Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 09 December 2016
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 8 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00300

Cognitive Change during the Life Course and Leukocyte Telomere Length in Late Middle-Aged Men

Lene Rask1,2,3* Laila Bendix4 Maria Harbo5 Birgitte Fagerlund6 Erik L. Mortensen3,7 Martin J. Lauritzen1,2,3* Merete Osler7,8
  • 1Department of Neuroscience and Pharmacology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 2Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, Rigshospitalet - Glostrup, Glostrup, Denmark
  • 3Center for Healthy Aging, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 4Pain Research Group, Department of Anaesthesiology and Intensive Care Medicine, Odense University Hospital, Odense, Denmark
  • 5Department of Clinical Genetics, Vejle Hospital, Vejle, Denmark
  • 6Center for Neuropsychiatric Schizophrenia Research and Lundbeck Foundation Centre for Clinical Intervention and Neuropsychiatric Schizophrenia Research, University of Copenhagen, Psychiatric Centre Glostrup, Glostrup, Denmark
  • 7Department of Public Health, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 8Research Center for Prevention and Health, Rigshospitalet - Glostrup, Glostrup, Denmark

Importance: Cognitive skills are known to decline through the lifespan with large individual differences. The molecular mechanisms for this decline are incompletely understood. Although leukocyte telomere length provides an index of cellular age that predicts the incidence of age-related diseases, it is unclear whether there is an association between cognitive decline and leukocyte telomere length.

Objective: To examine the association between changes in cognitive function during adult life and leukocyte telomere length after adjusting for confounding factors such as education, mental health and life style.

Design, Setting, and Participants: Two groups of men with negative (n = 97) and positive (n = 93) change in cognitive performance were selected from a birth cohort of 1985 Danish men born in 1953. Cognitive performance of each individual was assessed at age ~20 and 56 years. Leukocyte telomere length at age ~58 was measured using qPCR. Linear regression models were used to investigate the association between cognitive function and leukocyte telomere length.

Results: Men with negative change in cognitive performance during adult life had significantly shorter mean leukocyte telomere length than men with positive change in cognitive performance (unadjusted difference β = −0.09, 95% CI −0.16 to −0.02, p = 0.02). This association remained significant after adjusting for smoking, alcohol consumption, leisure time activity, body mass index (BMI) and cholesterol (adjusted difference β = −0.09, 95% CI −0.17 to −0.01, p = 0.02) but was non-significant after adjusting for smoking, alcohol consumption, leisure time activity, BMI, cholesterol, current cognitive function, depression and education (adjusted difference β = −0.07, 95% CI −0.16 to −0.01, p = 0.08).

Conclusion and Relevance: Preclinical cognitive changes may be associated with leukocyte telomere length.

Introduction

Cognitive performance seems to decline during adult life under the influence of genetic and environmental factors (Deary et al., 2009; Tucker-Drob and Briley, 2014). It has been suggested that cognitive decline is caused by a self-perpetuating cycle of oxidative stress triggering neuroinflammation, which further promotes oxidative stress as well as the suggested underlying neurodegeneration and cell death (Cai et al., 2013). Recently, cognitive decline was associated with leukocyte telomere length (LTL) and the LTL is assumed to reflect the accumulated burden of inflammation and oxidative stress occurring during the life course (Eitan et al., 2014). This relationship may therefore be influenced by same mechanisms such as oxidative stress and inflammation but the association may also be bi-directional.

In a number of cross-sectional studies (outlined in Supplementary Table 1), mainly among adults above 60 years, short LTL has been associated with cognitive impairment (Valdes et al., 2010; Der et al., 2012; Ma et al., 2013). Other studies have found no correlation between LTL and cognitive function (Harris et al., 2006; Bendix et al., 2011, 2014a; Cohen-Manheim et al., 2016). Furthermore, two cohort studies have found that short LTL predicts cognitive decline (Martin-Ruiz et al., 2006; Yaffe et al., 2011). In the Nurses' Health Study, short LTL in women above age 70 was modestly associated with subsequent cognitive decline (Devore et al., 2011), and in 70–79 year old adults from the Health ABC study short LTL predicted a 7 year decline in Mini Mental Test score (Yaffe et al., 2011). In two cohort studies, however, cognitive decline did not predict subsequent differences in LTL. In 70-year olds from the Lothian 1936 birth cohort, LTL at age 70 was not associated with cognitive changes occurring between ages 11 and 70 (Harris et al., 2012), nor was there any association between cognitive decline and LTL in an Australian population-based study (Mather et al., 2010).

