Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 26 July 2012
Sec. Sleep Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Sleep and cognition in the elderly View all 10 articles

Age-Related Changes in Sleep and Circadian Rhythms: Impact on Cognitive Performance and Underlying Neuroanatomical Networks

  • 1 Centre for Chronobiology, Psychiatric Hospital of the University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 2 Neuropsychology and Functional Neuroimaging Research Unit, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Bruxelles, Belgium

Circadian and homeostatic sleep-wake regulatory processes interact in a fine tuned manner to modulate human cognitive performance. Dampening of the circadian alertness signal and attenuated deterioration of psychomotor vigilance in response to elevated sleep pressure with aging change this interaction pattern. As evidenced by neuroimaging studies, both homeostatic sleep pressure and circadian sleep-wake promotion impact on cognition-related cortical and arousal-promoting subcortical brain regions including the thalamus, the anterior hypothalamus, and the brainstem locus coeruleus (LC). However, how age-related changes in circadian and homeostatic processes impact on the cerebral activity subtending waking performance remains largely unexplored. Post-mortem studies point to neuronal degeneration in the SCN and age-related modifications in the arousal-promoting LC. Alongside, cortical frontal brain areas are particularly susceptible both to aging and misalignment between circadian and homeostatic processes. In this perspective, we summarize and discuss here the potential neuroanatomical networks underlying age-related changes in circadian and homeostatic modulation of waking performance, ranging from basic arousal to higher order cognitive behaviors.

Introduction

Aging can be defined in terms of life and time (Martin, 1981) and it is often assumed that cognitive and health difficulties tend to increase as time advances. However, many researchers depart from this stereotype and put the concept of successful aging forward (for a review, see Lupien and Wan, 2004). Aging is considered as a multidimensional process in a way that environmental factors may protect for or conversely aggravate signs of aging in a non-linear manner with regard to physiological but also neurobehavioral processes. There is large and important heterogeneity both in cognitive and sleep-wake rhythm alterations that occur with normal aging, which have the potential to serve as a tool to better understand its underlying processes (Rowe and Kahn, 1987; Lupien and Wan, 2004; Eyler et al., 2011).

In the 1970s and 1980s, coupled oscillator models were shown to reproduce the basic features of the timing of human sleep and wake episodes, with one oscillator representing sleep/wake and the other representing the circadian pacemaker driving the temperature cycle (Wever, 1975; Kawato et al., 1982; Kronauer et al., 1982). Alternatively, the two process model of sleep regulation has been put forward at the same time by Borbely (1982) and Daan et al. (1984), and based on these models it was recently shown that a physiologically based model is able to account for many features of human sleep on self-selected schedules (Phillips et al., 2011). In this review we refer to the two process model, relying on a fined tuned interaction between the sleep-wake homeostatic and the circadian process to allow maintenance of sleep and wakefulness at appropriate times of day in order to explain time of day modulations in subjective sleepiness and cognitive performance.

There is ample evidence that the interplay of circadian and homeostatic processes also determines the temporal modulation of sleepiness and alertness levels across the day, which in turn affects performance for different cognitive domains (Cajochen et al., 2004; Dijk and von Schantz, 2005; Dijk and Archer, 2009). Disturbances or imbalance in the relationship between the circadian and homeostatic systems can lead to sleep and/or mood disorders and major difficulties in maintaining optimal cognitive performance during wake time.

Even in the absence of clinically significant sleep disorders, healthy aging is associated with a decline in night-time sleep quality and duration, decreases in sleep depth, sleep intensity, and sleep continuity (Bliwise, 2005). Concomitantly, a reduced amplitude of circadian rhythm output signals has been shown in older participants (Dijk et al., 1999; Duffy and Czeisler, 2002; Münch et al., 2005), suggesting that age-related changes in sleep may be partially due to a weaker circadian regulation of sleep and wakefulness. In parallel, it has been observed that older people may need less sleep (Klerman and Dijk, 2008) suggesting that in spite of marked changes in sleep physiology, excessive daytime sleepiness is not common during healthy aging (Duffy et al., 2009).

The underlying cerebral mechanisms of homeostatic and time of day-dependent modulation patterns in cognitive performance remain largely unexplored, in particular in relation to the healthy aging process. Recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in young volunteers yielded evidence that this interaction also influences cognition-related cortical (mainly frontal) and subcortical (thalamic, hypothalamic, and brain stem locus coeruleus, LC) brain activity (Schmidt et al., 2009, 2012; Vandewalle et al., 2009). Furthermore, there is evidence that cortical and subcortical task-related BOLD activity declines in those individuals presenting higher vulnerability to sleep loss and circadian misalignment while it increases in those participants who are less susceptible (e.g., Chuah et al., 2006; Vandewalle et al., 2009).

Similar fMRI studies are not yet available in older individuals. However, post-mortem studies revealed neuronal loss in the SCN of older people (Hofman and Swaab, 2006). Also, neuron density within the LC decreases with age due to a progressive loss of noradrenergic neurons, both in animals and humans (Samuels and Szabadi, 2008). Furthermore, the number of LC neurons projecting to areas such as the frontal cortex and the hippocampus declines with age, resulting in fewer synapses (Samuels and Szabadi, 2008). Since the LC is also involved in the regulation of cognitive performance (Usher et al., 1999), it can be hypothesized that age-related changes in these arousal-promoting structures may crucially contribute to circadian-related alterations in cognitive abilities. At the cortical level, frontal brain regions are particularly prone to both the aging process and to the misalignment between circadian and homeostatic processes, even though recent evidence indicates dissociation between these influences on frontal-activation-related executive functions (Cain et al., 2011; Tucker et al., 2011; Bratzke et al., 2012).

In this review, we will discuss the influence of circadian and homeostatic regulation on waking performance, including recent insights into the underlying cerebral correlates of the observed behavioral modulations. The impact of the age factor on these brain networks will then be discussed, considering that it is most likely that cognitive decline is a multifactorial process and that reserve factors may compensate for age-related modifications both in sleep features and cognitive functions (Bartres-Faz and Arenaza-Urquijo, 2011).

Cerebral Correlates Underlying Circadian and Homeostatic Regulation of Waking Performance throughout the 24-h Cycle

The specific timing and consolidation of sleep and wake episodes within the 24-h light-dark cycle are regulated by a coordinated action of homeostatic and circadian processes (Borbely, 1982; Daan et al., 1984; see also Figure 1). It is assumed that the circadian and homeostatic process represent independent drives on sleep-wake propensity but interact in a non-linear fashion across the 24-h light-dark cycle. Thus, circadian-based wake propensity is at its highest levels during the early evening hours (commonly after 12 h of wakefulness), when homeostatic sleep pressure is rather high, whereas circadian propensity for sleep reaches its maximum during the early morning (∼2 h before habitual wake up time), when homeostatic sleep pressure is low (Dijk and Czeisler, 1994). At any given time, the magnitude of sleepiness, alertness, and fatigue is thus determined by the interacting influences of these two processes (Figure 1). After homeostatic sleep pressure has mostly dissipated over the first hours of the night, it is the high circadian-based propensity for sleep that prevents us from prematurely waking up in the early morning hours. Conversely, it is the very low circadian-based propensity for sleep (i.e., circadian wake-promoting signal) that prevents us from falling asleep in the early evening hours when homeostatic sleep pressure is at its highest level. In both cases, circadian and homeostatic systems ideally work in opposition to ensure a consolidated period of sleep or wakefulness (Dijk and Czeisler, 1994, 1995; Dijk and von Schantz, 2005).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic illustration of the impact of circadian and homeostatic processes on sleep and wakefulness. The filled gray area illustrates variations in total sleep time during a constant routine protocol with regularly occurring naps (150 min of wakefulness followed by 75 min of naps), aiming at investigating circadian rhythm parameters under low homeostatic sleep pressure conditions. Black lines indicate superimposed subjective sleepiness as assessed by the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale over a similar nap (dashed line) and total sleep deprivation (straight line) protocol. The wake maintenance zone can be identified in the naps scheduled in the subjective evening hours, with minimal total sleep time (expressed in minutes). The sleep-promoting signal in the biological night is accompanied by rapid increases in subjective sleepiness in both the low (naps) and high (sleep deprivation) sleep pressure conditions. Over the course of the second biological day, subjective sleepiness decreases, even when homeostatic sleep pressure increases (in the sleep deprivation protocol, straight line), indicating that circadian wake promotion rises or that circadian sleep promotion diminishes [modified from Cajochen et al. (2001) and Münch et al. (2005)].

