Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 12 December 2014
Sec. Individual and Social Behaviors
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00430

State-dependent μ-opioid modulation of social motivation

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
  • 2Department of Physiology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
  • 3The Intervention Centre, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway

Social mammals engage in affiliative interactions both when seeking relief from negative affect and when searching for pleasure and joy. These two motivational states are both modulated by μ-opioid transmission. The μ-opioid receptor (MOR) system in the brain mediates pain relief and reward behaviors, and is implicated in social reward processing and affiliative bonding across mammalian species. However, pharmacological manipulation of the μ-opioid system has yielded opposite effects on rodents and primates: in rodents, social motivation is generally increased by MOR agonists and reduced by antagonists, whereas the opposite pattern has been shown in primates. Here, we address this paradox by taking into account differences in motivational state. We first review evidence for μ-opioid mediation of reward processing, emotion regulation, and affiliation in humans, non-human primates, rodents and other species. Based on the consistent cross-species similarities in opioid functioning, we propose a unified, state-dependent model for μ-opioid modulation of affiliation across the mammalian species. Finally, we show that this state-dependent model is supported by evidence from both rodent and primate studies, when species and age differences in social separation response are taken into account.

Introduction

It is a popular belief that endorphins make us feel good. Surprisingly little evidence for this notion can be found in the scientific literature, however. It is well established that μ-opioids help us to regulate pain (Zubieta et al., 2001). The importance of the μ-opioid receptor (MOR) system for reward processes in non-human animals is also well documented (e.g., Berridge and Kringelbach, 2013; Laurent et al., 2014). In contrast, little evidence links endorphin release to the experience of positive emotion in humans (but see these molecular imaging studies; Boecker et al., 2008; Koepp et al., 2009; Hsu et al., 2013). Here, we propose a model for opioid modulation of social well-being across mammalian species. There are three main types of opioid receptors in the brain, μ-, δ - and κ-receptors, and although there is increasing evidence implicating the δ - and κ-receptors in social behavior (Lutz and Kieffer, 2013; Resendez and Aragona, 2013), this review will focus on the considerably more studied μ-opioid system. The μ-opioid receptor (MOR) system is known to interact with the dopamine system in brain regions implicated in reward processing (e.g., Hagelberg et al., 2002; Lintas et al., 2011; Colasanti et al., 2012). The MOR system is also proposed to interact with oxytocin and dopamine in social bonding and social reward processing (Depue and Morrone-Strupinsky, 2005; Tops et al., 2014). The neural processes modulating and mediating social interactions are without doubt very complex. By focusing here on state-dependent μ-opioid modulation of social motivation we aim to further the understanding of how basic reward processing influences social interactions.

Engaging in affiliative interactions such as social play and social grooming is associated with endogenous μ-opioid release in brain reward circuitry in both rodents and primates (see Figure 1). In juvenile rats, rough-and-tumble play leads to increased central μ-opioid receptor activation, as suggested by both in vitro (Panksepp and Bishop, 1981) and in vivo studies (Vanderschuren et al., 1995e). In non-human primates, social grooming can elicit endogenous μ-opioid release, as demonstrated in a study where large increases in cerebrospinal levels of β-endorphins were observed following social grooming (Keverne et al., 1989). Both social play and grooming involve a great deal of tactile contact, and facilitate social joy and comfort (for a quick introduction to the most relevant social touch behaviors mentioned in this review, see Textbox 1). Grooming-induced MOR activation is thought to be a key facilitator of long-term relationship formation and maintenance in species of non-human primates that frequently engage in mutual partner-specific social grooming (Dunbar, 2010; Machin and Dunbar, 2011). Intriguingly, a series of studies report that adult mice engage more in social grooming when they interact with siblings, and subsequently display larger μ-opioid-mediated decreases in pain sensitivity, as compared to mice interacting with unrelated cage mates (D'Amato and Pavone, 1993, 1996; D'Amato, 1998). These findings suggest that endogenous μ-opioid responses to social interactions vary according to the nature and quality of the relationship between animals engaging with each other. MOR mechanisms have long been hypothesized to play a central role in reward processes promoting and maintaining affiliation (Depue and Morrone-Strupinsky, 2005; Machin and Dunbar, 2011). However, the question of whether brain endorphin release is either a prerequisite that causes attachment and social motivation, or is something that follows as an effect of social contact and successful bonding—or perhaps both—is still being explored.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Reward circuits in the brain. (A) Reward related brain areas in the rodent and human brain. These circuits are densely innervated with μ-opioid receptors, in addition to dopamine receptors and receptors for other neurotransmitters (Kringelbach and Berridge, 2009). Reward can be parsed into three components: hedonic “liking,” motivational “wanting” and cognitive “learning” (Berridge and Robinson, 2003; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2008; Berridge et al., 2009), and similarly to other primary rewards such as food or drugs, social rewards create feelings of wellbeing and pleasure (“liking”); motivate approach behaviors (“wanting”) and elicit associative learning of socially relevant cues (“learning”) (Trezza et al., 2011a). Reprinted from Kringelbach and Berridge (2009), with permission from Elsevier. (B) Hedonic hotspots have been identified in the rodent nucleus accumbens (NAc) and ventral pallidum (VP). Microinjections of the specific μ-opioid agonist DAMGO within the hotspots generate or intensify “liking”-responses to sweet taste rewards (Berridge and Kringelbach, 2013; Castro and Berridge, 2014), while injecting DAMGO into striatal areas outside these hotspots selectively enhances “wanting” without affecting “liking” (Mahler and Berridge, 2012) similarly to what is found with dopamine injections both within and outside of the hotspots (Peciña and Berridge, 2013). Reprinted from Berridge et al. (2009), with permission from Elsevier.

Textbox 1: Affiliative touch behaviors in rodents and non-human primates.

Throughout the life span of social animals, touch fosters and communicates intimacy and cooperation, and works as a tangible sign of social support (Hertenstein et al., 2006). Touch interactions such as grooming, huddling, and playing are common in most mammalian species, and aid establishment and maintenance of social, romantic, sexual and parental relationships. Here we outline the touch behaviors that are the most commonly observed in studies of μ-opioid involvement in social comfort and affiliation.

Social grooming in non-human primates forms the basis of long term relationships that provide social support and protection (Dunbar, 1980; Seyfarth and Cheney, 1984). Primate grooming involves sweeping through someone else's fur with hands and lips and removing impurities—the relaxing effect can be measured physiologically, e.g., reduced heart rate (Dunbar, 2010), as well as behaviorally, e.g., reduced stress-related scratching (Schino et al., 1988). Rodent maternal licking and grooming of the pup is central to mother-infant bond formation (Broad et al., 2006) and supports the development of resilient stress regulation systems (Liu et al., 1997; Caldji et al., 1998, 2000; Weaver et al., 2004) and is also associated with sociability in adolescence (Moore and Power, 1992; Parent and Meaney, 2008; Van Hasselt et al., 2012).

www.frontiersin.org

Photo by Dan Shouse is licenced under CC-BY-NC-ND 2.0.

Huddling can be defined as motionless physical contact, and typically occurs when animals are resting side by side. This mediates pair bonding in adult prairie voles (Burkett et al., 2011) and sibling bonding in rat pups (Alberts, 2007); aids infant well-being in primates (Harlow and Zimmermann, 1959), and homeostatic regulation in infant rodents (Nelson and Panksepp, 1998). Rat pups respond to separation from the huddle group with distress behaviors such as ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs), which are greatly attenuated by reinstatement of physical contact (Hofer and Shair, 1987; Stanton et al., 1987; Carden and Hofer, 1992; Hofer et al., 1993).

www.frontiersin.org

Photo by braindamaged217 is licenced under CC-BY-NC-ND 2.0.

Social play involves a great deal of tactile contact, and inhibiting the sense of touch with a local anesthetic significantly reduces playing in juvenile rats (Siviy and Panksepp, 1987). Physical play interactions such as wrestling or tickling are typically accompanied by laughter in both humans and non-human primates (Vettin and Todt, 2005; Davila Ross et al., 2009) and positive USVs in rodents (Panksepp and Burgdorf, 2000, 2003; Burgdorf et al., 2005), reflecting positive play-induced affect (Bachorowski and Owren, 2001; Gervais and Wilson, 2005; Panksepp, 2007, 2011; Burgdorf et al., 2011). Similar forms of physical play are common across mammalian species, especially in juveniles (Bekoff and Byers, 1981, 1998; Fagen, 1981). Play interactions help establish social organization and interaction patterns in human and non-human primates (Maestripieri and Ross, 2004; Palagi et al., 2004; Mancini and Palagi, 2009; Cordoni and Palagi, 2011; Norscia and Palagi, 2011; Ciani et al., 2012; Shimada, 2013) and in rodents (Meaney and Stewart, 1981; Van Den Berg et al., 1999; Pellis et al., 2010).

www.frontiersin.org

Photo by Clint Koehler is licenced under CC-BY-NC-ND 2.0.