The previous cohort studies have examined the relationship between LTL and cognitive decline in follow-ups of 2–7 years (Martin-Ruiz et al., 2006; Mather et al., 2010; Devore et al., 2011; Yaffe et al., 2011), mostly in persons above 70 years (Martin-Ruiz et al., 2006; Devore et al., 2011; Yaffe et al., 2011; Harris et al., 2012) (Supplementary Table 1). Thus, it remains uncertain as to whether or not LTL is a marker of cognitive decline in early aging.

The aim of the present study was to examine the association of change in cognitive function occurring between age ~20 and 56 with LTL measured at age ~58, as well as to investigate the extent to which potentially confounding factors: smoking, alcohol consumption, leisure time activity, Body Mass Index (BMI), serum cholesterol, indicators of mental function and education might influence any such association.

Materials and Methods

Study Population

Participants were recruited from the Metropolit 1953 Danish male birth cohort, which includes 11,532 men born in 1953 in Copenhagen (Osler et al., 2009). These men were all cognitively assessed at age ~20 and again in 2009–10 at age ~56 as part of the Copenhagen Aging and Midlife Biobank project (CAMB) (Avlund et al., 2014; Hansen et al., 2014). Among the 1985 members of the Metropolit cohort who had participated in both cognitive tests, a linear regression analysis was carried out with the cognitive test score at age ~20 as single predictor and the cognitive test score at age ~56 as outcome. Each individual's standardized residual from the regression line was considered a measure of the relative cognitive change between the two tests for that particular individual. To ensure sufficient variance in the magnitude of cognitive change, we invited the participants with the largest positive residuals [termed group 1 (residual range 0.96–2.80)] and the largest negative residuals [termed group 2 (residual range −2.83 to −0.99)] to participate in the study. Outliers defined as individuals with standardized residuals larger than 3 were excluded. In total, 552 participants were invited by letter to participate in a neurophysiological examination and blood sampling (249 in group 1; 303 in group 2). Based on participant interviews 36 persons were excluded (abuse of alcohol or drugs, certain neurological disorders, certain neurological, mental or psychiatric diagnoses, certain depressive diseases, former traumatic brain injury or contraindication of MR scanning). Of the remaining participants, 207 (102 in group 1; 105 in group 2) chose to participate in the examination period from 2010 to 2013 (~58 years). LTL was not measured for 13 participants (7 and 6 from group 1 and 2, respectively) (due to lack of blood material or unknown reasons). One included participant had a persistent depression (group 2). Within-group comparisons of residuals showed no differences between included participants, participants who were invited but did not want to participate, and those who were excluded. The inclusion procedure is described in detail in previous publications (e.g., Hansen et al., 2014).

The study was approved by the local ethical committee (De Videnskabsetiske Komiteer for Region Hovedstaden). All participants provided written informed consent. The study has also been registered by the Danish Data Protection Agency.

Measurements

Cognitive Change

At age ~20 years the participants completed the Danish draft board intelligence test Børge Prien's Prøve (BPP) as a part of the draft board assessment program. The 45-min BPP test comprises letter matrices, verbal analogies, number series, and geometric figures. The score, which is the total number of correct answers out of 78 questions, has proved to correlate to 0.82 with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Teasdale, 2009). In the CAMB follow-up examination in 2009/2010 participants completed a short version of the Intellegenz-Struktur-test 2000R (IST), which matches the BPP test and comprises verbal analogies, number series, and sentence completion (Osler et al., 2013; Mortensen et al., 2014). Relative cognitive change was assessed by standardized residual from regression analyses on the two tests as described above in the section “Study population.”

Leukocyte Telomere Length (LTL)

Non-fasting blood samples were collected during the examination day. Buffy coat was prepared from whole blood and DNA was purified using Maxwell system for automated extraction from buffy coat samples. DNA purity was measured based on absorbance at 260 and 280 nm (NanoDrop Spectrophotometer) and all samples had a 260/280 nm ratio of 1.8–2.0.

