Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 03 August 2016
Sec. Motivation and Reward
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00154

Compulsivity Across the Pathological Misuse of Drug and Non-Drug Rewards

Paula Banca1 Neil A. Harrison2 Valerie Voon1,3,4*
  • 1Behavioral and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
  • 2Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
  • 4Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK

Behavioral adaptation is required for the successful navigation of a constantly changing environment. Impairments in behavioral flexibility are commonly observed in psychiatric disorders including those of addiction. This study investigates two distinct facets of compulsivity, namely reversal learning and attentional set shifting, implicating orbitofrontal and lateral prefrontal regions respectively, across disorders of primary and secondary rewards. Obese subjects with and without binge eating disorder (BED), individuals with compulsive sexual behaviors (CSB), alcohol dependence (AD) and pathological video-gaming (VG) were tested with two computerized tasks: the probabilistic reversal task (trials to criterion and win-stay/lose-shift errors) and the intra/extra-dimensional set shift task (IED). Individuals with AD and pathological VG were slower at reversal learning irrespective of valence, with AD subjects more likely to perseverate after losses. Compared to obese subjects without BED, BED subjects were worse at reversal learning to wins but better at losses highlighting valence effects as a function of binge eating. CSB subjects demonstrated enhanced sensitivity to reward outcomes with faster acquisition and greater perseveration with higher magnitude rewards. We further show an impairment in attentional set shifting in individuals with BED and AD relative to healthy volunteers (HV). This study provides evidence for commonalities and differences in two distinct dimensions of behavioral inflexibility across disorders of compulsivity. We summarize studies on compulsivity subtypes within this same patient population. We emphasize commonalities in AD and BED with impairments across a range of compulsivity indices, perhaps supporting pathological binge eating as a form of behavioral addiction. We further emphasize commonalities in reversal learning across disorders and the crucial role of valence effects. These findings highlight the role of behavioral inflexibility and compulsivity as a relevant domain in defining dimensional psychiatry and the identification of relevant cognitive endophenotypes as targets for therapeutic modulation.

Introduction

The ability to adjust behaviors is crucial for optimal navigation in a constantly changing environment. Behavioral inflexibility has wide individual variability and is a typical cognitive feature in disorders of addiction, commonly linked to compulsivity. Compulsivity has been suggested to be a heterogeneous construct, which can be divided into subtypes with distinct but overlapping neural networks (Dalley et al., 2011). Here we focus on two specific facets of compulsivity namely reversal learning and attentional set shifting. Reversal learning measures the capacity to flexibly switch choices with changes in contingencies and implicates orbitofrontal cortices whereas attentional set shifting is the ability to shift response sets to a previously irrelevant dimension and implicates lateral prefrontal cortices (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2003; McAlonan and Brown, 2003; O’Doherty et al., 2003; Hornak et al., 2004; Robbins, 2007). Deficits in these markers of cognitive inflexibility have been classically studied across species (for review on reversal learning see Izquierdo et al., 2016; and on attentional set shifting see Brown and Tait, 2016; Izquierdo et al., 2016). Many of these studies have assessed these two markers in disorders of addiction to drug rewards but, although there is a large number of studies on cognitive flexibility in experimental species for natural rewards, fewer studies in humans have focused on disorders of natural rewards. We investigate these cognitive processes and the influence of the role of outcome valence across disorders characterized by the pathological misuse of drug and non-drug rewards. In keeping with the trend towards dimensional psychiatry (Insel et al., 2010), we seek to compare the cross-diagnostic neurocognitive profile focusing on compulsivity to enhance our understanding of current psychiatric disorder classifications (Robbins et al., 2012).

In reversal learning paradigms, subjects are required to adapt their internal representations and choices when feedback demonstrates an outcome change (Cools et al., 2002; Clarke et al., 2005). Reversal learning is strongly impaired in cocaine use disorders (Fillmore and Rush, 2006; Ersche et al., 2008; Camchong et al., 2011; Fernández-Serrano et al., 2012). Data from amphetamine is less consistent. One study has found unimpaired reversal learning in amphetamine and opiate users (Ersche et al., 2008) but others have shown that even brief exposure to methamphetamine results in selective impairments on reversal tasks in rats (Cheng et al., 2007; White et al., 2009; Izquierdo et al., 2010; for review of reversal learning in addiction see Izquierdo and Jentsch, 2012). Reversal learning impairments are also not prominent in alcohol dependence (AD). Impaired discrimination reversal has been shown following aversive eye-blink conditioning in abstinent chronic alcoholics (Fortier et al., 2008) and AD individuals have also been shown to be slower at reversal learning in a deterministic reversal task although there were no differences in the primary outcome measures (Vanes et al., 2014). Impairments have also been found in pathological gambling using a probabilistic reversal learning task for gain and loss (Patterson et al., 2006; de Ruiter et al., 2009). In obese women, deficits in both acquisition and reversal learning have been shown specific to food cues using an appetitive reversal learning task (Zhang et al., 2014).

The distinct yet related measure of cognitive inflexibility, cognitive set-shifting, as measured by the wisconsin card sorting task (WCST) or Intra-/Extra-dimensional set shifting (IED) task, probes the capacity to switch responding to previously irrelevant stimuli, requiring attentional flexibility. Studies with current amphetamine or methamphetamine users report reduced set shifting in users (Ornstein et al., 2000; Clark et al., 2006) with improvement in performance with prolonged drug abstinence (Toomey et al., 2003; Johanson et al., 2006; van den Hout et al., 2009). In AD individuals, alcohol misuse severity and impairments in set-shifting have previously been shown to be linked to relapse (Pothiyil and Alex, 2013). Impairments in the WCST have been shown particularly in chronic alcoholics with a history in alcoholism of greater than 10 years (Tarter, 1973). Furthermore, set-shifting impairments associated with AD have been shown to not improve after a period of abstinence (Nowakowska et al., 2007).

Poor set-shifting and increased perseveration have also been shown in obese individuals with and without binge eating disorder (BED; Duchesne et al., 2010; Wu et al., 2014) and in children and adolescent with excess weight (Cserjési et al., 2007; Verdejo-García et al., 2010) using the WCST and the Trail-making test. Contradictory results have been shown in pathological gambling, where both reduced set-shifting in the IED task (Grant et al., 2011) and normal performance on the WCST task (Goudriaan et al., 2006) has been demonstrated.

