitch, scale-violated melody or environmental sound reflects anatomical degeneration in dementia patients and the processing of non-verbal sounds are mediated by distinct neural circuits.

Introduction

Auditory agnosia is a rare disorder characterized by a relatively isolated deficit in auditory comprehension despite normal hearing. Different terms delineate separate entities of auditory agnosia, including word deafness (Ziegler, 1952), environmental sound agnosia (Oppenheimer and Newcombe, 1978) and music agnosia (Ayotte et al., 2002). Lesion studies point to a clear dissociation between the perceptions of different types of sounds, which give hints regarding the anatomical distribution of these unique human abilities (Mendez and Geehan, 1988; Peretz et al., 1994, 1997; Godefroy et al., 1995; Piccirilli et al., 2000; Ayotte et al., 2002; Galaburda et al., 2003; Levitin et al., 2004). Studies on auditory agnosia suggest that the processing of sound is not mediated by a general-purpose auditory architecture but by specialized cortical sub-systems (Thierry et al., 2003). In healthy participants using functional neuroimaging approaches, a dual access route specific for verbal and nonverbal material has been reported, respectively (Thierry and Price, 2006; Hocking and Price, 2009; Gainotti, 2015). Whether these networks also present in patients with degenerative process were not fully examined.

In terms of functional relationships, literature in verbal comprehension emphasizes the importance of the left posterior temporal region (Kreisler et al., 2000). In contrast, melody perception requires an intact auditory cortex and/or posterior superior temporal gyrus on the right hemisphere (Zatorre, 1988; Griffiths et al., 2001; Patterson et al., 2002; Gutschalk et al., 2004). The associated lesions leading to the environmental sound agnosia are not particularly consistent and have included left (Saygin et al., 2003; Vignolo, 2003), right (Griffiths et al., 1997; Mendez, 2001; Vignolo, 2003), bilateral cortical (Matthews et al., 2009), and sub-cortical areas (Tanaka et al., 2002).

Studies on the retention of musical abilities in dementia patients during the degeneration of other cognitive processes provide great insights into the organization of the brain. Bolero and the concerto for the left hand composed by Ravel after he had cortico-basal degeneration syndrome represents great musical talent that is innovative and distinct from his previous works (Amaducci et al., 2002). Four patients diagnosed of frontal-temporal dementia (FTD) or semantic dementia (SD) has retained or gained musical skills (Miller et al., 2000) while their loss of left anterior temporal lobes functions was prominent. Studies also indicated a loss of music and non-verbal sound recognition in dementia patients with other diagnosis (Halpern and O’Connor, 2000; Ghacibeh and Heilman, 2003; Matthews et al., 2009). However, no study has explored or compared the perception ability for non-verbal sounds in different types of dementia groups. The investigation of patients having different focal degenerative regions may help to test the hypothesis of conceptual representations of the non-verbal format in the brain (Gainotti, 2015).

Because environmental sound and music perception is so variable, this study aimed to explore the relationships of non-verbal sound perception in different dementia sub-groups and provide anatomical correlations using auditory cognitive tests and voxel-based morphometry (VBM). Based on the review, we hypothesize that dementia patients with greater right temporal atrophy may have worse music perception ability and that environmental sound perception may be related to left, right, or both temporal atrophy. The dementia group with a clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer dementia (AD), behavior variant fronto-temporal dementia (bv-FTD) and SD were selected because they present different degrees of gray matter atrophy in the frontal or temporal lobe. This would allow for group comparisons with controls based on the sound perception scores and elucidation of the anatomic structures related to the task results.

Materials and Methods

Subjects, Clinical Evaluation and Cognitive Testing

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by Chang Gung Memorial Hospital’s Institutional Review Committee on Human Research (97-0255B). The patients and controls were right handed and gave written informed consent to take part. Clinical diagnosis in both patients and controls was determined after a detailed history, neurologic examination, 1-h neuro-psychological battery (Chang et al., 2016a), laboratory screening, and visual inspection of a 3.0-tesla brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI; Chen et al., 2015). Patients diagnosed with fronto-temporal lobar degeneration met the Neary criteria (Neary et al., 1998). AD was diagnosed according to the International Working Group criteria (McKhann et al., 2011). None of the controls had evidence of impairment during the neuro-psychological testing nor did they have any history of neurologic or psychiatric disorder (Huang et al., 2015). Two sub-groups of fronto-temporal lobar degeneration, the behavior variant, also called bv-FTD, and the temporal variant, also called SD, were included.