LTL measurement was carried out with an adaptation of the qPCR methods described by Cawthon (2002, 2009). The method is described and validated in more detail elsewhere (Bendix et al., 2014b).

For measurement of telomere repeat copies (T), primers Telg—5′ -ACACTAAGGTTTGGGTTTGGGTTT GGGTTTGGGTTAGTGT-3′ and Telc—5′-TGTTAGGTATCCC TATCCCTATCCCTATCCCTATCCCTAACA-3′ were used. The single copy gene (S) chosen was TaqMan Copy Number Reference Assay RNase P (Applied Biosystems Inc., CA, USA). The qPCR was performed on a 7900HT Fast Real-Time PCR System in 384-plates. Analyses were carried out using the 7900HT Sequence Detection System (SDS) version 2.3 (Applied Biosystems INC., CA, USA). Cycling conditions for telomere runs were 50°C for 2 min, 95°C for 2 min, followed by two cycles of 95°C for 15 s, 52°C for 15 s, and 36 cycles of 95°C for 15 s, 62°C for 15 s, and 71°C for 15 s. LTL was measured in T/S ratio.

LTL was measured twice in two independent batches (n = 95 group 1 and n = 99 group 2).

All the analyses presented in this paper were carried out separately on each of the two telomere measurements (12 results failed for one measurement and 4 results failed for the other measurement). As the results were nearly identical only data for one of the measurements is presented (n = 93 group 1 and n = 97 group 2).

Covariables

A clinical neurocognitive test battery was used to assess functions proved to be sensitive to normal and pathological cognitive decline related to aging. For this study we included the Addenbrooke's (ACE) test (Mathuranath et al., 2000), a brief cognitive screening battery that is sensitive to mild cognitive impairment. As depressive symptoms might influence cognitive performance, data from the Major Depression Inventory (MDI), which measures the frequency of 12 depressive symptoms over the past 2 weeks (Bech et al., 2001) collected in the CAMB study, were included. The two mental scores were analyzed as continuous variables. In addition, covariables which have been associated with cognitive decline and LTL (information on vocational education, current smoking status, alcohol consumption, physical activity during leisure, BMI and serum cholesterol) were included in the analyses. These variables were obtained in the CAMB examination in 2009-10 (participants aged ~56 year).

Statistical Analysis

Differences in LTL and covariables between the two cognitive groups were analyzed by t-test and univariate linear regression models for categorical variables. We then conducted series of multiple regression analyses to evaluate the individual and mutually adjusted influences of the potentially confounding covariables (smoking, alcohol consumption, leisure time activity, BMI, serum cholesterol, MDI, ACE, and vocational education) on the association between the two groups with different change in cognitive function and LTL. In the first regression model we included smoking, alcohol, leisure time activity, BMI and cholesterol. Subsequently ACE and MDI were added while vocational education was also included in the final model. The basic assumption for linear regression [linearity, normality and colinarity (Variance Inflation Factor)] was examined using the diagnostic facilities in STATA.

Results

Due to selection procedure, there was no difference between the two groups on BPP scores (measured ~20 years), but substantial differences on the IST scores (measured ~56 years). Group 2 also obtained lower ACE scores, had less education, higher cholesterol and higher MDI depression scores, while there was no difference between the groups with regard to prevalence of smoking, sedentary leisure time activity, alcohol consumption or BMI (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean leukocyte telomere length (LTL) (age ~58) and the distribution of covariables in 190 middle-aged men with positive (Group 1) and negative (Group 2) cognitive change between age ~20 and 56 years.