Panksepp and colleagues first formulated the “The Brain Opioid Theory of Social Attachment” (BOTSA) theory in 1978. They argued that firstly, given the importance of social behaviors such as parental care and altruistic behaviors for survival of both the individual and the species, affiliative behaviors were likely to have evolved from more basic systems sub-serving pain perception. Secondly, several lines of evidence pointed to striking parallels between social attachment and drug addiction (Panksepp et al., 1978). Both phenomena are associated with a similar behavioral repertoire of proximity- seeking and approach behaviors, as well as profound distress and physiological stress responses to separation or deprivation that are promptly alleviated by contact restoration with the drug or object of attachment (Panksepp et al., 1978; Nelson and Panksepp, 1998; Insel, 2003). BOTSA proposes that social contact alleviates isolation distress and induces positive emotions through the release of endogenous opioids, while separation or social isolation causes opioid withdrawal symptoms and negative affect. This way, the pleasant effects of endorphin release and the aversive effects of endorphin withdrawal are thought to motivate us to seek social contact and maintain proximity to individuals we are emotionally attached to Panksepp et al. (1978, 1980b).

The first evidence supporting BOTSA came from a series of psychopharmacological studies in infants of several species, showing that μ-opioid agonists like morphine reduced and opioid antagonists like naltrexone and naloxone exacerbated distress calls and social contact seeking during and following social separation and isolation. Without apparent sedative effects, MOR agonists such as morphine profoundly reduced isolation-induced crying, while blocking the receptors with an opioid antagonist exacerbated or even induced separation distress behaviors in isolated puppies (Panksepp et al., 1978; Knowles et al., 1989), infant guinea pigs (Herman and Panksepp, 1978), rat pups (Panksepp et al., 1980b; Carden and Hofer, 1990b), chicks (Panksepp et al., 1980a), and infant rhesus monkeys (Kalin et al., 1988). In rat pups, socially mediated reductions in distress vocalizations were completely blocked by the opioid antagonist naltrexone (Carden and Hofer, 1990a), suggesting that social comfort was indeed mediated by the MOR system. This evidence consistently showed that across species, MOR deactivation increased the motivation to seek social contact for comfort in distressed infants, while MOR activation reduced this motivation and left the infant less distressed.

Studies of adult and juvenile non-human primates lent further support to the BOTSA postulations that exogenous opiates can replace the need for social contact, and that blocking the MOR system enhances overall motivation for social interaction. Following brief social isolation, animals injected with morphine engaged less in social grooming (Keverne et al., 1989), while those injected with μ-opioid antagonists such as naltrexone and naloxone made more solicitations and spent more time receiving grooming (Meller et al., 1980; Fabre-Nys et al., 1982; Keverne et al., 1989; Schino and Troisi, 1992; Martel et al., 1995; Graves et al., 2002).

Opposite μ-Opioid System Modulation of Social Motivation in Primates and Rodents?

In contrast to the findings in infants and socially isolated adult primates, social isolation leads to increases in social play interactions in juvenile rats. This increase is further amplified by low-dose morphine injections, while μ-opioid antagonist treatment leads to reductions in social play (Beatty and Costello, 1982; Panksepp et al., 1985; Siegel and Jensen, 1985, 1986; Vanderschuren et al., 1995a,c,d, 1996; Trezza and Vanderschuren, 2008a,b). Similar effects have been found in adult rats, where social grooming was increased by MOR agonists and decreased by MOR antagonists (Van Ree and Niesink, 1983; Niesink and van Ree, 1984, 1989). These effects of MOR manipulation on rodent adult and juvenile social behavior appear to be at odds with those observed in primate studies where MOR agonist treatment reduced social approach behaviors. In sum, the available literature indicates that increased MOR-activation reduces motivation for social contact in infants of several species including rodents, as well as in adult primates, but enhances motivation for social contact in juvenile and adult rodents. This could indicate that the MOR system plays opposite roles in social reward for rodents and primates, and that species differences become evident through development.

In the following, we address this paradox. To investigate whether there is indeed evidence supporting opposing functions of the opioid system in primate and rodent social reward processing, we first summarize the current knowledge on μ-opioid mediation of social reward processing and emotion regulation, and on μ-opioid mediation of affiliation, in both human and non-human animals. We propose a unified, state-dependent model for μ-opioid modulation of affiliation across the mammalian species. We then elaborate on this model by presenting further evidence, focusing on how differences in motivational state may affect social motivation and responses to pharmacological MOR manipulation.

The Role of the μ-Opioid System in Social Reward Processing and Emotion Regulation

Pain and pleasure are core driving forces behind behavior. We generally seek to avoid punishments and to achieve rewards. When the pursuit is successful, we experience subjective feelings of relief or pleasure. When we fail, the result is displeasure. We can call the pursuit of pleasure in the absence of a stressor pleasure seeking; whilst behavior aimed at reducing discomfort in the presence of a stressor can be called pain avoidance. Both pleasure seeking and pain avoidance are reward behaviors, driven by positive and negative reinforcement, respectively (Navratilova and Porreca, 2014). The endogenous μ-opioid receptor (MOR) system modulates both pain and pleasure (Leknes and Tracey, 2008), as well as behaviors related both to food (Berridge, 1996) and to social rewards such as sex (Mahler and Berridge, 2012) or affiliation (Lutz and Kieffer, 2013). Here, we summarize evidence that reveals consistent similarities in reward-related MOR functioning between rodents and humans. These cross-species correspondences substantiate an interpretation of the paradoxical effects MOR stimulation had on social motivation in rodents and primates as caused by differences in motivational state, rather than in neurobiological functioning.

Endogenous MOR transmission contributes to the encoding of relative reward value. Laboratory rats for instance ate less of their favorite food—chocolate chip cookies—if MOR were blocked by an antagonist, but they would still eat normal amounts of standard rat chow (Cooper and Turkish, 1989). Humans show a similar decrease in preference for high-fat, high-sugar foods after MOR antagonism (Mercer and Holder, 1997; Yeomans and Gray, 1997; Adolphs and Spezio, 2006; Nathan et al., 2012). If the μ-opioid receptors are instead stimulated with a MOR agonist, rodents' intake of sweet and fatty foods increases (Zhang et al., 1998; Peciña and Berridge, 2000; Kelley et al., 2002), and so do behaviors indicative of hedonic liking such as lip licking (Peciña and Berridge, 2000). This indicates that pharmacological enhancement of MOR system activity increases pleasure seeking specifically of high-value rewards (for a review of opioid signaling and food reward, see Taha, 2010).

A similar pattern of effects from MOR manipulations is found for sexual preference. Male rats displayed enhanced sexual “wanting” of females in oestrous after MOR stimulation, while their desire to mate with non-oestrous females remained the same (Mahler and Berridge, 2012). We recently reported an analogous finding in a laboratory study in humans. The human MOR system was manipulated with per-oral doses of an agonist (morphine) or antagonist (naltrexone). Male participants viewed pictures of female faces on a computer screen. In a liking task, they rated the attractiveness of the faces. In a wanting task, they pressed buttons to regulate the display time of each face. For both tasks, the largest effects of MOR manipulations were found for the high-reward stimuli, i.e., the most attractive female faces. Compared to placebo, morphine increased and naltrexone decreased attractiveness ratings and number of presses on a “keep-picture-on-screen”-button (Chelnokova et al., 2014; See Figure 2 for more details). Facial attractiveness is considered a signal of health and fertility, i.e., of evolutionary valuable traits (Perrett et al., 1998; Little et al., 2011). Overall, the available evidence points to a μ-opioid mechanism developed through evolution to mark the most valuable options and make them more desirable to us.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The μ-opioid system promotes social motivation in humans. Figure from Chelnokova et al. (2014), where thirty healthy males received a μ-opioid agonist (morphine 10 mg), a non-selective opioid antagonist (naltrexone 50 mg) or placebo (per-oral) on three separate days. Error bars represent within-subject s.e.m. Results from the morphine condition are shown in white; placebo, light gray; naltrexone, dark gray. (A) In two “wanting” and “liking” tasks, participants viewed pictures of faces with varying attractiveness levels. In the “wanting” task, participants could press one of two arrow keys to view the image for longer (“keep”) or shorter (“change”) time than the pre-set 5 s, without altering total task duration. In the “liking” task, participants rated attractiveness of each face using a VAS scale. (B) Morphine treatment enhanced and naltrexone treatment decreased men's “liking” of the most beautiful female faces (ratings on the VAS scale from 0 to 10). (C) “Wanting” of attractive females, as measured by the total of “keep” button presses, was similarly affected by opioid manipulations. However, morphine also increased motivation to avoid viewing the least attractive female faces, as measured by the total of “change” button presses. *P < 0.05. Reprinted from Chelnokova et al. (2014) by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd.

MOR activation is also involved in formation of partner preference, as indicated by recent evidence on social bonding in voles. Compared to the promiscuous meadow vole, the prairie vole has a significantly higher density of μ-opioid receptors in the nucleus accumbens shell, caudate and putamen. This is thought to contribute to species differences in social attachment behaviors (Inoue et al., 2013). A recent study showed that μ-opioid receptors were necessary for formation of lasting bonds between monogamous prairie vole mates (Burkett et al., 2011). Another study found that blocking μ-opioid receptors in the dorsomedial nucleus accumbens shell (dmNAcS) effectively impaired partner preference formation without affecting mating behavior. The authors proposed that MOR in the dmNAcS mediated pair bond formation by eliciting positive hedonic feelings associated with mating (Resendez et al., 2013). In other words, MOR activation may mediate a “hedonic marking” of available choices, making one partner stand out as preferable to the others.