Experimental Design and Materials

Neuropsychological Tests

All of the participants were tested for hearing ability using brainstem auditory evoked potentials with the measurement of waves I–V peak latency and waves I–III inter-peak latency. All had corrected-to-normal vision. Participants with a prolonged wave I peak latency (>1.8 ms) were excluded (Burdo, 1989). General intellectual function was assessed using the Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975). Verbal and non-verbal episodic memory was assessed using a modified California Verbal Learning Test-Mental Status (Chang et al., 2010) and the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test after a 10-min delay (Boone, 2000). The language screening included the 15-item Boston Naming test (Kaplan et al., 1983) and semantic verbal fluency tests. The subjects’ visual-spatial abilities were assessed by a modified Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and the number-location test from the Visual Object and Space Perception Battery (Warrington and James, 1991). In addition, the frontal lobe function was assessed using the digit-backwards.

Auditory Perceptual Tasks

We used three auditory perceptual tasks to determine the pitch, melody and environmental sound perception. The tasks were performed using a desktop computer connected to two speakers. Before the test, the examiners ensured that the volume of the speakers was sufficiently audible. Following oral instructions and trials, responses were scored on answer sheets. Repetition of the stimuli was allowed once for the environmental sound task if the participant failed to respond to the sound.

Pitch (Tasks 1) and Melody Perception Tests (Task 2)

The pitch and melody perception tests required the participants to determine whether two presenting pitches or melodies were identical or not. Instructions were given orally as follows: “I am going to play two notes (melodies). I would like you to tell me if they are the same or different. I will start by playing a sample for you.” A target tone (melody) was followed by a comparison tone (melody) that was the same or different. Participants were given two examples to practice. The examiner ensured that they understood the task and could answer either “same” or “different” after the stimulus.

For the pitch discrimination test, twenty paired tones were played. The pitches were first recorded into a musical instrument digital interface file and then converted to a wave file, using a grand piano as the playback instrument and a sound module (Roland Canvas SC-8850) as the sound source. The pitch interval between the two different tones varied from a minimum of 5.96% and a maximum of 25.9%. The time gap between two notes was 1 s. The frequency of each tone was as follows: C1 = 261.6 Hz; C#1 = 277.2 Hz; D1 = 293.7 Hz; Eb1 = 311.1 Hz; E1 = 329.6 Hz; F1 = 349.2 Hz; F#1 = 370.0 Hz; G1 = 392.0 Hz; Ab1 = 415.3 Hz; A1 = 440.0 Hz; Bb1 = 466.2 Hz; and B1 = 493.9 Hz. For the 20 paired notes, 10 were the same and the other 10 were different. They were mixed randomly to avoid analysis errors.

For the melody perception test, the scale discrimination test of the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia (MBEA) was used (Peretz et al., 2003). It was a “same-different” discrimination test with manipulation made to a comparison melody that consisted of a scale-violated tune. The pitch was modified to be out of scale, while retaining the original melodic contour; the change was salient because the modified pitch sound was out of tune. The duration of each melody lasted from 3.8 to 6.4 s [mean, 5.1 s; Peretz et al., 2003]. Subjects were required to use a “same-different” classification: on each trial, they had to judge whether the target and the comparison sequence were the same or not. After two practice trials, 31 paired melodies were presented, with each new stimulus preceded by a warning beep. The average time to perform the whole battery was 12 min.

Environmental Sound Naming and Matching Test (Task 3)

Fifty-five environmental sounds were selected as the sound stimuli (Marcell et al., 2000). These were divided into four categories: animal (n = 13), human (n = 15), musical instruments (n = 12), and environmental noise (n = 15). For each category, the sounds were matched by familiarity and complexity based on normative data (Marcell et al., 2000). First, the participants were instructed to listen to the sound and verbally answer what it represented (Marcell et al., 2000). After each response, four pictures appeared on the computer screen (Figure 1). The participants were instructed to point to the most appropriate picture corresponding to the sound even if they could name the sound. The pictures were mostly selected from the Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) set. Pictures not present in this set were drawn manually using the same black and white sketch.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Example of the environmental sound naming and matching test. The sound icon was connected to the wave file that played the sound of a car horn. The four pictures represent irrelevant living item (a dog), answer (car), category-related item (helicopter) and supra-ordinate non-living item (violin).

All of the pictures represented unique scenes corresponding to unique sounds. Ninety-four different pictures (with different picture complexity and familiarity scores) appeared randomly in various combinations. On average, each picture appeared as a stimulus or distracter 2–3 times. To further categorize the sound stimuli, the pictures were grouped in four categories using the following criteria: (1) correct natural source of the sound (car horn); (2) natural sound source within the same category of stimulus (helicopter); (3) natural sound within the living vs. non-living supra-ordinate category (musical instrument: violin); and (4) irrelevant in any category (dog). Task 3 required 30 min to complete.