Mean LTL measured at age ~58 was shortest in group 2, i.e., in the participants with negative residuals reflecting relative cognitive decline (P < 0.01) (Table 1). The covariables were not or only moderately associated with LTL (Table 1, last column). In the regression model (Table 2) with LTL as outcome and group as main predictor participants with negative change in cognitive performance during adult life had shorter mean LTL than did participants with positive change in cognitive performance (unadjusted difference β = −0.09, 95% CI −0.16 to −0.02, p = 0.02). This association remained significant after adjusting for smoking, alcohol consumption, leisure time activity, BMI and cholesterol (adjusted difference β = −0.09, 95% CI −0.17 to −0.01, p = 0.02) and a p-value of 0.05 was obtained after adjusting for smoking, alcohol consumption, leisure time activity, BMI, cholesterol, MDI and ACE (β = −0.08, 95% CI −0.16 to −0.00, p = 0.05). Association was non-significant after adjusting for smoking, alcohol consumption, leisure time activity, BMI, cholesterol, current cognitive function, depression and education (adjusted difference β = −0.07, 95% CI −0.16 to −0.01, p = 0.08).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The relationship in beta-coefficient and 95% confidence intervals (CI) of negative cognitive change (Group 2) vs. positive cognitive change (Group 1) between age ~20 and 56 years with leukocyte telomere length (LTL) (age ~58) in 190 middle-aged Danish men.

Discussion

In the present study we investigated the association between age-related change in cognitive function and LTL. In a sample of middle-aged men selected on the basis of change in cognitive function between age ~20 and 56, LTL measured at age ~58 was significantly shorter in the group with negative change in cognitive function than it was in the group with a positive change. This relationship attenuated after adjustment for education, smoking, alcohol consumption, BMI, leisure time activity, serum cholesterol and scores for current cognitive function and depression. Repeated measurements of LTL were not available. Therefore, association of cognitive decline and LTL measured at different ages can't be examined or whether cognitive decline precede changes in LTL. Furthermore, the effect of the current cognitive function measured by the ACE test on LTL compared to the impact of cognitive changes over the life course on the telomere length can't be sufficiently concluded. These results do not concur with another study in the literature of ~1000 relatively healthy men and women from Scotland in which no relationship was found between LTL and changes in cognitive function over the life course (Harris et al., 2012). As noted by the authors of this study, their population was socially homogenous and the variance possibly too limited to detect any association. Two other cohort studies with shorter follow-up times found that short LTL predicted cognitive decline in old age (Devore et al., 2011; Yaffe et al., 2011), while a previous study on geriatric patients with a mean age of 85 did not find that low LTL predicted progression from normal to mild cognitive impairment after 2 years of follow-up (Zekry et al., 2010).

The direction of the association between cognition and telomere length is unclear. The association may be bi-directional or it may be caused by related underlying processes. As an example increasing obesity is negatively correlated with cognitive performance supported by both human clinical and animal studies but the mechanisms have not been fully clarified. Studies have suggested obesity to negatively impact cerebral vascular function, hippocampal brain atrophy, blood-brain barrier functions and inflammation contributing to cognitive deficits (Nguyen et al., 2014). Furthermore, telomere length has been suggested to be affected by obesity among other factors such as smoking, physical activity, and alcohol consumption (Mather et al., 2011). On the other hand, telomere length may impact pulse pressure and the risk of stroke which in turn may lead to cognitive impairment. An comprehensive causal explanation of these associations and the importance of each involved factor have not been fully clarified (Jeanclos et al., 2000; Waldstein et al., 2008; Cai et al., 2013; Eitan et al., 2014; D'Mello et al., 2015).

The strength of the present study is that it was based on prospectively collected data on cognitive performance in a sample of men with no clinical signs of mental disorder or dementia. To obtain sufficient variance, the two groups were selected to consist of men with extreme residuals, i.e., those with the most negative and positive relative change in cognitive function. In order to account for variation in LTL measurements, qPCR measurements were repeated in two independent batches. Results from the analyses based on each of the two measurements showed the same pattern of associations. The assessment of cognitive function was conducted with two different tests at age ~20 (BPP) and at age ~56 (IST), but with an almost 40 year retest interval a correlation of 0.70 has been observed between the two tests in the larger cohort. This would suggest that errors of measurement and differences between the two tests contribute relatively little variance and that the residuals reflect true change in global cognitive function (Osler et al., 2013). It would have been advantageous for the present study if longitudinal LTL data were available. Longitudinal LTL data would provide information whether individual short telomere length was innate or had in fact decreased during life due to for example inflammation and oxidative stress. Moreover, repeated measurements of LTL and of cognitive abilities across lifespan would provide valuable insight into the onset and rate of changes as well as strength of relationship between cognition and LTL at different stages of life.