The ability to experience MOR-mediated reward from social stimuli appears to be necessary for adequate social attachment. Genetic knock-out mouse pups lacking the μ-opioid receptor gene OPRM1 displayed diminished reward from mother-related cues as well as diminished distress in responses to separation from the mother, while distress responses to environmental threats remained normal (Moles et al., 2004). In other words, social reward is impaired without MOR, and both bond formation and the distress otherwise expressed when bonds are threatened by social separation are inhibited. The OPRM1 knockout model is now considered a comprehensive mouse model of autism spectrum disorder (Becker et al., 2014). The social reward deficiency in OPRM1 knockout mice mirrors the diminished reward responses to for instance cocaine and ethanol that have been observed in OPRM1 knock-out mice in other studies (Hall et al., 2001, 2004; Hall and Uhl, 2006), consistent with the notion of a shared MOR-driven mechanism underlying social motivation and addiction disorders (Panksepp et al., 1978; Machin and Dunbar, 2011). Research on natural variation of the μ-opioid receptor gene OPRM1 further supports a role for the MOR system both in modulating negative responses to social isolation, and in mediating the reward of social contact. Behaviorally, the OPRM1 118G-allele variant in humans and the OPRM1 77G in non-human primates are associated both with increased negative reactions to social separation and increased positive responses to social affiliation, i.e., enhanced social reward and rejection reactivity (Barr et al., 2008; Way et al., 2009; Higham et al., 2011; Troisi et al., 2011). For more details on OPRM1 and social behaviors, see Textbox 2.

Textbox 2: The OPRM1 A118G/C77G genotype and affiliation.

Single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNP) on the μ-opioid receptor gene OPRM1 occur naturally and result in functional variants of the receptor. In humans, the OPRM1 A118G SNP is one of the most studied SNPs on the OPRM1 gene, and an analog is found in rhesus macaque monkeys (OPRM1 C77G). While the exact molecular function of the OPRM 118/77 G-variant is still unclear (Zhang et al., 2005; Sia et al., 2008), the evidence for OPRM1 A118G/C77G modulation of social reward behaviors is building.

Compared to homozygous 77C-carriers, infant rhesus macaques carrying the 77G-allele exhibit stronger baseline attachment to their mothers, and after repeated mother-infant separation they display more intense distress responses to separation and greater contact time with mothers during reunion (Barr et al., 2008). Further, adult female rhesus macaques carrying the 77G-allele display increased maternal behaviors aimed at restraining their infant's movements to maintain physical proximity than C-allele homozygous females, suggesting that the G-allele is associated with greater maternal motivation to prevent separation (Higham et al., 2011).

In humans, the A118G polymorphism was associated with a greater dispositional sensitivity to social rejection, and with fMRI A118G carriers showed increased reactivity to social rejection in brain regions associated with processing of negative affect related to pain compared to A-homozygotes (Way et al., 2009). One study found that human adults carrying the OPRM1 G-allele self-reported higher tendencies to become engaged in affectionate relationships and experience more pleasure in social situations (Troisi et al., 2010). There is also some evidence that children with G-variant show stronger neural responses to faces with both angry and happy facial expressions compared to A-allele homozygotes (Bertoletti et al., 2012).

To sum up, the current evidence from human and rodent studies suggests that enhanced MOR system transmission increases appetitive reward responses for the preferred option available, both for non-social and social rewards. Disrupting the MOR system affects social reward processing and thereby the ability to form social bonds to a partner or parent. When the ability to experience the rewarding aspects of social interactions is impaired, being socially isolated or separated from a caregiver becomes less aversive. In contrast, high social reward responsiveness enhances separation distress.

Separable μ-Opioid Modulation of Positive and Negative Social Affect

A recent molecular imaging study identified separable MOR mechanisms for modulation of negative and positive affective states caused by social interaction in humans. Hsu et al. (2013) let participants undergo a social feedback task in the scanner, where they were exposed to acceptance or rejection from persons they had rated as highly attractive and considered likely to reciprocate romantic interest (Figure 3). Compared to a neutral control condition, rejection led to increased MOR activation in the amygdala, periaqueductal gray (PAG), and right ventral striatum. High activity in these regions also predicted lower levels of sadness and feelings of rejection (Hsu et al., 2013), perhaps analogous to a coping mechanism for physical pain that is activated in stressful or threatening contexts (Fields, 2004).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. MOR regulation of social affect in humans during social rejection and acceptance. This figure, from Hsu et al. (2013), shows results from a PET study measuring endogenous MOR responses to conditions of social rejection or acceptance. During the Rejection state (receipt of social rejection signals from individuals in whom participants had indicated an interest), MOR activation in the amygdala (A), periacqueductal gray (PAG) (B) and subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (C) correlated positively with measures of the trait ego resiliency, associated with feeling less sad and rejected during Rejection. This indicates that greater MOR activation in pain-modulating areas might be a neural substrate for resiliency, serving as a protective mechanism that makes the individual respond to social rejection with less distress. (D) Increased feelings of sadness and rejection were associated with lower activation of MOR in the pregenual anterior cingulate cortex during Rejection. (E) Increased social motivation correlated positively with increased MOR activation in the left ventral striatum during receipt of signals of social Acceptance. Reprinted from Hsu et al. (2013) by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd.

Positive emotions of happiness and acceptance were increased during social acceptance, and this state change was accompanied by increases in MOR activation in the right anterior insula and left amygdala (Hsu et al., 2013). Similar MOR activation patterns were reported during positive mood induced by an amusing video clip (Koepp et al., 2009) and following amphetamine administration in humans (Colasanti et al., 2012). Interestingly, an increase in motivation to seek social contact was also observed in the acceptance condition, and this correlated positively with increased MOR activation in the ventral striatum (Hsu et al., 2013). In rats, stimulation of MOR in this region increase motivation for social interactions (Trezza et al., 2011b). Importantly, if motivational state (i.e., whether you are being socially accepted or rejected) alters the function of the MOR system in humans, could such state-dependence also explain the seemingly paradoxical, opposite effects of MOR manipulations in primates compared to juvenile and adult rodents?

Does μ-Opioid Modulation of Social Motivation Depend on State or Species Characteristics?

A large proportion of the studies investigating the role of the MOR system in affiliation have applied experimental paradigms where animals are subjected to a stressor such as separation from their mother, offspring, mate or social group in primates (Meller et al., 1980; Fabre-Nys et al., 1982; Keverne et al., 1989; Schino and Troisi, 1992; Martel et al., 1995; Graves et al., 2002) and rodents (Beatty and Costello, 1982; Panksepp et al., 1985; Siegel and Jensen, 1985, 1986; Vanderschuren et al., 1995c,d, 1996; Trezza and Vanderschuren, 2008a,b). Following the separation period, the MOR system is manipulated by central or peripheral administration of a MOR agonist or antagonist, and subsequent effects on distress-related behaviors, physiological stress responses, and social interactions are observed. Since social isolation is aversive to many social animals, experiments where the animal is taken away from its normal social setting before being reintroduced after a period of separation can be called relief paradigms.

Seeking social support is a central coping strategy for social animals facing some form of adversity (Cobb, 1976). It is useful here to distinguish distress from stress. The terms are often used interchangeably, yet distress implies another level of difficulty. When available coping mechanisms are insufficient to deal with stress, distress—which is characterized by more intense negative affect—ensues (NRC, 2008). Accordingly, animals that are more dependent on social comfort to handle stress should react with greater distress during isolation. This mechanism is illustrated by age-related differences in social distress. All mammalian infants rely on parental care to survive, and respond promptly to separation from the caregiver by emitting distress vocalizations and seeking physical proximity. The distress behaviors cease if comforting social contact is reinstated (Blumberg et al., 1992; Kalin et al., 1992; Hofer et al., 1993; Van Oers et al., 1998), and this comforting effect is thought to be mediated by MOR activation (Panksepp et al., 1980b; Kalin et al., 1988; Carden and Hofer, 1990b).

Later in the lifespan, species differences in social dependence become more apparent. Juvenile and adult rats respond to temporary social isolation with few signs of distress (Nelson and Panksepp, 1998) and no increase in stress hormone levels (Van Den Berg et al., 1999). Non-human primates on the other hand continue to display behavioral signs of distress upon social separation (Kalin, 1995; Rilling et al., 2001; Levine, 2005; Tardif et al., 2013). Depressive-like behavior similar to that observed in infant primates is displayed by adult primates separated from their family environment (Suomi et al., 1975), and physiological indications of stress such as increases in cortisol levels are observed in both juvenile and adult primates during social separation (Higley et al., 1992; Lyons et al., 1999; Ragen et al., 2013).

