Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 12 February 2016
Sec. Pathological Conditions
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00015

Editorial: Avoidance: From Basic Science to Psychopathology

  • Neuroscience, Syracuse DVA Medical Center, Stress and Motivated Behavior Institute, Rutgers Biomedical Health Sciences, Newark, NJ, USA

The Editorial on the Research Topic
Avoidance: From Basic Science to Psychopathology

As a means of coping, avoidance encompasses thoughts and efforts toward prevention of future aversive experiences and events. Avoidance has been and remains controversial. Avoidance is accepted as a construct in many areas of research, but is roundly disdained in others. Why is such a critical feature of coping both acknowledged as such, but almost reluctantly studied?

For one, avoidance is often conflated with fear. Fear is an emotion. Threat conditions which engender fear also engender a host of physiological and behavioral responses (Ledoux, 2013). In animals, exposure to aversive stimuli or cues associated with aversive stimuli induce freezing, fleeing, or aggressive displays depending on the context of exposure—all behavioral manifestations of threat (Osada et al.). Responses to threat are relatively simple, engendered and refined through a circumscribed neural circuitry (Ledoux and Muller, 1997; Delgado et al., 2008). Fear and defensive responses to threat are readily and almost universally acquired. Those under threat (Shors and Servatius, 1997), stress (Servatius and Shors, 1994), and fearful (Mosig et al.) have a generalized facilitation of associative learning making threat and fear more pervasive. The engendering of fear and its expression is a highly researched concept; advancements in fear and the neurobiology subsuming fear is among the most notable and exhaustive neurobiological achievements in the past half century.

By comparison, avoidance is a fairly sophisticated construct. Avoidance is the situational evaluation of likelihoods, efficacy of responses, and costs. Avoidance is often weighed against alternatives; alternatives with differing or competing motivations (Beck et al., 2011; Fernando et al., 2014; Ilango et al.; Sheynin et al.). For many applications and circumstances fear and avoidance seem to be inseparable, so the terms become conflated. In the vernacular, fear is an immediate response to stressors and fear motivates avoidance. Therefore, in many circumstances those avoiding are expected to be experiencing fear. However, the empirical literature provides ample evidence that the processes are distinct (Bolles, 1968; Seligman and Johnston, 1973; Rio-Alamos et al.) and while the neurocircuitry, such as the lateral habenula (Shumake et al., 2010; Ilango et al., 2013) and cerebellum (Steinmetz et al., 1993) overlaps (Freeman et al., 1996, 1997; Bravo-Rivera et al., 2014; Campese et al.; McCue et al.; Jiao et al.), their influences on these processes potentially do not. Further distinguishing fear and threat from avoidance, septal (Thomas and Van Atta, 1972; Hedges et al., 1975) and hippocampal lesions (Cominski et al.) are known to facilitate avoidance acquisition, whereas these brain regions are critically involved in fear conditioning when intact (Kim et al., 1993; Desmedt et al., 1998; Knight et al., 2004).

As a research topic, avoidance all but disappeared through the 1990's, a phenomenon that has been noted in a number of recent reviews (Dymond and Roche, 2009; Krypotos et al.). Reduction in the study of avoidance stemmed from theoretical and practical considerations. In humans, the rise of institutional review boards and the reluctance of institutions and investigators to study reactions to aversive, painful stimulation or uncomfortable situations stymied progress. Added to these concerns, there were growing controversies regarding the role of awareness and instructional sets in human associative learning. Explicit information stemming from the consent form and instructions complicated experimental designs and interpretations of acquisition. Now there are but a few laboratories across the world with a vested interest in studying avoidance acquisition and extinction in humans, the TOPIC highlights several (Myers et al., 2013; Schlund et al., 2013; Sheynin et al., 2015; Cameron et al.; Moustafa et al.). Otherwise, avoidance and coping are primarily studied through self-report survey instruments which document coping strategies (Snell et al., 2011; Ayers et al., 2014).