Here we investigate the behavioral markers of compulsivity focusing on reversal learning and set shifting across several different addictive disorders. The reversal learning task was modified to investigate whether different pathologies would show different sensitivity to reward and loss magnitude outcomes. This is important considering that several studies have provided evidence for dissociable neural responses to reward and punishment outcomes (O’Doherty et al., 2001; Remijnse et al., 2005; Robinson et al., 2010). We compare abstinent AD individuals and obese subjects with and without BED with their own healthy volunteers (HV). The rodent model of sucrose binge eating (Avena et al., 2008), which has been showing many similarities with models of substance-use disorders, has been suggesting the binge-eating pattern of food intake to be a crucial subtype that differentiates obese individuals. We subdivided our obese group into individuals with and without binge eating behaviors to assess the relationship between this important feature (pattern of food intake) and the constructs assessed by these cognitive tasks. On an exploratory basis, we also compare individuals with pathological video-gaming (VG) behaviors and with compulsive sexual behaviors (CSB) with their own matched healthy controls. We hypothesize that AD and obese individuals with, but not without, BED will be characterized by impairments in reversal learning and set shifting cognitive processes. Furthermore, we expect to see a differential influence of valence (reward/loss) in behavior across disorders, similarly to our previous findings in risk-taking reporting that pathological choices are influenced by factors of valence, probability, and magnitude (Voon et al., 2015c).

Materials and Methods

Recruitment

Healthy controls and AD, Obese with and without BED, VG and CSB individuals were recruited via community-and university-based advertisements in Cambridge. All subject groups were age- and gender-matched with their own HV. All volunteers completed the same basic interview assessment to detect all types of compulsive behaviors and exclude other psychiatric disorders. All diagnoses were made by a psychiatrist. Obese individuals had a body mass index (BMI) of ≥30 and those with BED fulfilled DSM-IV-TR BED criteria in addition (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Individuals with AD fulfilled DSM-IV criteria for AD, and were abstinent for at least 2 weeks to 1 year prior to testing. VG participants were included if they fulfilled VG criteria adapted from DSM-IV pathological gambling criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) in keeping with other published studies (Gentile et al., 2011). CSB individuals were also recruited via advertisements placed on internet sites including on Reddit and from therapist referrals. CSB subjects were screened using the internet sex screening test (ISST; Delmonico and Miller, 2003) and an exhaustive experimenter-designed questionnaire which included items pertaining to age of onset, frequency, duration, attempts to control use, abstinence, patterns of use, treatment and negative consequences. CSB participants were interviewed by a psychiatrist to confirm they fulfilled two sets of diagnostic criteria for CSB (proposed diagnostic criteria for Hypersexual Disorder; criteria for sexual addiction; Carnes et al., 2001; Kafka, 2010; Reid et al., 2012), focusing on compulsive use of online sexually explicit material. These criteria emphasize failure to cut down or control sexual behaviors, including consumption of pornography, despite social, financial, psychological and academic or vocational problems. Detailed description of CSB symptoms are described in Voon et al. (2014).

All the participants were included if they were aged ≥18 years and excluded if they had a current major depression or a history of other severe psychiatric disorder (e.g., bipolar affective disorder or schizophrenia) or a current substance use disorder including regular cannabis use. Participants were also excluded if they tested positive for a drug urine screen (including cannabis) or alcohol breathalyzer test on the day of testing. CSB and their matched HV were male and heterosexual.

Procedure

Following provision of written consent, all participants underwent urine and breathalyzer tests on the day of testing. All participants completed the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck et al., 1996) to assess depressive symptoms, the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale to assess impulsivity (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001), the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger et al., 1983) to assess depression and anxiety, and the ADs Identification Test (ADIT; Saunders et al., 1993). The National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson, 1982) provided an index of premorbid IQ. Subjects were screened for comorbid psychiatric disorders with the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Inventory (MINI; Sheehan et al., 1998). All participants refrained from consuming alcohol for at least 24 h prior to their study visit. The study was approved by the University of Cambridge Research Ethics Committee. Subjects were remunerated at a rate of £7.50 per hour including travel costs, with an additional £5 contingent on task performance.

Behavioral Measures

Probabilistic Reversal Task

The probabilistic reversal task comprised two phases, acquisition and reversal, with three conditions varying by magnitude of reward or loss outcome (reward, neutral and loss) in each phase. In the acquisition phase, subjects chose from one of the three stimulus-pairs associated with the following probabilistic outcomes: Loss (Stimulus A: P = 0.30, Win +£1/P = 0.70, Lose −£2 (mean −£1.1); Stimulus B: P = 0.70, Win +£1/P = 0.30, Lose −£2 (mean +£0.1)), Neutral (Stimulus C: P = 0.70, Win +£1/P = 0.30, Lose −£1 (mean +£0.4); Stimulus D: P = 0.30, Win +£1/P = 0.70, Lose −£1 (mean −£0.4)) or Reward (Stimulus E: P = 0.70, Win +£2/P = 0.30, Lose −£1 (mean +£1,1); Stimulus F: P = 0.30, Win +£2/P = 0.70, Lose −£1 (mean −£0.1); Figure 1A). Subjects were shown and chosen from one stimuli-pair at a time followed by the outcome. After 30 trials per condition in the acquisition phase, the contingencies for each stimulus-pair switched and were followed by 30 trials per condition in the reversal phase (e.g., Stimulus A: P = 0.70, Win +£1/P = 0.3, Lose −£2; Stimulus B: P = 0.30, Win +£1/P = 0.70, Lose −£2). There were a total of 180 trials (60 trials per condition). The position of the stimuli within each stimulus-pair was counterbalanced on either side of the screen. The stimuli-pairs of the different conditions were randomly presented; thus, subjects were exposed to different trial sequences.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Reversal learning. (A) Probabilistic reversal learning task. (B) The number of trials to criterion for reversal learning in the context of reward (solid line) and loss (dashed line) is depicted for Obese subjects with and without binge eating disorder (BED), alcohol dependence (AD), compulsive sexual behavior (CSB) and pathological video-gaming (VG), each with their own matched healthy volunteer (HV) group.

