Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 01 September 2015
Sec. Sleep Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Brainstem regulation of sleep, wake, and behavior View all 8 articles

Evaluating the Evidence Surrounding Pontine Cholinergic Involvement in REM Sleep Generation

  • 1Department of Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 2Department of Physiology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada

Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep – characterized by vivid dreaming, motor paralysis, and heightened neural activity – is one of the fundamental states of the mammalian central nervous system. Initial theories of REM sleep generation posited that induction of the state required activation of the “pontine REM sleep generator” by cholinergic inputs. Here, we review and evaluate the evidence surrounding cholinergic involvement in REM sleep generation. We submit that: (i) the capacity of pontine cholinergic neurotransmission to generate REM sleep has been firmly established by gain-of-function experiments, (ii) the function of endogenous cholinergic input to REM sleep generating sites cannot be determined by gain-of-function experiments; rather, loss-of-function studies are required, (iii) loss-of-function studies show that endogenous cholinergic input to the PTF is not required for REM sleep generation, and (iv) cholinergic input to the pontine REM sleep generating sites serve an accessory role in REM sleep generation: reinforcing non-REM-to-REM sleep transitions making them quicker and less likely to fail.

Background

Characterizing REM Sleep as a Unique State of the Central Nervous System

Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep – characterized by vivid dreaming, electroencephalographic activation, and motor atonia – is a fundamental operating mode of the mammalian central nervous system. Before REM sleep was discovered, sleep was assumed to be a homogenous brain state (1). However, electrophysiological recording of activity in the sleeping brain revealed sleep’s dual nature (2, 3). Pioneering electroencephalography (EEG) studies showed that cortical activity in sleeping subjects is dominated by high amplitude, low frequency oscillations; however, beginning in the 1930s, several groups reported that, during sleep, such slow-wave activity is periodically interrupted by periods of low-amplitude fast-waves resembling waking EEG (36). The seminal characterization of this novel sleep stage and its physiological correlates were made by Aserinsky, Kleitman, and Dement in the 1950s. They observed that periodic EEG activations during sleep occurred together with rapid, jerky, and binocularly symmetrical eye movements, which differed from the slow, rolling, and pendular eye movements that had been previously described in sleeping subjects. The authors also observed other phenomena typical of the state, including surges in respiratory rate, heightened arousal threshold, and the occurrence of “fine movements” in the extremities (i.e., muscle twitching). Perhaps their most intriguing observation was the association between this collection of phenomena and dreaming. Subjects awoken from periods of REM reported an increased incidence of highly visual and detailed dreams.

The discovery of the REM stage of sleep presented a conceptual challenge to the field of sleep research. Whether this stage was a distinct sleep state, or whether all sleep stages stemmed from a common neural mechanism was still uncertain. The distinction between these two perspectives is non-trivial. REM sleep being a distinct state would mean that distinct neural circuitry is responsible for its generation, circuitry giving rise to unique physiology, pathophysiology, and functionality. Aserinsky, Kleitman, and Dement described the REM and non-REM sleep stages, not as distinct states but, as different “depths” of the same biological phenomenon. It was suggested that sleep cycling is a process by which the brain progresses through deeper and deeper stages of sleep followed by a “lightening” associated with the occurrence of EEG activation with REMs (3). The REM sleep stage was characterized as a “manifestation of a particular level of cortical activity which is encountered normally during sleep” (2); that level of cortical activity being one that is intermediate between wakefulness and slow-wave sleep. Aserinsky, Kleitman, and Dement characterized the activated EEG associated with REMs as being equivalent to the electroencephalographic signature of Stage 1 sleep, which corresponded to the “drowsy” stage that had been previously described by Gibbs and Gibbs (7). Speaking of the Stage 1 sleep that occurs at sleep onset, Dement and Kleitman commented that “this Stage 1 EEG seemed to be identical with those occurring later in the night concomitant with actual eye movements” (3).

The suggestion that REM sleep represents one of the lightest stages of sleep conflicted with data showing that arousal threshold is highest during periods of REM and EEG activation (3). Building on the descriptive work of Aserinsky, Kleitman, and Dement, a series of experiments and philosophical arguments (1958–1960) authored by Michel Jouvet is largely responsible for the current conception of REM sleep as a brain state distinct from both non-REM sleep and wakefulness (8, 9). Jouvet reported that the occurrence of rapid cortical activity together with the so-called “somato-vegetative phenomena” (i.e., the disappearance of all muscle activity as well as variation in cardiac and respiratory rhythms) persisted following various supra-pontine transections in cats, but were eliminated following sectioning between the pons and trapezoid bodies (8). Subsequent lesion experiments showed that destroying portions of the pontine tegmental field (PTF) (specifically the caudal pontine reticular formation and the posterior part of the oral pontine reticular formation) caused a selective loss of the REM sleep stage (8). Moreover, the REM sleep stage could be selectively triggered by timed electrical stimulation of this same region. These findings were difficult to reconcile with the perspective of Kleitman and colleagues that REM sleep is an intermediary stage between wakefulness and non-REM sleep produced by the combined action of wake and slow-wave sleep circuitry. These data indicated that the REM sleep stage was mechanistically unique because it depended upon the integrity of pontine control circuitry that was not involved in the generation of other sleep stages. Having identified a neuroanatomical locus for the REM sleep stage, Jouvet was able to argue that the phenomenological components of the REM sleep stage constituted a distinct state of the central nervous system (8, 1012).

A Cholinergic Mechanism of REM Sleep Generation

Early Developments

The recognition of REM sleep as an independent sleep state led to speculation regarding the possible mechanisms responsible for its generation. At the CIBA Foundation symposium on the nature of sleep in 1960, Michel Jouvet reported that the induction of REM sleep by electrical stimulation of the PTF was followed by a “refractory” period during which identical stimulations resulted in wakefulness accompanied by hypertonia and agitation. He remarked that the existence of this refractory period was suggestive of a “neurohumoral mechanism [of REM sleep generation] which would “discharge” periodically during behavioural sleep but which could not be brought into play until a sufficient “stock” of neurohormones was gathered.” He further speculated that this neurohumoral mechanism may importantly involve cholinergic neurotransmission (8). Stemming from Jouvet’s initial hypothesis, numerous studies have investigated the potential involvement of cholinergic neurotransmission in REM sleep generation; however, there is presently no consensus viewpoint. This main focus of this review will be to evaluate the evidence relating to the potential involvement of cholinergic neurotransmission in REM sleep generation. It is our position that a consensus viewpoint can be reached because the bulk of available evidence can be brought into agreement when appropriately interpreted and considered in light of recent findings.

Speculation over cholinergic involvement in the mechanism of REM sleep generation stemmed from studies showing the REM sleep modulating effects of systemically administered cholinergic drugs (10, 11). In cats, systemic administration of the cholinergic receptor antagonist atropine strongly disrupts sleep and wakefulness. Systemically administered atropine results in what was described as a “dissociation between the behaviour of the animal and its electrical activity” (11). While the EEG of atropinized cats was marked by slow-waves and bursting activity similar to that observed during natural sleep and anesthesia, they were nevertheless awake. These animals had tonically elevated muscle activity, adopted standing, and crouching postures, and remained responsive to auditory stimulation. However, auditory or noxious stimulation did not elicit cortical activation. Atropinized cats did not have REM sleep. Nevertheless, consistent with there being some capacity of cholinergic neurotransmission to promote REM sleep generation, increasing endogenous levels of acetylcholine, by systemic administration of an acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, increased REM sleep bout durations as well as the intensity of phasic REM phenomena (10, 11).

Cholinergic Stimulation of the Putative REM Sleep Generator in the Pontine Tegmental Field

Features of REM Sleep Induction by Cholinergic Agonism in the Pontine Tegmental Field

Overview of seminal findings. The reported link between cholinergic neurotransmission and REM sleep generation gave rise to the hypothesis that cholinergic activation of the putative REM sleep generating circuitry in the PTF is the critical event leading to the initiation of the state. To-date, the bulk of evidence cited in support of this hypothesis comes from studies showing that stimulating sites in the pons with cholinergic agonists is sufficient to induce REM sleep. The seminal cholinergic stimulation study by George et al. (13) showed that microinjections of cholinomimetics (i.e., carbachol and oxotremorine) in cats, at PTF sites where lesioning eliminated REM sleep, produced extended REM sleep-like states. These states were characterized by atonia, electrocortical activation, reflex inactivation, and REMs. Induced REM sleep persisted for 45–50 min and occurred at a short latency following microinjection (1–5 min). The latency to and magnitude of the induced REM sleep are important details because they suggest that the PTF REM sleep circuitry is structured such that cholinergic inputs are capable of overwhelming competing influences. This “capacity to overwhelm” was the primary indicator that cholinergic neurotransmission might play a major role in REM sleep generation. Cholinergic PTF stimulation also induced dissociated states where motor atonia or a REM sleep-like EEG theta rhythm occurred apart from other REM sleep phenomena. The finding that dissociated states could be triggered by acetylcholine in addition to fully orchestrated REM sleep is significant for three reasons. First, the induction of dissociated states suggests that the PTF is a source of multiple efferent pathways that can independently control components of the REM sleep state. Second, the occurrence of fully orchestrated REM sleep (i.e., marked by the synchronized onset and offset of the full complement of REM sleep components) indicates that the PTF is also a likely source of controller/switching circuitry – responsible for coordinating the activation of REM sleep components, while preventing the intrusion of other behavioral states. Finally, these findings suggest that the phenomenological afferents and the executive controller are cholinoceptive.