The impact of the circadian timing system goes beyond compelling the body to fall asleep and to wake up again (Blatter and Cajochen, 2007; Schmidt et al., 2007; Wright et al., 2012). Forced desynchrony, constant routine, and sleep deprivation studies have identified the respective contributions of homeostatic sleep pressure and circadian rhythmicity on neurobehavioral performance measures (Dijk et al., 1992; Johnson et al., 1992; Cajochen et al., 1999, 2004; Wyatt et al., 1999; Carrier and Monk, 2000; Horowitz et al., 2003; Rogers et al., 2003). Two important observations have been made from these controlled studies: (1) while performance deterioration is mostly seen when the wake episode is extended into the biological night, modulations can also be seen throughout a usual waking day episode (<16 h of wakefulness) which can lead to significant deteriorations in the cognitive output and (2) the observed effects of the circadian and sleep-wake homeostatic system do not simply add up to characterize daily performance modulations. In particular, the circadian amplitude of cognitive performance modulation clearly depends on homeostatic sleep pressure levels (Dijk and Franken, 2005).

The proposal mentioned above that circadian and homeostatic systems interact at the neurobehavioral level has been supported by anatomical findings. In terms of circadian sleep-wake regulation, the SCN is the central circadian pacemaker regulating sleep-wake timing. The SCN sends an indirect projection – relayed via the dorsomedian hypothalamus – to the noradrenergic LC, which in turns sends wide projections to the entire cortex (Aston-Jones et al., 2001; Aston-Jones, 2005). Consequently, the LC has been proposed to be implicated in the circadian regulation of higher order cognitive behaviors (Gompf and Aston-Jones, 2008). On the other hand, the cerebral correlates and exact anatomical location of the sleep homeostat are still unknown. It most likely represents a diffuse system that includes the accumulation of at least one sleep-promoting substance, which enhances the activity of sleep-promoting, and reduces the activity of wake-promoting neurons (Landolt, 2008). Accordingly, sleep homeostasis has been related to plastic processes occurring during wakefulness that result in a net increase in synaptic strength in many brain circuits (Tononi and Cirelli, 2003). From this perspective, sleep would serve to downscale synaptic strength to a baseline level that is energetically sustainable with the aim of a homeostatic regulation of the total synaptic weight impinging on neurons.

Recent evidence from fMRI investigations in young morning and evening chronotypes indicate that homeostatic sleep pressure exerts an influence on attention-related cerebral activity in anterior hypothalamic structures, putatively implicated in the regulation of the circadian wake-promoting signal (Schmidt et al., 2009; see Figure 2). In particular, maintenance of optimal attentional performance in a vigilance task (PVT; psychomotor vigilance) after accumulated sleep pressure (i.e., during the subjective evening) was associated with higher activity in evening than morning chronotypes in the LC and in the anterior hypothalamus, two key structures crucially involved in the generation of the circadian wake-promoting signal. Furthermore, activity in the anterior hypothalamus decreased with increasing homeostatic sleep pressure as indexed by electroencephalographic (EEG) slow wave activity [SWA; EEG power density during non-rapid eye movement (Non-REM) sleep in the range of 0.75–4.5 Hz] in the first sleep cycle, suggesting that homeostatic and circadian interactions influence the neural activity underpinning diurnal variations in human behavior. Interestingly, this activation pattern was observed solely for the 10% of fastest reaction times that reflect the phasic ability to recruit the attentional network above normal levels (Drummond et al., 2005a).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (Left, top panel) higher task-related thalamic activation in morning as compared to evening types for intermediate reaction times (“global alertness”) during the subjective evening hours [modified from Schmidt et al. (2009)] (right, top panel) Higher BOLD activity in locus coeruleus and anterior hypothalamic regions in evening as compared to morning types for “optimal alertness” (10% of fastest reaction times, as compared to intermediate reaction times). (Left, bottom panel) both regions have been implicated in circadian arousal regulation, as illustrated by the model of Aston-Jones et al. (2001). (right, bottom panel) Finally, optimal alertness-related activity in the anterior hypothalamus (i.e., suprachiasmatic area) is negatively related to the amounts of EEG slow wave activity at the beginning of the night, which can be considered as a reliable marker of homeostatic sleep pressure build-up [modified from Schmidt et al. (2009)].

Recently, a 24-h sleep deprivation study (Vandewalle et al., 2009) took advantage of a genetic trait (the hPER3 polymorphism; Viola et al., 2007) associated with differential vulnerability to the deleterious effects of sleep deprivation on neurobehavioral performance. This study revealed that from the morning (1.5 h of wakefulness) to the evening (14 h of wakefulness) of a normal waking day, the more resistant PER34/4 individuals did not exhibit significant changes in brain responses to a working memory task, whereas the more vulnerable PER35/5 participants presented decreased activity in the posterior dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex. When further challenging the sleep homeostat by 25 h of total sleep deprivation, the more vulnerable PER35/5 subjects presented various decreased task-related cortical activations in the morning after sleep loss. In contrast, PER34/4 still did not show decreased brain responses to the task, but rather recruited supplemental brain areas located in right inferior frontal, middle temporal, parahippocampal gyri, as well as in bilateral thalamic areas. Similarly, morning types, more vulnerable to the accumulation of time spent awake throughout a normal waking day (Kerkhof, 1991; Mongrain et al., 2006a,b) show decreased BOLD responses in brain areas involved in conflict resolution over a normal waking day while performing the Stroop paradigm (Schmidt et al., 2012). In contrast, evening chronotypes, less affected by accumulated homeostatic sleep pressure during the evening exhibited the reversed profile or presented stable BOLD responses from morning to evening hours in task-related brain regions (Schmidt et al., 2012).