In animals, the meteoric rise of electrophysiological and molecular techniques made reductionistic procedures ever more popular. This was in the face of Bolles formulation of species specific defense reactions (SSDRs) (Bolles, 1970). A reading of Bolles strongly suggests that the most popular applications of avoidance learning in animals were reducible to reflexes. Avoidance that relied on SSDRs would be difficult to distinguish from fear responses or their modification and would be better studied in clearer procedures. Bolles did not negate avoidance learning, but argued that avoidance was obscured by SSDRs and arbitrary avoidances provided clear evidence of avoidance, which would be slowly and incrementally acquired. The Bolles position muddled already difficult discussions concerning reinforcement in avoidance acquisition (Bersh, 2001; Dinsmoor, 2001; Hineline, 2001). The many criticisms of avoidance learning and its proper interpretation became more and more inaccessible to the average reader and more esoteric in argument. The zeitgeist is avoidance responses either a SSDR or require the suppression of SSDRs. SSDRs reflect fear and fear is more clearly examined in freezing (Fanselow and Poulos, 2005) or by examining its exaggeration of acoustic startle responses (Davis, 2006) under conditions in which control procedures are established to reveal associativity. Although arbitrary responses provide clear evidence of avoidance (Avcu et al.; Bravo-Rivera et al., 2014; Servatius et al.), these procedures became more and more unpopular. An increase in demand for throughput (self-contained, relatively short, and easily scored procedures) is at odds with the seemingly slow development of avoidance. In an unfortunate happenchance, “passive avoidance” remains in the parlance of behavioral neuroscience, but the high-throughput tasks and protocols to study “passive avoidance” are essentially assessing punishment.

Modern theorists of avoidance have moved away from response dynamics to cognitive processes driving response dynamics. Humans and mammals form expectancies. Avoidance expression reflects propositional knowledge but also the context in which knowledge is to be expressed (Seligman and Johnston, 1973; Lovibond et al., 2008, 2009; Dymond and Roche, 2009). Knowledge is subject to error and error correction (Myers et al.; Sheynin et al.). The difficulties encountered in learning arbitrary responses may not rest in how unnatural such responses are to humans and animals (Dinsmoor, 2001), but in the pressures of time/distance (Fanselow and Lester, 1988) and a cost/benefit analysis. There is a need for conceptual bridges between propositional knowledge central to expectancy models of avoidance and animal research in which processes are resolvable to response dynamics (response selectivity, strength of responding, and probability of responding; Krypotos et al.).

Recently, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) in the United States embarked on research domain criteria (RDoC) to facilitate integration across levels of analysis and between diagnostic boundaries. The Negative Valence System encompasses acute responses to threat (fear) and inferred threat (anxiety), with escape/avoidance learning and expression emerging with sustained threat. In the NIMH working group discussion, ambivalence was expressed concerning whether sustained threat is distinct from acute threat, except for the time dimension. An undercurrent is that the sustained threat dimension, and by implication avoidance and escape, is not distinctive of acute conditions. The bounding conditions of avoidance are not only the duration of threat (acute/sustained), but the perceived intensity of threat, its perceived proximity, and the utility of responses or efforts. For perceived proximity of time, parametric manipulations of signal-shock intervals illustrate this point. Shuttling as the requisite response (a modified SSDR) is efficiently acquired with CS-US intervals of 10–20 s (Black, 1963). In lever press (not an SSDR) avoidance, escape behaviors predominate when signal-shock intervals are less than 20-s (Berger and Brush, 1975), with very few avoidance responses expressed after days of training (Servatius et al.). However, knowledge about avoidance is acquired; avoidance is not expressed (Servatius et al.). Using a crossover design those trained with a 10-s warning signal and exhibiting nominal avoidance rates displayed greater than 60% avoidance when switched to 60-s warning signal—nearly asymptotic performance of those trained initially with a 60-s warning signal. As to stressor intensity, shuttle box avoidance is efficiently acquired with foot shocks of moderately low intensity (0.2–0.5 mA) (Levine, 1966) with decrements apparent with shock intensity greater than 1.0 mA (Moyer and Korn, 1964). In contrast, lever press avoidance is efficiently acquired with shock intensities of 1.0–2.0 mA (Berger and Brush, 1975; Servatius et al., 2008; Avcu et al.). These features illustrate that avoidance acquired with arbitrary responses differ in a number of parameters from those modifying reflexive responses or “natural” responses, which are in turn distinct from fear responses. On the other hand, recent work also shows fear is more nuanced as fear contributes to sustained processes such as foraging (Kim et al., 2014).