REM sleep induction by cholinergic PTF stimulation has been repeated by numerous other studies in both cats and rats (1436). The experimental details of these studies are listed in Table 1. REM sleep can be elicited by microinjection of the mixed acetylcholine receptor agonist carbachol, selective muscarinic acetylcholine receptor agonists (e.g., oxotremorine and bethanechol), and acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (e.g., neostigmine). REM sleep effects exhibit dose dependency and can be blocked by co-application of cholinergic receptor antagonists (e.g., atropine). REM sleep-like states have also been induced in decerebrate and anesthetized animals [for a review of REM sleep induction in reduced preparations refer to the review by Kubin (37)].

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The effects of modulating pontine cholinergic neurotransmission on REM sleep.

Neuroanatomy of REM sleep induction sites. Figure 1 shows the region of the PTF in which delivery of cholinomimetic drugs can induce REM sleep in cats (Figure 1A) and rats (Figure 1B). Table 1 provides more specific details about the locations of cholinomimetic drug delivery and the resulting effects on sleep and motor behavior.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PTF regions sensitive to cholinomimetic induction of REM sleep. Coronal (left) and sagittal (right) maps of the cat (A) and rat (B) pons. The shaded regions are inclusive of all the effective REM sleep inducing injection sites from the studies listed in Table 1. Anatomical abbreviations: 5M, fifth motor nucleus; 7M, seventh motor nucleus; BC, brachium conjunctivum; DpMe, deep mesencephalic reticular nucleus; FTC, central tegmental field; FTG, gigantocellular tegmental field; FTM, magnocellular tegmental field; LC, locus coeruleus; LDT, laterodorsal tegmental nucleus; Me5, mesencephalic 5 nucleus; Peri-LCα, peri-locus coeruleus alpha; PnC, caudal part of the pontine reticular nucleus; PnO, oral part of the pontine reticular nucleus; PPT, pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus; PTF, pontine tegmental field; RM, raphe magnus; RPo, raphe pontis; scp, superior cerebellar peduncle; SubC, subcoeruleus nucleus including alpha (A), dorsal (D), and ventral parts (V); Tv, ventral tegmental nucleus of Gudden; Tz, nucleus of the trapezoid body. Anatomical maps were adapted from Ref. (44, 45).

Initial studies generated disagreement regarding the location of the pontine site(s) in the cat where cholinergic stimulation generates REM sleep. Studies by Baghdoyan et al. (21, 22) and Hobson et al. (20) reported potent REM sleep inducing effects (40–50 min episodes and four- to ninefold increases in REM sleep time) of cholinergic receptor agonism in the posterior portion the ventral PTF (roughly from the posterior boundary of the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus [LDT; P2 according to Berman (45) to the bisection of the PTF by the facial nerve (P7) and ventral to the level of the trigeminal motor pool]. However, lesioning studies have showed that unlike electrolytic ablation (that destroy neurons and axons of passage) of the ventral PTF, kainic acid (which destroy neurons only) lesions in these regions did not affect REM sleep or its component phenomena (46, 47). These findings suggested that fibers passing through the ventral PTF are important for REM sleep generation rather than the neuronal perikarya. In contrast to posteroventral sites, REM sleep time is greatly reduced or eliminated by chemical lesioning of the anterodorsal pontine tegmentum (ventral to the level of the trigeminal motor pool and anterior to P4) (4850).

While REM sleep is induced at both anterodorsal and posteroventral PTF sites, the former are associated with inducing REM sleep at the shortest latencies following drug microinjection (i.e., <20 min) (27, 29). Due to drug diffusion following microinjection, longer latencies to REM sleep induction raise the possibility that the site of drug action is at a distance from the delivery site. Gnadt and Pegram (23) showed that a relatively small volume of solution (0.1 μl) containing titrated choline and carbachol spread as much as 1–1.5 mm over the course of an hour. Similarly, Myers and Hoch (51) reported that a 0.5 μl volume of radiolabeled dopamine spread 0.5–1.0 mm after 15 min, while Macklis and Quattrochi (52) found that 0.050 μl of radiolabeled carbachol spread into an area with a 4–5 mm diameter within 1 h of the microinjection. Therefore, longer latencies to REM sleep induction following microinjection into posteroventral PTF sites (injection volume range = 0.25–3 μl) may be the result of the time taken for drug to diffuse to REM sleep generating circuitry located in the anterodorsal PTF. However this interpretation has been challenged by Reinoso-Suarez et al. (32). They found that carbachol microinjections within the dorsal PTF induced persistent waking states punctuated by periodic bouts of muscle atonia, consistent with the findings of Mitler and Dement (38) and van Dongen et al. (17). Only after several hours of such arousal were increases in REM sleep time observed. Moreover, they showed that carbachol microinjections in the ventral portion of the PTF strongly induced REM sleep, with latency to induction being less than 5 min on average. Garzon et al. (35) also found that REM sleep could be induced at a short latency following ventral PTF microinjections of carbachol.

As in cats, REM enhancing sites in the rat extend along an anteroposterior axis from the posterior pole of the ventral tegmental nucleus of Gudden (Tv) to the root of the facial nerve (7n). There is no particular zone within this portion of the rat PTF where REM sleep can be reliably enhanced; responses to cholinergic stimulation range from no effect, to REM sleep enhancement, to arousal and motor impairment (for details on response variety, see the following sections and Table 1). However, despite the lack of reliability, REM sleep enhancing sites do tend to cluster in the subcoeruleus/sublaterodorsal portion of the rat PTF medial to the trigeminal motor pool (34).

The differences between cat and rodent studies including wakefulness effects. There are notable differences in the features of cholinergic-induced REM sleep between cats and rodents. Unlike in cats, where REM sleep episodes can be triggered within minutes of drug microinjection, reported latencies to REM sleep induction in rats exceed 30 min (Table 1). As noted previously for cats, episodes of cholinergic-induced REM sleep can last as long as 50 min and that REM sleep time increases to levels 3–9 times normal. By contrast, following PTF cholinergic stimulation in rats, REM sleep time increases to only 1.5–2 times normal levels and extended bouts of REM sleep do not occur (Table 1). Rather, increases in REM sleep time stem from increased bout frequency. Recall that the magnitude of REM sleep effects in cats – “the capacity to overwhelm” – was the primary indicator that cholinergic neurotransmission might play a major role in REM sleep generation. Consequently, the weaker effects of cholinergic stimulation observed in rats have led to alternative hypotheses suggesting that cholinergic neurotransmission in the PTF serves a modulatory, rather than a critical, role in REM sleep generation.

In rodents, the primary effect of cholinergic stimulation is often increased wakefulness rather than increased REM sleep (23, 33, 34, 41). Pollock and Mistlberger (42) reported that microinjection of neostigmine into the PTF of mice induces extended periods of wakefulness characterized by elevated muscle tone, delaying REM sleep onset for up to 3 h. Similarly, Boissard et al. (41) showed that at dorsal PTF sites where GABAA receptor antagonism induced REM sleep and carbachol microinjection induced a waking state characterized by heightened muscle tone. Mastrangelo et al. (31) showed that carbachol microinjections in the rat PTF induced extended periods of wakefulness lasting 20–80 min; thereafter, REM sleep time increased by 40%. In a large mapping study of the rat PTF, Deurveilher et al. (34) showed that carbachol microinjections induced wakefulness as often as REM sleep (13% of cases); 74% of injections produced no effect. Persistent wakefulness following cholinergic stimulation has also been reported in the cat. Baghdoyan et al. (22) found that carbachol microinjections at posterodorsal PTF sites induced wakefulness marked by ataxia, poor hind limb control, and aggressive behaviors. Mitler and Dement (38) showed that carbachol microinjection into the peri-locus coeruleus region of the anterodorsal PTF induced a persistent waking state within 1–10 min of injection that could last upwards of 30 h. In that study, motor atonia did appear but only in 6 of 15 injections after extended periods of wakefulness.

The arousing effects of cholinergic stimulation in the PTF are often accompanied by unusual motor activity. Reporting on the effects of carbachol microinjection into the PTF of the rat, Gnadt and Pegram (23) noted that cholinergic stimulation sometimes produced “ipsilateral contraction of the axial musculature, particularly in the neck and thorax. In its mildest form, this appeared as an ipsiversive circling behavior. In its most severe form, the contracture completely drew the rat into an incapacitating ipsilateral flexion of the body.” Similar results have been reported in cats by van Dongen (19). Reports of increased wakefulness following delivery of cholinomimetics into the PTF are often associated with such motor abnormalities. In the mapping study by Deurveilher et al. (34), induced wakefulness was accompanied by asymmetric body flexion or hyperactive circling in 90% of cases. The association between motor effects and arousal raises the possibility that cholinomimetic-induced arousal is an epiphenomenon of motor disruption. Therefore, particularly in rats, the activation of motor circuitry and the resulting state of arousal may conceal what capacity exists for cholinergic stimulation to induce REM sleep in the PTF. It is important to acknowledge that cholinoceptive REM sleep-related neurons in the pons are intermingled amongst cholinoceptive locomotion-related reticulospinal neurons that provide input to spinal central pattern generators (53). Experimental stimulation of these reticulospinal cells and the mesencephalic locomotor region that drives their activity produces similar patterns of muscle contraction and behavior as those noted above (54).