MRI Acquisition

Brain MRI was obtained using a 3.0-T scanner (Excite®, GE Medical System, Milwaukee, WI, USA) equipped with echo-planar capability. The T1 inversion recovery prepared three-dimensional spoiled gradient-recalled acquisition in steady state sequence, which were prepared with the following parameters: TR 8.6, TE minimal, Prep time 400, FOV 24, slice thickness 1.5 mm, bandwidth 25, flip angle 15, 128 slabs, 256 × 256 matrix, 1 NEX, phase FOV 1, frequency direction A/P for axial sections.

Statistical Analysis

Neuropsychological and Perceptual Tasks

Statistical analysis explored differences among the groups in terms of demographic data, neuro-psychological performances, and three non-verbal perceptual tests. Statistical analysis was carried out using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) software package (version 10.05 for Windows®, SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL, USA). Because the samples were small and were not in a normal distribution, non-parametric tests were applied for comparison between sub-groups. Kruksal-Wallis test was used for overall group statistics while the Mann-Whitney test was used between two group comparisons. Pearson’s correlation or partial correlation was used to calculate the strength of the relationship between continuous variables. A p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

VBM

The VBM protocol was carried out using the Statistical Parametric Mapping 8 package1 and standard procedures. Normalized, segmented, and modulated gray matter images were spatially smoothened with a 12-mm full width at half-maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel. Age, gender, and MMSE were regarded as nuisance variables. The global level of atrophy was assessed by measuring the total intra-cranial volume in each image. The significance of each effect of interest was determined using the theory of Gaussian fields. A statistical threshold of p < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons was accepted.

Scores from the pitch perception test were not included in the analysis of VBM because of variable frequencies between each pitch interval.

For melody perception analysis, patients who scored below the cut-off point of 23 (Ayotte et al., 2002) in the MBEA scale-violated test were first selected as one group and compared with the controls to obtain the region of interest. In addition, morphometric data from the significant region of interest were extracted and correlated with task scores. A linear regression model was constructed for functional interpretation.

For the environmental sound test, a covariant-only statistical model was used to correlate picture-matching scores and gray matter volumes. All of the patients were merged into a single group, regardless of clinical diagnosis.

Results

Demographic Data

The 60 subjects were divided into four groups: 16 AD patients, 15 bv-FTD, 14 SD, and 15 controls. Of the SD group, nine were categorized as left temporal variants (SD-L) and five were right temporal variants (SD-R) based on the asymmetry of temporal lobe atrophy (Thompson et al., 2003). There were no between-group differences in age and education (Table 1). The three dementia groups had a significantly lower mini-mental status examination (MMSE) and clinical dementia rating (CDR) scores compared to the controls. The results of other neuro-psychological evaluations were summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data of participants in the study.

Pitch Perception

Patients with SD scored lowest among four groups (χ2 = 11.35, df = 3, Kruksal-Wallis test, p = 0.01) while the AD, bv-FTD, and control groups were not different. Inspection of the mean scores showed that scores of SD-R contributed to the differences (Figure 2). Further analysis between SD-R and controls showed a significant difference (χ2 = 9.71, Mann-Whitney test, p < 0.01) while SD-L was not significant (χ2 = 2.23, Mann-Whitney test, p > 0.05). Among the dementia patients, the pitch score inversely correlated with the CDR (r= −0.336, p = 0.042) but not the MMSE score (r = 0.285, p = 0.087). On the account of near-ceiling performance on the task, we did not perform the correlation analysis.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Bar graph with y-axis indicating mean and error bar as standard deviation of the pitch (maximal score = 20) or melody scores (maximal score = 31) in the dementia and control groups; *p < 0.01. AD: Alzheimer’s disease; FTD: behavior variant fronto-temporal dementia; SD-R: right temporal variant sementic dementia; SD-L: left temporal variant semantic dementia.

Melody Perception

The SD group had the lowest overall ranking among the groups in melody perception test (χ2 = 16.27, df = 3, Kruksal-Wallis test, p = 0.001). The SD-R but not the SD-L showed a significant difference with controls (χ2 = 10.1, Mann-Whitney test, p < 0.01, Figure 2). The AD, bv-FTD, and control groups were not different. There was a strong positive partial correlation between pitch and melody scores (r = 0.53, p = 0.0001) when controlling for the MMSE and CDR scores.

Environmental Sound Naming and Matching Test

Spontaneous Naming

For the spontaneous naming of environmental sounds, the SD group was the only group that showed group differences (χ2 = 25.085, df = 3, Kruksal-Wallis test, p < 0.001, Table 2, Figure 3). Total scores in environmental sound naming did not correlate with pitch scores (r = −0.36, p = 0.40) or melody scores (r = 0.02, p = 0.458) when controlling for the CDR and MMSE scores. There was no category specificity in naming scores, whether comparing the four categories or the living vs. non-living categories among the dementia groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Individual scores in the environmental sound test.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Bar graph with y-axis indicating mean and error bar as standard deviation of environmental sound naming (blue; maximal score = 55) and picture matching scores (red; maximal score = 55) in the dementia and control groups; *p < 0.01, **p < 0.001. AD: Alzheimer’s disease; FTD: behavior variant fronto-temporal dementia; SD-R: right temporal variant sementic dementia; SD-L: left temporal variant semantic dementia.

