Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 08 March 2013
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00050

Peer influence: neural mechanisms underlying in-group conformity

  • 1Department of Marketing Management, Rotterdam School of Management, Erasmus University, Rotterdam, Netherlands
  • 2Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands

People often conform to the behavior of others with whom they identify. However, it is unclear what fundamental mechanisms underlie this type of conformity. Here, we investigate the processes mediating in-group conformity by using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Participants completed a perceptual decision-making task while undergoing fMRI, during which they were exposed to the judgments of both in-group and out-group members. Our data suggest that conformity to the in-group is mediated by both positive affect as well as the cognitive capacity of perspective taking. Examining the processes that drive in-group conformity by utilizing a basic decision-making paradigm combined with neuroimaging methods provides important insights into the potential mechanisms of conformity. These results may provide an integral step in developing more effective campaigns using group conformity as a tool for behavioral change.

Introduction

People are often influenced by others with whom they identify. They buy clothes similar to those of their peers, visit restaurants because their colleagues go there, and download music their friends listen to. By adopting the tastes of others, people show they belong to a specific group. This social factor, whereby people follow the behavior or advice of others they associate with, has been labeled in-group influence. It is not limited to product choice (Bearden and Etzel, 1982; Berger and Heath, 2007, 2008), but influences behavior even when identity signaling is not an issue. For instance, a field experiment on household energy conservation showed that informing people about their neighborhood's average home energy usage resulted in a change in household energy consumption, specifically toward the mean of their neighborhood (Schultz et al., 2007). Similarly, a study on conservation behavior found that hotel guests were more likely to reuse towels when informed that guests who had stayed in that same room had reused towels than if they were informed about the behavior of guests in general (Goldstein et al., 2008).

Given the powerful influence of the in-group, it therefore comes as no surprise that there has been an increase in the use of group conformity as a tool for behavioral change. People often overestimate both the degree of approval and the prevalence of negative behavior among peers, such as drinking, drug use, violence, littering, or cigarette smoking (Baer et al., 1991; Donaldson et al., 1994; Schultz, 1999; Neighbors et al., 2004; Berkowitz, 2010). Social influence-programs seek to correct these misperceptions by exposing their target groups to the actual attitudes of their peers and the real frequency of the undesirable behaviors. However, despite the initial popularity of these programs, the evidence for their success in establishing behavioral change has been mixed. Over time, many programs failed to change behavior substantially (Peeler et al., 2000; Clapp et al., 2003), and some social influence-programs even showed effects of increasing the undesirable behavior they tried to reduce (Granfield, 2005; Wechsler et al., 2003). The mixed findings on the effectiveness of social influence-programs demonstrate that it is still unclear exactly what psychological processes may mediate in-group conformity. In order to understand why and when people conform to their in-group, we need to understand the mechanisms that drive in-group conformity.

The aim of the present study was to gain greater insight into the processes underlying in-group conformity. To examine the mechanism of in-group conformity, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), a modern neuroscientific method that provides a non-invasive measure of neural activity by assessing regional changes in blood oxygenation [blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) response]. Using fMRI enables us to make inferences about the processes that underlie in-group conformity, which is difficult to assess using behavioral measures alone. In addition, to investigate the basic underlying processes, we measured in-group conformity using an artificial group manipulation and using a domain that was neither relevant for identity signaling nor related to actual choice. Examining the neural processes driving in-group conformity under these minimal conditions provides fundamental insights into the basic brain mechanisms, and may help in designing more effective social norm campaigns.

Although the application of neuroimaging methods in decision-making research has increased in popularity during the last decade, only recently have neuroscientists started to identify the brain networks implicated in social influence, for example examining the influence of experts (Klucharev et al., 2008; Campbell-Meiklejohn et al., 2010), the persuasiveness of celebrities (Stallen et al., 2010), the mechanisms of racial bias (Beer et al., 2008; Van Bavel et al., 2008; Gonsalkorale et al., 2011), the influence of majority behavior on individual decision-making (Berns et al., 2005, 2010; Klucharev et al., 2009, 2011; Mason et al., 2009), and, most relevant to the current investigation, the influence of in-group membership on both money allocation (Volz et al., 2009) and helping behavior (Hein et al., 2010). Volz and colleagues (2009) investigated the neural implementation of social identity theory, which assumes that each individual has both a personal and a social identity, and that the way information is processed depends on which identity of the individual is salient at the time of decision-making (Tajfel and Turner, 1986). The results of Volz and colleagues (2009) support social identity theory by demonstrating that the social self is derived from the same cognitive processes as the individual self, as activation of both types of identities resulted in similar neural patterns in the prefrontal and parietal network. A second study on in-group influence by Hein and colleagues (2010) investigated the neurobiological basis of the decision to help either an in-group or out-group member in pain. Their results showed that seeing an in-group member in pain evoked more empathy-related responses in the brain than seeing an out-group member in pain, and demonstrated that the degree of this empathy-related response predicted in-group favoritism in actual helping behavior at a later point in time. Importantly however, none of these studies on social influence in the brain examined the processes that underlie conformity to the in-group.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Design