Age-Related Modulation in Circadian and Homeostatic Regulation of Sleep and Waking Performance

It has been controversial whether age-related sleep changes result from alterations in circadian and homeostatic processes or in their precise interaction (see Figure 3 for a schematic illustration of age-related changes on circadian and homeostatic sleep-wake regulation). The age-related decline in absolute levels of slow wave sleep (SWS) represents one of the most common reported features in the ageing and sleep literature (Bliwise, 2005). Studies demonstrated that older adults respond to sleep loss with an increase in EEG SWA (Dijk et al., 2001) indicating that, even though older persons present lower absolute SWS levels, the homeostatic response to increasing sleep need is basically operational. However, older adults also showed a shallower decline in homeostatic sleep pressure after sleep deprivation, particularly in frontal brain regions (Dijk et al., 1989; Münch et al., 2004). Together with the recently observed age-related reduction in asymptotic sleep duration under extended sleep conditions, these data favor the assumption that older adults have a generally lower homeostatic need for sleep (Klerman and Dijk, 2008). In the same perspective, healthy aging was associated with a reduction in daytime sleep propensity, while sleep continuity and SWS were reduced (Dijk et al., 2010).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Schematic illustration of age-related modifications in circadian and homeostatic sleep-wake regulation in humans. Filled areas illustrate variations in sleep efficiency over a nap protocol (10 episodes of 150 min of wake, followed by 75 min of scheduled sleep episodes) modified from Münch et al. (2005). Circadian sleep-wake promotion, as expressed by the amount of wakefulness throughout nap episodes seems attenuated in older (dark gray) as compared to young individuals (light gray area). Line plots indicate the superimposed time course of subjective sleepiness over a 40-h sleep deprivation protocol in young (light gray line) and older (dark gray line) adults [modified from Adam et al. (2006)]. These values indicate less pronounced effects of increasing homeostatic sleep pressure on subjective sleepiness in older, as compared to young individuals.

From a circadian perspective, older adults present a reduced amplitude of circadian rhythmicity in endogenous core body temperature (Dijk and Duffy, 1999) and melatonin (Münch et al., 2005), suggesting that age-related changes in sleep can also be related to a weaker circadian regulation. Whether age merely affects the wake- or sleep-consolidating function of the circadian signal has been a topic of debate. Dijk and colleagues found evidence that sleep latencies were rather similar between age groups throughout the circadian cycle, even though the shortest sleep latency values located around the temperature nadir were slightly longer in older participants (Dijk and Duffy, 1999; Dijk et al., 1999). Concomitantly, Duffy et al. (1998) reported that sleepiness and alertness levels in the older were less affected than in young adults, when the scheduled wake period occurred in the early morning hours coinciding with the maximal circadian drive for sleep. Finally, a nap study revealed that the circadian wake-promoting signal in the evening hours was weaker in older participants, with higher subjective sleepiness ratings and more sleep occurring during the wake maintenance zone in the late afternoon (Strogatz et al., 1987) in older than in young adults (Münch et al., 2005). Thus, the age-related lower homeostatic sleep need may account for the observed less consolidated and shorter sleep during night-time, while reduced circadian wake promotion during the biological day might favor daytime naps in older adults.

Cross-sectional studies indicate a preference for earlier habitual bedtime and getting-up time in older adults as compared to younger individuals (Carrier et al., 1997; Duffy et al., 1998; Duffy and Czeisler 2002). This morningness preference has been associated with an advance in the circadian phase at the physiological level, which could theoretically be associated to differences in the intrinsic period of the circadian oscillator (Brown et al., 2011). In this perspective, Pagani et al. (2010) showed proportionality between the physiological period length of the human circadian clock in vivo and the period in human fibroblasts in young and older participants. Interestingly, measurement of human fibroblasts in the presence of human serum from older donors highlighted shortened period length and advanced phase of cellular circadian rhythms as compared with serum from young donors, indicating that a circulating factor might alter human chronotype (Pagani et al., 2011). However, in vivo under conditions of experimentally induced misalignment between the sleep-wake cycle and endogenous circadian rhythmicity, the investigation of the circadian period in melatonin secretion or core body temperature revealed very similar period lengths across age groups (Czeisler et al., 1999; Duffy and Czeisler, 2002). However, the phase angle of entrainment as indexed by the timing of the biological clock (i.e., circadian phase) in relation to the timing of sleep (i.e., usual bedtime) was different in young and older participants: while young morning types woke up later within their circadian cycle (i.e., longer phase angles; e.g., Duffy et al., 1999; see Emens et al., 2009 for naturalistic conditions), older morning types woke up earlier within the circadian cycle (i.e., shorter phase angles; Duffy et al., 1999).

These age-related alterations in circadian and homeostatic sleep regulation significantly impact on an individual’s daytime cognitive performance level. Thus, from a clinical point of view, taking time of day and the individual’s circadian preference into account when assessing cognitive functions across age groups is rather important and has been emphasized in a series of reports (Hasher et al., 2005). Indeed, studies carried out under normal day–night conditions have generally revealed that, whereas the cognitive performance of young evening type adults often improves over the day, old morning type adults markedly deteriorate (May et al., 1993, 2005; Yoon, 1997; May and Hasher, 1998; Hasher et al., 1999, 2002, 2005; May, 1999; Yoon et al., 2003; Schmidt et al., 2007; Yang et al., 2007). This effect has been referred to as the synchrony effect, or the beneficial impact of temporal matching between task timing and preferred time of day for diurnal activities (May et al., 1993). The synchrony effect applies to different cognitive domains, including short-term memory tasks such as word span measures (Yoon, 1997), performance on different long-term memory tasks (May and Hasher, 1998; Intons-Peterson et al., 1999; Winocur and Hasher, 2002), and executive functions, especially cognitive inhibition abilities (Intons-Peterson et al., 1998; May and Hasher, 1998; May, 1999; West et al., 2002). We have recently observed that adapting testing time according to the specific individual’s sleep-wake schedule can attenuate synchrony effects in PVT and Stroop tasks (Schmidt et al., 2012), suggesting that part of the reported synchrony effects in aging may be accounted for by a series of confounders (e.g., differences in socio-professional timing constraints, the amount of accumulated sleep need or circadian phase position, all modulating arousal level at testing) rather than being inherent to the chronotypical profile of an individual. In the same vein, time of season may also affect cognitive functions, especially in clinical populations, such as bipolar I disorder (Rajajarvi et al., 2010). In the healthy population, there are indications that seasonal variation in mood can impact on cognitive performance (Merikanto et al., 2011).

Additionally, it is worth noting that the extent of age-related changes in circadian and sleep physiology substantially differs between individuals. For instance, the above mentioned polymorphism in the human clock gene PER3 may contribute to inter-individual differences in sleep and circadian physiology in older people (Viola et al., 2012). Homozygosity for the longer allele (PER35/5) was associated with a phase-advance in the circadian melatonin profile and an earlier occurrence of the melatonin peak within the sleep episode. Furthermore, older PER35/5 participants accumulated more nocturnal wakefulness, had increased EEG frontal delta activity (0.75–1.5 Hz), and decreased EEG frontal sigma activity (11–13 Hz) during (Non-REM) sleep compared with PER34/4 participants.