In subsequent position papers concerning RDoCs, fear and threat processes feature prominently, whereas avoidance and coping do not (Cuthbert et al., 2003; Insel et al., 2010; Cuthbert, 2015). This is indeed unfortunate. An opportunity to intensify efforts in avoidance research is being missed. The mental health implications are extensive. Psychologically healthy coping strikes a balance between avoidance (responding in anticipation of aversive stimulation) and escape (responding in the presence of the stimulation) and competing motivations of approach (Ilango et al.; Ilango et al.). Deviant forms of avoidance are evident in autism (Richer, 1976), anxiety (Ly and Roelofs, 2009); (Kashdan et al., 2014), phobias, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; North et al., 2004; Kashdan et al., 2009), major depression (Ottenbreit et al., 2014) and suicide (Dixon et al., 1991). Over-expression of avoidance, as in anxiety disorders and PTSD, insulates one from aversive thoughts or experiences at the expense of self-limiting interpersonal and environmental interactions. Under-expression of avoidance, as in depression or suicidality, unduly exposes one to aversive thoughts and experiences that would be otherwise controllable, severely depleting resources and progressing down a demoralizing spiral.

Diathesis models of mental illness capture avoidance biases as dynamic interactions of vulnerabilities (genes, epigenetics, personality, and developmental phases) with risk factors (psychological stressors, physical injuries) ultimately expressed as psychopathology. For example, behaviorally inhibited temperament, withdrawal in the face of social and nonsocial challenges, is a vulnerability factor for anxiety disorders (Moffitt et al., 2007). Humans expressing behavioral inhibition (BI) display enhanced avoidance expression (Sheynin et al.), and enhanced new motor learning (Caulfield et al.; Holloway et al., 2014), especially under degraded contingencies (Holloway et al., 2014; Allen et al.). Facilitated avoidance acquisition (Avcu et al.; Beck et al.; Jiao et al.; Servatius et al.) and new motor learning (Ricart et al., 2011a,b) are also apparent in Wistar-Kyoto rats, an animal model of BI temperament. Further, avoidance extinction is typically more difficult to obtain than extinction of fear. This is likely amplified by individual differences (Avcu et al.; Cominski et al.). Uncovering of neurobiological processes biasing avoidance expression and extinction has the promise of providing targets for individualized therapeutics and treatments for a number of psychopathological disorders.

Hence, there continues to be a need for an integration of human and animal research focused on coping and in particular avoidance coping. Model systems of avoidance that allow for bidirectional modifications of acquisition, expression, and extinction—protocols that allow for increased as well as decreased expression—are useful in translating basic science to psychopathology. By extension, RDoC constructs should be sensitive to individual differences, both accentuating and diminishing in the appearance of avoidance.

An open discussion of what features constitute fear, threat, anxiety, and avoidance would not only benefit basic science and psychopathology, but areas of research that are otherwise ignoring the infighting and are making substantial progress in improving health (e.g., fear-avoidance model of pain Vlaeyen et al., 1995; Crombez et al., 2012).

Approach Avoidance: Have no fear!

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and approved it for publication.