State dissociation. In addition to the study by George et al. (13), several additional studies in cats have reported state dissociation following cholinergic stimulation of the PTF. Cholinergic stimulation produced dissociated REM sleep states at sites scattered throughout the PTF; however, state dissociation was associated most frequently with microinjections in the peri-locus coeruleus region of the anterodorsal PTF (i.e., the same region associated with cholinomimetic-induced motor abnormalities and arousal). Vanni-Mercier et al. (27) reported that carbachol microinjection at points near the peri-locus coeruleus alpha in cats produced several distinct REM sleep-like states. In some cases, cats entered a state very similar to normal REM sleep; however, hippocampal theta activity was more reminiscent of wakefulness being described as medium amplitude and lower frequency (i.e., 3–5 Hz). This state contrasted with periods where carbachol induced a REM sleep-like hippocampal theta rhythm that persisted into non-REM sleep. In other cases, cats entered a state characterized by the occurrence of atonia, EEG desynchronization, ponto-geniculo-occipital waves, and REMs typical of normal REM sleep; however, the animals were otherwise alert, able to track visual stimuli, and orient toward low intensity auditory stimuli. The most common form of state dissociation reported in cats following cholinergic PTF stimulation is persistent motor atonia – occurring in 80% of studies reporting state dissociation.

Interpreting Gain-of Function Stimulation Studies

The induction of REM sleep by cholinergic stimulation of the PTF is often regarded as indirect evidence supporting the involvement of endogenous PTF cholinergic neurotransmission in REM sleep generation. Many investigators interpret the results of gain-of-function cholinergic stimulation studies as supporting the same conclusions as loss-of-function experiments, just to a lesser degree. It is our position that this should be considered a misinterpretation of gain-of-function stimulation studies. We submit that, in principle, the results of such cholinergic stimulation do not necessarily support or oppose any measure of endogenous cholinergic involvement in REM sleep generation.

Cholinergic stimulation of the PTF tests whether or not the endogenous cholinergic inputs to this region have a capacity, under the conditions of the experiment, to generate REM sleep or its component parts. In spite of the variable and species-dependent nature of cholinergic REM sleep enhancement, the capacity of endogenous cholinergic inputs to the PTF to generate REM sleep has been established. However, we should not infer the function of cholinergic PTF afferents in REM sleep generation from their demonstrated capacity – our reasoning is as follows (55). The function, if any, of PTF cholinergic afferents in REM sleep generation is a product of the REM sleep control network’s response to their input. This response depends on two factors. The first factor is the initial state of the REM sleep control network at the time of cholinergic input. The second factor is the magnitude (and perhaps the pattern) of the cholinergic input. Therefore, the role of PTF cholinergic afferents in REM sleep generation is ultimately a function of the timing and the intensity of their activation. The refractory period following carbachol-induced REM sleep illustrates how the effect of cholinergic input to the PTF depends on timing and initial conditions. During the refractory period, an additional bolus of carbachol induces a waking state marked by high muscle tone rather than REM sleep (13). Also, optogenetic stimulation of PTF cholinergic afferents in the LDT and pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus (PPT) is only effective at enhancing REM sleep when stimulation occurs in non-REM sleep – waking and REM sleep stimulations have no effect [(56), see “Selective Modulation of PPT and LDT Subpopulations” for details]. Similar results were obtained by Lopez-Rodriguez et al. (40) who showed that direct cholinergic stimulation in the PTF most often induced REM sleep when microinjections were made during non-REM sleep. Subsequent microinjections at the same locations during wakefulness preferentially induced a dissociated state characterized by motor atonia and sensory awareness. The importance of stimulus intensity is illustrated by the fact that, depending on the dose applied, cholinergic input to the PTF can have no effect, induce REM sleep, induce wakefulness, or produce dissociated states. Gain-of-function stimulation experiments simulate the effects of neural activation with intensity and timing that are not necessarily relevant to the physiological case. The effects of cholinomimetic stimulation of the PTF approximate the function of endogenous cholinergic inputs only in the case that those inputs are a major source of inductive REM sleep drive to the PTF. However, a priori, it is equally possible that cholinergic inputs are only minor sources of inductive drive relative to other afferents. It is also possible that cholinergic PTF input is non-inductive; the activation of REM sleep generating circuits in the PTF may precede the activation of their cholinergic afferents. In either of these alternative cases (i.e., where cholinergic PTF input is minor or non-inductive), stimulation of the PTF with cholinomimetic drugs could still have a major capacity to induce REM sleep despite endogenous cholinergic input to the PTF being potentially insignificant in the natural initiation of the state. Determining the function of PTF cholinergic inputs in REM sleep generation requires loss-of-function experiments that remove or block this afferent input. This is the only approach that can preserve the sequence of events and the initial conditions leading up to the activation of PTF cholinergic inputs.

Case study: optogenetics and the involvement of MCH containing neurons in REM sleep generation. There is an emerging consensus that melanin concentrating hormone (MCH) containing neurons in the hypothalamus play a role in the generation and maintenance of REM sleep. Studies reporting REM sleep enhancement following optogenetic stimulation of MCH neurons are cited in support of this claim. Konadhode et al. (57) have shown that optogenetic stimulation of MCH containing neurons shortens sleep latency and increases time spent in both non-REM and REM sleep at the expense of wakefulness. A similar study by Jego et al. (58) showed that optogenetic activation of MCH neurons during non-REM sleep facilitated non-REM-to-REM sleep transitions, while activation at the onset of REM sleep bouts increased their duration. Likewise, Tsunematsu et al. (59) showed that optogenetic activation of MCH neurons induced transitions into REM sleep and increased REM sleep time at the expense of time spent in non-REM sleep. Many in the field have interpreted these findings as implicating hypothalamic MCH neurons in the mechanism of REM sleep generation (5762). We submit that while these data demonstrate the capacity of MCH containing neurons to generate and maintain the state of REM sleep, they do not necessarily implicate MCH neurons in this function. More importantly, the role of MCH containing neurons in REM sleep generation ought to be inferred from loss-of-function experiments rather than gain-of-function experiments. However, neither optogenetic silencing (58) nor targeted ablation (59) of MCH neurons has any effect on the generation or maintenance of REM sleep. These loss-of-function silencing experiments demonstrate that MCH neurons do not play a major role in the induction or maintenance of REM sleep, in spite of the REM sleep enhancing effects of MCH neuron stimulation.

Blockade of Cholinergic Input to the Putative REM Sleep Generator

Identifying the function of endogenous cholinergic inputs to the PTF in REM sleep generation requires the removal or blockade of cholinergic inputs. A small number of studies have examined the effects of antagonizing cholinergic receptors in the PTF. In the seminal study of cholinomimetic-induced REM sleep, George et al. (13) anecdotally reported that, in two cats, atropine microinjections into the PTF had no “visible effects.” Similarly, Gnadt and Pegram (23) microinjected atropine in five rats and reported that it had no effect on REM sleep time (no data provided). Bourgin et al. (33) reported that in three rats, microinjection of atropine alone did not affect REM sleep time at the same sites where carbachol induced REM sleep (no data provided). By contrast, two studies have reported decreases in REM sleep following pharmacological blockade of cholinergic receptors in the pons. Firstly, Shiromani and Fishbein (24) reported significant reductions of REM sleep in rats following pontine infusions of the muscarinic acetylcholine receptor antagonist scopolamine. However, infusions took place continuously over 5 days using a chronically implanted osmotic mini-pump. Over such an extended period of time, the infused scopolamine would have spread far beyond the target site located in the posteromedial PTF. Similar reductions in REM sleep occurred with medullary and ventricular infusions. In a second study, Imeri et al. (30) reported increases in REM sleep latency in rats following pontine microinjections of the muscarinic type-2 receptor antagonist methoctramine. Methoctramine microinjection resulted in a dose-dependent increase in the latency to REM sleep. Total REM sleep time was also reduced following methoctramine microinjections. However, the reduced REM sleep time and the increased REM sleep latencies may have been a secondary effect of arousal, because non-REM sleep time was also reduced while wake time increased, particularly at the highest doses. Taken together, these data neither confirm nor refute the involvement of pontine cholinergic neurotransmission in the generation of REM sleep.

In a recently published study, we aimed to determine the role of endogenous cholinergic input to the PTF in the generation of REM sleep in rats (63).Using bilateral reverse microdialysis of the cholinergic receptor antagonist scopolamine, we showed that blocking cholinergic input to the PTF had no effect on REM sleep time or the frequency of REM sleep episodes. Importantly, the concentration of antagonist used was sufficient to block the effects of carbachol microperfusion (63). In our study, carbachol mainly induced extended episodes of wakefulness marked by abnormal motor behavior [similar to Ref. (17, 23, 34, 38)]. In the event that cholinergic receptor antagonism in the PTF does not block or reduce REM sleep, there is always the possibility that drug delivery was unable to completely block cholinergic input to the PTF. We aimed to minimize this possibility (63). Having previously measured the extent of drug diffusion using reverse microdialysis (43), we would expect microperfused antagonist to permeate the cholinoceptive REM sleep induction zone of the rat (approximately 14 mm3) within the recording period. Most importantly, we showed that scopolamine microperfusion in the PTF effectively blocked the REM sleep enhancement produced by selectively activating PTF cholinergic afferents in the PPT (see “Selective Modulation of PPT and LDT Subpopulations” for details) (63). Accordingly, we expect that our microperfusions of scopolamine were sufficient to block the endogenous cholinergic input to REM sleep generating sites in the PTF. Since scopolamine microperfusion into the PTF still had no effect on REM sleep time, we submit that endogenous cholinergic input to the PTF is not involved in the induction of REM sleep (63). Despite not effecting REM sleep time, antagonism of cholinergic inputs to the PTF did increase the duration and failure rate of transitions from non-REM-to-REM sleep (63). Therefore, while cholinergic inputs to the PTF are not required for inducing transitions into REM sleep, they may serve an accessory role in reinforcing transitions after their initiation.