Sound-picture Matching

For sound-picture matching, the SD group ranked lowest among the four groups (χ2 = 26.66, df = 3, Kruksal-Wallis test, p < 0.001, Table 2, Figure 3) while the AD, bv-FTD, and control groups were not different. Scores in the sound-picture matching correlated positively with sound naming (r = 0.88, p = 0.001) and MMSE scores (r = 0.293, p = 0.04), but not with CDR (r= −0.055, p = 0.38), pitch (r= −0.68, p = 0.349) or melody scores (r = 0.1, p = 0.29). In each group, there was no difference in scores for the living vs. non-living supra-ordinate category or in scores for all of the four sub-categories. The SD-L group was more apt at sound matching than the SD-R group (Figure 3).

In error analysis, all of the patients made more errors by subcategorizing pictures as “same” for the living vs. non-living category rather than as “supra-ordinate” or “irrelevant.” For the SD patients, the percentage of errors were 63.4% (same), 25.2% (supra-ordinate) and 1.14% (irrelevant).

VBM with Functional Correlation

Melody Perception Test

Eleven patients scored below the cut-off point of 23 on the MBEA scale violated scores: four with AD, one bv-FTD, one SD-L and five SD-R. A voxel-wise comparison of the gray matter between the poor melody score group and the controls showed a significant gray matter loss in the right supra-marginal and angular gyri (Brodmann’s area 39 and 40), as well as in the posterior third of the right superior and middle temporal gyri (Figure 4; Table 3).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Red clusters indicate a comparison in the poor melody group (n = 11) in dementia patients with control group overlaid on the Statistical Parametric Mapping template. It shows regions of the right supra-marginal and angular, superior temporal and middle temporal areas.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Result of the Voxel-based Morphometry (VBM) analysis.

Meanwhile, morphometric data extracted from the supra-marginal gyrus were shown in Figure 5. The right supra-marginal gyrus in the poor melody score group (n = 11) was significantly atrophic compared to the patient group with scored above the cut-off values (n = 34) or with controls. Further, the correlation between melody scores and morphometric data by VBM showed that the right supra-marginal, superior and middle temporal region, and primary and secondary auditory cortex volumes were inversely correlated with melody scores (p < 0.05). However, only the right supra-marginal and superior temporal regions fitted the linear regression model (Rsq = 0.203, p = 0.005).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Morphologic data from both supra-marginal gyri in three groups. Difference (right minus left) and the numbers in the parenthesis indicated the Montreal Neurological Institute coordinate located within the supra-marginal gyrus.

Environmental Sound Naming and Matching Test

When the total scores in sound-picture matching were entered as covariates in the statistical model, there was a correlation between low scores and clusters of atrophy along the left medial temporal and bilateral anterior temporal regions (Figure 6; Table 3).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Red clusters overlaid on the Statistical Parametric Mapping template indicates atrophy in the bilateral anterior and left medial temporal regions that correlates with lower scores in the environmental sound picture matching test.

Discussion

Major Findings

From the experiment design, the pitch and melody tests are more of a perceptual test, whereas the environmental sound naming and matching test is more of a semantic association test. The results validated the initial hypothesis that right superior temporal and supra-marginal region atrophy are related to poor melody perception, while the left medial temporal and bilateral anterior temporal regions is related to environmental sound perception. In sub-group comparisons, SD is the only group that has impairment in both tasks. These anatomy-driven differences may explain why the SD-R group has significant impairment in both melody and environmental sound tasks.

Dementia Severity May Interfere with Perceptual Attention

The pitch discrimination test can also be regarded as an attention test rather than a frequency discrimination test due to the wide frequency ranges between each pitch interval in our experiment. As such, the inverse relationship between pitch score and dementia severity suggests that the auditory attention deficits as the dementia progress. Of specific notes, as the dementia subgroups in this study have no difference in CDR scores, the lower scores in SD, especially SD-R, suggest additional pitch perceptual impairment rather than mere inattention to the task. As the case numbers were small, further validation is needed.