Funding

Supported by the Stress and Motivated Behavior Institute through funding from the Department of Defense, Armaments Research Development Engineering Center.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ayers, C. R., Castriotta, N., Dozier, M. E., Espejo, E. P., and Porter, B. (2014). Behavioral and experiential avoidance in patients with hoarding disorder. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 45, 408–414. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, K. D., Jiao, X., Ricart, T. M., Myers, C. E., Minor, T. R., Pang, K. C., et al. (2011). Vulnerability factors in anxiety: strain and sex differences in the use of signals associated with non-threat during the acquisition and extinction of active-avoidance behavior. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35, 1659–1670. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, D. F., and Brush, F. R. (1975). Rapid acquisition of discrete-trial lever-press avoidance: effects of signal-shock interval. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 24, 227–239. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1975.24-227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bersh, P. J. (2001). The molarity of molecular theory and the molecularity of molar theory. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 75, 348–350; discussion: 367–378. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2001.75-348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Black, A. H. (1963). The effects of CS-US interval on avoidance conditioning in the rat. Can. J. Psychol. 17, 174–182.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bolles, R. C. (1968). Autonomic indices of fear: the collapse of an idea. Psychol. Rep. 23, 1249–1250. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1968.23.3f.1249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolles, R. C. (1970). Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychol. Rev. 77, 32. doi: 10.1037/h0028589

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bravo-Rivera, C., Roman-Ortiz, C., Brignoni-Perez, E., Sotres-Bayon, F., and Quirk, G. J. (2014). Neural structures mediating expression and extinction of platform-mediated avoidance. J. Neurosci. 34, 9736–9742. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0191-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., Van Damme, S., Vlaeyen, J. W., and Karoly, P. (2012). Fear-avoidance model of chronic pain: the next generation. Clin. J. Pain 28, 475–483. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e3182385392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuthbert, B. N. (2015). Research Domain Criteria: toward future psychiatric nosologies. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 17, 89–97.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cuthbert, B. N., Lang, P. J., Strauss, C., Drobes, D., Patrick, C. J., and Bradley, M. M. (2003). The psychophysiology of anxiety disorder: fear memory imagery. Psychophysiology 40, 407–422. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. (2006). Neural systems involved in fear and anxiety measured with fear-potentiated startle. Am. Psychol. 61, 741–756. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.8.741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delgado, M. R., Nearing, K. I., Ledoux, J. E., and Phelps, E. A. (2008). Neural circuitry underlying the regulation of conditioned fear and its relation to extinction. Neuron 59, 829–838. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmedt, A., Garcia, R., and Jaffard, R. (1998). Differential modulation of changes in hippocampal-septal synaptic excitability by the amygdala as a function of either elemental or contextual fear conditioning in mice. J. Neurosci. 18, 480–487.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dinsmoor, J. A. (2001). Stimuli inevitably generated by behavior that avoids electric shock are inherently reinforcing. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 75, 311. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2001.75-311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, W. A., Heppner, P. P., and Anderson, W. P. (1991). Problem-solving appraisal, stress, hopelessness, and suicide ideation in a college population. J. Couns. Psychol. 38, 51. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.38.1.51

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dymond, S., and Roche, B. (2009). A contemporary behavior analysis of anxiety and avoidance. Behav. Anal. 32, 7–27.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S., and Lester, L. S. (1988). “A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior,” in Evolution and Learning, eds R. C. Bolles and M. D. Beecher (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 185–211.