Another limitation of the study is the measurement of telomere length in a heterogeneous blood sample and the used method doesn't discriminate between telomere lengths in different cell types or different chromosomes. It would have been advantageous to measure telomere length in neural tissue such as neurons or glial cells if materials like this were available. An additional benefit for the study could be to measure telomere length not just by one method to correct for PCR measurement uncertainty.

A recent study based on the Jerusalem Lipid Research Clinic study has examined longitudinal LTL data and association with cognitive ability (Cohen-Manheim et al., 2016). The LTL dynamics was measured over 13 years in young adults (measured at ages 28–32 and 41–46 years) and compared with cognitive function at ages 48–52. In this study increased LTL shortening was found to be associated with poorer cognitive function. However, another new study measuring telomere attrition and cognitive function at later ages (from 70 to 76 years and from 79 to 92 years) did not find the rate of telomere attrition to correlate with the rate of cognitive decline at these high ages (Harris et al., 2016). The rate of LTL changes may depend on different factors that vary across the life span and telomere length dynamics may differ at different stages in life. Therefore, it is likely that the association with age-related cognitive decline will change according to the timing of telomere analysis and age at cognitive assessment (Mather et al., 2011).

In conclusion, our study suggests that preclinical cognitive changes in middle-aged men are associated with LTL at age 58.

Author Contributions

LR: Design of the work, analysis and interpretation of data for the work. Revision of the work for intellectual content and according to referee suggestions. LB: Design of the work, analysis and interpretation of data for the work. Revision of the work for intellectual content. MH: Experimental work and analysis of data for the work. Revision of the work for intellectual content. BF: Design of the work and interpretation of data for the work. Revision of the work for intellectual content. EM: Conception and design of the work and interpretation of data for the work. Revision of the work for intellectual content. ML: Conception and supervision of the work. Revision of the work for intellectual content. MO: Conception and design of the work including statistical calculations, analysis and interpretation of data for the work. Drafting and revision of the work for intellectual content and according to referee suggestions.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all study participants for valuable contribution to the study and all researchers involved in data collection for the project. We thank Louise Juhl Boni, Ulla Seehusen Kruchov van Deurs, Naja Liv Hansen, and Katja Linda Waller for collection of cognitive data. We are grateful to Catherine Aubel for her careful proofreading of the manuscript. This work was funded by Nordea Fonden. The Copenhagen Aging and Midlife Biobank project has been supported by a generous grant from the Velux Foundation. Metropolit: We thank all those who initiated and/or continued the Metropolit study: K Svalastoga, E Høgh, P Wolf, T Rishøj, G Strande-Sørensen, E Manniche, B Holten, IA Weibull, and A Ortman. CAMB: The authors also thank the staff at the Department of Public Health and the National Research Center for the Working Environment who undertook the data collection for the Copenhagen Aging and Midlife Biobank (CAMB). Further thanks to Helle Bruunsgaard, Nils-Erik Fiehn, Åse Marie Hansen, Poul Holm-Pedersen, and Rikke Lund who initiated and established the Copenhagen Aging and Midlife Biobank from 2009 to 2011 together with Kirsten Avlund, EM and MO. The authors acknowledge the crucial role of the initiators and steering groups of the Metropolit Cohort, The Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort and The Danish Longitudinal Study on Work Unemployment and Health.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00300/full#supplementary-material