Subjects were given 10 practice trials of a stimuli-pair in which one stimulus was associated with P = 0.70, Win +£1/P = 0.30, Lose −£1 and the other stimulus associated with P = 0.30, Win +£1/P = 0.70, Lose −£1). The stimulus phase was shown for 2.5 s during which the participants needed to respond. Subjects indicated a response with their dominant hand pressing the left arrow on the keyboard for the stimulus on the left and the right arrow on the keyboard for the stimulus on the right. The stimulus phase was followed by a 1 s outcome phase with the words “You WON!!” and an image of a £2 or £1 coin or “You LOST!!” and an image of a large red “X” over the £2 or £1 coin. If subjects were too slow, this was followed by the words: “You were too slow. Respond faster”. The trial was followed by a variable inter-trial interval of a mean of 0.75 s varying between 0.5 and 1 s.

Participants were told they would choose from three different pairs of symbols and that one symbol within each pair was more likely to be associated with winning money or not losing money. They were also told that at some point the relationship between the symbols and the likelihood of winning and not losing money might change. Participants were instructed that their goal was to make as much money as possible of which a proportion would be paid to them at the end of the study.

The primary outcome measure of an index of a learning rate was the number of trials to criterion of four correct sequential choices within each condition (Voon et al., 2015a; Morris et al., 2016). This outcome provides an index of how quickly subjects learn contingencies during acquisition and reversal. As a secondary analysis, the outcome measure of win-stay (the proportion of trials in which the same stimulus was selected following a win outcome = win-stay/(win-stay + win-shift)) or lose-shift (the proportion of trials in which the opposite stimulus was selected following a loss outcome), were assessed. This outcome provides an index of sensitivity to outcome and behavioral strategy as a function of the most recent outcome. Outcome measures were calculated separately for acquisition and reversal phases. The task was coded in E-prime Version 2 and took approximately 13 min to get completed.

Intra/Extra-Dimensional Set Shift Task (IED)

The IED is a deterministic reward reversal task from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB; Sahakian and Owen, 1992; Figure 2). It is performed on a touch screen monitor. The task is composed of nine stages beginning with a simple discrimination and its reversal of one dimension (pink shapes). Compound discrimination and its reversal are then tested with the addition of another dimension (adjacent or overlying white lines). Success is dependent on responding to the previous relevant dimension and on ignoring the new, irrelevant dimension. On any given trial participants choose which of a pair of stimuli are “correct” by touching that stimulus. In stage 1, the stimuli varied along only one dimension (pink shapes) after six correct choices and the next stage (stage 2) commences in which contingencies reverse such that the other paired stimulus is correct. Stage 3 introduces a new dimension (adjacent white lines) to get the subject familiar with a compound stimulus. To succeed, participants had to continue responding to the correct stimulus of previous stages. Stage 4 and the subsequent stages were also compound but the two stimulus from the different dimensions were superimposed (the white lines overlying the pink shapes). The contingencies again remained unchanged from those for the previous stage. A reversal then occurred in stage 5. Stage 6 introduced new stimuli for both dimensions, with the relevant dimension unchanged. It required the subject to continue to attend to the shape dimension and learn which of the two new exemplars is correct (“intra-dimensional shift”). Following reversal (stage 7), a final set of compound stimuli are presented. In stage 8, the subject is now required to attend to the previously-redundant dimension of line (“extra-dimensional shift”). Finally, contingencies were reversed to the previously incorrect exemplar of the new dimension (stage 9). If a subject fails to complete six correct responses in a row, in any given stage, the test terminates. Of the nine blocks, four (2, 5, 7 and 9) represent reversal stages. The task duration is approximately 7 min. The primary outcome measure was the number of errors in the extra-dimensional (ED) shift stage. This outcome provides an index of set shifting or how many errors occurred prior to learning the shift to the previously redundant dimension.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Attentional set-shifting. (A) Intra- and extra-dimensional set shifting task. (B) Extra-dimensional (ED) shift errors in Obese subjects with and without BED, subjects with AD, CSB and pathological VG compared to each groups’ own matched healthy volunteers (HV). *p < 0.05.

Statistics

The data was inspected for outliers and normality of distribution tested using Shapiro-Wilkes test. Subject characteristics were analyzed using independent t-tests. As the data for reversal trials to criterion was left-skewed, the data was transformed by squaring. As the data for acquisition trials to criterion was right-skewed, the data was transformed by square root. The number of trials to criterion of the reversal phase was analyzed using a mixed measures ANOVA with Group as a between-subjects factor and Valence (Reward and Loss) as a within-subjects factor. The number of trials to criterion for the acquisition phase was similarly assessed. For the reversal learning task, the outcome of Win-Stay and Lose-Shift were separately assessed using a mixed measures ANOVA with Group as a between-subjects factor, Valence condition (Reward, Loss and Neutral) and Phase (Acquisition vs. Reversal) as a within-subjects factor. For the IED, the primary outcome of errors during the ED shift was assessed between groups using independent t-tests. A value of p < 0.05 was taken to indicate statistical significance.

Results

Age- and gender-matched HVs were compared with each group (AD: N = 32; Obese: N = 31; BED: N = 32; VG: N = 26; CSB: N = 25). Subject characteristics, behavioral measures and sample sizes are reported in Tables 1, 2. AD subjects had the following alcohol use (weeks abstinent 16.53 (SD 16.99); years of dependence 12.92 (SD 8.17); Units/day 28.51 (SD 14.32). Three subjects in the AD group were on the following medications (acamprosate 2; disulfiram 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Subject characteristics and behavioral measures.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Subject characteristics and behavioral measures in pathological videogaming and compulsive sexual behaviors.

Probabilistic Reversal Task

Obese Subjects With and Without BED

Trials to criterion

In the Reversal phase (Figure 1) in BED (N = 32) subjects compared to HVs (N = 64) there was no main effect of Valence (F(1,94) = 0.12, p = 0.726), or Group (F(1,94) = 1.19, p = 0.278) or Group × Valence interaction (F(1,94) = 3.04, p = 0.085).