The discrepancy between MOR effects in rodents and primates could reflect a qualitative difference between social bonds in primates and rodents. Most rodents typically form transient bonds that serve to facilitate mating and nurturing of offspring, and that hinge on the hormonal context induced by copulation and parturition. They do not typically depend on enduring parental or social bonds for survival or successful offspring rearing (Broad et al., 2006). In contrast, primates are capable of forming long-lasting bonds even without such hormonal context (Broad et al., 2006). Primates live in highly complex social groups, on which they depend for protection from predators, as well as for cooperation on rearing of offspring and foraging for food (Dunbar, 2012). While social separation is a potential stressor for all social mammals, the occurrence and intensity of distress should reflect the potential harm from social isolation. Social bonds and interactions are valued higher in the more socially dependent primates, and it is possible that this is underpinned by an evolutionary shift in MOR system functioning—which in turn could explain why effects of MOR manipulation on social interactions following social separation are opposite for juveniles and adults of the two species.

However, considering the striking neurobiological correspondence between rodent and primate reward systems in general (Berridge and Kringelbach, 2008), and in μ-opioid effects on pain and social distress in particular (e.g., Eisenberger, 2012), we propose that the observed difference between these species rather reflects a difference in motivational state caused by affective responses to the experimental conditions.

The SOMSOM: A Model of State-Dependent μ-Opioid Modulation of Social Motivation

Pain provides a powerful illustration of how the function of the MOR system can be modulated by motivational states. During high-pain states, large MOR agonist doses provide pain relief. Compared to non-pain states, acute pain is thought to reduce other common effects of opiate drugs, such as respiratory depression (Borgbjerg et al., 1996) and addiction potential (Ballantyne and Laforge, 2007), illustrating that the effect of exogenous μ-opioid manipulation is state dependent. According to the Motivation-Decision Model of Pain, the motivational context also determines the action of endogenous μ-opioids in the brainstem, such that pain signals can be either up- or down-regulated (Fields, 2006). If the context is dominated by a threat or pain signal, MOR deactivation can up-regulate that signal, amplify the experience of pain, and consequently facilitate rest and healing. In a context of potential high reward however, increased MOR activation may inhibit pain and thus enable reward-seeking in spite of risk. Conversely, pain inhibition can also facilitate escape from high threats. The purpose of this context-dependent mechanism is to enable decisions that maximize evolutionary benefits such as food or safety. We hypothesize that the MOR system affects social approach in a similarly context-dependent manner:

1. In a negative motivational state, social animals will seek out safe social contact as a mean of coping or comfort, i.e., to downregulate negative emotion through contact-mediated μ-opioid release. Stimulating MOR with opiates will then reduce social comfort seeking, because the need for comfort and endogenous MOR activation is diminished. Conversely, blocking MOR can enhance comfort seeking behaviors by blocking endogenous coping mechanisms.

2. In a positive motivational state on the other hand, social interaction can have functions beyond comfort, such as formation and maintenance of close social bonds, testing the boundaries of social hierarchies, or exploring possible new sex partners, and can thus be a source of fun, joy, pleasure—but also challenge and risk. As we have seen, the MOR system promotes many of these behaviors. Exogenous MOR stimulation should similarly increase the resources recruited for approach of social rewards, whereas an inhibition of social exploration is expected after MOR antagonism in this type of context (see Figure 4 for a schematic presentation of the model and its predictions).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The State-dependent μ-opioid Modulation of Social Motivation (SOMSOM) Model. (A) During a separation distress state, motivation is predominately aversive, and social contact is sought out for comfort and relief. Positive social contact or MOR agonist treatment enhances MOR activation, thereby providing relief. The result is a comfort state where the need for social support is diminished, thus reducing social approach behaviors. When the MOR system instead is blocked with an antagonist, the resulting state is one of increased distress and enhanced aversive motivation. Social approach behaviors are thus increased, since the need for social support is greater. A similar reaction would be expected from further social isolation. (B) During a normal state of homeostatic balance, motivation is predominately appetitive, and social interaction is sought out for exploratory or affiliative purposes. Increased MOR transmission through MOR agonist treatment or positive social interaction leads to a state of exploration with increased appetitive motivation and increased social exploration and approach. If the MOR system instead is blocked by an antagonist, the resulting state is one of “indifference,” where appetitive social motivation is diminished and social approach is reduced.

When applying this model to the question of opioid modulation of affiliative behavior, we also draw on the ideas of Maslow, namely that motivation to meet higher needs is contingent on the prior fulfillment of more basic needs (Maslow, 1943). Specifically, if the μ-opioid system is needed to cope with pain or distress, this should be its primary function, and only after homeostatic equilibrium has been achieved would one expect MOR-mediated increases in social exploration. In both states, affiliative interaction is rewarding. During social distress, interaction is sought primarily for comfort, and achieving positive contact signals social support and aids down-regulation of negative affect. Comforting social contact is thus rewarding because it alleviates the unpleasant state, and the behaviors leading to social comfort are negatively reinforced. Otherwise, i.e., during homeostatic equilibrium, affiliative interaction is positively reinforced. When the interaction is sought primarily for fun or pleasure, affiliative behaviors indicate the establishment or maintenance of a social relationship, and are a source of joy. In the following section, we elaborate on our model by summarizing further evidence for state dependent μ-opioid modulation of social behaviors.

μ-Opioid Modulation of Social Motivation for Comfort or Joy

The State-dependent μ-Opioid Modulation of SOcial Motivation (SOMSOM) model postulates that the diverging and paradoxical findings showing that MOR agonists in some cases reduce and in other cases increase social approach are due to very different motivational contexts. Although the intervention of social isolation or separation formed part of all the studies reviewed above, we argue that social separation leads to different motivational states depending on how much the animal relies on social contact for survival and emotion regulation. According to SOMSOM, the motivational state of the animal determines the effects of both agonism and antagonism of the MOR receptor, depending on whether the main motivation is to reduce distress or seek pleasure.

While studies of animals known to find social separation distressing, such as infant rodents; infant, juvenile and adult primates; and infants from other species, consistently show that the comforting effect social contact has on separation distress is blocked by MOR antagonism (see Section Introduction), blocking MOR with an antagonist is not found sufficient to induce separation distress in itself. Naltrexone failed to induce distress vocalizations in infant rats when they were allowed to remain together with their siblings (Carden et al., 1993). In primates, MOR antagonist treatment reduced maternal infant grooming in mothers who were never separated from their infant (Martel et al., 1993). These findings indicate that MOR blockade itself does not increase the need for social comfort or induce distress in a context where there is no prior stress. Instead, blockade of the MOR system in animals that remained unstressed attenuated social interaction.

Since social play generally occurs only when an animal is free from physiological and social stress, it provides a useful behavior for investigating MOR effects in the absence of distress (Loizos, 1967; Baldwin and Baldwin, 1976; Fagen, 1981; Siviy and Panksepp, 1985; Vanderschuren et al., 1995b; De Oliveira et al., 2003). By the time rats have become juveniles, social separation is no longer associated with distress (Nelson and Panksepp, 1998; Van Den Berg et al., 1999), and social separation leads to subsequent increases in social play interactions upon reunion (Panksepp, 1981). Based on the observation that play is suppressed by stress, we presume that the juvenile rats are in a state of appetitive motivation when they are released from brief isolation, and that they seek out social contact for joy (Panksepp and Burgdorf, 2003), and not primarily for consolation or to reduce distress (see also Textbox 1 for more details on social play). According to the SOMSOM model, MOR agonist treatment should lead to enhanced pleasure seeking when the animal is in a state of appetitive motivation. This is indeed what has been consistently found in pharmacological experiments of rodent social play (reviewed in Section Opposite μ-opioid System Modulation of Social Motivation in Primates and Rodents?). Further, social play was associated with strong increases in endogenous μ-opioid release in the rostral nucleus accumbens (Vanderschuren et al., 1995e), which is central to processing of other appetitive rewards (Berridge and Robinson, 1998; Trezza et al., 2011b).

Increased MOR transmission was hypothesized to enhance the reward value of social interactions (Vanderschuren et al., 1995a). This view is empirically supported by a study where a selective MOR agonist was injected directly into the nucleus accumbens of adolescent rats and observed to increase play behavior specifically, while injections of a specific MOR antagonist into this region prevented the development of social play-induced conditioned place preference (Trezza et al., 2011b). Exposing young rats to novel environments normally delays social play due to exploration of the surroundings (Vanderschuren et al., 1995b), but animals injected with low doses of morphine before being introduced to an unfamiliar cage immediately started social play interactions, which could indicate that MOR activation directs the attentional focus toward social rewards rather than exploring the environment (Trezza and Vanderschuren, 2008a). An alternative interpretation could be that the enhanced MOR transmission increases confidence (Panksepp et al., 1985), and thus reduces the need to check whether there are potential dangers present. Given its function in establishing social hierarchies and dominance relationships, engaging in social play involves a level of risk that could make it a more challenging rather than comforting activity (Poirier and Smith, 1974; Panksepp et al., 1985; Blumstein et al., 2013).