Cholinergic Afferents of the Pontine Tegmental Field

Non-Selective Modulation of the PPT and LDT

The claim that cholinergic neurotransmission in the PTF plays a major role in REM sleep generation entails that PTF cholinergic afferents located in the PPT and LDT are principal sources of the inductive drive that initiates REM sleep. PPT and LDT cell groups contain subpopulations of neurons that increase their activity in anticipation of and during REM sleep (i.e., REM sleep-active) (6466). The activation of cholinergic REM sleep-active PPT and LDT neurons is likely responsible for the increase in endogenous acetylcholine release in the PTF during REM sleep (67). Electrical or pharmacological stimulation of PPT and LDT neurons has been shown to increase REM sleep (66, 6870), induce acetylcholine release in the PTF (71), and evoke scopolamine-sensitive excitatory postsynaptic potentials in PTF neurons (72). These data further support the claim that cholinergic PTF afferents have a capacity to generate REM sleep.

Determining the function of cholinergic PPT and LDT neurons in REM sleep generation requires loss-of-function experiments. Reductions in REM sleep have been reported following electrolytic or chemical lesioning of the PPT and the LDT in cats, consistent with their causal involvement in REM sleep generation (49, 73). However, in these studies, lesions were not restricted to cholinergic cell areas and are therefore difficult to interpret. Lesions included other important components of the REM sleep generating circuitry, including the deep mesencephalic reticular nucleus (DpMe), the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray (vlPAG), and the anterodorsal PTF including the peri-locus coeruleus region.

The effects of smaller, more selective chemical lesions of the PPT and LDT in rats (50) indicate that neither of these cell groups is needed to generate REM sleep. Lu and colleagues reported that LDT lesions did not affect REM sleep per se, but did increase the number of total state transitions per hour. Surprisingly, PPT lesions increased, rather than decreased, REM sleep time. This finding raises the possibility that some PPT subpopulations may function to suppress, rather than promote, REM sleep. An inhibitory influence on REM sleep by PPT neurons is further supported by the finding that pharmacological inactivation of the PPT, by GABAA receptor agonism, increases REM sleep both as a percentage of total recording and total sleep time – i.e., independent of concurrent changes in wakefulness (7476). By contrast, Petrovic et al. (77) reported no change in REM sleep amounts following bilateral PPT lesions; however, PPT lesions resulted in an increased number non-REM-to-REM sleep and wake-to-REM sleep transitions. Moreover, REM sleep split into two electroencephalographically distinct states (i.e., “sigma coherent” and “theta coherent” REM sleep).

The lack of consistency between PPT/LDT modulating studies notwithstanding, the REM sleep effects of manipulating the PPT or LDT en masse should not be taken to reflect the function of the REM sleep-active cholinergic neurons in these regions. Cholinergic REM sleep-active PPT/LDT neurons are vastly outnumbered by other cell types (7880). The composition of the LDT and PPT cell groups is highly heterogeneous. While the boundaries of the PPT and LDT are defined by their cholinergic subpopulations, 74–81% of PPT/LDT neurons are GABAergic or glutamatergic (81). Furthermore, immunostaining for c-Fos following REM sleep deprivation and recovery has revealed that only a small proportion (5–15%) of PPT/LDT neurons exhibiting heightened activity in REM sleep are cholinergic (78, 79). Much larger proportions (50–85%) are GABAergic (78, 82, 83). Moreover, Clement et al. (84) have shown that more than 50% of LDT neurons found to be c-Fos positive after REM sleep recovery are glutamatergic.

REM sleep-active neurons in the PPT and LDT can be subdivided into two groups based on their firing rate profile across the sleep–wake cycle (6466, 80). First, some REM sleep-active neurons exhibit maximal activity in REM sleep and minimal activity in wakefulness (i.e., REM sleep-max active). Within this group, some neurons have firing rates that increase gradually from active wakefulness through non-REM sleep to REM sleep, while other neurons only markedly increase their firing immediately prior to and during REM sleep (80). Second, some REM sleep-active neurons in the PPT and LDT discharge minimally during non-REM sleep. Neurons of this type exhibit increased firing during wakefulness in association with muscle activation, firing rate deceleration in transition to non-REM sleep, and acceleration of firing rate in transition to REM sleep (i.e., REM sleep/wake-max active). Within this group, some neurons discharge maximally in active wakefulness while others discharge maximally in REM sleep (80). In addition to REM sleep-active neurons, some PPT and LDT neurons discharge minimally in REM sleep and maximally during active wakefulness (80). Wake-active subpopulations of the PPT/LDT are hypothesized components of the ascending reticular activating system that maintains wakefulness (85).

Using juxtacellular recording and labeling in the PPT and LDT, Boucetta et al. (80) reported that these groups are neurochemically diverse, consisting of cholinergic, GABAergic, and glutamatergic cell types. Previously, cholinergic neurons in the PPT and LDT were assumed to be both REM sleep-max active and REM sleep/wake-max active. However, Boucetta et al. (80) found that recorded cholinergic neurons in the LDT and PPT were exclusively REM sleep/wake-max active. Since the sample size was small [six neurons; LDT(5) and PPT(1)] further studies are needed to definitively determine whether or not any REM sleep-active cholinergic PPT/LDT neurons are REM sleep-max active. Boucetta et al. (80) showed that the majority (4/6) of REM sleep/wake-max active cholinergic neurons in the LDT and PPT discharged maximally in REM sleep. On average, discharge rates of cholinergic REM sleep/wake-max active LDT and PPT neurons were 63 and 348% greater than those in active and quiet wake, respectively. The existence of PPT and LDT cholinergic neurons that discharge maximally in REM sleep (irrespective of their activity in wakefulness) is also supported by positive immunostaining for c-Fos in cholinergic LDT/PPT neurons following recovery from selective REM sleep deprivation. If REM sleep-active cholinergic neurons in the LDT and PPT were exclusively REM sleep/wake-max active and did not discharge at significantly higher rates in REM sleep as compared to wakefulness, it would be difficult to explain the observations of Maloney et al. (78) and Verret et al. (79) that c-Fos expression increases in rebound versus deprived conditions when the combined time spent in wakefulness and REM sleep remains constant. Taken together, these studies demonstrate that there are distinct subpopulations of neurons defined by unique combinations of neurotransmitter expression and activity profile within the PPT and LDT. These subpopulations could be functionally distinct and therefore the inconsistencies between studies that manipulate these subpopulations en masse could be a reflection of this functional diversity.

Selective Modulation of PPT and LDT Subpopulations

Determining the functional importance of individual PPT and LDT subpopulations requires that they be targeted selectively. PPT and LDT cholinergic neurons selectively express receptors for Urotensin II, a vasomodulatory peptide (86). Urotensin II has been shown to selectively excite cholinergic PPT and LDT neurons in vitro (87, 88). Local infusion of Urotensin II in the PPT of rats increased the number of REM sleep episodes and the total time spent in REM sleep both as a percentage of total recording and total sleep time (63, 88). Urotensin II microperfusion in the PPT also decreased non-REM-to-REM sleep transition duration and increased non-REM-to-REM sleep transition efficiency (i.e., attempts to transition from non-REM to REM sleep failed less often) (63). The effects of Urotensin II at the PPT were blocked by simultaneous antagonism of cholinergic receptors in the PTF (63). Consistent with the effects of selective pharmacological activation of PPT and LDT cholinergic neurons, selective optogenetic activation of PPT and LDT cholinergic neurons increased the frequency of REM sleep episodes as well as time spent in REM sleep, at the expense of time spent in non-REM sleep (56). Optogenetic stimulation increased the probability of transitioning into REM sleep only when stimulation occurred in non-REM sleep. REM sleep was unaffected by stimulations occurring in wakefulness or REM sleep (56), for commentary see Ref. (55, 89). Taken together, these gain-of-function interventions confirm that, free of the potentially confounding effects of modulating intermingled non-cholinergic neurons, cholinergic PPT and LDT neurons have the capacity to enhance non-REM-to-REM sleep transitioning and induce REM sleep. These data also show that, at least in the case of PPT cholinergic neurons, this capacity for REM sleep enhancement is exerted through cholinoceptive PTF neurons. However, we submit that these studies do not necessitate that cholinergic LDT and PPT neurons are involved in the induction of REM sleep. We do not know what would be the effect, if any, of selectively inhibiting cholinergic PPT and LDT subpopulations – e.g., optogenetic inactivation or selective neurotoxic lesioning with Urotensin II conjugated to diphtheria toxin (90). However, the effects of cholinergic antagonism at the PTF (see Blockade of Cholinergic Input to the Putative REM Sleep Generator for details) would suggest that PPT and LDT cholinergic inputs to the PTF increase the efficiency of non-REM-to-REM sleep transitioning, but are otherwise not needed for the occurrence of REM sleep (63).

REM sleep-max active PPT and LDT neurons can be selectively inhibited by the 5-HT1A agonist 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino) (8-OH-DPAT), leaving the activity of REM sleep/wake-max unaffected (65). Surprisingly, selective inhibition of the REM sleep-max active subpopulation of the PPT increased REM sleep, as a percentage of total recording and total sleep time (91). The increase in REM sleep time stemmed from an increase in the frequency of REM sleep episodes particularly during periods of low REM sleep drive/propensity (91) quantified electroencephalographically using a modified algorithm developed by Benington and Heller (92) and Benington et al. (93). Recall that, based on c-Fos immunostaining following REM sleep deprivation and recovery, PPT REM sleep-active neurons are predominately GABAergic and glutamatergic. It is possible that, PPT subpopulations exert opposing yet complementary influences of REM sleep. Non-cholinergic REM sleep-max active PPT neurons may function to raise the drive threshold for REM sleep induction in order to limit REM sleep episodes to periods of high propensity. Following the onset of a transition into REM sleep, cholinergic PPT neurons may be recruited to reinforce switching and increase the probability of transition success (63). We hypothesize that the combined action of these PPT subpopulations may function to reduce the sensitivity of the REM sleep switch to noise.