In the Reversal phase, Obese subjects (N = 31) compared to HV (N = 62) presented a main effect of Valence (F(1,91) = 7.31, p = 0.008), a trend towards a Group effect (F(1,91) = 3.53, p = 0.063) and no interaction effect (F(1,91) = 1.77, p = 0.186).

In the comparison of Obese subjects with and without BED, there was a Group × Valence interaction (F(1,61) = 7.60, p = 0.008) in which Obese subjects with BED required more trials to reach criterion in the reversal phase to Reward and less to Loss with the opposite observed in Obese subjects without BED. There was no main effect of Group (F(1,61) = 0.43, p = 0.510) or Valence (F(1,61) = 0.79, p = 0.377). The interaction effect remained significant with the inclusion of BDI as a covariate of no interest (p = 0.023).

In the Acquisition phase, Obese subjects compared to HV (Reward: HV 9.84 (SD 7.84); Obese 10.44 (SD 9.00); Loss: HV 7.93 (SD 6.59); Obese 7.96 (SD 9.00)) presented a main effect of Valence (F(1,91) = 2.68, p = 0.008) but not effects of Group (F(1,91) = 0.12, p = 0.726) or interaction effect (F(1,91) = 1.07, p = 0.745). In the Acquisition phase in BED subjects compared to HV (Reward: HV 9.49 (SD 7.46); BED 9.97 (SD 8.29); Loss: HV 7.73 (SD 6.33); BED 7.36 (SD 6.64)) there were no main effects of Valence (F(1,94) = 2.82, p = 0.105), Group (F(1,94) = 0.285, p = 0.595) or Interaction (F(1,94) = 0.10, p = 0.749).

Lose-shift/win-stay

In the comparison (Table 3; Figure 3) of BED and HV, in both the Lose-Shift and Win-Stay analyses there was an interaction between Group × Phase. In the posthoc analysis, during the Reversal compared to the Acquisition phase, BED subjects had greater Lose-Shift or were more likely to shift or select the opposite stimulus after losing compared to HV. Similarly, during the Reversal compared to Acquisition phase, BED subjects also had lower Win-stay or were less likely to perseverate or stay with the same stimulus after a win compared to HV.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Lose-shift and win-stay analysis.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Lose-shift vs. win-stay. The significant mixed measures ANOVA findings are shown for Lose-shift (A) vs. Win-stay (B). Abbreviations: Neu, neutral; Rew, reward; HV, healthy volunteers; Acq, acquisition; Rev, Reversal; BED, Obese with binge eating disorder; AD, abstinent alcohol dependent subjects; Obese, Obese without BED; CSB, compulsive sexual behaviors.

In the comparison of obese and HV, there were no significant differences in Lose-Shift or Win-Stay.

In the comparison of BED and Obese, there were no interactions with Group or main Group effect with Lose-Shift. In the Win-Stay analysis comparing BED and Obese, there was a Group × Valence effect, which on posthoc analysis the BED subjects had greater Win-Stay across both Reversal and Acquisition phases or were overall more likely to perseverate after a win in Loss (p = 0.024) and Reward (p = 0.001) conditions relative to Neutral.

Summary

BED subjects compared to obese non-BED subjects showed poorer reversal learning (greater trials to criterion) specifically to reward and not to the loss condition. This converges with a greater Win-Stay in BED or enhanced tendency to perseverate or stay after a win. BED subjects compared to HV showed a greater tendency to switch after any outcome in the reversal phase.

Alcohol Dependence

Trials to criterion

In the Reversal phase in the AD subjects (N = 32) vs. HV (N = 64) there was a main effect of Group F(1,94) = 5.78, p = 0.018) in which AD subjects required more trials to reach criterion compared to HV (Figure 1). These Group results remained significant with a subgroup analysis without the three medicated subjects. There was no effect of Valence (F(1,94) = 2.41, p = 0.124) or interaction (F(1,94) = 0.08, p = 0.779). In the Acquisition phase in the AD subjects compared to HV (Reward: HV 9.43 (SD 7.64); AD 9.63 (SD 7.58); Loss: HV 7.51 (SD 6.63); AD 11.61 (SD 9.12)) there was no main effects of Valence (F(1,94) = 0.002, p = 0.967), Group (F(1,94) = 1.70, p = 0.196) or interaction (F(1,94) = 2.25, p = 0.137).

Win-stay/lose-shift

In the Lose-Shift analysis, there was a main Group effect (Table 3; Figure 2) in which AD had lower Lose-Shift scores or more likelihood to stay or perseverate after a loss across all trial types compared to HV. The Group effect remained significant with a subgroup analysis without the 3 medicated subjects. There were no differences in the Win-Stay analysis.

Summary

AD subjects had slower reversal learning compared to HV and had lower Lose-Shift or a greater tendency to stay or perseverate after a loss.

Pathological Videogaming

Trials to criterion

In the Reversal phase in the VG subjects (N = 26) compared to HV (N = 52) there was a main effect of Group (F(1,76) = 5.17, p = 0.026) in which VG subjects required more trials to reach criterion (Figure 1). There was no Valence (F(1,76) = 3.05, p = 0.085) or interaction effect (F(1,76) = 0.384, p = 0.537). In the Acquisition phase (Reward: HV 9.49 (SD 7.51); VG 10.46 (SD 6.36); Loss: HV 7.23 (SD 6.73); VG 7.69 (SD 6.90)) there was an effect of Valence (F(1,76) = 7.910, p = 0.006) but no effect of Group (F(1,76) = 0.86, p = 0.356) or interaction (F(1,76) = 0.51, p = 0.478).

Win-stay/lose-shift

There were no Lose-Shift or Win-Stay differences in VG compared to HV.

Summary

VG subjects had slower reversal learning compared to HV.