Finally, when looking at the impact of circadian and homeostatic modulations on neurobehavioral performance in aging, weaker variations in circadian output measures were observed for both subjective and objective vigilance measures in older study participants. Declines in PVT performance seem less susceptible to circadian and homeostatic misalignment in older people (Blatter et al., 2006). Intriguingly, a reversal of age-related differences in PVT performance speed has been reported when testing was scheduled to the early morning hours during SD, i.e., when elevated homeostatic sleep pressure and minimal circadian wake promotion coincide, such that the older reacted faster than the young participants (Adam et al., 2006; Duffy et al., 2009). More recently, a forced desynchrony study similarly revealed that response speed on a sustained attention task and the ability to perform mental arithmetic are less deteriorated by the cumulative effects of repeated exposure to adverse circadian phase in older as compared to young individuals (Silva et al., 2010). In the same line, during a sleep fragmentation protocol, older participants were less sensitive to the imposed sleep disturbance in terms of performance decrements than young participants (Bonnet, 1989). Together, these data suggest an age-related attenuation of the wake-dependent homeostatic influence on cognitive performance (Silva et al., 2010). Alternatively, weakening of the circadian signal promoting wakefulness in the late biological day, also called the wake maintenance zone, may be responsible for the observed effects (Cajochen et al., 2006). However, in the above mentioned study (Silva et al., 2010), it was found that older participants had fewer lapses of attention in the circadian phase bins corresponding to the late biological day/early biological evening. It is also worth noting that optimal reaction time performance (i.e., the fastest 10% reaction times in the PVT) revealed that neither the wake-dependent homeostatic nor the circadian influence showed a significant interaction with age, suggesting that for both age groups, optimal reaction time performance is affected similarly by wake-dependent and circadian influences.

Cerebral Underpinnings of the Circadian and Homeostatic Regulation of Waking Performance: Age-Related Influences?

Overall, more disruption in sleep and circadian rhythm outputs have been linked to increased disease susceptibility (Hastings et al., 2003), which both occur more often with advanced age. However, with increasing age, circadian, and sleep-wake related neural areas or the connections within the functional neuroanatomical networks may compensate for initial dysfunction (van Someren et al., 2002).

Hypothalamic dysfunction may potentially trigger some age-related physiological and behavioral changes in sleep-wake patterns, but the effects of senescence on specific hypothalamic nuclei that mediate these alterations have still to be elucidated (Kessler et al., 2011). Even though controversial, post-mortem studies indicate neuronal degeneration of the SCN in senescence, which suggests that the circadian pacemaker in the human brain becomes progressively disrupted during aging (Hofman and Swaab, 2006). Although no age-related changes were found in the number or size of SCN cells in the rhesus (Roberts et al., 2012), changes in spontaneous SCN neuron firing activity have been reported in aged rodents, together with alterations in the expression of certain genes and peptides similarly to findings in non-human primates (see Bertini et al., 2010 for a review). Also, Nakamura et al. (2011) reported reduced amplitude of day–night differences in neural activity with increasing age in mice, together with an alteration in neural activity in the subparaventricular zone, one of the main neural outputs of the SCN. As parallel studies indicate that the molecular clockwork in the SCN as measured by PER2 exhibits only minor deficits at the same age of those presenting reduced day–night amplitude and neural activity in the subparaventricular zone, it is suggested that the circadian output measured at the level of neural activity rhythms in the SCN is degraded by aging, before disruption becomes evident in key components of the molecular clockwork (Roberts et al., 2012).

Circadian arousal regulation acts via indirect projections from the SCN to the arousal-promoting LC in the brainstem (Aston-Jones et al., 2001). Interestingly, the LC has the potential to impact on higher order cognitive performance and shows early vulnerability to the aging process, such that neuron density within the LC decreases with age due to a progressive loss of cell number and size of noradrenergic neurons both in animals and humans (Samuels and Szabadi, 2008). Furthermore, the loss of noradrenergic LC axons innervating the frontal cortex has been associated with modifications in the electrophysiological properties of the remaining LC terminals (Samuels and Szabadi, 2008). The SCN has also a weak direct projection to the wake-active orexin-producing neurons in the lateral hypothalamic area (Saper et al., 2001). Since the densest projection of orexin fibers terminates in the LC, it has also been suggested that the LC controls the activity of orexin neurons directly by inhibiting orexin firing and indirectly via the DMH. Interestingly, orexin secretion follows a circadian variation in rats (Zhang et al., 2004), monkeys (Zeitzer et al., 2003), and humans (Salomon et al., 2003), which might be the result of direct or indirect inputs from the SCN to the orexin circuits. Indeed lesions of the SCN suppress the daily orexin rhythm (Deboer et al., 2004; Adamantidis and de Lecea, 2008). On the other side, orexin levels increased in response to sleep deprivation in both control and SCN-lesioned animals, demonstrating that sleep homeostatic control of orexin occurs independently from the SCN (Deboer et al., 2004). It is known that orexin in the lateral hypothalamus influences many integrative homeostatic processes related to wakefulness and plays a crucial role in sleep architecture and state stabilization throughout the sleep-wake cycle (Saper et al., 2001). Kessler et al. (2011) observed that aged rats exhibited a loss of greater than 40% of orexin-immunoreactive neurons, suggesting that compromised orexin function could be an important mediator of age-related homeostatic disturbances of hypothalamic origin.

The cholinergic basal forebrain represents an additional important key area in the regulation of wakefulness-related cortical arousal that is selectively sensitive to both prolonged waking and aging (Cayetanot et al., 2005). Interestingly, the sleep-wake-dependent decline and rise in adenosine levels, a potential sleep-promoting substance, is more pronounced in the basal forebrain than in other cerebral regions (Strecker et al., 2000). Accordingly, it has been suggested that local release of adenosine in the basal forebrain provides a signal for the homeostatic regulation of Non-REM sleep (Landolt, 2008). Furthermore, age-related attenuation in basal forebrain function reduces its capacity to respond to increased neuronal activity during prolonged wakefulness (Cayetanot et al., 2005). In a similar perspective, cognitive stimulation in humans increases lactate expression in the prefrontal cortex of young, but not in an aged population (Urrila et al., 2004).

All of these cerebral systems involved in waking performance have the potential to modulate cognition through their widespread connections to the cerebral cortex and thus do not act in isolation. For instance, orexin neurons activate regions such as the prefrontal cortex and the basal forebrain cholinergic system, both of which are strongly implicated in normal cognitive function and in age-related cognitive decline.

Older age has also been associated with lower blood flow and resting metabolism, particularly in the prefrontal cortex (Meltzer and Francis, 2001) as well as reduced regional brain responses to challenging tasks (see Figure 4; Dennis et al., 2008; see Buckner, 2004; Hedden and Gabrieli, 2004 for a review). Conversely, when performing at optimal levels, older adults present higher functional responses in frontal cortices. Besides quantitative changes, there may be also qualitatively different brain response patterns, such as “over-activation” in an homologous region in the opposite hemisphere from the region typically responsive in young adults, a phenomenon referred to as to “hemispheric asymmetry reduction” in older adults (Eyler et al., 2011). Overall, supplemental recruitment of cerebral structures might reflect compensatory activity or strategic differences with advanced age. A general hypothesis is that increased brain recruitment represents a general response to increasing task difficulty and conveyed global factors that affect aging (Buckner, 2004).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Frontal activity changes in young and older populations. (A) Activations during a memory-encoding task in young adults, low-performing older adults and high-performing older adults. Low-performing older adults exhibit a similar pattern as do young adults, with lower overall levels of activation. High-performing older adults exhibit greater bilateral activation (with permission from Hedden and Gabrieli, 2004). (B) Total sleep deprivation-related patterns of cerebral activation during a verbal learning task. For each of these regions, the memorization of difficult words elicited greater activation after total sleep deprivation than after a night of sleep. Images show left hemisphere slices from 5 to 50 mm [adapted from Drummond et al. (2005b)].