Scale Violate Melody and Anatomy Correlation

To accurately perform on the melody perception test, the participants need not only perceive the individual pitch, but also store the incoming auditory information long enough for later comparisons. This necessary memory retention time is 3.8–6.4 s (mean, 5.1 s; Peretz et al., 2003). Because this very short implicit memory is at the “bottom” of the perceptual-cognitive system, the process by which it influences response to auditory stimuli is referred to as “bottom-up processing” and is based on immediate sensory experiences (Kubovy and Howard, 1976). In such situations, those with impaired pitch perception and those with limited musical echoic memory (Kubovy and Howard, 1976) may experience difficulties in the melody perception task. As the poor melody perception group was intermixed with different dementia diagnosis, our study results suggested the importance of the right supra-marginal and superior temporal regions for the “bottom-up processing” of violated scales in the dementia groups.

One epidemiologic study has suggested that SD-L occurs about three times more frequently than SD-R (Thompson et al., 2003). Some researchers consider that the lower prevalence of SD-R is related to tasks used in reaching a clinical diagnosis because language ability is relatively spared in the early stage of SD-R. From the functional correlation of melody scores with VBM in this study, we consider that the testing of melody perception may increase the sensitivity in detecting degenerative processes with greater right temporal lobe involvement.

Environmental Sound Perception Related to Semantic Knowledge

SD is the only group that showed impairment in environmental sound tests. The cognitive process in the environmental sound test requires higher-level processing and interaction with the long-term and semantic memory systems (i.e., “top-down processing”). Although this schema-driven grouping is not possible without prior primitive grouping, the environmental sound stimuli used here are simple so that the effect of bottom-up processing may be ignored. Clinical studies have revealed that word-finding and comprehension difficulties in SD patients are related to semantic deficits (Hodges, 2001; Hodges and Miller, 2001; Nyatsanza et al., 2003). It can be speculated that impaired environmental sounds task in SD may also be related to the inability to associate already learned sounds or pictures. A lack of correlation between environmental sound score and melody scores in our study also suggests that these two non-verbal modalities are mediated by different cortical systems. Our VBM study suggested that environmental association is more lateralized to the left hemisphere and anterior temporal regions, whereas melody perception is more on the right temporal-parietal regions.

No Category Specificity in the Environmental Perceptual Test

Neuro-psychological studies have shown that categorizing items is a basic operation of the semantic system (Gainotti, 2000). Various theoretical models have been proposed to explain the cognitive mechanism (Caramazza and Shelton, 1998; Ishai et al., 2000) but the anatomical organization of category-specific semantic information is relatively controversial. The posterior visual-association cortices (Mummery et al., 1998; Perani et al., 1999), anterior temporal lobes (Gainotti, 2000, 2015), left dorso-lateral peri-Sylvian regions, and left inferior frontal cortex (Chao et al., 1999; Devlin et al., 2002) have all been reported. However, the environmental sound study results here have not detected differences among four categories in the dementia groups, although the patients have major temporal or frontal lobe involvements.

Meanwhile, the environmental perceptual test with related anatomical correlates in this study was performed by the correlation analysis. Therefore, the finding emphasized on the left temporal structure was not driven by the patients of SD-L. According to the function MRI data on audiovisual object processing, the non-verbal audiovisual matching tasks were related to increased activation in the right fusiform area (Hocking and Price, 2009) which was not different from our result. Possible explanations for the discrepancy may be related to the shared left-lateralized network in both the verbal and nonverbal domains in conceptual processing (Thierry et al., 2003; Thierry and Price, 2006). The correct audio-visual matching of the environmental sound-picture task here required intactness for categorization, comprehension and selection with conceptions of objects. As these patients with dementia may show impairment in some of these tasks, the differences of degenerative processes may have driven part of the difference in the audio-visual networks.

Another possible explanation may be related to the compensatory mechanisms that serve as a protective buffer in the degenerative processes. The brain reserve implies differences in the quantity of available neural substrate (Freret et al., 2015) that reduces the pathological impacts (Chang et al., 2016b). Our correlation model could only point out the atrophic regions that were related to the impairment of task scores. The compensatory mechanism that also plays a role could only be delineated by the functional neuroimaging approach which is worth noting.

AD and bv-FTD Were not Different from Control in Nonverbal Sound Perception

Both the AD and bv-FTD groups have no differences with the controls in all three tasks, although they also have temporal region involvements (Rosen et al., 2002a,b). From our VBM results, both AD and bv-FTD showed relatively sparing on the anterior temporal and right superior temporal regions as compared with SD. Therefore, the ability of melody and environmental sound perception may not be affected. Other possible mechanisms include paradoxical functional facilitation from other spared Brain region (BAs; Miller et al., 2000) or relatively good integrity of the right hemisphere (Polk and Kertesz, 1993) in retaining musical abilities.