Compulsive Sexual Behavior

Trails to criterion

In the Reversal phase in the CSB subjects (N = 25) compared to HV (N = 50) there was no main effect of Group (F(1,73) = 1.33, p = 0.253), Valence (F(1,73) = 1.47, p = 0.229) or interaction effect (F(1,73) = 0.008, p = 0.928; Figure 1). In the Acquisition phase in the CSB subjects (Reward: HV 9.39 (SD 7.34); CSB 6.39 (SD 5.43); Loss: HV 7.26 (SD 6.53); CSB 8.69 (SD 7.83)) there was a Group × Valence interaction (F(1,73) = 4.35, p = 0.039) in which CSB subjects were faster to learn from Rewards and slower to learn from Losses as compared to HV. There was no Group (F(1,73) = 0.38, p = 0.539) or Valence effect (F(1,73) < 0.001, p = 0.983).

Win-stay/lose-shift

In the Lose-Shift analysis, there was a Group × Valence effect (Table 3; Figure 3); in the posthoc analysis, CSB subjects had lower Lose-Shift or were more likely to stay or perseverate after a loss in the Reward condition relative to Loss (p = 0.005) and Neutral (p < 0.001). Similarly, in the Win-Stay analysis, there was a Group × Valence effect; in the posthoc analysis, CSB had higher Win-Stay or were more likely to stay after a win in the Reward condition relative to Loss (p = 0.019) and Neutral (p = 0.007).

Summary

CSB subjects were faster to learning from rewards in the acquisition phase compared to HV and were more likely to perseverate or stay after either a loss or a win in the Reward condition.

Attentional Set Shifting

BED subjects (n = 31) compared to HV (N = 47) made more ED shift errors (t = −2.33, df = 77, p = 0.023). AD subjects (n = 30) compared to HV (N = 45) similarly made more ED shift errors (t = −3.04, df = 74, p = 0.003). In contrast, Obese subjects without BED (N = 30) compared to HV (n = 45) made a similar number of errors during the ED shift (t = −1.02, df = 74, p = 0.313). CSB subjects (N = 25) compared to HV (N = 37) made a similar number of errors during ED shift (t = 0.93, df = 61, p = 0.358). VG subjects (N = 24) compared to HV (N = 36) also made a similar number of errors during ED shifts (t = −0.52, df = 59, p = 0.605; Figure 2).

In AD subjects, the number of units per day was positively correlated with ED errors (reported as Pearson correlation coefficient, p-value: 0.394, p = 0.031). In Obese and BED subjects there were no correlations between ED errors and BMI or Binge Eating Severity (BES; p > 0.05).

Discussion

This study provides evidence for differential cognitive flexibility impairments across different pathologies of drug and non-drug rewards as measured by two distinct behavioral paradigms: reversal learning and attentional set-shifting. Although we did not measure the neural correlates of these distinct cognitive processes in the present study, agreement on the substrates of these different tasks has evolved with the use of higher precision technology in rodent and nonhuman primate species and fMRI in humans. The literature consistently implicates differing aspects of fronto-striatal circuitry in reversal learning and attentional set-shifting, namely orbitofrontal and lateral prefrontal cortices respectively. We have previously reported on these measures of reversal learning (number of trials to criterion) and ED shifting demonstrating a relationship between dissociable fronto-striatal circuits (Morris et al., 2016).

Reversal Learning Deficits

We found slower reversal learning in individuals with AD and VG across both reward and loss valences. Obese subjects with BED compared to those without BED also had slower reversal learning in the reward relative to loss condition whereas those without BED had slower reversal in the loss relative to reward condition.

Reversal learning impairments are common in cocaine use disorders (Camchong et al., 2011) with evidence for enhanced perseverative responding to previously rewarded stimuli (Ersche et al., 2008) but appears less prominent in studies in AD (Fortier et al., 2008; Vanes et al., 2014). Our findings converge with a study comparing AD, pathological gamblers and healthy controls which found no significant group differences in perseverative responding during reversals between alcohol-dependent patients and pathological gamblers and healthy controls but showed slower learning rates for reversal (Vanes et al., 2014). We expand on these findings demonstrating impairments in reversal trials to criterion to both reward and loss outcomes suggesting a generalized impairment in reversal learning in AD rather than a valence specific abnormality. Our version of the task is more difficult which may be more likely to induce reversal impairments. We further show that VG individuals had similar impairments in reversal learning across both valences in line with previously reported reversal learning deficits in pathological gambling, a non-substance behavioral addiction (Patterson et al., 2006).

Our findings in obese subjects with BED converge with reported preclinical and clinical findings. Rodents exposed to an unrestricted high-fat diet develop greater impairments in reversal learning (Kanoski et al., 2007). Obese mice have reduced striatal D2 receptors (Johnson and Kenny, 2010), a deficit which has been shown to impair the ability to inhibit previously rewarded responses to natural rewards in mice (Kruzich et al., 2006). In a human study of obesity, both acquisition and reversal learning were impaired specific to the food outcome but not the monetary outcome (Zhang et al., 2014). Here we focused on monetary outcomes and show that obese subjects with BED relative to those without BED were slower to learn during reversal in the reward condition but not in the loss condition. These findings suggest reversal learning impairments in BED as a function of valence. These differential effects of valence are also highly consistent with our previous report of enhanced risk taking to reward anticipation along with impaired sensitivity to reward value gradients in BED, which was not observed in obese subjects without BED (Voon et al., 2015c). Thus, these findings are similar to observations of lower goal-directed and enhanced habit formation in BED subjects relative to non-BED subjects. BED subjects may be more likely to choose actions based on expected prior rewarded actions whereas non-BED subjects may be more likely to avoid actions based on expected prior punished actions.

Attentional Set Shifting Deficits

We further show specific impairments in ED set shifting in AD and obese subjects with BED but not in those without BED. Previous studies have shown impairments in set-shifting tested with the WCST or the Trail-making test in obese individuals with and without BED (Duchesne et al., 2010; Wu et al., 2014) and in children and adolescents with excess weight (Cserjési et al., 2007; Verdejo-García et al., 2010). In line with previous studies demonstrating impairments in set shifting in AD (Tarter, 1973; Nowakowska et al., 2007) and as a predictor of relapse (Pothiyil and Alex, 2013), we show impairments in ED set shifting correlating with alcohol severity. The findings in BED are consistent with enhanced behavioral inflexibility across multiple domains (Voon, 2015).

