Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 08 September 2016
Sec. Pathological Conditions
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00172

Distribution of Response Time, Cortical, and Cardiac Correlates during Emotional Interference in Persons with Subclinical Psychotic Symptoms

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 2The Zurich Program for Sustainable Development of Mental Health Services, University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 3Translational Neuromodeling Unit, Institute for Biomedical Engineering, University of Zurich and ETH Zurich, Switzerland
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 5LEAD Graduate School, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 6Laboratory of Neuroscience (LIM27), Institute of Psychiatry, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • 7Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Charité University Medicine, Berlin, Germany

A psychosis phenotype can be observed below the threshold of clinical detection. The study aimed to investigate whether subclinical psychotic symptoms are associated with deficits in controlling emotional interference, and whether cortical brain and cardiac correlates of these deficits can be detected using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). A data set derived from a community sample was obtained from the Zurich Program for Sustainable Development of Mental Health Services. 174 subjects (mean age 29.67 ± 6.41, 91 females) were assigned to four groups ranging from low to high levels of subclinical psychotic symptoms (derived from the Symptom Checklist-90-R). Emotional interference was assessed using the emotional Stroop task comprising neutral, positive, and negative conditions. Statistical distributional methods based on delta plots [behavioral response time (RT) data] and quantile analysis (fNIRS data) were applied to evaluate the emotional interference effects. Results showed that both interference effects and disorder-specific (i.e., group-specific) effects could be detected, based on behavioral RTs, cortical hemodynamic signals (brain correlates), and heart rate variability (cardiac correlates). Subjects with high compared to low subclinical psychotic symptoms revealed significantly reduced amplitudes in dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (interference effect, p < 0.001) and middle temporal gyrus (disorder-specific group effect, p < 0.001), supported by behavioral and heart rate results. The present findings indicate that distributional analyses methods can support the detection of emotional interference effects in the emotional Stroop. The results suggested that subjects with high subclinical psychosis exhibit enhanced emotional interference effects. Based on these observations, subclinical psychosis may therefore prove to represent a valid extension of the clinical psychosis phenotype.

Introduction

Several studies in the past 20 years have demonstrated that the expression of a psychosis phenotype can be observed below the threshold of its clinical detection (van Os et al., 2000; Wiles et al., 2006; Rössler et al., 2007, 2013a,b, 2015). Subclinical psychotic symptoms are commonly referred to as psychotic-(like) experiences, proneness to psychosis, at-risk mental state, schizotypy, or exceptional experiences (Fach et al., 2013). These symptoms, although not always of clinical relevance (Johns and van Os, 2001), can have predictive power for the onset of clinical psychotic disorders later in life (van Os et al., 2000). People with a high score of subclinical psychotic symptoms have been reported to have a 10% increased risk to develop a schizophrenia-spectrum disorder (Meehl, 1990; Chapman et al., 1994; Hanssen et al., 2005). As such, the topic of subclinical psychosis has gained increased interest within the context of early identification and treatment of persons at risk for psychosis.

People with subclinical psychosis indicate the same symptoms as persons with full-blown schizophrenia, though in an attenuated form. For these reasons a new diagnostic entity, labeled ‘Attenuated Psychosis Syndrome,’ has been considered for inclusion in the new DSM-5. It is described as a condition ‘with recent onset of modest psychotic-like symptoms and clinically relevant distress and disability’ (Tsuang et al., 2013). However, because this new category did not possess any certainty of its validity, it was not yet included in the last revision of the DSM. Therefore, in order to validate subclinical psychotic states as an ‘valid’ extension of the psychosis phenotype (i.e., the typical symptoms of full-blown schizophrenia but in an attenuated form), it is necessary to demonstrate that subclinical psychosis is reflected also in other dimensions than psychopathology. In particular, it is of interest to detect underlying physiological markers that could serve as validation.

The present study aimed to answer the question of whether emotional interference effects (i.e., commonly associated with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders) can be detected in persons with subclinical psychotic symptoms. On an emotional level, persons with schizophrenia display deficits in the identification of information with emotional valence, together with impaired regulation of emotional behavior (Phillips et al., 2003). Typical examples of such emotional impairments have been recently reviewed (Kring and Moran, 2008), such as in the areas of reduced emotional expression (i.e., blunted or flat affect, less facially and vocally expressive responses), reduced emotional experience (i.e., anhedonia, inability to experience pleasure), and altered feelings ratings (i.e., experiencing emotions that are not necessarily associated with a given emotional valence). On a cognitive level, the range of cognitive impairments in individuals with schizophrenia is broad, with the more robust and replicable deficits typically found in the domains of processing speed, working memory, selective attention (Goff et al., 2011), and executive functions, such as updating, shifting, and inhibiting cognitive processes as assessed by the original Stroop task (Westerhausen et al., 2011). Impairments in these domains are associated with alterations in neural systems known to support these cognitive functions, including the prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Minzenberg et al., 2009) and the temporal lobe (Heckers, 2001; Reichenberg and Harvey, 2007). Taking these two levels together, deficits in emotion discrimination and cognitive inhibition are frequently co-occurring impairments in individuals with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders (Kohler et al., 2000).

We had the opportunity to test these issues in a data set from a community sample derived from the Epidemiology Survey of the Zurich Program for Sustainable Development of Mental Health Services (ZInEP) (Ajdacic-Gross et al., 2014). This sample was thoroughly characterized both on a psychopathological and neuropsychological level, complemented with a suitable brain imaging method, i.e., functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) (Ehlis et al., 2014) in order to investigate cortical responses associated with potential interference deficits.

The first objective of the present study was to examine in this community sample, how emotional interference would be modified in subjects with high versus low subclinical psychotic symptoms. Subclinical psychotic symptoms were assessed using a self-rating scale based on the subscales ‘Schizophrenia Nuclear Symptoms’ (SNS) and ‘Schizotypal Signs’ (STS) (Rössler et al., 2007), derived from the Symptom Checklist-90-R (SCL-90-R) subscales ‘Paranoid Ideation’ and ‘Psychoticism’ (Derogatis, 1977).

As measure of emotional interference, we applied the color-word emotional Stroop task (Williams et al., 1996; Dresler et al., 2012). In this task the slowing of response times (RTs) for color naming of emotional words (positive and negative) relative to neutral words serves as a measure of emotional interference. There have been promising findings in patient populations, where the emotional Stroop task has been shown to be sensitive in individuals with deficits in emotional processing (Williams et al., 1996; Bar-Haim et al., 2007). Behavioral studies investigating the emotional Stroop in persons with schizophrenia have shown overall significant emotional interference effects in these patients (Phillips et al., 2005; Mohanty et al., 2008; Strauss et al., 2008; Besnier et al., 2011, 2009) (exception Demily et al., 2010). In particular, performance in persons with schizotypy (Mohanty et al., 2008) and schizophrenia (Phillips et al., 2005) differed from healthy controls by means of increased RTs predominantly for the emotional valence of negative words. Based on these behavioral studies, we hypothesized that subjects with high subclinical psychotic symptoms would also exhibit increased RTs to words with emotional valence; we expected this contrast to be prominent for words with emotionally negative valence.