Limitation

There are several limitations to this study. First, we selected patients with mild to moderate stage dementia and each consisted of small sample sizes. The study design was to delineate the diagnostic differences. The small sample size in each diagnosis might limit the theory to a selected clinical dementia stage. Second, the conclusions with regards to the upstream and downstream relationships of auditory processing pathways were based on the literature review (Kubovy and Howard, 1976). This does not imply that all of these factors (i.e., musical echoic memory, melody task scores and pitch perceptions) are inter-related. The effect of melody task scores may be entirely independent of the effect of pitch perception or musical echoic memory. Finally, although we carefully select patients with similar age ranges, the role of aging process on nonverbal perception ability (Deal et al., 2016) may be highly variable that could form some of the discrepancies from the functional data tested in healthy younger subjects (Hocking and Price, 2009).

Conclusion

In conclusion, the anatomy of melody perception and semantic association of environmental sound explored in dementia patients show diverse cortical atrophy due to neuro-degenerative disease. This study provides lesion data suggesting that right supra-marginal and superior temporal atrophy are associated with poor melody perceptual ability. Atrophy of the left medial temporal and bilateral anterior temporal regions is linked to the impaired association of environmental sounds with words or pictures. These findings not only complement existing data on the effect of lesions, but also suggest that non-verbal tests may significantly facilitate the investigation of hemisphere laterality in the clinical setting.

Author Contributions

P-HL participated in the design of the study, drafted the manuscript and performed the statistical analysis. H-HC, N-CC, W-NC, C-WH, Y-TC, S-WH and C-WH participated in the sequence alignment, clinical evaluation of patients and helped draft the manuscript. C-CC helped to draft the work and revise it critically for important intellectual content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding and Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by grants from Chang Gung Memorial Hospital (CMRPG860171, CMRPG8D0771, CMRPG8E0541), and Ministry of Science and Technology (104-2314-B-182A-026 -MY2).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm

References

Amaducci, L., Grassi, E., and Boller, F. (2002). Maurice Ravel and right-hemisphere musical creativity: influence of disease on his last musical works? Eur. J. Neurol. 9, 75–82. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.2002.00351.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ayotte, J., Peretz, I., and Hyde, K. (2002). Congenital amusia: a group study of adults afflicted with a music-specific disorder. Brain 125, 238–251. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boone, K. B. (2000). TEST REVIEW The boston qualitative scoring system for the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 22, 430–434. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200006)22:3;1-V;FT430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burdo, S. (1989). Evoked potentials of the brain stem in normal and pathologic conditions: experience with 692 adult subjects. Acta Otorhinolaryngol. Ital. 9, 1–36.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Caramazza, A., and Shelton, J. R. (1998). Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain the animate-inanimate distinction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 1–34. doi: 10.1093/neucas/4.4.399-k

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C. C., Hsu, J. L., Chang, W. N., Huang, S. H., Huang, C. W., Chang, Y. T., et al. (2016a). Metabolic covariant network in relation to nigrostriatal degeneration in carbon monoxide intoxication-related Parkinsonism. Front. Neurosci. 10:187. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, Y. T., Huang, C. W., Chen, N. C., Lin, K. J., Huang, S. H., Chang, Y. H., et al. (2016b). Prefrontal lobe brain reserve capacity with resistance to higher global amyloid load and white matter hyperintensity burden in mild stage Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS One 11:e0149056. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C. C., Kramer, J. H., Lin, K. N., Chang, W. N., Wang, Y. L., Huang, C. W., et al. (2010). Validating the Chinese version of the verbal learning test for screening Alzheimer’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 244–251. doi: 10.1017/s1355617709991184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chao, L. L., Haxby, J. V., and Martin, A. (1999). Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 913–919. doi: 10.1038/13217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, N. C., Huang, C. W., Huang, S. H., Chang, W. N., Chang, Y. T., Lui, C. C., et al. (2015). Cognitive severity-specific neuronal degenerative network in charcoal burning suicide-related carbon monoxide intoxication: a multimodality neuroimaging study in Taiwan. Medicine (Baltimore) 94:e783. doi: 10.1097/md.0000000000000783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deal, J. A., Betz, J., Yaffe, K., Harris, T., Purchase-Helzner, E., Satterfield, S., et al. (2016). Hearing impairment and incident dementia and cognitive decline in older adults: the health ABC study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw069 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devlin, J. T., Moore, C. J., Mummery, C. J., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Phillips, J. A., Noppeney, U., et al. (2002). Anatomic constraints on cognitive theories of category specificity. Neuroimage 15, 675–685. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freret, T., Gaudreau, P., Schumann-Bard, P., Billard, J. M., and Popa-Wagner, A. (2015). Mechanisms underlying the neuroprotective effect of brain reserve against late life depression. J. Neural Transm. (Vienna) 122, S55–S61. doi: 10.1007/s00702-013-1154-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gainotti, G. (2000). What the locus of brain lesion tells us about the nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders: a review. Cortex 36, 539–559. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70537-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gainotti, G. (2015). Is the difference between right and left ATLs due to the distinction between general and social cognition or between verbal and non-verbal representations? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 51, 296–312. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galaburda, A. M., Holinger, D., Mills, D., Reiss, A., Korenberg, J. R., and Bellugi, U. (2003). Williams syndrome. A summary of cognitive, electrophysiological, anatomofunctional, microanatomical and genetic findings. Rev. Neurol. 36, S132–S137.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ghacibeh, G. A., and Heilman, K. M. (2003). Progressive affective aprosodia and prosoplegia. Neurology 60, 1192–1194. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000055870.48864.87