Perseveration

We further measured a more basic form of behavioral inflexibility, or the tendency to stay or shift following reward or loss outcomes to assess the use of outcome valence to guide behaviors. AD subjects were also overall more likely to stay or perseverate after loss outcomes rather than shift across both acquisition and reversal arms compared to HV suggesting an impaired ability to integrate loss outcomes to guide behaviors. In the comparison with Obese subjects without BED, those with BED were more likely to stay following larger rewarding outcomes consistent with impaired reversal learning specific to the reward condition.

Study Limitations

One limitation of the present study is the lack of direct confirmation of diagnostic in all the disorders assessed. We used alcohol breathalyzer tests on the day of testing to make sure the participants were not alcohol free to confirm abstinence in AD participants. However, a similar measure was not used in other groups where use or compulsive behavior cannot be easily confirmed in such a direct way, which may be seen as the alcohol dependent group being treated differently than the other groups. However, an extensive interview assessment was employed and all diagnosis was made by an experienced psychiatrist, which makes us believe that limitation was addressed. A second limitation is the fact that, in the reversal learning task, participants were instructed that they would be choosing from three different pairs of symbols but at some point the relationship between the symbols and the likelihood of winning and not losing money might change. This instruction may alter the way individuals solve the task and could change brain recruitment, redirecting from a model-free to a model-based learning strategy. However, if a model-based strategy was more predominant as consequence of the instruction, the likelihood of finding impairments in this task would be lower. Since the direction of the effect of a model-based strategy is opposite of our results, we do not think this limitation affected our findings in any way.

Behavioral Flexibility Deficits Across Disorders of Compulsivity

In Table 4 we summarize these results and studies on compulsivity indices within this same human patient cohort. Compulsivity has multiple subtypes with overlapping yet discrete neural substrates (Voon and Dalley, 2015). Here we define compulsivity and behavioral flexibility on the basis of flexible responding to context and can be conceptualized hierarchically from more complex to basic. Our previous studies have also described shifts in reinforcement learning in the context of task structure or goals (model-based goal-directed learning) relative to previously reinforced choices (model-free habitual learning; Voon et al., 2015b) and explore-exploit behaviors in the context of uncertainty (Morris et al., 2015). These compulsivity subtypes implicate lateral prefrontal and medial orbitofrontal and anterior prefrontal cortices respectively. In this current study, contextual changes may occur to a higher level explicit rule shift in attentional set shifting or to changes in reinforcement contingencies in reversal learning (Chase et al., 2011) implicating lateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortices respectively. More basic forms of behavioral inflexibility include perseverative responding either following valenced feedback or irrespective of feedback (Voon et al., 2015b).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Summary of compulsivity measures across disorders.

Together these findings suggest marked similarities in impairments across multiple forms of behavioral flexibility in AD and BED (Voon, 2015) across both orbitofrontal and lateral prefrontal cortices. Obese subjects with BED exhibit a wider range of impairments of behavioral flexibility relative to those without BED with the former demonstrating greater sensitivity to reward valence and the latter to loss valence. Deficits observed in VG focus on the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), suggesting a commonality across drug and non-drug addictions.

In contrast to other disorders, CSB compared to HV showed faster acquisition to reward outcomes along with a greater perseveration in the reward condition irrespective of outcome. The CSB subjects did not show any specific impairments in set shifting or reversal learning. These findings converge with our previous findings of enhanced preference for stimuli conditioned to either sexual or monetary outcomes, overall suggesting enhanced sensitivity to rewards (Banca et al., 2016). Further studies using salient rewards are indicated. Deficits in goal-directed or exploratory behaviors in CSB have not yet been reported.

Our findings have several important implications. First, these observations may be interpreted as commonalities across AD and BED supporting pathological binge eating behaviors in BED as a possible form of behavioral addiction. Alternatively, the degree of behavioral inflexibility may also be representative of severity. Second, commonalities across OFC reversal deficits may link these pathological drug and non-drug behaviors. Third, we emphasize the differential influence of valence on behavioral flexibility, particularly emphasizing its relevance in obesity with and without bingeing and CSB. Finally, our findings highlight compulsivity and behavioral inflexibility as a relevant domain in the trend towards defining relevant cognitive endophenotypes dimensionally across psychiatric disorders (Insel et al., 2010).

Author Contributions

VV was responsible for the study concept and design. PB and VV contributed to the acquisition of the data, performed the data analysis and wrote the article. All authors critically reviewed content and approved final version for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

PB was supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (individual fellowship: SFRH/BD/33889/2009). VV and NAH are Wellcome Trust Intermediate Fellows. The study was funded by the Wellcome Trust (WT093705/Z/10/Z).

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Avena, N. M., Rada, P., and Hoebel, B. G. (2008). Evidence for sugar addiction: behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 20–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banca, P., Morris, L. S., Mitchell, S., Harrison, N. A., Potenza, M. N., and Voon, V. (2016). Novelty, conditioning and attentional bias to sexual rewards. J. Psychiatr. Res. 72, 91–101. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Ball, R., and Ranieri, W. (1996). Comparison of beck depression inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J. Pers. Assess. 67, 588–597. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, V. J., and Tait, D. S. (2016). Attentional set-shifting across species. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. doi: 10.1007/7854_2015_5002 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camchong, J., MacDonald, A. W. III, Nelson, B., Bell, C., Mueller, B. A., Specker, S., et al. (2011). Frontal hyperconnectivity related to discounting and reversal learning in cocaine subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 1117–1123. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carnes, P., Delmonico, D. L., and Griffin, E. (2001). In the Shadows of the Net: Breaking Free from Compulsive Online Sexual Behaviour. Center City, Minnesota: Hazelden.