Few studies have investigated the effects of experimental MOR manipulation on play behaviors in primates, but the limited evidence suggests that the MOR system mediates social play in a similar manner to what is seen in rodents. In a study specifically designed to investigate MOR modulation of primate social play, marmoset juveniles living in a family group were injected with morphine, naloxone, or saline without prior social isolation-distress. Here, morphine specifically increased social play behaviors compared to both saline and naloxone, while naloxone led to slight, albeit not statistically significant, decreases in play behaviors compared to saline (Guard et al., 2002). Similarly, a study of long-tailed macaques that also were allowed to remain in their social context throughout the experiment, reported a decrease in social play after treatment with naloxone that approached statistical significance (Schino and Troisi, 1992). The weak effects of naloxone might indicate that other neurochemicals than μ-opioids are important for play behavior in primates. According to SOMSOM, MOR blockade during a state of distress should increase social motivation, specifically for comforting social interactions. As mentioned, social play is not a behavior usually sought out for comfort, so the increased social motivation during social distress would be expected to manifest in other behaviors. Indeed, a study where juvenile rhesus monkeys were socially isolated for 2 h found that the subsequent injection of naloxone, relative to saline, significantly decreased social play interactions when reunited with peers while contact with the mother increased (Martel et al., 1995). The authors hypothesized that the decrease in social play could reflect that MOR-blockade decreased social confidence and reduced the young monkeys' willingness to risk social play while increasing their need for social comfort (Martel et al., 1995).

In sum, and in line with SOMSOM, the evidence from rodent and primate studies shows that MOR activation in a context of positive motivation increased “appetitive” social play interactions, whilst MOR blockade reduced these behaviors, if to different degrees depending on species. The hypothesis that MOR activation enhances reward experienced during engagement in social play is in line with a large body of literature showing that MOR agonism enhances appetitive reward responses to food rewards (Berridge and Kringelbach, 2008; Difeliceantonio and Berridge, 2012; Mahler and Berridge, 2012).

Conclusion

We started this review by presenting a paradox in the current literature on MOR modulation of social motivation, namely that MOR activation increases social motivation in rats, but reduces social motivation in primates. The experimental paradigms in most studies of rodents and primates include social separation followed by drug administration, and then social reunion. We reasoned that social separation would affect motivational state differently according to social dependency, such that for instance a socially dependent infant or an adult primate would seek comfort to regulate social distress. In contrast, an adult or juvenile rat would tolerate social separation and instead react to MOR activation with social exploratory behaviors. Importantly, the consistent correspondence between species in the reviewed literature on MOR modulation of reward processing, emotion regulation, and affiliation in humans, non-human primates, and rodents substantiates an interpretation of the initial paradox as a result of differences in motivational state caused by affective responses to the experimental conditions, rather than differences in neurobiology.

Building on the Motivation-Decision Model for opioid modulation of pain (Fields, 2006), and on the idea that motivation is dictated by a hierarchy of needs where more basic needs must be fulfilled before seeking to satisfy higher needs (Maslow, 1943), we have proposed a model for State-dependent μ-opioid Modulation of Social Motivation (SOMSOM). This model postulates that during a distress state, motivation is predominately aversive, and social contact is sought out for comfort and relief. Social comfort reduces distress at least in part via μ-opioid receptor (MOR) activation. Activation of MOR, elicited by either by social interaction or exogenous MOR agonist treatment, provides relief and diminishes the need for social support, thus reducing social approach behaviors. In contrast, during a normal state of homeostatic balance, motivation is predominately appetitive, and social interaction is sought out for explorative or affiliative purposes. Increased MOR transmission increases social exploration and approach of social rewards. In non-distressed states, decreased MOR transmission due to antagonist treatment reduces social approach.

The SOMSOM is supported by the reviewed evidence from a large literature of psychopharmacological studies in rats and a few key studies of non-human primates. When factors such as species and age differences in social dependency are taken into account, effects of pharmacological MOR manipulation on social approach behaviors are similar across species. We therefore hold that the SOMSOM predicts state-dependent MOR modulation of social motivation in humans. While the reviewed evidence from human studies show MOR system modulation of social motivation and separable MOR activation for positive and negative social affect, future studies should empirically test for state dependency in MOR modulation of human social motivation. Little is known about the role of the human MOR system in social bond formation and attachment. μ-opioid mechanisms form part of a complex concert of neurobiological processes modulating social behavior, and future studies should aim to disentangle interactions between the μ-opioid system and other neurochemical systems relevant for social reward such as κ-opioids and δ-opioids, oxytocin, dopamine and serotonin.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adolphs, R., and Spezio, M. (2006). Role of the amygdala in processing visual social stimuli. Prog. Brain Res. 156, 363–378. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56020-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alberts, J. R. (2007). Huddling by rat pups: ontogeny of individual and group behavior. Dev. Psychobiol. 49, 22–32. doi: 10.1002/dev.20190

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bachorowski, J. A., and Owren, M. J. (2001). Not all laughs are alike: voiced but not unvoiced laughter readily elicits positive affect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 252–257. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00346

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, J. D., and Baldwin, J. I. (1976). Effects of food ecology on social play: a laboratory simulation3. Z. Tierpsychol. 40, 1–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1976.tb00922.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballantyne, J. C., and Laforge, K. S. (2007). Opioid dependence and addiction during opioid treatment of chronic pain. Pain 129, 235–255. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.03.028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barr, C. S., Schwandt, M. L., Lindell, S. G., Higley, J. D., Maestripieri, D., Goldman, D., et al. (2008). Variation at the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) influences attachment behavior in infant primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 5277–5281. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710225105

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beatty, W. W., and Costello, K. B. (1982). Naloxone and play fighting in juvenile rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 17, 905–907. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(82)90470-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, J. A. J., Clesse, D., Spiegelhalter, C., Schwab, Y., Le Merrer, J., and Kieffer, B. L. (2014). Autistic-Like syndrome in mu opioid receptor null mice is relieved by facilitated mGluR4 activity. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 2049–2060. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.59

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bekoff, M., and Byers, J. A. (1981). “A critical reanalysis of the ontogeny and phylogeny of mammalian social and locomotor play: an ethological hornet's nest,” in Behavioral Development, eds K. Immelmann, G. W. Barlow, L. Petrinovich, and M. Main (London: Cambridge University Press), 296–337.

Bekoff, M., and Byers, J. A. (1998). Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative, and Ecological Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Berridge, K. C. (1996). Food reward: brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 20, 1–25. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00033-B

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2008). Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 199, 457–480. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1099-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2013). Neuroscience of affect: brain mechanisms of pleasure and displeasure. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 294–303. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res. Rev. 28, 309–369. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00019-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Robinson, T. E. (2003). Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci. 26, 507–513. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., Robinson, T. E., and Aldridge, J. W. (2009). Dissecting components of reward: “liking,” “wanting,” and learning. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 9, 65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertoletti, E., Zanoni, A., Giorda, R., and Battaglia, M. (2012). Influence of the OPRM1 gene polymorphism upon children's degree of withdrawal and brain activation in response to facial expressions. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2, 103–109. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blumberg, M. S., Efimova, I. V., and Alberts, J. R. (1992). Ultrasonic vocalizations by rat pups: the primary importance of ambient temperature and the thermal significance of contact comfort. Dev. Psychobiol. 25, 229–250. doi: 10.1002/dev.420250402

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blumstein, D. T., Chung, L. K., and Smith, J. E. (2013). Early play may predict later dominance relationships in yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventris). Proc. Biol. Sci. 280:20130485. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.0485

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boecker, H., Sprenger, T., Spilker, M. E., Henriksen, G., Koppenhoefer, M., Wagner, K. J., et al. (2008). The Runner's high: opioidergic mechanisms in the human brain. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2523–2531. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borgbjerg, F. M., Nielsen, K., and Franks, J. (1996). Experimental pain stimulates respiration and attenuates morphine-induced respiratory depression: a controlled study in human volunteers. Pain 64, 123–128. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(95)00088-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broad, K. D., Curley, J. P., and Keverne, E. B. (2006). Mother–infant bonding and the evolution of mammalian social relationships. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 361, 2199–2214. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1940

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgdorf, J., Panksepp, J., and Moskal, J. R. (2011). Frequency-modulated 50kHz ultrasonic vocalizations: a tool for uncovering the molecular substrates of positive affect. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1831–1836. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgdorf, J., Panksepp, J., Brudzynski, S. M., Kroes, R., and Moskal, J. R. (2005). Breeding for 50-kHz positive affective vocalization in rats. Behav. Genet. 35, 67–72. doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-0856-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burkett, J. P., Spiegel, L. L., Inoue, K., Murphy, A. Z., and Young, L. J. (2011). Activation of-opioid receptors in the dorsal striatum is necessary for adult social attachment in monogamous prairie voles. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 2200–2210. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.117

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldji, C., Diorio, J., and Meaney, M. J. (2000). Variations in maternal care in infancy regulate the development of stress reactivity. Biol. Psychiatry 48, 1164–1174. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01084-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldji, C., Tannenbaum, B., Sharma, S., Francis, D., Plotsky, P. M., and Meaney, M. J. (1998). Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 5335–5340. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.9.5335