The REM Sleep Switch

The Reciprocal Interaction Hypothesis

Mapping the neuroanatomical network responsible for REM sleep generation, although necessary, is insufficient for an understanding of the network dynamics that actually give rise to cycling into and out of REM sleep. In 1975, Hobson and McCarley proposed the reciprocal interaction model: a structural/mathematical hypothesis meant to provide an explanation for the cyclical generation of REM sleep. This model posited that a reciprocal interaction between REM sleep-inactive cell groups and REM sleep-active cell groups form a pacemaker circuit that drives oscillations between sleep stages (94) (Figure 2A). Aminergic neurons in the locus coeruleus and dorsal raphe were hypothesized as the REM sleep-inactive cells. REM sleep-active neurons in the PTF innervating the LC were presumed to be cholinergic and excitatory (95). The model proposed that during wakefulness “cholinergic” PTF neurons would be inhibited by activated aminergic neurons. At non-REM sleep onset, waning aminergic neuron activity would disinhibit “cholinergic” PTF neurons. At a critical point, the combination of this disinhibition and auto-excitation within the “cholinergic” cell group would enable the exponential rise in PTF neuron activity that triggers the onset of REM sleep (96). During REM sleep, “cholinergic” PTF neurons would excite aminergic neurons resulting in their own inactivation and the termination of the REM sleep episode.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Hypothesized REM sleep control circuits. (A) The original reciprocal interaction hypothesis. (B) Modified reciprocal interaction hypothesis. (C) Flip-flop circuit proposed by Lu et al. (50). (D) Version of flip-flop circuit proposed by Sapin et al. (83), modified by Grace et al. (63). Anatomical abbreviations: DpMe, deep mesencephalic reticular nucleus; LDT, laterodorsal tegmental nucleus; PPT, pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus; PTF, pontine tegmental field; vlPAG, ventrolateral periaqueductal gray.

The reciprocal interaction model was proposed before cholinergic neurons could be immunohistochemically identified. The discovery that PTF neurons are predominately non-cholinergic forced a revision of the model (97) (Figure 2B). REM sleep-active cholinergic neurons in the PPT and LDT were included as the main REM sleep promoting group (96, 98). In the revised model, PPT and LDT cholinergic neurons triggered REM sleep by exciting REM sleep-related non-cholinergic neurons in the PTF. In this version of the structural model, a mutually excitatory connection between the PTF and the PPT/LDT was responsible for the exponential rise in PTF neuron activity at REM sleep onset (95, 99). This pacemaker circuit was modeled mathematically using equations of the Lotka–Volterra type derived from population models of predator–prey interactions (94). The simple Lotka–Volterra model formed the basis for the more robust limit cycle model, which incorporated circadian modulation (100). The time course of neuronal activity in the REM sleep-active and REM sleep-inactive cell groups predicted by the mathematical models, coincide with the actual long-term recordings of REM sleep-active and REM sleep-inactive neurons. However, models of this type have been criticized on the basis that they rely on the tuning of unconstrained parameters to achieve a particular ultradian period. Several lines of evidence argue against the reciprocal interaction model as being a workable explanation for REM sleep generation. Most notably, loss-of-function interventions targeting the structural components of the model – the PPT, LDT, LC, DRN, and cholinergic neurotransmission in the PRF – have little or no effects on the occurrence of REM sleep (50, 63, 7476, 91).

The Flip-Flop Hypothesis

More recently, it has been suggested that mutual inhibition between REM sleep-active and REM sleep-inactive GABAergic cell groups creates a flip-flop switch that is critical for REM sleep generation (50, 83). Unlike a pacemaker circuit (e.g., reciprocal interaction model), which is intrinsically capable of generating an ultradian rhythm, a flip-flop switch is triggered by an input signal external to the circuit. In other words, pacemaker network models assume that the periodicity of the sleep cycle is an intrinsic property of the circuit’s topology, whereas a flip-flop mechanism assumes that cycling between sleep stages occurs as a response to external demands. A flip-flop switch can convert a graded input signal, which changes slowly over time, into a two-state output that reduces time spent in intermediate states (i.e., bistability) (101). In comparison with a pacemaker model, a flip-flop mechanism can better account for (i) the occurrence of a “rebound” after REM sleep deprivation (102105) and (ii) the positive correlation between the length of a REM sleep bout and the length of the preceding non-REM bout (this correlation would likely be negative in the case of a pacemaker mechanism) (92).

In 2006, Lu and colleagues proposed that REM sleep is generated by a mutually inhibitory interaction between REM sleep-active GABAergic neurons in the sublaterodorsal region of the PTF and REM sleep-inactive GABAergic neurons in the vlPAG and the adjacent DpMe (Figure 2C). In support of this arrangement, retrograde and anterograde tracing experiments showed that the sublaterodorsal PTF and vlPAG/DpMe neuronal pools mutually innervate one another (50). Moreover, Boissard et al. (106) have shown that DpMe and vlPAG projections to the sublaterodorsal PTF are GABAergic; Lu et al. (50) have demonstrated that half of the sublaterodorsal pontine tegmental cells retrogradely labeled from the DpMe and vlPAG contain GAD67 mRNA.

In contrast to sublaterodorsal PTF inactivation, which suppresses REM sleep, vlPAG and DpMe inactivation potently increases REM sleep time, increasing the frequency and length of REM sleep episodes (50, 83, 107, 108). While these effects are consistent with those that one would expect in the case that the sublaterodorsal PTF and vlPAG/DpMe neuronal pools form a mutually inhibitory flip-flop switch, it has been shown that REM sleep-active neurons in the sublaterodorsal PTF are predominately glutamatergic rather than GABAergic (84). It is therefore unlikely that REM sleep-active sublaterodorsal PTF neurons form a mutually inhibitory flip-flop switch together with vlPAG/DpMe neurons. Sapin et al. (83) alternatively proposed that a mutually inhibitory flip-flop switch might be formed between GABAergic REM sleep-inactive vlPAG/DpMe neurons and the large number of GABAergic REM sleep-active neurons also located in the vlPAG/DpMe (Figure 2D). In either case, activation of REM sleep-related PTF neurons and initiation of REM sleep would result from the withdrawal of GABAergic inhibition by REM sleep-inactive vlPAG/DpMe neurons. Consistent with such an inductive mechanism, GABAA receptor antagonism in the PTF terminates non-REM sleep and induces persistent REM sleep (41, 109111).

Flip-flop hypotheses of REM sleep generation remain speculative; however, a recent modeling study has tested the theoretical validity of flip-flop switch involvement in the generation of the sleep cycle. Dunmyre et al. (112) constructed a physiological model based on coupled flip-flop oscillators for the control of transitioning between wakefulness, non-REM and REM sleep. With the inclusion of separate non-REM and REM sleep “homeostatic” drives, the modeled circuit was able to faithfully reproduce experimentally measured rodent sleep architecture under normal and sleep-deprived conditions. Importantly, unlike previous flip-flop models of behavioral state transitioning (113, 114), this study does not incorporate intrinsic oscillatory capability into the neuronal groups. Therefore, this study provides theoretical validation that “homeostatically” regulated interlocking flip-flop oscillators are sufficient to explain behavioral state transitioning.

A Hypothesis for the Structural Basis of the Cholinergic Effects on non-REM-to-REM Sleep Transitioning

It was previously noted that strong evidence has confirmed the capacity of cholinergic neurotransmission in the PTF to induce REM sleep; however, blockade of this cholinergic neurotransmission does not prevent REM sleep. Instead, we found evidence that cholinergic input to the PTF reinforces transitions making them quicker and less likely to fail. We also noted that the reciprocal interaction model posits that a mutually excitatory (i.e., positive feedback) interaction between cholinergic LDT/PPT neurons and the PTF causes the exponential rise in PTF neuron activity in advance of REM sleep onset. Consistent with the existence of such a mutually excitatory positive feedback loop, (i) neuroanatomical evidence shows that the PTF and LDT/PPT cell groups innervate one another (115117) and (ii) unilateral carbachol stimulation of the PTF causes endogenous acetylcholine release in the contralateral pontine tegmentum (118), possibly due to positive feedback onto PPT/LDT neurons from cholinoceptive PTF neurons.

Given that REM sleep time and the frequency of REM sleep bouts are both unaffected by cholinergic inputs to the PTF, it is unlikely that this cholinergic input provides inductive drive to trigger the activation of REM sleep-related PTF neurons. In other words, in the sequence of events giving rise to REM sleep, it is unlikely that PPT/LDT activation precedes PTF activation. The available evidence is best explained in the case where the activation of PPT/LDT cholinergic neurons is gated by activity in the PTF, and where the PPT/LDT provides cholinergic positive feedback to PTF neurons. In this case, cholinergic receptor antagonism could have little or no effect on REM sleep quantity despite there being a major capacity for exogenous cholinergic stimulation to initiate REM sleep or its component parts (see Case Study: Optogenetics and the Involvement of MCH Containing Neurons in REM Sleep Generation for further discussion of this point). Consistent with this hypothesis, the increase in firing rates of REM sleep-active GABAergic and glutamatergic neurons in the pontomesencephalic tegmentum that occurs during the non-REM to REM sleep transition period precedes that of cholinergic LDT/PPT neurons (80).