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godefroy, O., Leys, D., Furby, A., De Reuck, J., Daems, C., Rondepierre, P., et al. (1995). Psychoacoustical deficits related to bilateral subcortical hemorrhages. A case with apperceptive auditory agnosia. Cortex 31, 149–159. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80113-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, T. D., Rees, A., Witton, C., Cross, P. M., Shakir, R. A., and Green, G. G. (1997). Spatial and temporal auditory processing deficits following right hemisphere infarction. A psychophysical study. Brain 120, 785–794. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.5.785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, T. D., Uppenkamp, S., Johnsrude, I., Josephs, O., and Patterson, R. D. (2001). Encoding of the temporal regularity of sound in the human brainstem. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 633–637. doi: 10.1038/88459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutschalk, A., Patterson, R. D., Scherg, M., Uppenkamp, S., and Rupp, A. (2004). Temporal dynamics of pitch in human auditory cortex. Neuroimage 22, 755–766. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halpern, A. R., and O’Connor, M. G. (2000). Implicit memory for music in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 14, 391–397. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.14.3.391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hocking, J., and Price, C. J. (2009). Dissociating verbal and nonverbal audiovisual object processing. Brain Lang. 108, 89–96. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodges, J. R. (2001). Frontotemporal dementia (Pick’s disease): clinical features and assessment. Neurology 56, S6–S10. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.suppl_4.s6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodges, J. R., and Miller, B. (2001). The neuropsychology of frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. Introduction to the special topic papers: Part II. Neurocase 7, 113–121. doi: 10.1093/neucas/7.2.113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, C. W., Tsai, M. H., Chen, N. C., Chen, W. H., Lu, Y. T., Lui, C. C., et al. (2015). Clinical significance of circulating vascular cell adhesion molecule-1 to white matter disintegrity in Alzheimer’s dementia. Thromb. Haemost. 114, 1230–1240. doi: 10.1160/th14-11-0938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L. G., Martin, A., and Haxby, J. V. (2000). The representation of objects in the human occipital and temporal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 35–51. doi: 10.1162/089892900564055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., and Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea and Febiger.