The second objective was to examine, using fNIRS, whether there exist cortical hemodynamic and cardiac correlates of emotional interference during the emotional Stroop task. Neuroimaging studies investigating the emotional Stroop using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in healthy subjects reported increased activity in dorsolateral, orbitofrontal, parietal, and temporal cortices in response to color processing of emotionally valence negative compared to neutral words (Compton et al., 2003). However, so far few neuroimaging studies have investigated the emotional Stroop task in people with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders; therefore, consistent findings are scarce. An fMRI study in individuals with schizotypy reported increased activity in the right and decreased activity in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), predominantly in response to words with emotionally negative valence (Mohanty et al., 2005). An fMRI study in patients with schizophrenia reported that relative deactivations of the cingulate gyrus and the PFC were only observed in healthy controls, whereas relative activation in the DLPFC was observed only in the patient group (Park et al., 2008). We also included cardiac correlates derived from fNIRS, in order to see whether cardiovascular responses reported in previous studies in the original Stroop task (Fauvel et al., 1996; Renaud and Blondin, 1997; Boutcher and Boutcher, 2006; Satish et al., 2015), and in the emotional Stroop (Mathewson et al., 2011) could support the interpretation of the brain correlates in terms of potential interference effects.

Based on these described neuroimaging findings, a clear hypothesis could not be formulated However, we expected differences between subjects with high compared to low subclinical psychotic symptoms in the DLPFC, as these cortical areas have been consistently reported to be affected (Mohanty et al., 2005; Park et al., 2008); we expected this contrast to be prominent for emotionally valence words.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Data were obtained from the Epidemiology Survey of the Zurich Program for Sustainable Development of Mental Health Services (ZInEP), a longitudinal study on prevalence rates of common mental disorders in the Canton Zurich, Switzerland. The study design has been described in detail by Ajdacic-Gross et al. (2014). All subjects gave written informed consent. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Canton Zurich and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

The subsample included in this analysis comprising the emotional Stroop task and fNIRS examination consisted of 174 subjects, who were selected from a representative survey sample of 20–41 years old adults to participate in tests at the Center for Neuro- and Sociophysiology of our department. This subsample originated from a total of 9’829 subjects, from which 193 subjects underwent neuro-physiological testing; 19 subjects were excluded from the analysis due to missing data. Subjects receiving psychiatric medications or with a diagnosis of neuro-psychiatric diseases were excluded (Ajdacic-Gross et al., 2014).

Subjects were classified based on the subscales SNS and STS (Rössler et al., 2007), derived from the Symptom Checklist-90-R (SCL-90-R) (Derogatis, 1977). The SNS subscale assesses the items, ‘Someone else can control your thoughts,’ ‘Hearing voices other people do not hear,’ ‘Other being aware of your thoughts,’ ‘Having thoughts that are not your own.’ The STS subscale assesses the items, ‘Others are to blame for your troubles,’ ‘Feeling most people cannot be trusted,’ ‘Feeling you are watched by others,’ ‘Having ideas, others do not share,’ ‘Others not giving you proper credit,’ ‘Feeling lonely even when with people,’ ‘Feeling people take advantage of you,’ ‘Never feeling close to another person.’ The SNS scale thereby subsumes four items of the original ‘Psychoticism’ scale of the SCL-90-R (thought broadcasting, hearing voices), whereas the STS scale corresponds mostly to the original ‘Paranoid Ideation’ scale. The two subscales correlated moderately positively (r = 0.329, p < 0.001). Figure 1 illustrates the scatter plot of the four groups with the axes representing the subscores of STS and SNS, respectively.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. (Top) STS and SNS distribution. Illustration of a scatter plot demonstrating the four groups with respect to the subscores STS and SNS on the vertical and horizontal axes, respectively. (Bottom) RT and RTslow distribution. Illustration of histogram demonstrating the distribution of the two behavioral parameters, RT and RTslow.

Group CO (Control) consisted of subjects below the fifth quintiles of the two subscales (N = 27, 15.5%). Group PA (Paranoia) represented subjects within the fifth quintile of the STS subscale but below the fifth quintile of the SNS scale (N = 62, 35.6%). Group PS (Psychoticism) represented subjects within the fifth quintile of the SNS subscale but below the fifth quintile of the STS scale (N = 34, 19.5%). Group PA-PS (Paranoia-Psychoticism) represented subjects from both the fifth quintile of the SNS and STS subscales (N = 51, 29.3%). There were no significant differences between groups regarding gender, marital status, number of children, professional education, or occupation as assessed by multivariate ANOVA with the between-subject factor ‘group’ (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Data set.

Emotional Stroop Task

The color-word emotional Stroop task applied in this study consisted of a total of 120 stimuli (German words) varying in their emotional valence. Forty neutral trials [‘Monat’ (month), ‘Säule’ (column), ‘Papier’ (paper), ‘Bleistift’ (pencil), ‘Zeilen’ (rows), ‘Schnur’ (cord), ‘Laterne’ (lantern), ‘Kopien’ (copies), ‘Zitate’ (citations), ‘Rohre’ (pipes)], 40 positive trials [‘Geschenk’ (present), ‘Freundlichkeit’ (cordiality), ‘Schönheit’ (beauty), ‘Umarmung’ (embrace), ‘Beifall’ (applause), ‘Urlaub’ (holiday), ‘Schatz’ (treasure), ‘Zuneigung’ (sympathy), ‘Glück’ (luck), ‘Frühling’ (spring)], and 40 negative trials [‘Alptraum’ (nightmare), ‘Geiseln’ (hostage), ‘Gestank’ (stench), ‘Infektion’ (infection), ‘Leiche’ (cadaver), ‘Scheidung’ (divorce), ‘Stress’ (stress), ‘Tod’ (death), ‘Verräter’ (betrayer), ‘Trauer’ (sorrow)]. Each word was presented in red, green, blue, and yellow on a black computer screen using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, CA, USA). The words in all three conditions did not differ regarding the number of letters and syllables, and their frequency in German language was similar. The words were presented randomly in an event-related design with an inter-trial-interval varying between 4 and 8 s. Presentation of each word took 1500 ms and was preceded by a white fixation cross shown for 500 ms. After the stimulus presentation, subjects were asked to indicate the color of the word as fast as possible by pressing a corresponding color key on a keyboard. Performance in all conditions was assessed by the response time (RT) and the error rate in response to the cues. In particular, RT was recorded during the stimulus presentation period from 250 ms until the require time of 1500 ms. If subjects did respond after the required RT of 1500 ms the trial was marked as error trial, and the time was recorded as separate parameter, in the following defined as ‘slow’ response time (RTslow). Figure 1 illustrates the distribution of the two parameters, RT and RTslow. Trials with errors, such as no responses or corrections, were excluded.

fNIRS Instrumentation

An ETG-4000 Optical Topography System (Hitachi, Medical Corporation, Tokyo, Japan) was used consisting of a 52-channel probe setup with 17 laser diodes and 16 photo-detectors that covered parts of prefrontal and temporal cortices (Figure 4). fNIRS data were recorded with a sampling frequency of 10 Hz, transformed by the modified Beer–Lambert law to concentration changes of oxy- (O2Hb) and deoxy- (HHb) hemoglobin. Preprocessing was done using NIRS-SPM (Ye et al., 2009), we employed the wavelet minimum description length algorithm (Jang et al., 2009) to remove systemic confounds, and the precoloring method for estimating temporal correlation (Worsley and Friston, 1995).

Motion artifacts were thoroughly removed using the following procedures. First, the Wavelet motion correction function based on the hmrMotionCorrectWavelet algorithm included in the Homer2 software (Huppert et al., 2009) was applied to all subjects. Second, data were visually inspected for remaining motion artifacts, which were subsequently removedusing NIRS-SPM (in particular, ‘steps’ and ‘spikes’) in 21 subjects.

Total hemoglobin tHb, i.e., the sum of the O2Hb and HHb time series, was chosen as primary hemodynamic parameter of interest because it is thought to provide higher spatial specificity for mapping cerebral activity compared to O2Hb or HHb separately (Grubb et al., 1974; Gagnon et al., 2012).