Using computer modeling, we have shown that a positive feedback loop producing this sequence (i.e., PTF → PPT/LDT → PTF) can account for the transition reinforcement produced by PTF cholinergic neurotransmission in vivo (63) (Figure 2D). Using the flip-flop circuit proposed by Sapin et al. (83), we showed that varying “cholinergic” positive feedback to the “PTF” did not delay transition onset and therefore did not affect the activation threshold of the “PTF,” likely a major determinant of REM sleep quantity in vivo. Reducing “cholinergic” positive feedback to the “PTF” did lengthen transitions into REM sleep as observed in vivo (63). Under conditions of reduced positive feedback, peak firing rates of “PTF” neurons were also suppressed. Reduced firing of PTF neurons could underlie our observation that the normal increase in EEG theta power occurring in REM sleep was suppressed during cholinergic receptor blockade in the PTF (63). The PTF contains glutamatergic neurons projecting to the medial septum and lesioning these cells eliminates theta oscillations during REM sleep (50). Lastly, simulations showed that “cholinergic” positive feedback dampened potentially destabilizing surges in flip-flop switch REM-off neuron activity that could underlie transition failure in vivo (63).

Our hypothesis that a positive feedback mechanism underlies the function of PTF cholinergic afferents in REM sleep generation does not necessarily require that the upstream switching element be a flip-flop switch. However, in the case that the main switching element is a positive feedback flip-flop circuit, our suggestion of an accessory cholinergic positive feedback loop takes on special significance. Note that while only a single positive feedback loop is required to create bistability in a signaling pathway, nested feedback loops are nevertheless common in biology. For instance, the polarization of cell growth in yeast (119), mammalian calcium signaling (120), and the maturation of xenopus oocytes (121) all utilize interlocking positive feedback loops. Using multiple interlocked feedback loops produces more reliable switches that are rapidly inducible and resistant to extrinsic noise (122).

Conclusion

(1) The capacity of PTF cholinergic afferents to generate REM sleep has been firmly established by gain-of-function experiments. (2) The function of endogenous PTF cholinergic input in REM sleep generation cannot be determined by gain-of-function experiments; rather, loss-of-function studies are required. (3) Loss-of-function studies show that endogenous cholinergic input to the PTF is not required for REM sleep generation. (4) Cholinergic input to the PTF serves an accessory role in REM sleep generation: reinforcing non-REM-to-REM sleep transitions making them quicker and less likely to fail.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

KG received support from a CIHR Doctoral Award – Fredrick Banting and Charles Best Canada Graduate Scholarship (CGS-D), a CIHR Team Research and Training Grant in Sleep and Biological Rhythms, and a Queen Elizabeth II/Grace Lumsden/Margaret Nicholds Scholarship in Science and Technology (QEII-GSST). Supported by funds from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR, Grant MT-15563). RH is supported by a Tier I Canada Research Chair in Sleep and Respiratory Neurobiology.

References

1. Naquet R. The duality of sleep revealed: the history of paradoxical sleep. Arch Ital Biol (2004) 142:327–32.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

2. Aserinsky E, Kleitman N. Regularly occurring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleep. Science (1953) 118:273–4. doi:10.1126/science.118.3062.273

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Dement W, Kleitman N. Cyclic variations in EEG during sleep and their relation to eye movements, body motility, and dreaming. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol (1957) 9:673–90. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(57)90088-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Loomis AL, Harvey EN, Hobart G. Distribution of disturbance-patterns in the human EEG with special reference to sleep. J Neurophysiol (1938) 1:413–30.

Google Scholar

5. Rimbaud L, Passouant P, Cadilhac J. Participation of the hippocampus in regulation of sleep and wakeful states. Rev Neurol (Paris) (1955) 93:303–8.

Google Scholar

6. Roth B. [The Eeg in narcolepsy-cataplexy]. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol (1964) 16:170–90. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(64)90036-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Gibbs FA, Gibbs EL. Atlas of Electroencephalography. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley Press (1950).

Google Scholar

8. Jouvet M. Telencephalic and rhombencephalic sleep in the cat. In: Wolstenholme GEW, O’Connor M, editors. CIBA Foundation Symposium on the Nature of Sleep. London: J. & A. Churchhill (1960). p. 188–208.

Google Scholar

9. Jouvet M, Dechaume J, Michel F. [Study of the mechanisms of physiological sleep]. Lyon Med (1960) 92:479–521.

Google Scholar

10. Jouvet M, Michel F. [New research on the structures responsible for the “paradoxical phase” of sleep.]. J Physiol (Paris) (1960) 52:130–1.

Google Scholar

11. Jouvet M. [Research on the neural structures and responsible mechanisms in different phases of physiological sleep.]. Arch Ital Biol (1962) 100:125–206.

Google Scholar

12. Jouvet M. How sleep was dissociated into two states: telencephalic and rhombencephalic sleep? Arch Ital Biol (2004) 142:317–26.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

13. George R, Haslett WL, Jenden DJ. A cholinergic mechanism in the brainstem reticular formation: induction of paradoxical sleep. Int J Neuropharmacol (1964) 3:541–52. doi:10.1016/0028-3908(64)90076-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Cordeau JP, Moreau A, Beaulnes A, Laurin C. EEG and behavioral changes following microinjections of acetylcholine and adrenaline in the brain stem of cats. Arch Ital Biol (1963) 101:30–47.

Google Scholar

15. Kostowski W. Effects of some cholinergic and anticholinergic drugs injected intracerebrally to the midline pontine area. Neuropharmacology (1971) 10:595–605. doi:10.1016/0028-3908(71)90025-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Amatruda TT III, Black DA, Mckenna TM, Mccarley RW, Hobson JA. Sleep cycle control and cholinergic mechanisms: differential effects of carbachol injections at pontine brain stem sites. Brain Res (1975) 98:501–15. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(75)90369-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. van Dongen PA, Broekkamp CL, Cools AR. Atonia after carbachol microinjections near the locus coeruleus in cats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (1978) 8:527–32. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(78)90382-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Silberman EK, Vivaldi E, Garfield J, Mccarley RW, Hobson JA. Carbachol triggering of desynchronized sleep phenomena: enhancement via small volume infusions. Brain Res (1980) 191:215–24. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(80)90324-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. van Dongen PA. Locus ceruleus region: effects on behavior of cholinergic, noradrenergic, and opiate drugs injected intracerebrally into freely moving cats. Exp Neurol (1980) 67:52–78. doi:10.1016/0014-4886(80)90161-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Hobson JA, Goldberg M, Vivaldi E, Riew D. Enhancement of desynchronized sleep signs after pontine microinjection of the muscarinic agonist bethanechol. Brain Res (1983) 275:127–36. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(83)90424-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Baghdoyan HA, Monaco AP, Rodrigo-Angulo ML, Assens F, Mccarley RW, Hobson JA. Microinjection of neostigmine into the pontine reticular formation of cats enhances desynchronized sleep signs. J Pharmacol Exp Ther (1984) 231:173–80.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

22. Baghdoyan HA, Rodrigo-Angulo ML, Mccarley RW, Hobson JA. Site-specific enhancement and suppression of desynchronized sleep signs following cholinergic stimulation of three brainstem regions. Brain Res (1984) 306:39–52. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(84)90354-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Gnadt JW, Pegram GV. Cholinergic brainstem mechanisms of REM sleep in the rat. Brain Res (1986) 384:29–41. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(86)91216-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Shiromani PJ, Fishbein W. Continuous pontine cholinergic microinfusion via mini-pump induces sustained alterations in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (1986) 25:1253–61. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(86)90120-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Shiromani PJ, McGinty DJ. Pontine neuronal response to local cholinergic infusion: relation to REM sleep. Brain Res (1986) 386:20–31. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(86)90137-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Baghdoyan HA, Rodrigo-Angulo ML, Mccarley RW, Hobson JA. A neuroanatomical gradient in the pontine tegmentum for the cholinoceptive induction of desynchronized sleep signs. Brain Res (1987) 414:245–61. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(87)90005-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Vanni-Mercier G, Sakai K, Lin JS, Jouvet M. Mapping of cholinoceptive brainstem structures responsible for the generation of paradoxical sleep in the cat. Arch Ital Biol (1989) 127:133–64.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

28. Velazquez-Moctezuma J, Gillin JC, Shiromani PJ. Effect of specific M1, M2 muscarinic receptor agonists on REM sleep generation. Brain Res (1989) 503:128–31. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(89)91712-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Yamamoto K, Mamelak AN, Quattrochi JJ, Hobson JA. A cholinoceptive desynchronized sleep induction zone in the anterodorsal pontine tegmentum: locus of the sensitive region. Neuroscience (1990) 39:279–93. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(90)90268-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Imeri L, Bianchi S, Angeli P, Mancia M. Selective blockade of different brain stem muscarinic receptor subtypes: effects on the sleep-wake cycle. Brain Res (1994) 636:68–72. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(94)90176-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Mastrangelo D, De Saint Hilaire-Kafi Z, Gaillard JM. Effects of clonidine and alpha-methyl-p-tyrosine on the carbachol stimulation of paradoxical sleep. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (1994) 48:93–100. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(94)90503-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Reinoso-Suarez F, De Andres I, Rodrigo-Angulo ML, Rodriguez-Veiga E. Location and anatomical connections of a paradoxical sleep induction site in the cat ventral pontine tegmentum. Eur J Neurosci (1994) 6:1829–36. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.1994.tb00575.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Bourgin P, Escourrou P, Gaultier C, Adrien J. Induction of rapid eye movement sleep by carbachol infusion into the pontine reticular formation in the rat. Neuroreport (1995) 6:532–6. doi:10.1097/00001756-199502000-00031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Deurveilher S, Hars B, Hennevin E. Pontine microinjection of carbachol does not reliably enhance paradoxical sleep in rats. Sleep (1997) 20:593–607.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