Fanselow, M. S., and Poulos, A. M. (2005). The neuroscience of mammalian associative learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 207–234. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernando, A. B., Urcelay, G. P., Mar, A. C., Dickinson, A., and Robbins, T. W. (2014). Safety signals as instrumental reinforcers during free-operant avoidance. Learn. Mem. 21, 488–497. doi: 10.1101/lm.034603.114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, J. H. Jr., Cuppernell, C., Flannery, K., and Gabriel, M. (1996). Context-specific multi-site cingulate cortical, limbic thalamic, and hippocampal neuronal activity during concurrent discriminative approach and avoidance training in rabbits. J. Neurosci. 16, 1538–1549.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Freeman, J. H. Jr., Weible, A., Rossi, J., and Gabriel, M. (1997). Lesions of the entorhinal cortex disrupt behavioral and neuronal responses to context change during extinction of discriminative avoidance behavior. Exp. Brain Res. 115, 445–457. doi: 10.1007/PL00005714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedges, A. S., Van Atta, L., and Thomas, J. B. (1975). Septal lesions facilitate the shift from conditioned escape to conditioned avoidance behavior in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 14, 25–30. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(75)90136-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hineline, P. N. (2001). Beyond the molar-molecular distinction: we need multiscaled analyses. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 75, 342–347; discussion: 367–378. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2001.75-342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holloway, J. L., Allen, M. T., Myers, C. E., and Servatius, R. J. (2014). Behaviorally inhibited individuals demonstrate significantly enhanced conditioned response acquisition under non-optimal learning conditions. Behav. Brain Res. 261, 49–55. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.10.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilango, A., Shumake, J., Wetzel, W., Scheich, H., and Ohl, F. W. (2013). Electrical stimulation of lateral habenula during learning: frequency-dependent effects on acquisition but not retrieval of a two-way active avoidance response. PLoS ONE 8:e65684. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., et al. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashdan, T. B., Goodman, F. R., Machell, K. A., Kleiman, E. M., Monfort, S. S., Ciarrochi, J., et al. (2014). A contextual approach to experiential avoidance and social anxiety: evidence from an experimental interaction and daily interactions of people with social anxiety disorder. Emotion 14, 769–781. doi: 10.1037/a0035935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashdan, T. B., Morina, N., and Priebe, S. (2009). Post-traumatic stress disorder, social anxiety disorder, and depression in survivors of the Kosovo War: experiential avoidance as a contributor to distress and quality of life. J. Anxiety Disord. 23, 185–196. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, E., Kim, E. J., Yeh, R., Shin, M., Bobman, J., Krasne, F. B., et al. (2014). Amygdaloid and non-amygdaloid fear both influence avoidance of risky foraging in hungry rats. Proc. Biol. Sci. 281:20133357. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.3357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. J., Rison, R. A., and Fanselow, M. S. (1993). Effects of amygdala, hippocampus, and periaqueductal gray lesions on short- and long-term contextual fear. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 1093–1098. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.6.1093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, D. C., Cheng, D. T., Smith, C. N., Stein, E. A., and Helmstetter, F. J. (2004). Neural substrates mediating human delay and trace fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 24, 218–228. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0433-03.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ledoux, J. E. (2013). The slippery slope of fear. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 155–156. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ledoux, J. E., and Muller, J. (1997). Emotional memory and psychopathology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 352, 1719–1726. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1997.0154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, S. (1966). UCS intensity and avoidance learning. J. Exp. Psychol. 71, 163–164. doi: 10.1037/h0022690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, P. F., Mitchell, C. J., Minard, E., Brady, A., and Menzies, R. G. (2009). Safety behaviours preserve threat beliefs: protection from extinction of human fear conditioning by an avoidance response. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 716–720. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, P. F., Saunders, J. C., Weidemann, G., and Mitchell, C. J. (2008). Evidence for expectancy as a mediator of avoidance and anxiety in a laboratory model of human avoidance learning. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61, 1199–1216. doi: 10.1080/17470210701503229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ly, V., and Roelofs, K. (2009). Social anxiety and cognitive expectancy of aversive outcome in avoidance conditioning. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 840–847. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Milne, B. J., Melchior, M., Goldberg, D., et al. (2007). Generalized anxiety disorder and depression: childhood risk factors in a birth cohort followed to age 32. Psychol. Med. 37, 441–452. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moyer, K. E., and Korn, J. H. (1964). Effect of ucs intensity on the acquisition and extinction of an avoidance response. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 352–359. doi: 10.1037/h0039933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, C. E., Moustafa, A. A., Sheynin, J., Vanmeenen, K. M., Gilbertson, M. W., Orr, S. P., et al. (2013). Learning to obtain reward, but not avoid punishment, is affected by presence of PTSD symptoms in male veterans: empirical data and computational model. PLoS ONE 8:e72508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