Heart Rate Variability

Based on the raw fNIRS data, heart rate variability (HRV) was computed using the algorithm introduced by Scholkmann et al. (2012). The method, automatic multiscale-based peak detection (AMPD), is based on the calculation of the local heart rate maxima in the raw fNIRS time series. AMPD detects the heart rate peaks (i.e., the beats per minute), which were then used to calculate the inter-peak intervals frequency via interpolating the time difference signal. This calculation was done for each of the channels 1–52. Finally, an overall HRV estimate was obtained by computing the median heart rate frequency (Hz, beats/min) over all channels at each time point. The response variable for statistical analysis was then derived by calculating the HRV signal from each subject, trial, and condition.

Delta Plots (Behavioral Data)

Statistical analysis was performed using Matlab (Version R2014a, The Math Works). To obtain a detailed understanding of the behavioral RT data within the emotional Stroop task, we chose a graphical technique called delta plot (De Jong et al., 1994; Ridderinkhof et al., 2005; Speckman et al., 2008; Pratte et al., 2010). Delta plots are a specific form of distributional analysis built using the quantiles of a given RT distribution. Delta plots have been proposed, by going beyond mean RT, to provide useful clues about the evaluation of the latency mechanisms underlying tasks thought to assess cognitive control and inhibition (Pratte et al., 2010; Schwarz and Miller, 2012; Unsworth et al., 2012).

To construct the delta plots, we followed the procedure previously described by Schwarz and Miller (2012). The procedure was done for both RT parameters collected in this study, i.e., RT and RTslow. First, RTs were rank ordered for each subject and condition (neutral, positive, negative) separately and then collected to form bins of equal area. As shown in the first vertical panel of Figures 2 and 3, we used four bins with each bin containing 10 RTs per subject and condition (neutral, positive, negative). The group average RT within each bin was then computed and used as an estimate of the corresponding RT quantile intervals, i.e., the 0–25%, 25–50%, 50–75%, and 75–100% quantiles. Based on the RT quantiles an empirical estimate of the cumulative distribution functions (CDFs) was calculated for each condition. As shown in the second vertical panel of Figures 2 and 3, the delta plots were then constructed from the CDFs, by plotting the quantile differences on the y-axis against the quantile averages on the x-axis. Delta plots basically provide a visual comparison of the RT quantiles obtained in the three conditions (neutral, positive, negative). By definition, the horizontal separation of the empirical CDFs is the value of the delta plot. For example, an increase in the delta line reflects an increasing separation of two conditions (e.g., larger RTs in the neutral compared to the negative condition), whereas a drop below zero reflects a separation of two conditions in the opposite direction (e.g., larger RTs in the negative compared to the neutral condition).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Delta plots RT. Illustration of the quantile differences in RT per group (CO, PA, PS, PA-PS). First panel: Binned RT frequency distributions for the neutral (black), positive (blue), and negative (red) condition. The solid lines indicate edges of the four bins. Second panel: Delta plots of quantile differences (y-axis, i.e., neutral minus positive, and neutral minus negative) against quantile averages (x-axis), as computed from the cumulative distribution functions (CDFs). Note the different y-units between RT and RTslow (Figure 3), adapted for better visibility. See Tables 3 and 4 for statistical analysis. Third panel: QQ-plots of the frequency distributions between the neutral versus the positive (blue) and negative (red) conditions. Statistical significance was assessed using two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (k, p-value) comparing the CDFs of the neutral versus the positive and negative conditions.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Delta plots RTslow. Illustration of the quantile differences in RTslow per group (CO, PA, PS, PA-PS). First panel: Binned RTslow frequency distributions for the neutral (black), positive (blue), and negative (red) condition. The solid lines indicate edges of the four bins. Second panel: Delta plots of quantile differences (y-axis, i.e., neutral minus positive, and neutral minus negative) against quantile averages (x-axis), as computed from the cumulative distribution functions (CDFs). Note the different y-units between RT (Figure 2) and RTslow, adapted for better visibility. See Tables 3 and 4 for statistical analysis. Third panel: QQ-plots of the frequency distributions between the neutral versus the positive (blue) and negative (red) conditions. Statistical significance was assessed using two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (k, p-value) comparing the CDFs of the neutral versus the positive and negative conditions.

To quantify the statistical significance of the quantile differences between the three conditions (i.e., neutral minus positive, and neutral minus negative), we performed two methods. First, the standard deviations of the quantile differences were calculated per condition, and statistically assessed using paired- and independent-samples t-test. Second, as shown in the third vertical panel of Figures 2 and 3, QQ-plots were obtained to serve as a visual control of the differences in quantile distributions between the three conditions (Marden, 2004), with the significances assessed using two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test.

Quantile Analysis (fNIRS Data)

For the fNIRS data, a quantile analysis was performed following the procedure described by Grinband et al. (2011) for the original Stroop task. Analog to the behavioral delta plots, the quantile analysis aimed at generating averages for each subject’s four RT quantiles. First, the tHb time series from each of the 52 channels were time-locked to the RT offset assuming a peak of the hemodynamic response at 6 s following stimulus onset (baseline corrected) (Ye et al., 2009). Likewise, HRV time series were time-locked to the RT offset assuming a peak at 3 s following stimulus onset (baseline corrected).

Second, based on the RT and RTslow quantiles obtained in the behavioral analysis, Δ[tHb] beta estimates were computed using the General Linear Model (GLM) for each subject. In particular, each subject’s quantiles were computed from the RT distributions that contained all of the subject’s trials, i.e., neutral, positive, and negative, thus ensuring that the same quantiles were used for all condition types. This gave equal weight (1/4) to each quantile when comparing the Δ[tHb] beta estimates between conditions. For the HRV variable, the same quantile analysis was performed per subject.

To quantify the statistical significance of the Δ[tHb] and HRV quantile differences between the three conditions (i.e., positive minus neutral, and negative minus neutral), repeated measures ANOVA was performed using the within-subject factor ‘interference’ (neutral, positive, negative), the between-subject factor ‘group’ (CO, PA, PS, PA-PS), and the covariate ‘quantile’ (RT/RTslow Q1-Q4). The Bonferroni correction was applied to counteract the problem of multiple comparisons, and results were reported at a significance level of p < 0.05.

Results

To corroborate the initial group definition based on the subscales SNS and STS (Rössler et al., 2007), derived from the Symptom Checklist-90-R (SCL-90-R) (Derogatis, 1977), Fisher’s linear discriminant analysis with leave-one-out cross-validation was used. Classification results revealed that the subscales SNS and STS classified the groups to 89.5% of the cross-validated subjects correctly.

Delta Plots (Behavioral Data)

Table 2 illustrates an overview of the trial-based RT parameters, i.e., RT and RTslow. Repeated measures ANOVA revealed a main effect of ‘interference’ only for RTslow (F = 3.142, p = 0.043), but not RT (F = 1.135, p = 0.321). A main effect of ‘group’ was observed for both RT (F = 25.876, p < 0.001) and RTslow (F = 28.573, p < 0.001). An interaction effect of ‘interference’ × ‘group’ was observed only for RTslow (F = 2.829, p = 0.009), but not RT (F = 0.710, p = 0.642). Over all groups, the largest RTs and the highest percentage of RTslow were found in the negative condition (17%), followed by the positive (14%), and the neutral (13%) condition. Group CO showed a similar percentage of RTslow trials (12%), compared to group PS (9%), group PA (9%), and group PA-PS (14%). However, the mean RT durations were significantly longer in groups PA and PA-PS compared to group CO. See Table 2 for post-hoc pairwise comparisons.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Response times.