Chase, H. W., Swainson, R., Durham, L., Benham, L., and Cools, R. (2011). Feedback-related negativity codes prediction error but not behavioral adjustment during probabilistic reversal learning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 936–946. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, R. K., Etchegaray, M., and Meck, W. H. (2007). Impairments in timing, temporal memory and reversal learning linked to neurotoxic regimens of methamphetamine intoxication. Brain Res. 1186, 255–266. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, L., Robbins, T. W., Ersche, K. D., and Sahakian, B. J. (2006). Reflection impulsivity in current and former substance users. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 515–522. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, H. F., Walker, S. C., Crofts, H. S., Dalley, J. W., Robbins, T. W., and Roberts, A. C. (2005). Prefrontal serotonin depletion affects reversal learning but not attentional set shifting. J. Neurosci. 25, 532–538. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3690-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cools, R., Clark, L., Owen, A. M., and Robbins, T. W. (2002). Defining the neural mechanisms of probabilistic reversal learning using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 22, 4563–4567.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cserjési, R., Molnár, D., Luminet, O., and Lénárd, L. (2007). Is there any relationship between obesity and mental flexibility in children? Appetite 49, 675–678. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalley, J. W., Everitt, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2011). Impulsivity, compulsivity and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 69, 680–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delmonico, D. L., and Miller, J. A. (2003). The internet sex screening test: a comparison of sexual compulsives versus non-sexual compulsives. Sex. Relation. Ther. 18, 261–276. doi: 10.1080/1468199031000153900

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Ruiter, M. B., Veltman, D. J., Goudriaan, A. E., Oosterlaan, J., Sjoerds, Z., and van den Brink, W. (2009). Response perseveration and ventral prefrontal sensitivity to reward and punishment in male problem gamblers and smokers. Neuropsychopharmacology 34, 1027–1038. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duchesne, M., Mattos, P., Appolinario, J. C., de Freitas, S. R., Coutinho, G., Santos, C., et al. (2010). Assessment of executive functions in obese individuals with binge eating disorder. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 32, 381–388. doi: 10.1590/S1516-44462010000400011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ersche, K. D., Roiser, J. P., Robbins, T. W., and Sahakian, B. J. (2008). Chronic cocaine but not chronic amphetamine use is associated with perseverative responding in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 197, 421–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-1051-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Serrano, M. J., Perales, J. C., Moreno-López, L., Pérez-García, M., and Verdejo-García, A. (2012). Neuropsychological profiling of impulsivity and compulsivity in cocaine dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 219, 673–683. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2485-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fillmore, M. T., and Rush, C. R. (2006). Polydrug abusers display impaired discrimination-reversal learning in a model of behavioural control. J. Psychopharmacol. 20, 24–32. doi: 10.1177/0269881105057000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fortier, C. B., Steffen, E. M., Lafleche, G., Venne, J. R., Disterhoft, J. F., and McGlinchey, R. E. (2008). Delay discrimination and reversal eyeblink classical conditioning in abstinent chronic alcoholics. Neuropsychology 22, 196–208. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.2.196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentile, D. A., Choo, H., Liau, A., Sim, T., Li, D., Fung, D., et al. (2011). Pathological video game use among youths: a two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics 127, e319–e329. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goudriaan, A. E., Oosterlaan, J., de Beurs, E., and van den Brink, W. (2006). Neurocognitive functions in pathological gambling: a comparison with alcohol dependence, Tourette syndrome and normal controls. Addiction 101, 534–547. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01380.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, J. E., Odlaug, B. L., and Chamberlain, S. R. (2011). Neurocognitive response to deep brain stimulation for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a case report. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 1338–1339. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11071108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hornak, J., O’Doherty, J., Bramham, J., Rolls, E. T., Morris, R. G., Bullock, P. R., et al. (2004). Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbito-frontal or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 463–478. doi: 10.1162/089892904322926791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., et al. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izquierdo, A., Belcher, A. M., Scott, L., Cazares, V. A., Chen, J., O’Dell, S. J., et al. (2010). Reversal-specific learning impairments after a binge regimen of methamphetamine in rats: possible involvement of striatal dopamine. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 505–514. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izquierdo, A., Brigman, J. L., Radke, A. K., Rudebeck, P. H., and Holmes, A. (2016). The neural basis of reversal learning: an updated perspective. Neuroscience doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.03.021 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izquierdo, A., and Jentsch, J. D. (2012). Reversal learning as a measure of impulsive and compulsive behavior in addictions. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 219, 607–620. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2579-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johanson, C. E., Frey, K. A., Lundahl, L. H., Keenan, P., Lockhart, N., Roll, J., et al. (2006). Cognitive function and nigrostriatal markers in abstinent methamphetamine abusers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 185, 327–338. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0330-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, P. M., and Kenny, P. J. (2010). Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 635–641. doi: 10.1038/nn.2519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kafka, M. P. (2010). Hypersexual disorder: a proposed diagnosis for DSM-V. Arch. Sex. Behav. 39, 377–400. doi: 10.1007/s10508-009-9574-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanoski, S. E., Meisel, R. L., Mullins, A. J., and Davidson, T. L. (2007). The effects of energy-rich diets on discrimination reversal learning and on BDNF in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex of the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 182, 57–66. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kringelbach, M. L., and Rolls, E. T. (2003). Neural correlates of rapid reversal learning in a simple model of human social interaction. Neuroimage 20, 1371–1383. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00393-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruzich, P. J., Mitchell, S. H., Younkin, A., and Grandy, D. K. (2006). Dopamine D2 receptors mediate reversal learning in male C57BL/6J mice. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 6, 86–90. doi: 10.3758/cabn.6.1.86

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAlonan, K., and Brown, V. J. (2003). Orbital prefrontal cortex mediates reversal learning and not attentional set shifting in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 146, 97–103. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, L. S., Baek, K., Kundu, P., Harrison, N. A., Frank, M. J., and Voon, V. (2015). Biases in the explore-exploit tradeoff in addictions: the role of avoidance of uncertainty. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 940–948. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, L. S., Kundu, P., Dowell, N., Mechelmans, D., Favre, P., Irvine, M. A., et al. (2016). Fronto-striatal organization: defining functional and microstructural substrates of behavioural flexibility. Cortex 74, 118–133. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, H. E. (1982). National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.