North, C. S., Pfefferbaum, B., Tivis, L., Kawasaki, A., Reddy, C., and Spitznagel, E. L. (2004). The course of posttraumatic stress disorder in a follow-up study of survivors of the Oklahoma City bombing. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry 16, 209–215. doi: 10.1080/10401230490522034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ottenbreit, N. D., Dobson, K. S., and Quigley, L. (2014). An examination of avoidance in major depression in comparison to social anxiety disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 56, 82–90. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ricart, T. M., De Niear, M. A., Jiao, X., Pang, K. C., Beck, K. D., and Servatius, R. J. (2011a). Deficient proactive interference of eyeblink conditioning in Wistar-Kyoto rats. Behav. Brain Res. 216, 59–65. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ricart, T. M., Jiao, X., Pang, K. C., Beck, K. D., and Servatius, R. J. (2011b). Classical and instrumental conditioning of eyeblink responses in Wistar-Kyoto and Sprague-Dawley rats. Behav. Brain Res. 216, 414–418. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.08.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richer, J. (1976). The social-avoidance behaviour of autistic children. Anim. Behav. 24, 898–906. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(76)80020-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlund, M. W., Hudgins, C. D., Magee, S., and Dymond, S. (2013). Neuroimaging the temporal dynamics of human avoidance to sustained threat. Behav. Brain Res. 257, 148–155. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.09.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. P., and Johnston, J. C. (1973). “A cognitive theory of avoidance learning,” in Contemporary Approaches to Conditioning and Learning, eds F. J. McGuigan and D. B. Lumsden (Washington, DC: Winston and Sons), 69–110.

Servatius, R. J., Jiao, X., Beck, K. D., Pang, K. C., and Minor, T. R. (2008). Rapid avoidance acquisition in Wistar-Kyoto rats. Behav. Brain Res. 192, 191–197. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Servatius, R. J., and Shors, T. J. (1994). Exposure to inescapable stress persistently facilitates associative and nonassociative learning in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 108, 1101–1106. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.108.6.1101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheynin, J., Moustafa, A. A., Beck, K. D., Servatius, R. J., and Myers, C. E. (2015). Testing the role of reward and punishment sensitivity in avoidance behavior: a computational modeling approach. Behav. Brain Res. 283, 121–138. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shors, T. J., and Servatius, R. J. (1997). The contribution of stressor intensity, duration, and context to the stress-induced facilitation of associative learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 68, 92–96. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1997.3763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shumake, J., Ilango, A., Scheich, H., Wetzel, W., and Ohl, F. W. (2010). Differential neuromodulation of acquisition and retrieval of avoidance learning by the lateral habenula and ventral tegmental area. J. Neurosci. 30, 5876–5883. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3604-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snell, D. L., Siegert, R. J., Hay-Smith, E. J., and Surgenor, L. J. (2011). Associations between illness perceptions, coping styles and outcome after mild traumatic brain injury: preliminary results from a cohort study. Brain Inj. 25, 1126–1138. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2011.607786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinmetz, J. E., Logue, S. F., and Miller, D. P. (1993). Using signaled barpressing tasks to study the neural substrates of appetitive and aversive learning in rats: behavioral manipulations and cerebellar lesions. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 941–954. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.6.941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, J. B., and Van Atta, L. (1972). Hyperirritability, lever-press avoidance, and septal lesions in the albino rat. Physiol. Behav. 8, 225–232. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(72)90365-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlaeyen, J. W., Kole-Snijders, A. M., Boeren, R. G., and Van Eek, H. (1995). Fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain and its relation to behavioral performance. Pain 62, 363–372. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(94)00279-N

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: avoidance learning, anxiety disorders, depressive disorder, research domain criteria, expectancy, animal models of mental disorders, association learning, escape

Citation: Servatius RJ (2016) Editorial: Avoidance: From Basic Science to Psychopathology. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:15. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00015

Received: 08 January 2016; Accepted: 28 January 2016;
Published: 12 February 2016.

Edited and reviewed by: Nuno Sousa, University of Minho, Portugal

Copyright © 2016 Servatius. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Richard J. Servatius, richard.servatius@va.gov

Download