Next, we constructed the delta plots. A methodological aspect should be noted. Typically, delta plots enter the faster condition first, followed by the slower condition. We chose to enter the neutral condition always first in order to obtain the delta-difference-line always by subtracting the positive and negative conditions from the neutral condition. This approach was chosen to obtain a uniform pattern in the two other conditions potentially inducing interference (i.e., positive and negative). This aspect should be considered when interpreting the present results. For example, a delta line dropping below zero, indicated a significant slowing of the corresponding (positive or negative) emotional valence conditions.

Figures 2 and 3 illustrate the delta plots (Figures 2 and 3, second vertical panel) that were constructed based on the CDFs of the four bins of the RT and RTslow quantiles (Figures 2 and 3, first vertical panel). The QQ-plots (Figures 2 and 3, third vertical panel) served as a statistical control comparing the CDFs between the three conditions. It should be noted that although the overlay of the bar plots in Figures 2 and 3 may be hard to see, the overlay is an essential part of the delta plots in order to illustrate the relation to time (Schwarz and Miller, 2012).

In addition, statistical comparisons were performed for within-groups- (effect of ‘interference’) and between-groups- (effect of ‘group’) differences. The main findings regarding the effect of ‘interference’ (Table 3) was that groups PS (t = -2.619, p = 0.013) and PA-PS (t = -2.785, p = 0.008) responded significantly slower (larger RTs) in the negative compared to the neutral condition. This is visually illustrated by the drop of the delta line below zero. The effects were pronounced in group PA-PS (t = -3.301, p = 0.002) when considering RTslow. Group CO and group PA showed no ‘interference’ effects.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Delta plots (effect of ‘interference’).

The main findings regarding the effect of ‘group’ (Table 4) was that groups PS (t = -2.166, p = 0.034) and PA-PS (t = -2.966, p = 0.004) responded significantly slower (larger RTs) compared to group CO (predominantly, but not only in the negative condition). The effect was preserved when considering RTslow. Finally, the comparison of the QQ-plots (Figures 2 and 3, third vertical panel) based on two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test illustrated that only group PA-PS elicited a significant RT difference of the CDFs distributions between the negative compared to the neutral condition (ks = 0.046, p = 0.014). The effect was again pronounced when considering RTslow (ks = 0.048, p = 0.008).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Delta plots (effect of ‘group’).

Quantile Analysis (fNIRS Data)

The main effects obtained by the quantile analysis are illustrated in Figure 4. Two regions of interest (ROIs) elicited significant effects, i.e., the DLPFC and the middle temporal gyrus (MTG). While the DLPFC [channels 23/24 (right) and 29/30 (left)] reflected effects of ‘quantile,’ ‘interference,’ and ‘group,’ the MTG [channels 32/43 (right) and 42 (left)] elicited only an effect of ‘group.’ For the repeated measures ANOVA analysis as illustrated in Figure 5; Table 5, bilateral channels of the two ROIs were collapsed over hemispheres, since no significant differences were found between hemispheres.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Quantile analysis: Main effects. Schematic front and side views of the fNIRS probe setup with 52 channels (gray). ROI DLPFC: Channels 23/24 (right) and 29/30 (left) reflected significant main effects of ‘quantile,’ ‘interference,’ and ‘group’ (black). ROI MTG: Channels 32/43 (right) and 42 (left) reflected a significant main effect of ‘group’ (red). The MATLAB toolbox NFRI (Singh et al., 2005) was used to estimate the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates of the 10–20 positions. Channels were visualized using BrainNet Viewer (Xia et al., 2013) (Table 6).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Quantile analysis. Illustration of the quantile differences in heart rate variability (HRV), and the Δ[tHb] beta estimates (DLPFC, MTG) per group (CO, PA, PS, PA-PS). Positive values indicate larger responses in the positive or negative compared to the neutral condition (i.e., positive minus neutral, and negative minus neutral). Error bars indicate standard error of the mean. HRV: Main effect of ‘group’ (F = 19.116, p < 0.001). An effect of ‘quantile’ was observed in group CO between the first and fourth quantile (Q1 vs. Q4) for RT and RTslow. DLPFC: Main effects of ‘quantile’ (F = 25.266, p < 0.001) and ‘interference’ (F = 25.703, p < 0.001) for RTslow. An effect of ‘group’ was observed in the fourth quantile (Q4) for RT and RTslow. MTG: Main effect of ‘group’ (F = 8.755, p < 0.001) and interaction effect of ‘interference’ × ‘group’ (F = 4.481, p < 0.001).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Quantile analysis.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 6. Regions of interest (ROIs).

Repeated measures ANOVA of the Δ[tHb] beta estimates of the DLPFC revealed a main effect of ‘interference’ (F = 25.703, p < 0.001). The effect reached significance only when considering RTslow, reflecting the pronouncing effect of slow RT (RT vs. RTslow F = 13.876, p < 0.001). This indicated that, over all groups, larger Δ[tHb] signals were observed in the negative compared to the neutral condition. Although, no main effect of ‘group’ was observed in the DLPFC, separate pairwise comparisons revealed significant differences in the fourth quantile (Q4) for both RT and RTslow. This indicated that group PA-PS elicited reduced Δ[tHb] amplitudes compared to group CO in the negative condition (RT t = 2.304, p = 0.024; RTslow t = 2.241, p = 0.028). Further, the DLPFC revealed a main effect of ‘quantile’ (RTslow F = 25.266, p < 0.001), indicating a monotonically increasing pattern of brain activity over time.

In contrast, repeated measures ANOVA of the Δ[tHb] beta estimates in the MTG revealed a main effect of ‘group’ (RT F = 8.755, p < 0.001; RTslow F = 8.499, p < 0.001), and an interaction effect of ‘interference’ × ‘group’ (RT F = 4.356, p < 0.001; RTslow F = 4.481, p < 0.001). This indicated that groups PA and PA-PS elicited reduced Δ[tHb] amplitudes compared to group CO due to an interference effect during the emotionally negative valence condition. No main effect of ‘quantile’ was observed in the MTG, indicating a stable pattern of brain activity over time.

Repeated measures ANOVA of the HRV data revealed a main effect of ‘group’ (F = 19.116, p < 0.001), indicating larger HRV signals in groups PS and PA-PS, compared to group CO and PA. Although, no main effect of ‘quantile’ was observed, separate pairwise comparisons revealed significant differences between the first and the fourth quantile (Q1 vs. Q4) in group CO, both for RT and RTslow. This indicated that HRV signals significantly decreased over time from Q1 to Q4 in group CO in both the positive (RT t = 6.940, p < 0.001; RTslow 6.822, p < 0.001) and the negative (RT t = 6. 769, p < 0.001; RTslow 6.828, p < 0.001) condition. This effect was not observed in groups PA, PS, or PA-PS. Hence, the HRV reflected a monotonically decreasing pattern over time, but only in group CO (stable patterns in groups CO, PA, PA-PS).