Task-related BOLD activity decreases have been observed in young adults under sleep deprivation, while other studies showed compensatory mechanisms resulting in activity increases (e.g., Drummond et al., 2000; Drummond and Brown, 2001; Chee and Choo, 2004; Habeck et al., 2004; Choo et al., 2005; Mu et al., 2005a,b; Chee et al., 2006; Chuah et al., 2006; Chee and Chuah, 2007; see Chee and Chuah, 2008 for a review). Thus, similarly to what has been hypothesized in the aging literature, it is suggested that increasing task difficulty elicits “compensatory” prefrontal activation in some, but not all studies, as a function of the investigated cognitive domain. Thus sleep deprivation and aging may elicit similar deficits in executive functions due to similar alterations in the prefrontal cortex (e.g., Harrison et al., 2007). Executive functions encompass a series of high-level processes, the main function of which is to facilitate adaptation to new or complex situations when highly practiced cognitive abilities or behaviors no longer suffice (Collette et al., 2006). While the frontal lobes play a major role in executive function (Shallice, 1982), additional posterior cerebral regions, including the parietal lobes (Collette et al., 2006) also play a key role in executive functioning. Horne and colleagues first hypothesized that the waking function of the prefrontal cortex and the frontal predominance of EEG delta activity in sleep may be linked (Horne, 1993; see also Blatter and Cajochen, 2007). Since then, several studies have argued that cognitive functioning related to the prefrontal cortex is particularly vulnerable to sleep loss (Blatter and Cajochen, 2007). However, in a recent report, Tucker et al. (2011) observed heterogeneity in cognitive aspects impaired by sleep deprivation and aging when looking more specifically at the affected cognitive compounds during the performance of executive function tasks, raising questions about the similarity of the involved cognitive processes underlying sleep loss related and age-related modulations in cognitive performance.

Conclusion and Perspectives

Healthy older individuals may experience twice as much time awake during a night sleep episode than young adults, suggesting that impaired sleep consolidation is associated with aging per se, rather than being a by-product of co-ailments linked to aging. Whether age-related sleep disruption derives from either the circadian and/or the homeostatic facet of sleep regulation is still somewhat uncertain. However, we propose that the strength of the circadian/homeostatic interaction on modulating sleep and cognitive processes are weakened in older healthy people. The cortical underpinnings that account for these age-related modulations are virtually unexplored. A combined investigation of these issues at the cerebral level would allow the elaboration of theoretical concepts in order to explain the underlying mechanisms of age-related changes in circadian and homeostatic modulations in cognitive performance. Hence, this leads to the next question: are age-related sleep disruptions a consequence of alterations in the circadian or homeostatic sleep regulation, or is it ultimately caused by an attenuated interaction between the circadian timing system and sleep-wake homeostatic process at the cerebral level? Here we surmise that some of the subcortical structures involved in the generation of circadian wake promotion may at least in part be compromised in healthy aging. Alternatively, upstream from these structures, more integrative cortical areas underlying higher order cognitive behaviors may also be selectively altered with aging. Quantitative evidence for such a hypothesis is building up from the vulnerability of frontal cortical brain regions to both the effects of elevated sleep pressure and aging. Whether there is a direct relationship between age-related decline in cognitive performance and sleep disruption still remains a matter of investigation. Prospective or intervention studies (e.g., sleep extension) would help to elucidate their relationships.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Christina Schmidt and Christian Cajochen are funded by a research grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation (#310030_130689) and a post-doctoral fellowship from the AXA Research Fund.

References

Adam, M., Retey, J. V., Khatami, R., and Landolt, H. P. (2006). Age-related changes in the time course of vigilant attention during 40 hours without sleep in men. Sleep 29, 55–57.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Adamantidis, A., and de Lecea, L. (2008). Physiological arousal: a role for hypothalamic systems. Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 65, 1475–1488.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aston-Jones, G. (2005). Brain structures and receptors involved in alertness. Sleep Med. 6(Suppl. 1), S3–S7.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aston-Jones, G., Chen, S., Zhu, Y., and Oshinsky, M. L. (2001). A neural circuit for circadian regulation of arousal. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 732–738.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bartres-Faz, D., and Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M. (2011). Structural and functional imaging correlates of cognitive and brain reserve hypotheses in healthy and pathological aging. Brain Topogr. 24, 340–357.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bertini, G., Colavito, V., Tognoli, C., Etet, P. F., and Bentivoglio, M. (2010). The aging brain, neuroinflammatory signaling and sleep-wake regulation. Ital. J. Anat. Embryol. 115, 31–38.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Blatter, K., and Cajochen, C. (2007). Circadian rhythms in cognitive performance: methodological constraints, protocols, theoretical underpinnings. Physiol. Behav. 90, 196–208.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blatter, K., Graw, P., Munch, M., Knoblauch, V., Wirz-Justice, A., and Cajochen, C. (2006). Gender and age differences in psychomotor vigilance performance under differential sleep pressure conditions. Behav. Brain Res. 168, 312–317.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bliwise, D. L. (2005). “Normal aging,” in Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, eds T. Roth, M. H. Kryger, W. C. Dement (Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders), 24–38.

Bonnet, M. H. (1989). The effect of sleep fragmentation on sleep and performance in younger and older subjects. Neurobiol. Aging 10, 21–25.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Borbely, A. A. (1982). A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum. Neurobiol. 1, 195–204.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bratzke, D., Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., and Ulrich, R. (2012). Effects of sleep loss and circadian rhythm on executive inhibitory control in the Stroop and Simon tasks. Chronobiol. Int. 29, 55–61.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brown, S. A., Schmitt, K., and Eckert, A. (2011). Aging and circadian disruption: causes and effects. Aging (Albany N.Y.) 3, 813–817.

Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron 44, 195–208.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cain, S. W., Silva, E. J., Chang, A. M., Ronda, J. M., and Duffy, J. F. (2011). One night of sleep deprivation affects reaction time, but not interference or facilitation in a Stroop task. Brain Cogn. 76, 37–42.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cajochen, C., Blatter, K., and Wallach, D. (2004). Circadian and sleep-wake dependent impact on neurobehavioral function. Psychol. Belg. 44, 59–80.