Google Scholar

Nowakowska, K., Jablkowska, K., and Borkowska, A. (2007). Cognitive dysfunctions in patients with alcohol dependence. Psychiatr. Pol. 41, 693–702.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

O’Doherty, J., Critchley, H., Deichmann, R., and Dolan, R. J. (2003). Dissociating valence of outcome from behavioral control in human orbital and ventral prefrontal cortices. J. Neurosci. 23, 7931–7939.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

O’Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., and Andrews, C. (2001). Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 95–102. doi: 10.1038/82959

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ornstein, T. J., Iddon, J. L., Baldacchino, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., London, M., Everitt, B. J., et al. (2000). Profiles of cognitive dysfunction in chronic amphetamine and heroin abusers. Neuropsychopharmacology 23, 113–126. doi: 10.1016/s0893-133x(00)00097-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, J. C. 2nd, Holland, J., and Middleton, R. (2006). Neuropsychological performance, impulsivity and comorbid psychiatric illness in patients with pathological gambling undergoing treatment at the CORE inpatient treatment center. South. Med. J. 99, 36–43. doi: 10.1097/01.smj.0000197583.24072.f5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pothiyil, D. I., and Alex, J. (2013). Self-regulation and set-shifting in alcohol dependence syndrome. Indian J. Appl. Res. 3, 347–350. doi: 10.15373/2249555x/mar2013/118

CrossRef Full Text

Reid, R. C., Carpenter, B. N., Hook, J. N., Garos, S., Manning, J. C., Gilliland, R., et al. (2012). Report of findings in a DSM-5 field trial for hypersexual disorder. J. Sex. Med. 9, 2868–2877. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02936.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Remijnse, P. L., Nielen, M. M., Uylings, H. B., and Veltman, D. J. (2005). Neural correlates of a reversal learning task with an affectively neutral baseline: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 26, 609–618. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W. (2007). Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, neurochemical modulation and clinical implications. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 362, 917–932. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., Gillan, C. M., Smith, D. G., de Wit, S., and Ersche, K. D. (2012). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 81–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, O. J., Frank, M. J., Sahakian, B. J., and Cools, R. (2010). Dissociable responses to punishment in distinct striatal regions during reversal learning. Neuroimage 51, 1459–1467. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahakian, B. J., and Owen, A. M. (1992). Computerized assessment in neuropsychiatry using CANTAB: discussion paper. J. R. Soc. Med. 85, 399–402.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., de la Fuente, J. R., and Grant, M. (1993). Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): who collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption–II. Addiction 88, 791–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59, 22–33; quiz 34–57.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., V, P. R., and Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Tarter, R. E. (1973). An analysis of cognitive deficits in chronic alcoholics. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 157, 138–147. doi: 10.1097/00005053-197308000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toomey, R., Lyons, M. J., Eisen, S. A., Xian, H., Chantarujikapong, S., Seidman, L. J., et al. (2003). A twin study of the neuropsychological consequences of stimulant abuse. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 60, 303–310. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.3.303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Hout, M. A., Engelhard, I. M., Smeets, M., Dek, E. C., Turksma, K., and Saric, R. (2009). Uncertainty about perception and dissociation after compulsive-like staring: time course of effects. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 535–539. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanes, L. D., van Holst, R. J., Jansen, J. M., van den Brink, W., Oosterlaan, J., and Goudriaan, A. E. (2014). Contingency learning in alcohol dependence and pathological gambling: learning and unlearning reward contingencies. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 38, 1602–1610. doi: 10.1111/acer.12393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verdejo-García, A., Pérez-Expósito, M., Schmidt-Río-Valle, J., Fernández-Serrano, M. J., Cruz, F., Pérez-García, M., et al. (2010). Selective alterations within executive functions in adolescents with excess weight. Obesity (Silver Spring) 18, 1572–1578. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voon, V. (2015). Cognitive biases in binge eating disorder: the hijacking of decision making. CNS Spectr. 20, 566–573. doi: 10.1017/s1092852915000681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voon, V., Baek, K., Enander, J., Worbe, Y., Morris, L. S., Harrison, N. A., et al. (2015a). Motivation and value influences in the relative balance of goal-directed and habitual behaviours in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Transl. Psychiatry 5:e670. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voon, V., Derbyshire, K., Rück, C., Irvine, M. A., Worbe, Y., Enander, J., et al. (2015b). Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 345–352. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voon, V., Morris, L. S., Irvine, M. A., Ruck, C., Worbe, Y., Derbyshire, K., et al. (2015c). Risk-taking in disorders of natural and drug rewards: neural correlates and effects of probability, valence and magnitude. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 804–812. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voon, V., and Dalley, J. W. (2015). “Translatable and back-translatable measurement of impulsivity and compulsvity: convergent and divergent processes,” in Current Topics in Behavioural Neuroscience, eds T. W. Robbins and B. J. Sahakian (Switzerland: Springer International Publishing), 1–39.

Google Scholar

Voon, V., Mole, T. B., Banca, P., Porter, L., Morris, L., Mitchell, S., et al. (2014). Neural correlates of sexual cue reactivity in individuals with and without compulsive sexual behaviours. PLoS One 9:e102419. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0102419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, I. M., Minamoto, T., Odell, J. R., Mayhorn, J., and White, W. (2009). Brief exposure to methamphetamine (METH) and phencyclidine (PCP) during late development leads to long-term learning deficits in rats. Brain Res. 1266, 72–86. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whiteside, S. P., and Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Pers. Individ. Dif. 30, 669–689. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(00)00064-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, M., Brockmeyer, T., Hartmann, M., Skunde, M., Herzog, W., and Friederich, H. C. (2014). Set-shifting ability across the spectrum of eating disorders and in overweight and obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 44, 3365–3385. doi: 10.1017/s0033291714000294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Z., Manson, K. F., Schiller, D., and Levy, I. (2014). Impaired associative learning with food rewards in obese women. Curr. Biol. 24, 1731–1736. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: addiction, alcohol dependence, binge eating disorder, compulsivity, reversal learning, set-shifting

Citation: Banca P, Harrison NA and Voon V (2016) Compulsivity Across the Pathological Misuse of Drug and Non-Drug Rewards. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:154. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00154

Received: 14 April 2016; Accepted: 19 July 2016;
Published: 03 August 2016.

Edited by:

Matthias Brand, University of Duisburg, Germany

Reviewed by:

Alicia Izquierdo, University of California, Los Angeles, USA
Juan M. Dominguez, University of Texas, Austin, USA
Rudolf Stark, University of Giessen, Germany

Copyright © 2016 Banca, Harrison and Voon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Valerie Voon, vv247@cam.ac.uk

Download