Comparison of Subscales

As described in Section “Subjects,” subjects were assigned to the four groups (CO, PA, PS, PA-PS) based on the combination of the five quintiles of the two subscales STS (corresponding to SCL-90-R ‘Paranoid Ideation’) and SNS (corresponding to SCL-90-R ‘Psychoticism’). In Figure 6, we aimed to control whether the main effects of ‘group’ would also fit the subscales separately. The plots therefore illustrate the main effects of ‘group’ in comparison to the five original quintiles of the subscales ‘Paranoid Ideation’ (STS) and ‘Psychoticism’ (SNS). Overall, the results corresponded well to the findings in the quantile analysis. In particular, the MTG reflected a strong effect of ‘group,’ which fitted significantly with both subscales ‘Paranoid Ideation’ (PA) and ‘Psychoticism’ (PS), as indicated by a significant linear relationship for ‘group’ (R2 = 0.846), as well as for the two subscales (PA R2 = 0.754; PS R2 = 0.758). In contrast, the DLPFC showed, as expected, no effect of ‘group,’ as indicated by no linear relationship for ‘group’ (R2 = 0.051), or the two subscales (PA R2 = 0.083; PS R2= 0.505). Interestingly, the HRV data fitted significantly better with the subscale ‘Paranoid Ideation’ (PA R2 = 0.867), compared to ‘Psychoticism’ (PS R2 = 0.174; group R2 = 0.441). This may indicate that HRV as a measure of affective responsiveness reflected ‘emotional’ aspects associated with paranoid perception better, but not psychoticism. Note that for simplicity, Figure 6 has been plotted only for the difference between the negative and the neutral condition based on RTslow.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. Comparison of subscales. Comparison of the main effects of ‘group’ (CO, PA, PS, PA-PS) with the five original quintiles of the subscales ‘Paranoid Ideation’ (PA1–PA5) and ‘Psychoticism’ (PS1–PS5), for HRV, DLPFC, and MTG. Note that for simplicity, the plots are only shown for the difference between the negative and the neutral condition based on RTslow. Blue lines indicate the main effect and the corresponding fit expressed as the coefficient of determination (R2).

Discussion

The present work examined a community sample of individuals with low compared to high subclinical psychotic symptoms. A community sample increases the generalizability and distinguishes our work from other studies conducted in more likely biased convenience samples with subclinical psychosis (Fusar-Poli et al., 2010, 2011; Jacobson et al., 2010; Sabb et al., 2010; Modinos et al., 2011; Allen et al., 2012; Jung et al., 2012; Brent et al., 2014). The study aimed to assess deficits in the control of emotional interference in subclinical psychotic symptoms, because they have been suggested to represent a feature of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders (Kohler et al., 2000). As behavioral measure the color-word emotional Stroop task was applied. So far, no neuroimaging studies have been published evaluating the emotional Stroop in individuals with subclinical psychotic symptoms.

In the color-word emotional Stroop task (Williams et al., 1996; Dresler et al., 2012) the slowing of RTs for color naming of emotional words (positive and negative) relative to neutral words serves as a measure of emotional interference. The meaning of the emotional word stimuli is, however, semantically unrelated to the task-relevant information (ink color). Thus, it has been debated that this version of the emotional Stroop does not directly assess the interference of emotional processing with attentional processing. The lack of stable interference effects may therefore explain the contradictory findings and heterogeneity of previous results, especially in healthy controls (McKenna and Sharma, 2004; Frings et al., 2010). The primary limitation of the emotional Stroop task is that none of the previous studies could so far define a stable network related to its performance as opposed to the original color naming Stroop (Stroop, 1935) or an emotional Stroop-like task based on the semantic conflict between emotional distractor (emotional words) and emotional targets (faces) (Chechko et al., 2012). Hence, the lack of reliable behavioral effects limits the conclusions that can be drawn from previous imaging studies in healthy subjects.

For statistical quantification of the emotional interference effects, the present study applied distributional methods, i.e., delta plots and quantile analysis. These methods provided suitable frameworks to obtain a better understanding of the Stroop-dependent interference effects. The main conclusion that can be drawn from the distributional analyses is that it considers the dynamic aspect of response timing (both for the behavioral and the brain data) and that this contributed essential to the detection of interference effects and disorder-specific effects. Without considering these dynamic aspects, the present findings would have been missed. The results may therefore suggest that the contradictory findings and heterogeneity of previous studies on the emotional Stroop maybe better understood considering the present analysis.

Taking the behavioral, cortical, and cardiac level together, we could confirm all hypotheses stated in the introduction. The results therefore provided evidence that the emotional Stroop is sensitive enough to detect deficits in emotional interference in persons with subclinical psychotic symptoms, supporting an extension of the clinical psychosis phenotype.

Delta Plots (Behavioral Data)

To obtain a detailed understanding of the behavioral RT data within the emotional Stroop task, we applied delta plots (Figures 2 and 3). Previous studies applied delta plots to the original Stroop paradigm (Pratte et al., 2010). Although the original Stroop paradigm and the emotional Stroop are both formally forms of interference tasks, characterized by response-relevant and response-irrelevant stimuli that can potentially produce interference effects, the two versions may nevertheless differ in the basic form of delta plots they generate (for review see Pratte et al., 2010).

The present study showed that delta plots are a suitable framework for evaluating the interference effects in the emotional Stroop task (Figures 2 and 3, Tables 3 and 4). Our main finding was that subjects with high (but not low) subclinical psychotic symptoms elicited significant interference effects in terms of a slowing of RTs in the negative condition. A significant drop of the delta line below zero visually illustrated this finding, with the largest interference effects found in the higher quantiles.

Considering the slow RTs (RTslow) (Figures 2 and 3, Bottom), the high symptom group generated slower RTs (RTslow) and their RTs exceeded the longest RTs generated by the CO group. This indicated that subjects with high subclinical psychotic symptoms not only showed a higher frequency of RT overshoots (>1500 ms), but also a higher magnitude of the RT maximum (exceeding the RT maximum observed in group CO). From a psychophysiological point of view, this aspect may also be associated with what may be described as ‘effect of time pressure’ (Sharma and McKenna, 2001) in the emotional Stroop task. Sharma and McKenna (2001) showed that time pressure (i.e., restricting the response interval to a required interval, as in the present study) plays an important role in determining not only the magnitude of interference effects, but also in detecting any effect at all. The authors therefore argued that time pressure may act as a stressor on its own, based on their results that interference effects of emotional valence stimuli (in particular negative stimuli) were present under high time pressure but absent under low time pressure. Hence, without (actual/perceived) time pressure no differences between neutral and emotional stimuli may be occurred. This suggests that while negative emotional stimuli do produce interference, this interference is not inevitable and may, in fact, be eliminated with longer (or un-restricted) response intervals (Sharma and McKenna, 2001). Although, the previous results may not be directly transferred to our study (since we did not assess differences between restricted versus unrestricted versions of the task), one may suggest that subjects in group PA-PS may have perceived subjective time pressure, thus leading to the observed significant interference effects.

Quantile Analysis (fNIRS Data)

The quantile analysis allowed for a direct comparison of the dynamic aspects of the hemodynamic (Δ[tHb]) and HRV data with the behavioral delta plots. In line with previous studies (Compton et al., 2003; Mohanty et al., 2005; Park et al., 2008), brain activity in the DLPFC reflected an interference effect (Figure 4). Negative words stimulated significantly larger hemodynamic responses compared to neutral words. Moreover, we observed that this effect was monotonically increasing over time as a function of RT, with the largest amplitudes in the highest RT quantile. The very similar pattern in the DLPFC and the behavioral delta plots therefore suggested a strong physiological relationship of the hemodynamic with the behavioral RT data, dynamically increasing from low to high response quantiles.