Cajochen, C., Knoblauch, V., Krauchi, K., Renz, C., and Wirz-Justice, A. (2001). Dynamics of frontal EEG activity, sleepiness and body temperature under high and low sleep pressure. Neuroreport 12, 2277–2281.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cajochen, C., Khalsa, S. B., Wyatt, J. K., Czeisler, C. A., and Dijk, D. J. (1999). EEG and ocular correlates of circadian melatonin phase and human performance decrements during sleep loss. Am. J. Physiol. 277, R640–R649.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cajochen, C., Munch, M., Knoblauch, V., Blatter, K., and Wirz-Justice, A. (2006). Age-related changes in the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep. Chronobiol. Int. 23, 461–474.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carrier, J., and Monk, T. H. (2000). Circadian rhythms of performance: new trends. Chronobiol. Int. 17, 719–732.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carrier, J., Monk, T. H., Buysse, D. J., and Kupfer, D. J. (1997). Sleep and morningness-eveningness in the ‘middle’ years of life (20-59 y). J. Sleep Res. 6, 230–237.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cayetanot, F., Bentivoglio, M., and Aujard, F. (2005). Arginine-vasopressin and vasointestinal polypeptide rhythms in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the mouse lemur reveal aging-related alterations of circadian pacemaker neurons in a non-human primate. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 902–910.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chee, M. W., and Choo, W. C. (2004). Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. J. Neurosci. 24, 4560–4567.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chee, M. W., and Chuah, L. Y. (2008). Functional neuroimaging insights into how sleep and sleep deprivation affect memory and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 21, 417–423.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chee, M. W., Chuah, L. Y., Venkatraman, V., Chan, W. Y., Philip, P., and Dinges, D. F. (2006). Functional imaging of working memory following normal sleep and after 24 and 35 h of sleep deprivation: correlations of fronto-parietal activation with performance. Neuroimage 31, 419–428.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chee, M. W., and Chuah, Y. M. (2007). Functional neuroimaging and behavioral correlates of capacity decline in visual short-term memory after sleep deprivation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 9487–9492.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Choo, W. C., Lee, W. W., Venkatraman, V., Sheu, F. S., and Chee, M. W. (2005). Dissociation of cortical regions modulated by both working memory load and sleep deprivation and by sleep deprivation alone. Neuroimage 25, 579–587.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chuah, Y. M., Venkatraman, V., Dinges, D. F., and Chee, M. W. (2006). The neural basis of interindividual variability in inhibitory efficiency after sleep deprivation. J. Neurosci. 26, 7156–7162.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Collette, F., Hogge, M., Salmon, E., and van der Linden, M. (2006). Exploration of the neural substrates of executive functioning by functional neuroimaging. Neuroscience 139, 209–221.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Czeisler, C. A., Duffy, J. F., Shanahan, T. L., Brown, E. N., Mitchell, J. F., Rimmer, D. W., Ronda, J. M., Silva, E. J., Allan, J. S., Emens, J. S., Dijk, D. J., and Kronauer, R. E. (1999). Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker. Science 284, 2177–2181.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Daan, S., Beersma, D. G. M., and Borbély, A. A. (1984). Timing of human sleep: recovery process gated by a circadian pacemaker. Am. J. Physiol. 246, R161–R178.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Deboer, T., Overeem, S., Visser, N. A., Duindam, H., Frolich, M., Lammers, G. J., and Meijer, J. H. (2004). Convergence of circadian and sleep regulatory mechanisms on hypocretin-1. Neuroscience 129, 727–732.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dennis, N. A., Hayes, S. M., Prince, S. E., Madden, D. J., Huettel, S. A., and Cabeza, R. (2008). Effects of aging on the neural correlates of successful item and source memory encoding. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. 34, 791–808.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dijk, D. J., and Archer, S. N. (2009). Circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep and cognitive performance and its modulation by PERIOD3. Sleep Med. Clin. 4, 111–125.

CrossRef Full Text

Dijk, D. J., Beersma, D. G., and van den Hoofdakker, R. H. (1989). All night spectral analysis of EEG sleep in young adult and middle-aged male subjects. Neurobiol. Aging 10, 677–682.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dijk, D. J., and Czeisler, C. A. (1994). Paradoxical timing of the circadian rhythm of sleep propensity serves to consolidate sleep and wakefulness in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 166, 63–68.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dijk, D. J., and Czeisler, C. A. (1995). Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves and sleep spindle activity in humans. J. Neurosci. 15, 3526–3538.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Dijk, D. J., and Duffy, J. F. (1999). Circadian regulation of human sleep and age-related changes in its timing, consolidation and EEG characteristics. Ann. Med. 31, 130–140.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dijk, D. J., Duffy, J. F., and Czeisler, C. A. (1992). Circadian and sleep/wake dependent aspects of subjective alertness and cognitive performance. J. Sleep Res. 1, 112–117.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dijk, D. J., Duffy, J. F., and Czeisler, C. A. (2001). Age-related increase in awakenings: impaired consolidation of nonREM sleep at all circadian phases. Sleep 24, 565–577.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Dijk, D. J., Duffy, J. F., Riel, E., Shanahan, T. L., and Czeisler, C. A. (1999). Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 516(Pt 2), 611–627.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dijk, D. J., and Franken, P. (2005). “Interaction of sleep homeostatis and circadian rhythmicity: dependent or independent systems?,” in Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, eds M. H. Kryger, T. Roth, and W. C. Dement (Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders), 418–435.

Dijk, D. J., Groeger, J. A., Stanley, N., and Deacon, S. (2010). Age-related reduction in daytime sleep propensity and nocturnal slow wave sleep. Sleep 33, 211–223.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Dijk, D. J., and von Schantz, M. (2005). Timing and consolidation of human sleep, wakefulness, and performance by a symphony of oscillators. J. Biol. Rhythms 20, 279–290.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Drummond, S. P., Bischoff-Grethe, A., Dinges, D. F., Ayalon, L., Mednick, S. C., and Meloy, M. J. (2005a). The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. Sleep 28, 1059–1068.

Drummond, S. P., Meloy, M. J., Yanagi, M. A., Orff, H. J., and Brown, G. G. (2005b). Compensatory recruitment after sleep deprivation and the relationship with performance. Psychiatry Res. 140, 211–223.

CrossRef Full Text

Drummond, S. P., and Brown, G. G. (2001). The effects of total sleep deprivation on cerebral responses to cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology 25, S68–S73.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Drummond, S. P., Brown, G. G., Gillin, J. C., Stricker, J. L., Wong, E. C., and Buxton, R. B. (2000). Altered brain response to verbal learning following sleep deprivation. Nature 403, 655–657.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Duffy, J. F., and Czeisler, C. A. (2002). Age-related change in the relationship between circadian period, circadian phase, and diurnal preference in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 318, 117–120.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Duffy, J. F., Dijk, D. J., Hall, E. F., and Czeisler, C. A. (1999). Relationship of endogenous circadian melatonin and temperature rhythms to self-reported preference for morning or evening activity in young and older people. J. Investig. Med. 47, 141–150.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Duffy, J. F., Dijk, D. J., Klerman, E. B., and Czeisler, C. A. (1998). Later endogenous circadian temperature nadir relative to an earlier wake time in older people. Am. J. Physiol. 275, R1478–R1487.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Duffy, J. F., Willson, H. J., Wang, W., and Czeisler, C. A. (2009). Healthy older adults better tolerate sleep deprivation than young adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 57, 1245–1251.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Emens, J. S., Yuhas, K., Rough, J., Kochar, N., Peters, D., and Lewy, A. J. (2009). Phase angle of entrainment in morning- and evening-types under naturalistic conditions. Chronobiol. Int. 26, 474–493.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eyler, L. T., Sherzai, A., Kaup, A. R., and Jeste, D. V. (2011). A review of functional brain imaging correlates of successful cognitive aging. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 115–122.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gompf, H. S., and Aston-Jones, G. (2008). Role of orexin input in the diurnal rhythm of locus coeruleus impulse activity. Brain Res. 1224, 43–52.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Habeck, C., Rakitin, B. C., Moeller, J., Scarmeas, N., Zarahn, E., Brown, T., and Stern, Y. (2004). An event-related fMRI study of the neurobehavioral impact of sleep deprivation on performance of a delayed-match-to-sample task. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 18, 306–321.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harrison, Y., Jones, K., and Waterhouse, J. (2007). The influence of time awake and circadian rhythm upon performance on a frontal lobe task. Neuropsychologia 45, 1966–1972.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hasher, L., Chung, C., May, C. P., and Foong, N. (2002). Age, time of testing, and proactive interference. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 56, 200–207.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., and May, C. (2005). “It’s about time: circadian rhythms, memory and aging,” in Human Learning and Memory: Advances in Theory and Application, ed. C. I. N. Ohta (Kansas: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 199–217.

Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., and Rahhal, T. A. (1999). Timing, instructions, and inhibitory control: some missing factors in the age and memory debate. Gerontology 45, 355–357.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hastings, M. H., Reddy, A. B., and Maywood, E. S. (2003). A clockwork web: circadian timing in brain and periphery, in health and disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 649–661.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hedden, T., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 87–96.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hofman, M. A., and Swaab, D. F. (2006). Living by the clock: the circadian pacemaker in older people. Ageing Res. Rev. 5, 33–51.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Horne, J. A. (1993). Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour. Implications for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry 162, 413–419.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Horowitz, T. S., Cade, B. E., Wolfe, J. M., and Czeisler, C. A. (2003). Searching night and day: a dissociation of effects of circadian phase and time awake on visual selective attention and vigilance. Psychol. Sci. 14, 549–557.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Intons-Peterson, M. J., Rocchi, P., West, T., Mclellan, K., and Hackney, A. (1998). Aging, optimal testing times, and negative priming. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 24, 362–376.

CrossRef Full Text

Intons-Peterson, M. J., Rocchi, P., West, T., Mclellan, K., and Hackney, A. (1999). Age, testing at preferred or nonpreferred times (testing optimality), and false memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. 25, 23–40.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Johnson, M. P., Duffy, J. F., Dijk, D. J., Ronda, J. M., Dyal, C. M., and Czeisler, C. A. (1992). Short-term memory, alertness and performance: a reappraisal of their relationship to body temperature. J. Sleep Res. 1, 24–29.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kawato, M., Fujita, K., Suzuki, R., and Winfree, A. T. (1982). A three-oscillator model of the human circadian system controlling the core temperature rhythm and the sleep-wake cycle. J. Theor. Biol. 98, 369–392.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kerkhof, G. A. (1991). Differences between morning-types and evening-types in the dynamics of EEG slow wave activity during night sleep. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 78, 197–202.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kessler, B. A., Stanley, E. M., Frederick-Duus, D., and Fadel, J. (2011). Age-related loss of orexin/hypocretin neurons. Neuroscience 178, 82–88.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klerman, E. B., and Dijk, D. J. (2008). Age-related reduction in the maximal capacity for sleep – implications for insomnia. Curr. Biol. 18, 1118–1123.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kronauer, R. E., Czeisler, C. A., Pilato, S. F., Moore-Ede, M. C., and Weitzman, E. D. (1982). Mathematical model of the human circadian system with two interacting oscillators. Am. J. Physiol. 242, R3–R17.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Landolt, H. P. (2008). Sleep homeostasis: a role for adenosine in humans? Biochem. Pharmacol. 75, 2070–2079.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lupien, S. J., and Wan, N. (2004). Successful ageing: from cell to self. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 359, 1413–1426.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Martin, D. W. (1981). Why aging? West. J. Med. 135, 520.

May, C. P. (1999). Synchrony effects in cognition: the costs and a benefit. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 6, 142–147.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

May, C. P., and Hasher, L. (1998). Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 363–379.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

May, C. P., Hasher, L., and Foong, N. (2005). Implicit memory, age, and time of day: paradoxical priming effects. Psychol. Sci. 16, 96–100.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

May, C. P., Hasher, L., and Stoltzfus, E. R. (1993). Optimal time of day and the magnitude of age differences in memory. Psychol. Sci. 4, 326–330.

CrossRef Full Text

Meltzer, C. C., and Francis, P. T. (2001). Brain aging research at the close of the 20th century: from bench to bedside. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 3, 167–180.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Merikanto, I., Lahti, T., Castaneda, A. E., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Aalto-Setala, T., Suvisaari, J., and Partonen, T. (2011). Influence of seasonal variation in mood and behavior on cognitive test performance among young adults. Nord. J. Psychiatry. doi: 10.3109/08039488.2011.633618.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mongrain, V., Carrier, J., and Dumont, M. (2006a). Circadian and homeostatic sleep regulation in morningness-eveningness. J. Sleep Res. 15, 162–166.

CrossRef Full Text

Mongrain, V., Carrier, J., and Dumont, M. (2006b). Difference in sleep regulation between morning and evening circadian types as indexed by antero-posterior analyses of the sleep EEG. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 497–504.

CrossRef Full Text

Mu, Q., Mishory, A., Johnson, K. A., Nahas, Z., Kozel, F. A., Yamanaka, K., Bohning, D. E., and George, M. S. (2005a). Decreased brain activation during a working memory task at rested baseline is associated with vulnerability to sleep deprivation. Sleep 28, 433–446.

Mu, Q., Nahas, Z., Johnson, K. A., Yamanaka, K., Mishory, A., Koola, J., Hill, S., Horner, M. D., Bohning, D. E., and George, M. S. (2005b). Decreased cortical response to verbal working memory following sleep deprivation. Sleep 28, 55–67.

Münch, M., Knoblauch, V., Blatter, K., Schroder, C., Schnitzler, C., Krauchi, K., Wirz-Justice, A., and Cajochen, C. (2004). The frontal predominance in human EEG delta activity after sleep loss decreases with age. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 1402–1410.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Münch, M., Knoblauch, V., Blatter, K., Schroder, C., Schnitzler, C., Krauchi, K., Wirz-Justice, A., and Cajochen, C. (2005). Age-related attenuation of the evening circadian arousal signal in humans. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 1307–1319.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nakamura, T. J., Nakamura, W., Yamazaki, S., Kudo, T., Cutler, T., Colwell, C. S., and Block, G. D. (2011). Age-related decline in circadian output. J. Neurosci. 31, 10201–10205.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pagani, L., Schmitt, K., Meier, F., Izakovic, J., Roemer, K., Viola, A., Cajochen, C., Wirz-Justice, A., Brown, S. A., and Eckert, A. (2011). Serum factors in older individuals change cellular clock properties. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 7218–7223.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pagani, L., Semenova, E. A., Moriggi, E., Revell, V. L., Hack, L. M., Lockley, S. W., Arendt, J., Skene, D. J., Meier, F., Izakovic, J., Wirz-Justice, A., Cajochen, C., Sergeeva, O. J., Cheresiz, S. V., Danilenko, K. V., Eckert, A., and Brown, S. A. (2010). The physiological period length of the human circadian clock in vivo is directly proportional to period in human fibroblasts. PLoS ONE 5, e13376. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013376