35. Garzon M, De Andres I, Reinoso-Suarez F. Sleep patterns after carbachol delivery in the ventral oral pontine tegmentum of the cat. Neuroscience (1998) 83:1137–44. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00494-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Marks GA, Birabil CG. Enhancement of rapid eye movement sleep in the rat by cholinergic and adenosinergic agonists infused into the pontine reticular formation. Neuroscience (1998) 86:29–37. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(98)00005-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kubin L. Carbachol models of REM sleep: recent developments and new directions. Arch Ital Biol (2001) 139:147–68.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

38. Mitler MM, Dement WC. Cataplectic-like behavior in cats after micro-injections of carbachol in pontine reticular formation. Brain Res (1974) 68:335–43. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(74)90402-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Shiromani PJ, McGinty DJ. Pontine neuronal response to local cholinergic infusion: relation to REM sleep. Brain Res (1986) 386:20–31. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(86)90137-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Lopez-Rodriguez F, Kohlmeier K, Morales FR, Chase MH. State dependency of the effects of microinjection of cholinergic drugs into the nucleus pontis oralis. Brain Res (1994) 649:271–81. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(94)91074-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Boissard R, Gervasoni D, Schmidt MH, Barbagli B, Fort P, Luppi PH. The rat ponto-medullary network responsible for paradoxical sleep onset and maintenance: a combined microinjection and functional neuroanatomical study. Eur J Neurosci (2002) 16:1959–73. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02257.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Pollock MS, Mistlberger RE. Microinjection of neostigmine into the pontine reticular formation of the mouse: further evaluation of a proposed REM sleep enhancement technique. Brain Res (2005) 1031:253–67. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2004.10.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Grace KP, Hughes SW, Horner RL. Identification of a pharmacological target for genioglossus reactivation throughout sleep. Sleep (2014) 37:41–50. doi:10.5665/sleep.3304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press (1998).

Google Scholar

45. Berman AL. The Brain Stem of the Cat: A Cytoarchitectonic Atlas With Stereotaxic Coordinates. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press (1968).

Google Scholar

46. Sastre JP, Sakai K, Jouvet M. Are the gigantocellular tegmental field neurons responsible for paradoxical sleep? Brain Res (1981) 229:147–61. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(81)90752-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Drucker-Colin R, Pedraza JG. Kainic acid lesions of gigantocellular tegmental field (FTG) neurons does not abolish REM sleep. Brain Res (1983) 272:387–91. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(83)90590-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Carli G, Zanchetti A. A study of pontine lesions suppressing deep sleep in the cat. Arch Ital Biol (1965) 103:751–88.

Google Scholar

49. Webster HH, Jones BE. Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsolateral pontomesencephalic tegmentum-cholinergic cell area in the cat. II. Effects upon sleep-waking states. Brain Res (1988) 458:285–302. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(88)90471-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Lu J, Sherman D, Devor M, Saper CB. A putative flip-flop switch for control of REM sleep. Nature (2006) 441:589–94. doi:10.1038/nature04767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Myers RD, Hoch DB. 14C-dopamine microinjected into the brain-stem of the rat: dispersion kinetics, site content and functional dose. Brain Res Bull (1978) 3:601–9. doi:10.1016/0361-9230(78)90006-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Macklis JD, Quattrochi JJ. Restricted diffusion and stability of carbachol-fluorescent nanospheres in-vivo. Neuroreport (1991) 2:247–50. doi:10.1097/00001756-199105000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Lai YY, Siegel JM. Pontomedullary glutamate receptors mediating locomotion and muscle tone suppression. J Neurosci (1991) 11:2931–7.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

54. Depoortere R, Di Scala G, Sandner G. Treadmill locomotion and aversive effects induced by electrical stimulation of the mesencephalic locomotor region in the rat. Brain Res Bull (1990) 25:723–7. doi:10.1016/0361-9230(90)90049-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Grace KP. How useful is optogenetic activation in determining neuronal function within dynamic circuits? Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2015) 112(29):E3755. doi:10.1073/pnas.1506188112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Van Dort CJ, Zachs DP, Kenny JD, Zheng S, Goldblum RR, Gelwan NA, et al. Optogenetic activation of cholinergic neurons in the PPT or LDT induces REM sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2015) 112:584–9. doi:10.1073/pnas.1423136112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Konadhode RR, Pelluru D, Blanco-Centurion C, Zayachkivsky A, Liu M, Uhde T, et al. Optogenetic stimulation of MCH neurons increases sleep. J Neurosci (2013) 33:10257–63. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1225-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Jego S, Glasgow SD, Herrera CG, Ekstrand M, Reed SJ, Boyce R, et al. Optogenetic identification of a rapid eye movement sleep modulatory circuit in the hypothalamus. Nat Neurosci (2013) 16:1637–43. doi:10.1038/nn.3522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Tsunematsu T, Ueno T, Tabuchi S, Inutsuka A, Tanaka KF, Hasuwa H, et al. Optogenetic manipulation of activity and temporally controlled cell-specific ablation reveal a role for MCH neurons in sleep/wake regulation. J Neurosci (2014) 34:6896–909. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5344-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Fraigne JJ, Peever JH. Melanin-concentrating hormone neurons promote and stabilize sleep. Sleep (2013) 36:1767–8. doi:10.5665/sleep.3186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Jones BE, Hassani OK. The role of Hcrt/Orx and MCH neurons in sleep-wake state regulation. Sleep (2013) 36:1769–72. doi:10.5665/sleep.3188

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Luppi PH, Peyron C, Fort P. Role of MCH neurons in paradoxical (REM) sleep control. Sleep (2013) 36:1775–6. doi:10.5665/sleep.3192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Grace KP, Vanstone LE, Horner RL. Endogenous cholinergic input to the pontine REM sleep generator is not required for REM sleep to occur. J Neurosci (2014) 34:14198–209. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0274-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. el Mansari M, Sakai K, Jouvet M. Unitary characteristics of presumptive cholinergic tegmental neurons during the sleep-waking cycle in freely moving cats. Exp Brain Res (1989) 76:519–29. doi:10.1007/BF00248908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Thakkar MM, Strecker RE, Mccarley RW. Behavioral state control through differential serotonergic inhibition in the mesopontine cholinergic nuclei: a simultaneous unit recording and microdialysis study. J Neurosci (1998) 18:5490–7.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

66. Datta S, Siwek DF. Single cell activity patterns of pedunculopontine tegmentum neurons across the sleep-wake cycle in the freely moving rats. J Neurosci Res (2002) 70:611–21. doi:10.1002/jnr.10405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Leonard TO, Lydic R. Pontine nitric oxide modulates acetylcholine release, rapid eye movement sleep generation, and respiratory rate. J Neurosci (1997) 17:774–85.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

68. Thakkar M, Portas C, Mccarley RW. Chronic low-amplitude electrical stimulation of the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus of freely moving cats increases REM sleep. Brain Res (1996) 723:223–7. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(96)00256-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Datta S, Siwek DF. Excitation of the brain stem pedunculopontine tegmentum cholinergic cells induces wakefulness and REM sleep. J Neurophysiol (1997) 77:2975–88.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

70. Datta S. Evidence that REM sleep is controlled by the activation of brain stem pedunculopontine tegmental kainate receptor. J Neurophysiol (2002) 87:1790–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

71. Lydic R, Baghdoyan HA. Pedunculopontine stimulation alters respiration and increases ACh release in the pontine reticular formation. Am J Physiol (1993) 264:R544–54.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

72. Imon H, Ito K, Dauphin L, Mccarley RW. Electrical stimulation of the cholinergic laterodorsal tegmental nucleus elicits scopolamine-sensitive excitatory postsynaptic potentials in medial pontine reticular formation neurons. Neuroscience (1996) 74:393–401. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(96)00134-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Shouse MN, Siegel JM. Pontine regulation of REM sleep components in cats: integrity of the pedunculopontine tegmentum (PPT) is important for phasic events but unnecessary for atonia during REM sleep. Brain Res (1992) 571:50–63. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(92)90508-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Torterolo P, Morales FR, Chase MH. GABAergic mechanisms in the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus of the cat promote active (REM) sleep. Brain Res (2002) 944:1–9. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02475-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Pal D, Mallick BN. GABA in pedunculo pontine tegmentum regulates spontaneous rapid eye movement sleep by acting on GABAA receptors in freely moving rats. Neurosci Lett (2004) 365:200–4. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2004.04.080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Pal D, Mallick BN. GABA in pedunculopontine tegmentum increases rapid eye movement sleep in freely moving rats: possible role of GABA-ergic inputs from substantia nigra pars reticulata. Neuroscience (2009) 164:404–14. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Petrovic J, Lazic K, Kalauzi A, Saponjic J. REM sleep diversity following the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus lesion in rat. Behav Brain Res (2014) 271:258–68. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2014.06.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Maloney KJ, Mainville L, Jones BE. Differential c-Fos expression in cholinergic, monoaminergic, and GABAergic cell groups of the pontomesencephalic tegmentum after paradoxical sleep deprivation and recovery. J Neurosci (1999) 19:3057–72.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

79. Verret L, Leger L, Fort P, Luppi PH. Cholinergic and noncholinergic brainstem neurons expressing Fos after paradoxical (REM) sleep deprivation and recovery. Eur J Neurosci (2005) 21:2488–504. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04060.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Boucetta S, Cisse Y, Mainville L, Morales M, Jones BE. Discharge profiles across the sleep-waking cycle of identified cholinergic, GABAergic, and glutamatergic neurons in the pontomesencephalic tegmentum of the rat. J Neurosci (2014) 34:4708–27. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2617-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Wang HL, Morales M. Pedunculopontine and laterodorsal tegmental nuclei contain distinct populations of cholinergic, glutamatergic and GABAergic neurons in the rat. Eur J Neurosci (2009) 29:340–58. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06576.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Torterolo P, Yamuy J, Sampogna S, Morales FR, Chase MH. GABAergic neurons of the laterodorsal and pedunculopontine tegmental nuclei of the cat express c-fos during carbachol-induced active sleep. Brain Res (2001) 892:309–19. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(00)03264-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Sapin E, Lapray D, Berod A, Goutagny R, Leger L, Ravassard P, et al. Localization of the brainstem GABAergic neurons controlling paradoxical (REM) sleep. PLoS One (2009) 4:e4272. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0004272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Clement O, Sapin E, Berod A, Fort P, Luppi PH. Evidence that neurons of the sublaterodorsal tegmental nucleus triggering paradoxical (REM) sleep are glutamatergic. Sleep (2011) 34:419–23.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