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carden, S. E., and Hofer, M. A. (1990a). Socially mediated reduction of isolation distress in rat pups is blocked by naltrexone but not by Ro 15-1788. Behav. Neurosci. 104, 457–463. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.104.3.457

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carden, S. E., and Hofer, M. A. (1990b). Independence of benzodiazepine and opiate action in the suppression of isolation distress in rat pups. Behav. Neurosci. 104, 160–166. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.104.1.160

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carden, S. E., and Hofer, M. A. (1992). Effect of a social companion on the ultrasonic vocalizations and contact responses of 3-Day-Old rat pups. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 421–426. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.106.2.421

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carden, S. E., Bortot, A. T., and Hofer, M. A. (1993). Ultrasonic vocalizations are elicited from rat pups in the home cage by pentylenetetrazol and U50,488, but not naltrexone. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 851–859. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.5.851

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castro, D. C., and Berridge, K. C. (2014). Opioid hedonic hotspot in nucleus accumbens shell: Mu, delta, and kappa maps for enhancement of sweetness “liking” and “wanting.” J. Neurosci. 34, 4239–4250. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4458-13.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chelnokova, O., Laeng, B., Eikemo, M., Riegels, J., Loseth, G., Maurud, H., et al. (2014). Rewards of beauty: the opioid system mediates social motivation in humans. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 746–747. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciani, F., Dall'olio, S., Stanyon, R., and Palagi, E. (2012). Social tolerance and adult play in macaque societies: a comparison with different human cultures. Anim. Behav. 84, 1313–1322. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cobb, S. (1976). Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosom. Med. 38, 300–314. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197609000-00003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colasanti, A., Searle, G. E., Long, C. J., Hill, S. P., Reiley, R. R., Quelch, D., et al. (2012). Endogenous opioid release in the human brain reward system induced by acute amphetamine administration. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 371–377. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, S. J., and Turkish, S. (1989). Effects of naltrexone on food preference and concurrent behavioral responses in food-deprived rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 33, 17–20. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90422-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cordoni, G., and Palagi, E. (2011). Ontogenetic trajectories of chimpanzee social play: similarities with humans. PLoS ONE 6:e27344. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027344

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D'Amato, F. R. (1998). Kin interaction enhances morphine analgesia in male mice. Behav. Pharmacol. 9, 369–373.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

D'Amato, F. R., and Pavone, F. (1993). Endogenous opioids: a proximate reward mechanism for kin selection? Behav. Neural Biol. 60, 79–83. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(93)90768-D

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D'Amato, F. R., and Pavone, F. (1996). Reunion of separated sibling mice: neurobiological and behavioral aspects. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 65, 9–16. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.0002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davila Ross, M. J., Owren, M., and Zimmermann, E. (2009). Reconstructing the evolution of laughter in great apes and humans. Curr. Biol. 19, 1106–1111. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Oliveira, C. R., Ruiz-Miranda, C. R., Kleiman, D. G., and Beck, B. B. (2003). Play behavior in juvenile golden lion tamarins (Callitrichidae: Primates): organization in relation to costs. Ethology 109, 593–612. doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2003.00901.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Depue, R. A., and Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V. (2005). A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 313–350 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000063

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Difeliceantonio, A. G., and Berridge, K. C. (2012). Which cue to “want”? Opioid stimulation of central amygdala makes goal-trackers show stronger goal-tracking, just as sign-trackers show stronger sign-tracking. Behav. Brain Res. 230, 399–408. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.02.032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. I. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1980). Determinants and evolutionary consequences of dominance among female gelada baboons. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 7, 253–265. doi: 10.1007/BF00300665

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2012). Bridging the bonding gap: the transition from primates to humans. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 367, 1837–1846. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0217

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). The pain of social disconnection: examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 421–434. doi: 10.1038/nrn3231

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabre-Nys, C., Meller, R. E., and Keverne, E. B. (1982). Opiate antagonists stimulate affiliative behaviour in monkeys. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 16, 653–659. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(82)90432-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagen, R. (1981). Animal Play Behavior. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Fields, H. (2004). State-dependent opioid control of pain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 565–575. doi: 10.1038/nrn1431

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fields, H. L. (2006). “A Motivation-Decision model of pain: the role of opioids,” in Proceedings of the 11th World Congress on Pain, eds H. Flor, E. Kalso, and J. O. Dostrovsky (Seattle, WA: IASP Press), 449–459.

Gervais, M., and Wilson, D. S. (2005). The evolution and functions of laughter and humor: a synthetic approach. Q. Rev. Biol. 80, 395–430. doi: 10.1086/498281

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graves, F. C., Wallen, K., and Maestripieri, D. (2002). Opioids and attachment in rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) abusive mothers. Behav. Neurosci. 116, 489–493. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.116.3.489

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guard, H. J., Newman, J. D., and Lucille Roberts, R. (2002). Morphine administration selectively facilitates social play in common marmosets. Dev. Psychobiol. 41, 37–49. doi: 10.1002/dev.10043

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagelberg, N., Kajander, J. K., Någren, K., Hinkka, S., Hietala, J., and Scheinin, H. (2002). Mu-Receptor agonism with alfentanil increases striatal dopamine D2 receptor binding in man. Synapse 45, 25–30. doi: 10.1002/syn.10078

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, F. S., and Uhl, G. R. (2006). Transgenic mouse studies reveal substantial roles for opioid receptors in the rewarding effects of several classes of addictive drugs. Curr. Psychiatry Rev. 2, 27–37. doi: 10.2174/157340006775101526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, F. S., Goeb, M., Li, X.-F., Sora, I., and Uhl, G. R. (2004). μ-Opioid receptor knockout mice display reduced cocaine conditioned place preference but enhanced sensitization of cocaine-induced locomotion. Mol. Brain Res. 121, 123–130. doi: 10.1016/j.molbrainres.2003.10.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, F. S., Sora, I., and Uhl, G. R. (2001). Ethanol consumption and reward are decreased in μ-opiate receptor knockout mice. Psychopharmacology 154, 43–49. doi: 10.1007/s002130000622

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harlow, H. F., and Zimmermann, R. R. (1959). Affectional responses in the infant monkey: orphaned baby monkeys develop a strong and persistent attachment to inanimate surrogate mothers. Science 130, 421–432. doi: 10.1126/science.130.3373.421

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herman, B. H., and Panksepp, J. (1978). Effects of morphine and naloxone on separation distress and approach attachment: evidence for opiate mediation of social affect. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 9, 213–220. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(78)90167-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertenstein, M. J., Verkamp, J. M., Kerestes, A. M., and Holmes, R. M. (2006). The communicative functions of touch in humans, nonhuman primates, and rats: a review and synthesis of the empirical research. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 132, 5–94. doi: 10.3200/MONO.132.1.5-94

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higham, J. P., Barr, C. S., Hoffman, C. L., Mandalaywala, T. M., Parker, K. J., and Maestripieri, D. (2011). Mu-opioid receptor (OPRM1) variation, oxytocin levels and maternal attachment in free-ranging rhesus macaques Macaca mulatta. Behav. Neurosci. 125, 131–136. doi: 10.1037/a0022695

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higley, J. D., Suomi, S. J., and Linnoila, M. (1992). A longitudinal assessment of CSF monoamine metabolite and plasma cortisol concentrations in young rhesus monkeys. Biol. Psychiatry 32, 127–145. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(92)90016-S

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofer, M. A., and Shair, H. N. (1987). Isolation distress in two-week-old rats: influence of home cage, social companions, and prior experience with littermates. Dev. Psychobiol. 20, 465–476. doi: 10.1002/dev.420200410

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofer, M. A., Brunelli, S. A., and Shair, H. N. (1993). Ultrasonic vocalization responses of rat pups to acute separation and contact comfort do not depend on maternal thermal cues. Dev. Psychobiol. 26, 81–95. doi: 10.1002/dev.420260202

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, D. T., Sanford, B. J., Meyers, K. K., Love, T. M., Hazlett, K. E., Wang, H., et al. (2013). Response of the [mu]-opioid system to social rejection and acceptance. Mol. Psychiatry 18, 1211–1217. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.96

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inoue, K., Burkett, J. P., and Young, L. J. (2013). Neuroanatomical distribution of μ-opioid receptor mRNA and binding in monogamous prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and non-monogamous meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Neuroscience 244, 122–133. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.03.035

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T. R. (2003). Is social attachment an addictive disorder? Physiol. Behav. 79, 351–357. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00148-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalin, N. H. (1995). Opiate systems in mother and infant primates coordinate intimate contact during reunion. Psychoneuroendocrinology 20, 735–742. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(95)00023-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalin, N. H., Shelton, S. E., and Barksdale, C. M. (1988). Opiate modualtion of separation-induced distress in non-human primates. Brain Res. 440, 285–292. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90997-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalin, N. H., Shelton, S. E., and Snowdon, C. T. (1992). Affiliative vocalizations in infant rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J. Comp. Psychol. 106, 254–261. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.106.3.254