CrossRef Full Text

Phillips, A. J., Czeisler, C. A., and Klerman, E. B. (2011). Revisiting spontaneous internal desynchrony using a quantitative model of sleep physiology. J. Biol. Rhythms 26, 441–453.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rajajarvi, E., Antila, M., Kieseppa, T., Lonnqvist, J., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., and Partonen, T. (2010). The effect of seasons and seasonal variation on neuropsychological test performance in patients with bipolar I disorder and their first-degree relatives. J. Affect. Disord. 127, 58–65.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roberts, D., Killiany, R., and Rosene, D. (2012). Neuron numbers in the hypothalamus of the normal aging rhesus monkey: stability across the adult life-span and between the sexes. J. Comp. Neurol. 520, 1181–1197.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rogers, N. L., Dorrian, J., and Dinges, D. F. (2003). Sleep, waking and neurobehavioural performance. Front. Biosci. 8, S1056–S1067.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rowe, J. W., and Kahn, R. L. (1987). Human aging: usual and successful. Science 237, 143–149.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Salomon, R. M., Ripley, B., Kennedy, J. S., Johnson, B., Schmidt, D., Zeitzer, J. M., Nishino, S., and Mignot, E. (2003). Diurnal variation of cerebrospinal fluid hypocretin-1 (Orexin-A) levels in control and depressed subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 96–104.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Samuels, E. R., and Szabadi, E. (2008). Functional neuroanatomy of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus: its roles in the regulation of arousal and autonomic function part I: principles of functional organisation. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 6, 235–253.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saper, C. B., Chou, T. C., and Scammell, T. E. (2001). The sleep switch: hypothalamic control of sleep and wakefulness. Trends Neurosci. 24, 726–731.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., and Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 24, 755–789.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Leclercq, Y., Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., Berthomier, P., Berthomier, C., Phillips, C., Tinguely, G., Darsaud, A., Gais, S., Schabus, M., Desseilles, M., Dang-Vu, T. T., Salmon, E., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., Maquet, P., Cajochen, C., and Peigneux, P. (2009). Homeostatic sleep pressure and responses to sustained attention in the suprachiasmatic area. Science 324, 516–519.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmidt, C., Peigneux, P., Leclercq, Y., Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., Phillips, C., Berthomier, P., Berthomier, C., Tinguely, G., Gais, S., Schabus, M., Desseilles, M., Dang-Vu, T., Salmon, E., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E., Luxen, A., Cajochen, C., Maquet, P., and Collette, F. (2012). Circadian preference modulates the neural substrate of conflict processing across the day. PLoS ONE 7, e29658. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0029658

CrossRef Full Text

Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 298, 199–209.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Silva, E. J., Wang, W., Ronda, J. M., Wyatt, J. K., and Duffy, J. F. (2010). Circadian and wake-dependent influences on subjective sleepiness, cognitive throughput, and reaction time performance in older and young adults. Sleep 33, 481–490.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Strecker, R. E., Morairty, S., Thakkar, M. M., Porkka-Heiskanen, T., Basheer, R., Dauphin, L. J., Rainnie, D. G., Portas, C. M., Greene, R. W., and Mccarley, R. W. (2000). Adenosinergic modulation of basal forebrain and preoptic/anterior hypothalamic neuronal activity in the control of behavioral state. Behav. Brain Res. 115, 183–204.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Strogatz, S. H., Kronauer, R. E., and Czeisler, C. A. (1987). Circadian pacemaker interferes with sleep onset at specific times each day: role in insomnia. Am. J. Physiol. 253, R172–R178.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2003). Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain Res. Bull. 62, 143–150.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tucker, A. M., Stern, Y., Basner, R. C., and Rakitin, B. C. (2011). The prefrontal model revisited: double dissociations between young sleep deprived and elderly subjects on cognitive components of performance. Sleep 34, 1039–1050.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Urrila, A. S., Hakkarainen, A., Heikkinen, S., Vuori, K., Stenberg, D., Hakkinen, A. M., Lundbom, N., and Porkka-Heiskanen, T. (2004). Stimulus-induced brain lactate: effects of aging and prolonged wakefulness. J. Sleep Res. 13, 111–119.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Usher, M., Cohen, J. D., Servan-Schreiber, D., Rajkowski, J., and Aston-Jones, G. (1999). The role of locus coeruleus in the regulation of cognitive performance. Science 283, 549–554.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Someren, E. J., Riemersma, R. F., and Swaab, D. F. (2002). Functional plasticity of the circadian timing system in old age: light exposure. Prog. Brain Res. 138, 205–231.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vandewalle, G., Archer, S. N., Wuillaume, C., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., Maquet, P., and Dijk, D. J. (2009). Functional magnetic resonance imaging-assessed brain responses during an executive task depend on interaction of sleep homeostasis, circadian phase, and PER3 genotype. J. Neurosci. 29, 7948–7956.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Viola, A. U., Archer, S. N., James, L. M., Groeger, J. A., Lo, J. C., Skene, D. J., von Schantz, M., and Dijk, D. J. (2007). PER3 polymorphism predicts sleep structure and waking performance. Curr. Biol. 17, 613–618.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Viola, A. U., Chellappa, S. L., Archer, S. N., Pugin, F., Gotz, T., Dijk, D. J., and Cajochen, C. (2012). Interindividual differences in circadian rhythmicity and sleep homeostasis in older people: effect of a PER3 polymorphism. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 1010.e17–1010.e27.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

West, R., Murphy, K. J., Armilio, M. L., Craik, F. I., and Stuss, D. T. (2002). Effects of time of day on age differences in working memory. J. Gerontol. B. Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 57, P3–P10.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wever, R. (1975). The circadian multi-oscillatory system of man. Int. J. Chronobiol. 3, 19–55.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Winocur, G., and Hasher, L. (2002). “Circadian rhythms and memory in aged humans and animals,” in Neuropsychology of Memory 3rd Edition, eds L. R. Squire and D. L. Schacter (New York: The Guilford Press), 273–285.

Wright, K. P., Lowry, C. A., and Lebourgeois, M. K. (2012). Circadian and wakefulness-sleep modulation of cognition in humans. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 5:50. doi:10.3389/fnmol.2012.00050

CrossRef Full Text

Wyatt, J. K., Ritz-De Cecco, A., Czeisler, C. A., and Dijk, D. J. (1999). Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 277, R1152–R1163.

Yang, L., Hasher, L., and Wilson, D. E. (2007). Synchrony effects in automatic and controlled retrieval. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14, 51–56.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yoon, C. (1997). Age differences in consumers’ processing strategies: an investigation of moderating influences. J. Consum. Res. 24, 329–342.

CrossRef Full Text

Yoon, I. Y., Kripke, D. F., Elliott, J. A., Youngstedt, S. D., Rex, K. M., and Hauger, R. L. (2003). Age-related changes of circadian rhythms and sleep-wake cycles. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 51, 1085–1091.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zeitzer, J. M., Buckmaster, C. L., Parker, K. J., Hauck, C. M., Lyons, D. M., and Mignot, E. (2003). Circadian and homeostatic regulation of hypocretin in a primate model: implications for the consolidation of wakefulness. J. Neurosci. 23, 3555–3560.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zhang, S., Zeitzer, J. M., Yoshida, Y., Wisor, J. P., Nishino, S., Edgar, D. M., and Mignot, E. (2004). Lesions of the suprachiasmatic nucleus eliminate the daily rhythm of hypocretin-1 release. Sleep 27, 619–627.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Keywords: aging, sleep-wake regulation, cognition, functional magnetic resonance imaging, circadian rhythms, sleep homeostasis

Citation: Schmidt C, Peigneux P and Cajochen C (2012) Age-related changes in sleep and circadian rhythms: impact on cognitive performance and underlying neuroanatomical networks. Front. Neur. 3:118. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00118

Received: 24 February 2012; Paper pending published: 05 April 2012;
Accepted: 08 July 2012; Published online: 26 July 2012.

Edited by:

Julie Carrier, Université de Montréal, Canada

Reviewed by:

Michael W. L. Chee, Duke NUS Graduate Medical School, Singapore
Timo Partonen, University of Helsinki, Finland
Stuart Fogel, University of Montreal, Canada

Copyright: © 2012 Schmidt, Peigneux and Cajochen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Philippe Peigneux, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Campus du Solbosch CP191, Avenue F.D. Roosevelt 50, B-1050 Bruxelles, Belgium. e-mail: philippe.peigneux@ulb.ac.be

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.