Kreisler, A., Godefroy, O., Delmaire, C., Debachy, B., Leclercq, M., Pruvo, J. P., et al. (2000). The anatomy of aphasia revisited. Neurology 54, 1117–1123. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.5.1117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubovy, M., and Howard, F. P. (1976). Persistence of a pitch-segregating echoic memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2, 531–537. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.2.4.531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levitin, D. J., Cole, K., Chiles, M., Lai, Z., Lincoln, A., and Bellugi, U. (2004). Characterizing the musical phenotype in individuals with Williams Syndrome. Child Neuropsychol. 10, 223–247. doi: 10.1080/09297040490909288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcell, M. M., Borella, D., Greene, M., Kerr, E., and Rogers, S. (2000). Confrontation naming of environmental sounds. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 22, 830–864. doi: 10.1076/jcen.22.6.830.949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, B. R., Chang, C. C., De May, M., Engstrom, J., and Miller, B. L. (2009). Pleasurable emotional response to music: a case of neurodegenerative generalized auditory agnosia. Neurocase 15, 248–259. doi: 10.1080/13554790802632934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R. Jr., Kawas, C. H., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendez, M. F. (2001). Generalized auditory agnosia with spared music recognition in a left-hander. Analysis of a case with a right temporal stroke. Cortex 37, 139–150. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70563-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendez, M. F., and Geehan, G. R. Jr. (1988). Cortical auditory disorders: clinical and psychoacoustic features. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 51, 1–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.51.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, B. L., Boone, K., Cummings, J. L., Read, S. L., and Mishkin, F. (2000). Functional correlates of musical and visual ability in frontotemporal dementia. Br. J. Psychiatry 176, 458–463. doi: 10.1192/bjp.176.5.458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mummery, C. J., Patterson, K., Hodges, J. R., and Price, C. J. (1998). Functional neuroanatomy of the semantic system: divisible by what? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 766–777. doi: 10.1162/089892998563059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neary, D., Snowden, J. S., Gustafson, L., Passant, U., Stuss, D., Black, S., et al. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 51, 1546–1554. doi: 10.1212/wnl.51.6.1546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyatsanza, S., Shetty, T., Gregory, C., Lough, S., Dawson, K., and Hodges, J. R. (2003). A study of stereotypic behaviours in Alzheimer’s disease and frontal and temporal variant frontotemporal dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 74, 1398–1402. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.10.1398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oppenheimer, D. R., and Newcombe, F. (1978). Clinical and anatomic findings in a case of auditory agnosia. Arch. Neurol. 35, 712–719. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500350016004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, R. D., Uppenkamp, S., Johnsrude, I. S., and Griffiths, T. D. (2002). The processing of temporal pitch and melody information in auditory cortex. Neuron 36, 767–776. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01060-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perani, D., Schnur, T., Tettamanti, M., Gorno-Tempini, M., Cappa, S. F., and Fazio, F. (1999). Word and picture matching: a PET study of semantic category effects. Neuropsychologia 37, 293–306. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00073-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peretz, I., Belleville, S., and Fontaine, S. (1997). Dissociations between music and language functions after cerebral resection: a new case of amusia without aphasia. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 51, 354–368. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.51.4.354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peretz, I., Champod, A. S., and Hyde, K. (2003). Varieties of musical disorders. The Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 999, 58–75. doi: 10.1196/annals.1284.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peretz, I., Kolinsky, R., Tramo, M., Labrecque, R., Hublet, C., Demeurisse, G., et al. (1994). Functional dissociations following bilateral lesions of auditory cortex. Brain 117, 1283–1301. doi: 10.1093/brain/117.6.1283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piccirilli, M., Sciarma, T., and Luzzi, S. (2000). Modularity of music: evidence from a case of pure amusia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 69, 541–545. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.69.4.541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polk, M., and Kertesz, A. (1993). Music and language in degenerative disease of the brain. Brain Cogn. 22, 98–117. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1993.1027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, H. J., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Goldman, W. P., Perry, R. J., Schuff, N., Weiner, M., et al. (2002a). Patterns of brain atrophy in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. Neurology 58, 198–208. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.2.198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, H. J., Perry, R. J., Murphy, J., Kramer, J. H., Mychack, P., Schuff, N., et al. (2002b). Emotion comprehension in the temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 125, 2286–2295. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saygin, A. P., Dick, F., Wilson, S. M., Dronkers, N. F., and Bates, E. (2003). Neural resources for processing language and environmental sounds: evidence from aphasia. Brain 126, 928–945. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snodgrass, J. G., and Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity and visual complexity. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. 6, 174–215. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.6.2.174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, Y., Nakano, I., and Obayashi, T. (2002). Environmental sound recognition after unilateral subcortical lesions. Cortex 38, 69–76. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70639-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thierry, G., Giraud, A. L., and Price, C. (2003). Hemispheric dissociation in access to the human semantic system. Neuron 38, 499–506. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00199-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thierry, G., and Price, C. J. (2006). Dissociating verbal and nonverbal conceptual processing in the human brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1018–1028. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.1018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, S. A., Patterson, K., and Hodges, J. R. (2003). Left/right asymmetry of atrophy in semantic dementia: behavioral-cognitive implications. Neurology 61, 1196–1203. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000091868.28557.b8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vignolo, L. A. (2003). Music agnosia and auditory agnosia. Dissociations in stroke patients. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 999, 50–57. doi: 10.1196/annals.1284.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warrington, E. K., and James, M. (1991). Visual Object and Space Perception Battery. Bury St. Edmunds, Suffolk, UK: Thames Valley Test Co.

Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J. (1988). Pitch perception of complex tones and human temporal-lobe function. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 84, 566–572. doi: 10.1121/1.396834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegler, D. K. (1952). Word deafness and Wernicke’s aphasia; report of cases and discussion of the syndrome. AMA. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 67, 323–331. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1952.02320150056005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dementia, environmental sound, music, scale-violated melody, semantic dementia

Citation: Lin P-H, Chen H-H, Chen N-C, Chang W-N, Huang C-W, Chang Y-T, Hsu S-W, Hsu C-W and Chang C-C (2016) Anatomical Correlates of Non-Verbal Perception in Dementia Patients. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:207. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00207

Received: 20 June 2016; Accepted: 15 August 2016;
Published: 31 August 2016.

Edited by:

Aurel Popa-Wagner, University of Rostock, Germany

Reviewed by:

Guido Gainotti, Policlinico Gemelli, Italy
Raluca Sandu Vintilescu, University of Medicine and Pharmacy of Craiova, Romania

Copyright © 2016 Lin, Chen, Chen, Chang, Huang, Chang, Hsu, Hsu and Chang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chiung-Chih Chang, neur099@adm.cgmh.org.tw

Download