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelley, A. E., Bakshi, V. P., Haber, S. N., Steininger, T. L., Will, M. J., and Zhang, M. (2002). Opioid modulation of taste hedonics within the ventral striatum. Physiol. Behav. 76, 365–377. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00751-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keverne, E. B., Martensz, N. D., and Tuite, B. (1989). Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology 14, 155–161. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(89)90065-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knowles, P. A., Conner, R. L., and Panksepp, J. (1989). Opiate effects on social behavior of juvenile dogs as a function of social deprivation. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 33, 533–537. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90382-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koepp, M. J., Hammers, A., Lawrence, A. D., Asselin, M. C., Grasby, P. M., and Bench, C. J. (2009). Evidence for endogenous opioid release in the amygdala during positive emotion. Neuroimage 44, 252–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kringelbach, M. L., and Berridge, K. C. (2009). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 479–487. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laurent, V., Morse, A. K., and Balleine, B. W. (2014). The role of opioid processes in reward and decision-making. Br. J. Pharmacol. doi: 10.1111/bph.12818. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leknes, S., and Tracey, I. (2008). A common neurobiology for pain and pleasure. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 314–320. doi: 10.1038/nrn2333

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, S. (2005). Developmental determinants of sensitivity and resistance to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 939–946. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.03.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lintas, A., Chi, N., Lauzon, N. M., Bishop, S. F., Gholizadeh, S., Sun, N., et al. (2011). Identification of a dopamine receptor-mediated opiate reward memory switch in the basolateral amygdala-nucleus accumbens circuit. J. Neurosci. 31, 11172–11183. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1781-11.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., and Debruine, L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 366, 1638–1659. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0404

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, B., Caldji, C., Francis, D., Freedman, A., et al. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science 277, 1659–1662. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5332.1659

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loizos, C. (1967). “Play behaviour in higher primates: a review,” in Symposia of the Zoological Society of London, 18 (London: AldineTransaction).

Google Scholar

Lutz, P. E., and Kieffer, B. L. (2013). The multiple facets of opioid receptor function: implications for addiction. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 473–479. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.02.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, D. M., Wang, O. J., Lindley, S. E., Levine, S., Kalin, N. H., and Schatzberg, A. F. (1999). Separation induced changes in squirrel monkey hypothalamic-pituitary- adrenal physiology resemble aspects of hypercortisolism in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology 24, 131–142. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00065-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Machin, A. J., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2011). The brain opioid theory of social attachment: a review of the evidence. Behaviour 148, 985–1025. doi: 10.1163/000579511X596624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maestripieri, D., and Ross, S. R. (2004). Sex differences in play among western lowland gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) infants: implications for adult behavior and social structure. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 123, 52–61. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10295

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahler, S. V., and Berridge, K. C. (2012). What and when to “want”? Amygdala-based focusing of incentive salience upon sugar and sex. Psychopharmacology 221, 407–426. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2588-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mancini, G., and Palagi, E. (2009). Play and social dynamics in a captive herd of gelada baboons (Theropithecus gelada). Behav. Process. 82, 286–292. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.07.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martel, F. L., Nevison, C. M., Rayment, F. D., Simpson, M. J., and Keverne, E. B. (1993). Opioid receptor blockade reduces maternal affect and social grooming in rhesus monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology 18, 307–321. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(93)90027-I

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martel, F. L., Nevison, C. M., Simpson, M. J., and Keverne, E. B. (1995). Effects of opioid receptor blockade on the social behavior of rhesus monkeys living in large family groups. Dev. Psychobiol. 28, 71–84. doi: 10.1002/dev.420280202

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychol. Rev. 50, 370–396. doi: 10.1037/h0054346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meaney, M. J., and Stewart, J. (1981). A descriptive study of social development in the rat (Rattus norvegicus). Anim. Behav. 29, 34–45. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80149-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meller, R. E., Keverne, E. B., and Herbert, J. (1980). Behavioural and endocrine effects of naltrexone in male talapoin monkeys. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 13, 663–672. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(80)90010-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercer, M. E., and Holder, M. D. (1997). Antinociceptive effects of palatable sweet ingesta on human responsivity to pressure pain. Physiol. Behav. 61, 311–318. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00400-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moles, A., Kieffer, B. L., and D'Amato, F. R. (2004). Deficit in attachment behavior in mice lacking the μ-opioid receptor gene. Science 304, 1983–1986. doi: 10.1126/science.1095943

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, C. L., and Power, K. L. (1992). Variation in maternal care and individual differences in play, exploration, and grooming of juvenile Norway rat offspring. Dev. Psychobiol. 25, 165–182. doi: 10.1002/dev.420250303

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nathan, P. J., O'neill, B. V., Bush, M. A., Koch, A., Tao, W. X., Maltby, K., et al. (2012). Opioid receptor modulation of hedonic taste preference and food intake. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 52, 464–474. doi: 10.1177/0091270011399577

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navratilova, E., and Porreca, F. (2014). Reward and motivation in pain and pain relief. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1304–1312. doi: 10.1038/nn.3811

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, E. E., and Panksepp, J. (1998). Brain substrates of infant-mother attachment: contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 22, 437–452. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00052-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niesink, R. J., and van Ree, J. M. (1984). Neuropeptides and social behavior of rats tested in dyadic encounters. Neuropeptides 4, 483–496. doi: 10.1016/0143-4179(84)90092-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niesink, R. J., and van Ree, J. M. (1989). Involvement of opioid and dopaminergic systems in isolation-induced pinning and social grooming of young rats. Neuropharmacology 28, 411–418. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(89)90038-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norscia, I., and Palagi, E. (2011). When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy, and possible stress reduction in common marmosets. Primates 52, 101–104. doi: 10.1007/s10329-010-0228-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

NRC (2008). Recognition and Alleviation of Distress in Laboratory Animals. Washington, DC: National Academies Press (US).

Palagi, E., Cordoni, G., and Borgognini Tarli, S. M. (2004). Immediate and delayed benefits of play behaviour: new evidence from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology 110, 949–962. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.01035.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (1981). The ontogeny of play in rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 14, 327–332. doi: 10.1002/dev.420140405

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2007). Neuroevolutionary sources of laughter and social joy: modeling primal human laughter in laboratory rats. Behav. Brain Res. 182, 231–244. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.02.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2011). Cross-Species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals. PLoS ONE 6:e21236. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021236

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Bishop, P. (1981). An autoradiographic map of (3H) diprenorphine binding in rat brain: effects of social interaction. Brain Res. Bull. 7, 405–410. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(81)90038-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Burgdorf, J. (2000). 50-kHz chirping (laughter?) in response to conditioned and unconditioned tickle-induced reward in rats: effects of social housing and genetic variables. Behav. Brain Res. 115, 25–38. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00238-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Burgdorf, J. (2003). “Laughing” rats and the evolutionary antecedents of human joy? Physiol. Behav. 79, 533–547. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00159-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Bean, N. J., Bishop, P., Vilberg, T., and Sahley, T. L. (1980a). Opioid blockade and social comfort in chicks. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 13, 673–683. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(80)90011-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Herman, B. H., Vilberg, T., Bishop, P., and Deeskinazi, F. G. (1980b). Endogenous opioids and social behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 4, 473–487. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(80)90036-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Herman, B., Conner, R., Bishop, P., and Scott, J. P. (1978). The biology of social attachments: opiates alleviate separation distress. Biol. Psychiatry 13, 607–618.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Jalowiec, J., Deeskinazi, F. G., and Bishop, P. (1985). Opiates and play dominance in juvenile rats. Behav. Neurosci. 99, 441–453. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.99.3.441

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parent, C. I., and Meaney, M. J. (2008). The influence of natural variations in maternal care on play fighting in the rat. Dev. Psychobiol. 50, 767–776. doi: 10.1002/dev.20342

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peciña, S., and Berridge, K. C. (2000). Opioid site in nucleus accumbens shell mediates eating and hedonic “liking” for food: map based on microinjection Fos plumes. Brain Res. 863, 71–86. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02102-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peciña, S., and Berridge, K. C. (2013). Dopamine or opioid stimulation of nucleus accumbens similarly amplify cue-triggered “wanting” for reward: entire core and medial shell mapped as substrates for PIT enhancement. Eur. J. Neurosci. 37, 1529–1540. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12174

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellis, S. M., Pellis, V. C., and Bell, H. C. (2010). The function of play in the development of the social brain. Am. J. Play 2, 278–298.