From a theoretical point of view, this effect may appear similar to what has been described as time-on-task effect in the original Stroop task (Taylor et al., 2013, 2014), typically found in prefrontal areas. The time-on-task effect describes that PFC can monotonically increase neural activity with time-on-task, even when no task-related decisions are made. It has therefore been argued, that greater PFC activity on interference trials (incompatible trials in the original Stroop task) may stem from longer RTs rather than response conflict. Hence, in case of a time-on-task effect, both incompatible (high-interference) and compatible (low-interference) trials would show monotonically increasing activity. This argument is in contrast to the traditional conflict monitoring model (Botvinick et al., 2001), which describes that PFC is primarily involved in detecting interference between competing responses thus signaling the need for attentional control. The conflict monitoring model thereby predicts monotonically increasing neural activity with increasing RT for incompatible (high-interference) but not compatible (low-interference) trials. Based on these arguments, we suggest that the present results in the DLPFC do not represent a time-on-task effect, but a real interference effect based on our observation of monotonically increasing activity with increasing RT for negative (high-interference) but not neutral (low-interference) trials. In other words, our data suggest that DLPFC activity reflects interference effects.

In contrast to the DLPFC, the hemodynamic response in the MTG showed a stable pattern over time (Figure 4). The MTG rather reflected a strong disorder-specific group-effect, with reduced response in subjects with high compared to low subclinical psychotic symptoms. Generally, the temporal gyrus has been described to have gray matter volume reductions not only in patients with schizophrenia (Matsumoto et al., 2001; Onitsuka et al., 2004), but also in subclinical psychosis (Jacobson et al., 2010; Brent et al., 2014). These gray volume reductions have been reported in superior, middle, and inferior regions of the temporal gyrus, have been shown to progress over the time (Kasai et al., 2003; Takahashi et al., 2009), and to be associated with positive psychosis symptoms (Onitsuka et al., 2004). Further, fMRI studies investigating emotional priming in the original Stroop task in healthy subjects, showed a smaller response of the temporal gyrus to aversive (negative) compared to neutral priming (Hart et al., 2010). The smaller interference effect has therefore been argued to reflect functional and structural abnormalities in areas associated with language processing in the (superior) temporal gyrus in patients with schizophrenia (Li et al., 2009). The present data may be in line with this previous work, reflecting reduced magnitudes of the hemodynamic responses in temporal gyrus due to disorder-specific, but task-independent, reductions in activity. This finding might indicate that the MTG is less involved in the interference effect (compared to the DLPFC), but rather reflects disorder-specific effects.

Taking the results of the DLPFC and the MTG together, the present analysis demonstrated both interference effects and disorder-specific effects. Extending previous research, the present study thereby suggests that emotional interference in response to the emotional Stroop task can already be detected on the subclinical level of psychotic symptoms.

Heart Rate Variability

In addition to the cortical data, the present analysis assessed individual cardiac responses. Cardiovascular responses have been reported in the original Stroop task (Fauvel et al., 1996; Renaud and Blondin, 1997; Boutcher and Boutcher, 2006; Satish et al., 2015), and in one study in the emotional Stroop (Mathewson et al., 2011). These studies consistently showed that Stroop performance was accompanied by heightened HRV levels (although also controversial results were reported, Boutcher and Boutcher, 2006). Extending previous work, we addressed disorder-specific interference effects on HRV. In particular, we show that the effects on HRV differed between groups. Subjects with low subclinical psychotic symptoms monotonically decreased in HRV levels after stimulus onset. In contrast, subjects with high subclinical psychotic symptoms maintained an increased HRV level until the end of the trial.

A potential factor that may have contributed to these findings may be the perception of stress or time pressure [see also Delta plots (behavioral data)] based on differences in subjective anxiety. For example, a previous study reported that state-anxiety scores increased during original Stroop performance, but only among subjects who completed a large number of trials with restricted response intervals, i.e., performance likely to induce stress or time pressure (Renaud and Blondin, 1997). This and other studies showed that subjective anxiety may influence performance in the emotional Stroop task. In particular, it has been argued that inconsistent findings regarding the emotional Stroop effect in healthy subjects may be explained not only by effects of stimulus valence and arousal, but also by confounding individual differences in state anxiety (not trait anxiety) (Dresler et al., 2009). This state anxiety-related effect has even been suggested in healthy subjects, indicating that arousal produces emotional interference independent of word valence, and that state anxiety exacerbates interference of emotional words by further biasing attention toward emotionally salient stimuli (Dresler et al., 2009). Taking together, it may be argued that differences in subjective perception of stress, anxiety, or time pressure played a role in the increasing HRV response observed on the present study.

Conclusion

The present findings are the first that provide evidence of cortical hemodynamic correlates of emotional interference in subjects with subclinical psychotic symptoms using fNIRS. The results based on distributional analysis suggest that beyond the evaluation of averaged RTs, the evaluation of the dynamic aspects of response timing may support the detection of both interference effects and disorder-specific effects. Taking the behavioral, cortical, and cardiac level together, the presented study proposes that further careful investigations of prefrontal and temporal cortices may provide neurobiological correlates of subclinical psychotic symptoms during emotional Stroop performance.

The specific value of the present work is that the observed brain correlates of subclinical psychotic symptoms were obtained in a community sample. This sample presented symptoms similar to persons with schizophrenia, though in an attenuated form. It may consequently be suggested that these individuals represent what has been earlier proposed as a new DSM-5 diagnostic entity (‘Attenuated Psychosis Syndrome’) (Tsuang et al., 2013). Although the validity of our observations needs to be confirmed, the brain correlates of subclinical psychotic symptoms reported in the present work may indicate a step toward possible markers of such attenuated psychotic symptoms.

Author Contributions

Authors WR, WK, HH, VA-G, and AF, designed the study and wrote the protocol according to the Epidemiology Survey of the Zurich Program for Sustainable Development of Mental Health Services (ZInEP). Authors FH, AA, and MM, collected the data. Author LH undertook the statistical analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors critically revised and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer VP and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation, and the handling Editor states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.

Acknowledgments

The work was supported by the Zurich Program for Sustainable Development of Mental Health Services (ZInEP), University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, and the academic career program Filling the Gap, University of Zurich (Grant number: FTG-1415-007).

References

Ajdacic-Gross, V., Müller, M., Rodgers, S., Warnke, I., Hengartner, M. P., Landolt, K., et al. (2014). The ZInEP epidemiology survey: background, design and methods. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 23, 451–468. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1441

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, P., Luigjes, J., Howes, O. D., Egerton, A., Hirao, K., Valli, I., et al. (2012). Transition to psychosis associated with prefrontal and subcortical dysfunction in ultra high-risk individuals. Schizophr. Bull. 38, 1268–1276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr194

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Bull. 133, 1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besnier, N., Kaladjian, A., Mazzola-Pomietto, P., Adida, M., Fakra, E., Jeanningros, R., et al. (2011). Differential responses to emotional interference in paranoid schizophrenia and bipolar mania. Psychopathology 44, 1–11. doi: 10.1159/000322097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besnier, N., Richard, F., Zendjidjian, X., Kaladjian, A., Mazzola-Pomietto, P., Adida, M., et al. (2009). Stroop and emotional Stroop interference in unaffected relatives of patients with schizophrenic and bipolar disorders: distinct markers of vulnerability? World J. Biol. Psychiatry 10, 809–818. doi: 10.1080/15622970903131589