85. Steriade M, McCarley RW. Brainstem Control of Wakefulness and Sleep. New York, NY: Plenum Press (2005).

Google Scholar

86. Clark SD, Nothacker HP, Wang Z, Saito Y, Leslie FM, Civelli O. The urotensin II receptor is expressed in the cholinergic mesopontine tegmentum of the rat. Brain Res (2001) 923:120–7. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(01)03208-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Clark SD, Nothacker HP, Blaha CD, Tyler CJ, Duangdao DM, Grupke SL, et al. Urotensin II acts as a modulator of mesopontine cholinergic neurons. Brain Res (2005) 1059:139–48. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2005.08.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Huitron-Resendiz S, Kristensen MP, Sanchez-Alavez M, Clark SD, Grupke SL, Tyler C, et al. Urotensin II modulates rapid eye movement sleep through activation of brainstem cholinergic neurons. J Neurosci (2005) 25:5465–74. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4501-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Van Dort CJ, Brown EN. Reply to grace: role of cholinergic neurons in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep control. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2015) 112:E3756. doi:10.1073/pnas.1506854112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Clark SD, Alderson HL, Winn P, Latimer MP, Nothacker HP, Civelli O. Fusion of diphtheria toxin and urotensin II produces a neurotoxin selective for cholinergic neurons in the rat mesopontine tegmentum. J Neurochem (2007) 102:112–20. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.04529.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Grace KP, Liu H, Horner RL. 5-HT1A receptor-responsive pedunculopontine tegmental neurons suppress REM sleep and respiratory motor activity. J Neurosci (2012) 32:1622–33. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5700-10.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Benington JH, Heller HC. REM-sleep timing is controlled homeostatically by accumulation of REM-sleep propensity in non-REM sleep. Am J Physiol (1994) 266:R1992–2000.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

93. Benington JH, Kodali SK, Heller HC. Scoring transitions to REM sleep in rats based on the EEG phenomena of pre-REM sleep: an improved analysis of sleep structure. Sleep (1994) 17:28–36.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

94. McCarley RW, Hobson JA. Neuronal excitability modulation over the sleep cycle: a structural and mathematical model. Science (1975) 189:58–60. doi:10.1126/science.1135627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Hobson JA, Mccarley RW, Wyzinski PW. Sleep cycle oscillation: reciprocal discharge by two brainstem neuronal groups. Science (1975) 189:55–8. doi:10.1126/science.1094539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Steriade M, McCarley RW. REM sleep as a biological rhythm: the phenomenology and a structural and mathematical model with application to depression. 2nd ed. Brainstem Control of Wakefulness and Sleep. New York, NY: Plenum Press (2005). p. 513–60.

Google Scholar

97. Armstrong DM, Saper CB, Levey AI, Wainer BH, Terry RD. Distribution of cholinergic neurons in rat brain: demonstrated by the immunocytochemical localization of choline acetyltransferase. J Comp Neurol (1983) 216:53–68. doi:10.1002/cne.902160106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Pace-Schott EF, Hobson JA. The neurobiology of sleep: genetics, cellular physiology and subcortical networks. Nat Rev Neurosci (2002) 3:591–605. doi:10.1038/nrn895

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Steriade M, McCarley RW. Neuronal control of REM sleep. 2nd ed. Brainstem Control of Wakefulness and Sleep. New York, NY: Plenum Press (2005). p. 461–511.

Google Scholar

100. Massaquoi SG, McCarley RW. Extension of the limit cycle reciprocal interaction model of REM cycle control. An integrated sleep control model. J Sleep Res (1992) 1:138–43. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.1992.tb00027.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Brandman O, Meyer T. Feedback loops shape cellular signals in space and time. Science (2008) 322:390–5. doi:10.1126/science.1160617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Dement W, Greenberg S, Klein R. The effect of partial REM sleep deprivation and delayed recovery. J Psychiatr Res (1966) 4:141–52. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(66)90003-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Morden B, Mitchell G, Dement W. Selective REM sleep deprivation and compensation phenomena in the rat. Brain Res (1967) 5:339–49. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(67)90042-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Brunner DP, Dijk DJ, Tobler I, Borbely AA. Effect of partial sleep deprivation on sleep stages and EEG power spectra: evidence for non-REM and REM sleep homeostasis. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol (1990) 75:492–9. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(90)90136-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Endo T, Roth C, Landolt HP, Werth E, Aeschbach D, Achermann P, et al. Selective REM sleep deprivation in humans: effects on sleep and sleep EEG. Am J Physiol (1998) 274:R1186–94.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

106. Boissard R, Fort P, Gervasoni D, Barbagli B, Luppi PH. Localization of the GABAergic and non-GABAergic neurons projecting to the sublaterodorsal nucleus and potentially gating paradoxical sleep onset. Eur J Neurosci (2003) 18:1627–39. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02861.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Sastre JP, Buda C, Kitahama K, Jouvet M. Importance of the ventrolateral region of the periaqueductal gray and adjacent tegmentum in the control of paradoxical sleep as studied by muscimol microinjections in the cat. Neuroscience (1996) 74:415–26. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(96)00190-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Crochet S, Onoe H, Sakai K. A potent non-monoaminergic paradoxical sleep inhibitory system: a reverse microdialysis and single-unit recording study. Eur J Neurosci (2006) 24:1404–12. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04995.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Xi MC, Morales FR, Chase MH. Evidence that wakefulness and REM sleep are controlled by a GABAergic pontine mechanism. J Neurophysiol (1999) 82:2015–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

110. Pollock MS, Mistlberger RE. Rapid eye movement sleep induction by microinjection of the GABA-A antagonist bicuculline into the dorsal subcoeruleus area of the rat. Brain Res (2003) 962:68–77. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(02)03956-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Sanford LD, Tang X, Xiao J, Ross RJ, Morrison AR. GABAergic regulation of REM sleep in reticularis pontis oralis and caudalis in rats. J Neurophysiol (2003) 90:938–45. doi:10.1152/jn.00993.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Dunmyre JR, Mashour GA, Booth V. Coupled flip-flop model for REM sleep regulation in the rat. PLoS One (2014) 9:e94481. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0094481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Rempe MJ, Best J, Terman D. A mathematical model of the sleep/wake cycle. J Math Biol (2010) 60:615–44. doi:10.1007/s00285-009-0276-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Kumar R, Bose A, Mallick BN. A mathematical model towards understanding the mechanism of neuronal regulation of wake-NREMS-REMS states. PLoS One (2012) 7:e42059. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0042059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Quattrochi JJ, Mamelak AN, Madison RD, Macklis JD, Hobson JA. Mapping neuronal inputs to REM sleep induction sites with carbachol-fluorescent microspheres. Science (1989) 245:984–6. doi:10.1126/science.2475910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Semba K, Reiner PB, Fibiger HC. Single cholinergic mesopontine tegmental neurons project to both the pontine reticular formation and the thalamus in the rat. Neuroscience (1990) 38:643–54. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(90)90058-C

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Semba K, Fibiger HC. Afferent connections of the laterodorsal and the pedunculopontine tegmental nuclei in the rat: a retro- and antero-grade transport and immunohistochemical study. J Comp Neurol (1992) 323:387–410. doi:10.1002/cne.903230307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Lydic R, Baghdoyan HA, Lorinc Z. Microdialysis of cat pons reveals enhanced acetylcholine release during state-dependent respiratory depression. Am J Physiol (1991) 261:R766–70.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

119. Wedlich-Soldner R, Wai SC, Schmidt T, Li R. Robust cell polarity is a dynamic state established by coupling transport and GTPase signaling. J Cell Biol (2004) 166:889–900. doi:10.1083/jcb.200405061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Lewis RS. Calcium signaling mechanisms in T lymphocytes. Annu Rev Immunol (2001) 19:497–521. doi:10.1146/annurev.immunol.19.1.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Abrieu A, Doree M, Fisher D. The interplay between cyclin-B-Cdc2 kinase (MPF) and MAP kinase during maturation of oocytes. J Cell Sci (2001) 114:257–67.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

122. Brandman O, Ferrell JE Jr, Li R, Meyer T. Interlinked fast and slow positive feedback loops drive reliable cell decisions. Science (2005) 310:496–8. doi:10.1126/science.1113834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: acetylcholine, REM sleep, pons, gain-of-function, loss-of-function, sleep dynamics, cholinomimetic

Citation: Grace KP and Horner RL (2015) Evaluating the evidence surrounding pontine cholinergic involvement in REM sleep generation. Front. Neurol. 6:190. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00190

Received: 15 March 2015; Accepted: 17 August 2015;
Published: 01 September 2015

Edited by:

Jun Lu, Harvard Medical School, USA

Reviewed by:

Axel Steiger, Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry, Germany
Pablo Torterolo, Universidad de la República, Uruguay

Copyright: © 2015 Grace and Horner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kevin P. Grace, Department of Medicine, University of Toronto, Room 3206 Medical Sciences Building, 1 King’s College Circle, Toronto, ON M5S 1A8, Canada, kevin.grace@utoronto.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.