Google Scholar

Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., et al. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature 394, 884–887. doi: 10.1038/29772

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poirier, F. E., and Smith, E. O. (1974). Socializing functions of primate play. Integr. Comp. Biol. 14, 275–287. doi: 10.1093/icb/14.1.275

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ragen, B. J., Maninger, N., Mendoza, S. P., Jarcho, M. R., and Bales, K. L. (2013). Presence of a pair-mate regulates the behavioral and physiological effects of opioid manipulation in the monogamous titi monkey (Callicebus cupreus). Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 2448–2461. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.05.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Resendez, S. L., and Aragona, B. J. (2013). Aversive motivation and the maintenance of monogamous pair bonding. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 51–60. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0068

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Resendez, S. L., Dome, M., Gormley, G., Franco, D., Nevárez, N., Hamid, A. A., et al. (2013). μ-Opioid receptors within subregions of the striatum mediate pair bond formation through parallel yet distinct reward mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 33, 9140–9149. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4123-12.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rilling, J. K., Winslow, J. T., O'brien, D., Gutman, D. A., Hoffman, J. M., and Kilts, C. D. (2001). Neural correlates of maternal separation in rhesus monkeys. Biol. Psychiatry 49, 146–157. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00977-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schino, G., and Troisi, A. (1992). Opiate receptor blockade in juvenile macaques: effect on affiliative interactions with their mothers and group companions. Brain Res. 576, 125–130. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90617-I

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schino, G., Scucchi, S., Maestripieri, D., and Turillazzi, P. G. (1988). Allogrooming as a tension-reduction mechanism: a behavioral approach. Am. J. Primatol. 16, 43–50. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350160106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (1984). Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature 308, 541–543. doi: 10.1038/308541a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimada, M. (2013). Dynamics of the temporal structures of playing clusters and cliques among wild chimpanzees in Mahale Mountains national park. Primates 54, 245–257. doi: 10.1007/s10329-013-0348-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sia, A. T., Lim, Y., Lim, E. C. P., Goh, R. W. C., Law, H. Y., Landau, R., et al. (2008). A118G single nucleotide polymorphism of human μ-opioid receptor gene influences pain perception and patient-controlled intravenous morphine consumption after intrathecal morphine for postcesarean analgesia. Anesthesiology 109, 520–526. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318182af21

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, M. A., and Jensen, R. A. (1985). The prolonged effects of naloxone on play behavior and feeding in the rat. Behav. Neural Biol. 44, 509–514. doi: 10.1016/S0163-1047(85)91024-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, M. A., and Jensen, R. A. (1986). The effects of naloxone and cage size on social play and activity in isolated young rats. Behav. Neural Biol. 45, 155–168. doi: 10.1016/S0163-1047(86)90739-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siviy, S. M., and Panksepp, J. (1985). Energy balance and play in juvenile rats. Physiol. Behav. 35, 435–441. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(85)90320-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siviy, S. M., and Panksepp, J. (1987). Sensory modulation of juvenile play in rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 20, 39–55. doi: 10.1002/dev.420200108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanton, M. E., Wallstrom, J., and Levine, S. (1987). Maternal contact inhibits pituitary-adrenal stress responses in preweanling rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 20, 131–145. doi: 10.1002/dev.420200204

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suomi, S. J., Eisele, C. D., Grady, S. A., and Harlow, H. F. (1975). Depressive behavior in adult monkeys following separation from family environment. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 84, 576–578. doi: 10.1037/h0077066

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taha, S. A. (2010). Preference or fat? Revisiting opioid effects on food intake. Physiol. Behav. 100, 429–437. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.02.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tardif, S. D., Coleman, K., Hobbs, T. R., and Lutz, C. (2013). IACUC Review of nonhuman primate research. ILAR J. 54, 234–245. doi: 10.1093/ilar/ilt040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tops, M., Koole, S. L., Ijzerman, H., and Buisman-Pijlman, F. T. A. (2014). Why social attachment and oxytocin protect against addiction and stress: insights from the dynamics between ventral and dorsal corticostriatal systems. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 119, 39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.07.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trezza, V., and Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J. (2008a). Cannabinoid and opioid modulation of social play behavior in adolescent rats: differential behavioral mechanisms. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18, 519–530. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.03.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trezza, V., and Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J. (2008b). Bidirectional cannabinoid modulation of social behavior in adolescent rats. Psychopharmacology 197, 217–227. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-1025-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trezza, V., Campolongo, P., and Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J. (2011a). Evaluating the rewarding nature of social interactions in laboratory animals. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 444–458. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trezza, V., Damsteegt, R., Achterberg, E. J. M., and Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J. (2011b). Nucleus Accumbens μ-Opioid receptors mediate social reward. J. Neurosci. 31, 6362–6370. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5492-10.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troisi, A., Frazzetto, G., and Carola, V. (2010). Social hedonic capacity is associated with the A118G polymorphism of the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) in adult healthy volunteers and psychiatric patients. Soc. Neurosci. 17, 1–10. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2010.482786

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troisi, A., Frazzetto, G., Carola, V., Di Lorenzo, G., Coviello, M., D'Aamato, F. R., et al. (2011). Social hedonic capacity is associated with the A118G polymorphism of the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) in adult healthy volunteers and psychiatric patients. Soc. Neurosci. 6, 88–97. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2010.482786

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Den Berg, C. L., Hol, T., Van Ree, J. M., Spruijt, B. M., Everts, H., and Koolhaas, J. M. (1999). Play is indispensable for an adequate development of coping with social challenges in the rat. Dev. Psychobiol. 34, 129–138.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Hasselt, F. N., Tieskens, J. M., Trezza, V., Krugers, H. J., Vanderschuren, L. J., and Joëls, M. (2012). Within-litter variation in maternal care received by individual pups correlates with adolescent social play behavior in male rats. Physiol. Behav. 106, 701–706. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.12.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Oers, H. J. J., De Kloet, E. R., Whelan, T., and Levine, S. (1998). Maternal deprivation effect on the infant's neural stress markers is reversed by tactile stimulation and feeding but not by suppressing corticosterone. J. Neurosci. 18, 10171–10179.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Ree, J. M., and Niesink, R. J. (1983). Low doses of beta-endorphin increase social contacts of rats tested in dyadic encounters. Life Sci. 33(Suppl. 1), 611–614. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(83)90577-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J., Niesink, R. J. M., Spruijt, B. M., and Van Ree, J. M. (1995c). Effects of morphine on different aspects of social play in juvenile rats. Psychopharmacology 117, 225–231. doi: 10.1007/BF02245191

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J., Niesink, R. J. M., Spruijt, B. M., and Van Ree, J. M. (1995d). μ- and κ-opioid receptor-mediated opioid effects on social play in juvenile rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 276, 257–266. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(95)00040-R

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J., Spruijt, B. M., Hol, T., Niesink, R. J. M., and Van Ree, J. M. (1996). Sequential analysis of social play behavior in juvenile rats: effects of morphine. Behav. Brain Res. 72, 89–95. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(96)00060-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J., Stein, E. A., Wiegant, V. M., and Van Ree, J. M. (1995e). Social play alters regional brain opioid receptor binding in juvenile rats. Brain Res. 680, 148–156. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00256-P

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderschuren, L. J. M., Niesink, R. J. M., Spruijt, B. M., and Van Ree, J. M. (1995b). Influence of environmental factors on social play behavior of juvenile rats. Physiol. Behav. 58, 119–123. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)00385-I

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderschuren, L. J., Spruijt, B. M., Hol, T., Niesink, R. J., and Van Ree, J. M. (1995a). Sequential analysis of social play behavior in juvenile rats: effects of morphine. Behav. Brain Res. 72, 89–95. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(96)00060-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vettin, J., and Todt, D. (2005). Human laughter, social play, and play vocalizations of non-human primates: an evolutionary approach. Behaviour 142, 217–240. doi: 10.1163/1568539053627640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Way, B. M., Taylor, S. E., and Eisenberger, N. I. (2009). Variation in the μ-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) is associated with dispositional and neural sensitivity to social rejection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 15079–15084. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812612106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D'Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., et al. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 847–854. doi: 10.1038/nn1276

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeomans, M. R., and Gray, R. W. (1997). Effects of naltrexone on food intake and changes in subjective appetite during eating: evidence for opioid involvement in the appetizer effect. Physiol. Behav. 62, 15–21. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00101-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, M., Gosnell, B. A., and Kelley, A. E. (1998). Intake of high-fat food is selectively enhanced by Mu opioid receptor stimulation within the nucleus accumbens. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 285, 908–914.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Wang, D., Johnson, A. D., Papp, A. C., and Sadee, W. (2005). Allelic expression imbalance of human mu opioid receptor (OPRM1) caused by variant A118G. J. Biol. Chem. 280, 32618–32624. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M504942200

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zubieta, J.-K., Smith, Y. R., Bueller, J. A., Xu, Y., Kilbourn, M. R., Jewett, D. M., et al. (2001). Regional mu opioid receptor regulation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain. Science 293, 311–315. doi: 10.1126/science.1060952

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: OPRM1, affiliative behaviors, opioids, social reward, social touch, social comfort, social support, μ-opioid

Citation: Loseth GE, Ellingsen D-M and Leknes S (2014) State-dependent μ-opioid modulation of social motivation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:430. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00430

Received: 09 July 2014; Accepted: 25 November 2014;
Published online: 12 December 2014.

Edited by:

India Morrison, Linköping University, Sweden

Reviewed by:

Annett Schirmer, National University of Singapore, Singapore
Mikhail Votinov, RWTH Aachen University Hospital, Germany

Copyright © 2014 Loseth, Ellingsen and Leknes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Guro E. Loseth, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Forskningsveien 3A, Oslo 0553, Norway e-mail: guroelo@student.uio.no

Download