References

Avlund, K., Osler, M., Mortensen, E. L., Christensen, U., Bruunsgaard, H., Holm-Pedersen, P., et al. (2014). Copenhagen aging and midlife biobank (CAMB): an introduction. J. Aging Health 26, 5–20. doi: 10.1177/0898264313509277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bech, P., Rasmussen, N. A., Olsen, L. R., Noerholm, V., and Abildgaard, W. (2001). The sensitivity and specificity of the Major Depression Inventory, using the Present State Examination as the index of diagnostic validity. J. Affect. Disord. 66, 159–164. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00309-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bendix, L., Gade, M. M., Staun, P. W., Kimura, M., Jeune, B., Hjelmborg, J. V., et al. (2011). Leukocyte telomere length and physical ability among danish twins age 70+. Mech. Ageing Dev. 132, 568–572. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2011.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bendix, L., Thinggaard, M., Fenger, M., Kolvraa, S., Avlund, K., Linneberg, A., et al. (2014b). Longitudinal changes in leukocyte telomere length and mortality in humans. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 69, 231–239. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bendix, L., Thinggaard, M., Kimura, M., Aviv, A., Christensen, K., Osler, M., et al. (2014a). Association of leucocyte telomere length with fatigue in non-disabled older adults. J. Aging Res. 2014:403253. doi: 10.1155/2014/403253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, Z., Yan, L. J., and Ratka, A. (2013). Telomere shortening and Alzheimer's disease. Neuromol. Med. 15, 25–48. doi: 10.1007/s12017-012-8207-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cawthon, R. M. (2002). Telomere measurement by quantitative PCR. Nucleic Acids Res. 30:e47. doi: 10.1093/nar/30.10.e47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cawthon, R. M. (2009). Telomere length measurement by a novel monochrome multiplex quantitative PCR method. Nucleic Acids Res. 37:e21. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkn1027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen-Manheim, I., Doniger, G. M., Sinnreich, R., Simon, E. S., Pinchas, R., Aviv, A., et al. (2016). Increased attrition of leukocyte telomere length in young adults is associated with poorer cognitive function in midlife. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 31, 147–157. doi: 10.1007/s10654-015-0051-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deary, I. J., Corley, J., Gow, A. J., Harris, S. E., Houlihan, L. M., Marioni, R. E., et al. (2009). Age-associated cognitive decline. Br. Med. Bullet. 92, 135–152. doi: 10.1093/bmb/ldp033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Der, G., Batty, G. D., Benzeval, M., Deary, I. J., Green, M. J., McGlynn, L., et al. (2012). Is telomere length a biomarker for aging: cross-sectional evidence from the west of Scotland? PLoS ONE 7:e45166. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devore, E. E., Prescott, J., De Vivo, I., and Grodstein, F. (2011). Relative telomere length and cognitive decline in the Nurses' Health Study. Neurosci. Lett. 492, 15–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.01.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D'Mello, M. J., Ross, S. A., Briel M Anand, S. S., Gerstein, H., and Paré, G. (2015). Association between shortened leukocyte telomere length and cardiometabolic outcomes: systematic review and meta-analysis. Circ. Cardiovasc. Genet. 8, 82–90. doi: 10.1161/CIRCGENETICS.113.000485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eitan, E., Hutcison, E. R., and Mattson, M. P. (2014). Telomere shortening in neurological disorders: an abundance of unanswered questions. Cell Press 37, 256–262. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, N. L., Lauritzen, M., Mortensen, E. L., Osler, M., Avlund, K., Fagerlund, B., et al. (2014). Subclinical cognitive decline in middle-age is associated with reduced task-induced deactivation of the brain's default mode network. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4488–4498. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, S. E., Deary, I. J., MacIntyre, A., Lamb, K. J., Radhakrishnan, K., Starr, J. M., et al. (2006). The association between telomere length, physical health, cognitive ageing and mortality in non-demented people. Neurosci. Lett. 406, 260–264. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.07.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, S. E., Marioni, R. E., Martin-Ruiz, C., Pattie, A., Gow, A. J., Cox, S. R., et al. (2016). Longitudinal telomere length shortening and cognitive and physical decline in later life: the Lothian Birth Cohorts 1936 and 1921. Mech. Ageing Dev. 154, 43–48. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2016.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, S. E., Martin-Ruiz, C., von Zglinicki, T., Starr, J. M., and Deary, I. J. (2012). Telomere length and aging biomarkers in 70-year olds: the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 1486e3-8. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeanclos, E., Schork, N. J., Kyvik, K. O., Kimura, M., Skurmick, J. H., and Aviv, A. (2000). Telomere length inversely correlates with pulse pressure and is highly familial. Hypertension 36, 195–200. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.36.2.195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, S. L., Lau, E. S., Suen, E. W., Lam, L. C., Leung, P. C., Woo, J., et al. (2013). Telomere length and cognitive function in southern Chinese community-dwelling male elders. Age Ageing 42, 450–455. doi: 10.1093/ageing/aft036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin-Ruiz, C., Dickinson, H. O., Keys, B., Rowan, E., Kenny, R. A., and Von Zglinicki, T. (2006). Telomere length predicts poststroke mortality, dementia, and cognitive decline. Ann. Neurol. 6, 174–180. doi: 10.1002/ana.20869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, K. A., Jorm, A. F., Anstey, K. J., Milburn, P. J., Easteal, S., and Christensen, H. (2010). Cognitive performance and leukocyte telomere length in two narrow age cohorts: a population study. BMC Geriatr. 10:62. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-10-62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, K. A., Jorm, A. F., Parslow, R. A., and Christensen, H. (2011). Is telomere length a biomarker of aging? A review. J Gerontol A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 66, 202–213. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathuranath, P. S., Nestor, P. J., Berrios, G. E., Rakowicz, W., and Hodges, J. R. (2000). A brief cognitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 55, 1613–1620. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000434309.85312.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mortensen, E. L., Flensborg-Madsen, T., Molbo, D., Fagerlund, B., Christensen, U., Lund, R., et al. (2014). The relationship between cognitive ability and demographic factors in late midlife. J. Aging Health 26, 37–53. doi: 10.1177/0898264313508780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, J. C., Killcross, A. S., and Jenkings, T. A. (2014). Obesity and cognitive decline: role of inflammation and vascular changes. Front. Neurosci. 8:375. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osler, M., Avlund, K., and Mortensen, E. L. (2013). Socio-economic position early in life, cognitive development and cognitive change from young adulthood to middle age. Eur. J. Public Health 23, 974–980. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cks140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osler, M., Lund, R., Christensen, U., and Andersen, A. M. (2009). Cohort profile: the metropolit 1953 danish male birth cohort. Int. J. Epidemiol. 35, 541–545. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyi300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teasdale, T. W. (2009). The Danish draft board's intelligence test, Børge Priens Prøve: psychometric properties and research applications through 50 years. Scand. J. Psychol. 50, 633–638. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00789.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker-Drob, E. M., and Briley, D. A. (2014). Continuity of genetic and environmental influences on cognition across the life span: a meta-analysis of longitudinal twin and adoption studies. Psychol. Bull. 140, 949–979. doi: 10.1037/a0035893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdes, A. M., Deary, I., Gardner, J., Kimura, M., Lu, X., Spector, T. D., et al. (2010). Leucocyte telomere length is associated with cognitive performance in healthy women. Neurobiol. Ageing 31, 986–992. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldstein, S. R., Rice, S. C., Thayer, J. F., Najjar, S. S., Scuteri, A., and Zonderman, A. B. (2008). Pulse pressure and pulse wave velocity are related to cognitive decline in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Hypertension 51, 99–104. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.107.093674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yaffe, K., Lindquist, K., Kluse, M., Cawthon, R., Harris, T., Hsueh, W. C., et al. (2011). Telomere length and cognitive function in community-dwelling elders: findings from the Health ABC study. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 2055–2060. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zekry, D., Hermann, F. R., Irminger-Finger, I., Ortolan, L., Genet, C., Vitale, A. M., et al. (2010). Telomere length is not predictive of dementia or MCI conversion in the oldest old. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 719–720. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive function, telomere length, mental health, birth cohort study, aging

Citation: Rask L, Bendix L, Harbo M, Fagerlund B, Mortensen EL, Lauritzen MJ and Osler M (2016) Cognitive Change during the Life Course and Leukocyte Telomere Length in Late Middle-Aged Men. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:300. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00300

Received: 08 July 2016; Accepted: 23 November 2016;
Published: 09 December 2016.

Edited by:

Junming Wang, University of Mississippi Medical Center School of Dentistry, USA

Reviewed by:

Alexander Vaiserman, Institute of Gerontology (NAN Ukraine), Ukraine
Sara Hägg, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden

Copyright © 2016 Rask, Bendix, Harbo, Fagerlund, Mortensen, Lauritzen and Osler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lene Rask, lene.rask.01@regionh.dk
Martin J. Lauritzen, martin.johannes.lauritzen@regionh.dk

Download