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M., Braver, T., Barch, D., Carter, C., and Cohen, J. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108, 624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boutcher, Y. N., and Boutcher, S. H. (2006). Cardiovascular response to Stroop: effect of verbal response and task difficulty. Biol. Psychol. 73, 235–241. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.04.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brent, B. K., Coombs, G., Keshavan, M. S., Seidman, L. J., Moran, J. M., and Holt, D. J. (2014). Subclinical delusional thinking predicts lateral temporal cortex responses during social reflection. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 273–282. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, L., Chapman, J., Kwapil, T., Eckbald, M., and Zinser, M. (1994). Putatively psychosis-prone subjects 10 years later. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 103, 171–183. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.103.2.171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chechko, N., Kellermann, T., Zvyagintsev, M., Augustin, M., Schneider, F., and Habel, U. (2012). Brain circuitries involved in semantic interference by demands of emotional and non-emotional distractors. PLoS ONE 7:e38155. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Compton, R., Banich, M., Mohanty, A., Milham, M., Herrington, J., Miller, G., et al. (2003). Paying attention to emotion: an fMRI investigation of cognitive and emotional stroop tasks. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 81–96. doi: 10.3758/CABN.3.2.81

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., and Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 20, 731–750. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demily, C., Attala, N., Fouldrin, G., Czernecki, V., Ménard, J.-F., Lamy, S., et al. (2010). The emotional Stroop task: a comparison between schizophrenic subjects and controls. Eur. Psychiatry 25, 75–79. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2009.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derogatis, L. (1977). SCL-90. Administration, Scoring and Procedures Manual for the R (Revised) Version and Other Instruments of the Psychopathology Rating Scale Series. Chicago, IL: John Hopkins University School of Medicine.

Dresler, T., Attar, C. H., Spitzer, C., Löwe, B., Deckert, J., Büchel, C., et al. (2012). Neural correlates of the emotional Stroop task in panic disorder patients: an event-related fMRI study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 46, 1627–1634. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dresler, T., Mériau, K., Heekeren, H., and van der Meer, E. (2009). Emotional Stroop task: effect of word arousal and subject anxiety on emotional interference. Psychol. Res. 73, 364–371. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0154-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehlis, A. C., Schneider, S., Dresler, T., and Fallgatter, A. (2014). Application of functional near-infrared spectroscopy in psychiatry. Neuroimage 15, 478–488. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fach, W., Atmanspacher, H., Landolt, K., Wyss, T., and Rössler, W. (2013). A comparative study of exceptional experiences of clients seeking advice and of subjects in an ordinary population. Front. Psychol. 4:65. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fauvel, J. P., Bernard, N., Laville, M., Daoud, S., Pozet, N., and Zech, P. (1996). Reproducibility of the cardiovascular reactivity to a computerized version of the Stroop stress test in normotensive and hypertensive subjects. Clin. Auton. Res. 6, 219–224. doi: 10.1007/BF02291137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frings, C., Englert, J., Wentura, D., and Bermeitinger, C. (2010). Decomposing the emotional Stroop effect. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Hove 63, 42–49. doi: 10.1080/17470210903156594

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusar-Poli, P., Broome, M. R., Matthiasson, P., Woolley, J. B., Johns, L. C., Tabraham, P., et al. (2010). Spatial working memory in individuals at high risk for psychosis: longitudinal fMRI study. Schizophr. Res. 123, 45–52. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusar-Poli, P., Broome, M. R., Matthiasson, P., Woolley, J. B., Mechelli, A., Johns, L. C., et al. (2011). Prefrontal function at presentation directly related to clinical outcome in people at ultrahigh risk of psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 37, 189–198. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagnon, L., Yücel, M. A., Dehaes, M., Cooper, R. J., Perdue, K. L., Selb, J., et al. (2012). Quantification of the cortical contribution to the NIRS signal over the motor cortex using concurrent NIRS-fMRI measurements. Neuroimage 59, 3933–3940. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goff, D. C., Hill, M., and Barch, D. (2011). The treatment of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 99, 245–253. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grinband, J., Savitskaya, J., Wager, T. D., Teichert, T., Ferrera, V. P., and Hirsch, J. (2011). The dorsal medial frontal cortex is sensitive to time on task, not response conflict or error likelihood. Neuroimage 57, 303–311. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grubb, R., Raichle, M., Eichling, J., and Ter-Pogossian, M. (1974). The effects of changes in PaCO2 cerebral blood volume, blood flow, and vascular mean transit time. Stroke 5, 630–639. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.5.5.630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanssen, M., Bak, M., Bijl, R., Vollebergh, W., and van Os, J. (2005). The incidence and outcome of subclinical psychotic experiences in the general population. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 44, 181–191. doi: 10.1348/014466505X29611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, S. J., Green, S. R., Casp, M., and Belger, A. (2010). Emotional priming effects during Stroop task performance. Neuroimage 49, 2662–2670. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heckers, S. (2001). Neuroimaging studies of the hippocampus in schizophrenia. Hippocampus 11, 520–528. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, T., Diamond, S., Franceschini, M., and Boas, D. (2009). HomER: a review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Appl. Opt. 48, D280–D298. doi: 10.1364/AO.48.00D280

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, S., Kelleher, I., Harley, M., Murtagh, A., Clarke, M., Blanchard, M., et al. (2010). Structural and functional brain correlates of subclinical psychotic symptoms in 11–13 year old schoolchildren. Neuroimage 49, 1875–1885. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jang, K., Tak, S., Jung, J., Jang, J., Jeong, Y., and Ye, J. (2009). Wavelet minimum description length detrending for near-infrared spectroscopy. J. Biomed. Opt. 14, 34004. doi: 10.1117/1.3127204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johns, L. C., and van Os, J. (2001). The continuity of psychotic experiences in the general population. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 21, 1125–1141. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00103-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, W., Jang, J., Shin, N., Kim, S., Choi, C., An, S., et al. (2012). Regional brain atrophy and functional disconnection in Broca’s area in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and schizophrenia. PLoS ONE 7:e51975. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051975

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasai, K., Shenton, M. E., Salisbury, D. F., Hirayasu, Y., Lee, C.-U., Ciszewski, A. A., et al. (2003). Progressive decrease of left superior temporal gyrus gray matter volume in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 156–164. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.156

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohler, C. G., Bilker, W., Hagendoorn, M., Gur, R. E., and Gur, R. C. (2000). Emotion recognition deficit in schizophrenia: association with symptomatology and cognition. Biol. Psychiatry 48, 127–136. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00847-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kring, A. M., and Moran, E. K. (2008). Emotional response deficits in schizophrenia: insights from affective science. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 819–834. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Branch, C. A., and DeLisi, L. E. (2009). Language pathway abnormalities in schizophrenia: a review of fMRI and other imaging studies. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 22, 131–139. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328324bc43

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marden, J. I. (2004). Positions and QQ Plots. Stat. Sci. 19, 606–614. doi: 10.1214/088342304000000512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathewson, K. J., Drmic, I. E., Jetha, M. K., Bryson, S. E., Goldberg, J. O., Hall, G. B., et al. (2011). Behavioral and cardiac responses to emotional stroop in adults with autism spectrum disorders: influence of medication. Autism Res. 4, 98–108. doi: 10.1002/aur.176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, H., Simmons, A., Williams, S., Hadjulis, M., Pipe, R., Murray, R., et al. (2001). Superior temporal gyrus abnormalities in early-onset schizophrenia: similarities and differences with adult-onset schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 1299–1304. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKenna, F. P., and Sharma, D. (2004). Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: the role of fast and slow components. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 30, 382–392. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.382

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meehl, P. (1990). Toward an integrated theory of schizotaxia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia. J. Personal. Disord. 4, 1–99. doi: 10.1521/pedi.1990.4.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minzenberg, M. J., Laird, A. R., Thelen, S., Carter, C. S., and Glahn, D. C. (2009). Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 811–822. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.91

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Modinos, G., Renken, R., Ormel, J., and Aleman, A. (2011). Self-reflection and the psychosis-prone brain: an fMRI study. Neuropsychology 25, 295–305. doi: 10.1037/a0021747

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohanty, A., Heller, W., Koven, N. S., Fisher, J. E., Herrington, J. D., and Miller, G. A. (2008). Specificity of emotion-related effects on attentional processing in schizotypy. Schizophr. Res. 103, 129–137. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohanty, A., Herrington, J., Koven, N., Fisher, J., Wenzel, E., Webb, A., et al. (2005). Neural mechanisms of affective interference in schizotypy. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114, 16–27. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onitsuka, T., Shenton, M. E., Salisbury, D. F., Dickey, C. C., Kasai, K., Toner, S. K., et al. (2004). Middle and inferior temporal gyrus gray matter volume abnormalities in chronic schizophrenia: an MRI study. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 1603–1611. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.9.1603

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, I. H., Park, H.-J., Chun, J.-W., Kim, E. Y., and Kim, J.-J. (2008). Dysfunctional modulation of emotional interference in the medial prefrontal cortex in patients with schizophrenia. Neurosci. Lett. 440, 119–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.05.094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, L. K., Deldin, P. J., Voglmaier, M. M., and Rabbitt, S. (2005). Emotional Stroop performance predicts disorganization in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 77, 141–149. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, M. L., Drevets, W. C., Rauch, S. L., and Lane, R. (2003). Neurobiology of emotion perception II: implications for major psychiatric disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 515–528. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00171-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratte, M., Rouder, J., Morey, R., and Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 72, 2013–2025. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.7.2013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reichenberg, A., and Harvey, P. D. (2007). Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings. Psychol. Bull. 133, 833–858. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renaud, P., and Blondin, J.-P. (1997). The stress of Stroop performance: physiological and emotional responses to color–word interference, task pacing, and pacing speed. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 27, 87–97. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00049-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridderinkhof, K. R., Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J., and Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/HD that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114, 197–215. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.197

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rössler, W., Ajdacic-Gross, V., Haker, H., Rodgers, S., Müller, M., and Hengartner, M. P. (2013a). Subclinical psychosis syndromes in the general population: results from a large-scale epidemiological survey among residents of the canton of Zurich, Switzerland. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 24, 69–77. doi: 10.1017/S2045796013000681

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rössler, W., Ajdacic-Gross, V., Müller, M., Rodgers, S., Haker, H., and Hengartner, M. P. (2015). Assessing sub-clinical psychosis phenotypes in the general population – a multidimensional approach. Schizophr. Res. 161, 194–201. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.11.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rössler, W., Hengartner, M. P., Ajdacic-Gross, V., Haker, H., and Angst, J. (2013b). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence rates of sub-clinical psychosis symptoms in a community cohort of 50-year-old individuals. Eur. Psychiatry 28, 302–307. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2012.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rössler, W., Riecher-Rössler, A., Angst, J., Murray, R., Gamma, A., Eich, D., et al. (2007). Psychotic experiences in the general population: a twenty-year prospective community study. Schizophr. Res. 92, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabb, F. W., van Erp, T. G. M., Hardt, M. E., Dapretto, M., Caplan, R., Cannon, T. D., et al. (2010). Language network dysfunction as a predictor of outcome in youth at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 116, 173–183. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.09.042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satish, P., Muralikrishnan, K., Balasubramanian, K., and Shanmugapriya. (2015). Heart rate variability changes during stroop color and word test among genders. Indian J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 59, 9–15.

Google Scholar

Scholkmann, F., Boss, J., and Wolf, M. (2012). An efficient algorithm for automatic peak detection in noisy periodic and quasi-periodic signals. Algorithms 5, 588–603. doi: 10.3390/a5040588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, W., and Miller, J. (2012). Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 19, 555–574. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0254-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, D., and McKenna, F. P. (2001). The role of time pressure on the emotional Stroop task. Br. J. Psychol. 92, 471–481. doi: 10.1348/000712601162293

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, A., Okamoto, M., Dan, H., Jurcak, V., and Dan, I. (2005). Spatial registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without MRI. Neuroimage 27, 842–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speckman, P. L., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., and Pratte, M. S. (2008). Delta plots and coherent distribution ordering. Am. Stat. 62, 262–266. doi: 10.1198/000313008X333493

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, G. P., Allen, D. N., Duke, L. A., Ross, S. A., and Schwartz, J. (2008). Automatic affective processing impairments in patients with deficit syndrome schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 102, 76–87. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.01.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, T., Wood, S., Yung, A., Soulsby, B., McGorry, P., Suzuki, M., et al. (2009). Progressive gray matter reduction of the superior temporal gyrus during transition to psychosis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 366–376. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. S. H., Rastle, K., and Davis, M. (2013). Can cognitive models explain brain activation during word and pseudoword reading? A meta-analysis of 36 neuroimaging studies. Psychol. Bull. 139, 766–791. doi: 10.1037/a0030266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. S. H., Rastle, K., and Davis, M. H. (2014). Interpreting response time effects in functional imaging studies. Neuroimage 99, 419–433. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsuang, M., Van Os, J., Tandon, R., Barch, D. M., Bustillo, J., Gaebel, W., et al. (2013). Attenuated psychosis syndrome in DSM-5. Schizophr. Res. 5, 31–35. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unsworth, N., Redick, T., Spillers, G., and Brewer, G. (2012). Variation in working memory capacity and cognitive control: goal maintenance and microadjustments of control. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Hove 65, 326–355. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2011.597865

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Os, J., Hanssen, M., Bijl, R. V., and Ravelli, A. (2000). Strauss (1969) revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophr. Res. 45, 11–20. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00224-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westerhausen, R., Kompus, K., and Hugdahl, K. (2011). Impaired cognitive inhibition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the Stroop interference effect. Schizophr. Res. 133, 172–181. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.08.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiles, N. J., Zammit, S., Bebbington, P., Singleton, N., Meltzer, H., and Lewis, G. (2006). Self-reported psychotic symptoms in the general population: results from the longitudinal study of the British National Psychiatric Morbidity Survey. Br. J. Psychiatry 188, 519–526. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.012179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. M, Mathews, A., and MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol. Bull. 120, 3–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worsley, K. J., and Friston, K. (1995). Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited—again. Neuroimage 2, 173–181. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xia, M., Wang, J., and He, Y. (2013). BrainNet viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS ONE 8:e68910. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, J. C., Tak, S., Jang, K. E., Jung, J., and Jang, J. (2009). NIRS-SPM: statistical parametric mapping for near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuroimage 44, 428–447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: epidemiology, paranoid ideation, psychoticism, emotional Stroop, delta plot, quantile analysis, fNIRS

Citation: Holper LKB, Aleksandrowicz A, Müller M, Ajdacic-Gross V, Haker H, Fallgatter AJ, Hagenmuller F, Kawohl W and Rössler W (2016) Distribution of Response Time, Cortical, and Cardiac Correlates during Emotional Interference in Persons with Subclinical Psychotic Symptoms. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:172. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00172

Received: 28 December 2015; Accepted: 25 August 2016;
Published: 08 September 2016.

Edited by:

Nuno Sousa, University of Minho, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Shinsuke Koike, The University of Tokyo, Japan
Vitor Hugo Pereira, Life and Health Sciences Research Institute, Portugal

Copyright © 2016 Holper, Aleksandrowicz, Müller, Ajdacic-Gross, Haker, Fallgatter, Hagenmuller, Kawohl and Rössler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wulf Rössler, wulf.roessler@uzh.ch

Download