Twenty-eight healthy right-handed participants (mean age 20.7 years) took part in the experiment. All were free of neurological or psychiatric illness, head trauma or drug abuse, and none were taking medication. Written informed consent was obtained according to the local medical ethics committee, and participants were compensated financially. Data from three participants were discarded due to technical problems, and one participant was excluded because he guessed the study aim. This resulted in 24 subjects for final analyses (12 males). We used a repeated measures design with the identity of the group member (in-group or out-group member) as a single within-subject factor.

Materials and Methods

Subjects arrived alone to the experiment. Upon arrival, participants' group membership was manipulated using a minimal group paradigm approach (Tajfel et al., 1971). In the task, adapted from Volz et al. (2009), five perceptual illusions, such as the young girl-old woman illusion, were shown for 2 s each, After each illusion, two possible answers were displayed on the screen and participants were asked to choose between them. Then, participants were informed that they had been categorized as people who either focus on the foreground of visual illusions, people who focus on the background, or as people who could not be classified into either of these two categories. Unbeknownst to participants, everyone was classified as a foreground perceiver (in-group). The other two groups (background and unclassified) will be referred to as the out-group. We manipulated group membership artificially instead of using real, existing groups, as this allowed us to control the (minimal) information participants had about their in-group and out-group members, and hence ensured that the hypothesized in-group conformity effect could not be explained by factors other than group membership, such as for example perceived differences in expertise in perceptual decision-making.

Decision-Making Task

After the perceptual illusion task, participants completed the decision-making task while undergoing MRI (Figure 1). First, participants were instructed to look at a dot pattern on a computer screen for 1.5 s. The number of dots on display ranged from 5 to 30 (M = 15, SD = 7.5), and the participants' goal was to estimate the number of dots as accurately as possible. The number of dots used was based on pre-tests conducted with a different set of participants (N = 42). Pre-tests showed that, on average, participants were able to estimate about 11 dots (SD = 2.2 dots) correctly within 1.5 s. Because we required our experiment to include both easy and difficult trials (easy trials were included to ensure motivation), we varied the number of dots from 5 to 30 across trials. After the brief presentation of the dots, participants were instructed to think about their estimate (duration jittered between 2.5 and 6 s). Next, the estimate of a previous participant was displayed. This estimate came from either a member of the same group as the participant, that is, a foreground perceiver (in-group member) or from a member of a different group (out-group member). Group membership of the other participant (foreground perceiver, background perceiver or unclassified perceiver) was indicated by a colored cartoon of either yellow, purple or blue. Colors were counterbalanced to ensure no confound between the color of the cartoon and group membership.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Trial outline with duration times in seconds. Group membership of the other participant was indicated by the colored cartoon.

After presentation of the estimates, a response screen appeared. This screen was identical to the previous screen on which the estimate was presented, except for a response bar displayed at the bottom of the screen. This bar consisted of a row of numbers from 5 to 30, on which participants were instructed to indicate their estimate. Responses were indicated by scrolling to the number of their choice and pressing a confirmation button. The estimates provided by in-group and out-group members were predetermined by a computer script and, unbeknownst to the participant, were always correct. Finally, to enhance motivation, participants were told that the group who performed best would win an (unspecified) prize, with the winning group notified at the end of the study.

The presentation of a fixation screen (duration jittered between 3.5 to 5 s) signaled the start of a new trial. Participants performed 214 trials. To maintain attention, 6 self-paced breaks were included. The total scanning session took approx. 55 min.

MRI Acquisition Parameters

Functional images were acquired with a 1.5T Siemens Sonata scanner, using an ascending slice acquisition and a T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR 2.29 s, TE 30 ms, flip angle 70°, slice matrix 64 × 64 mm, slice thickness 3.0 mm, slice gap 0.5 mm, FOV 224 mm). Anatomical scans were acquired with a T1-weighted MP-RAGE sequence (176 sagittal slices, TR 2.25 s, TE 3.93 ms, flip angle 15°, slice matrix 256 × 256, slice thickness 1.0 mm, no gap, FOV 256 mm).

Dependent Variables

Behavioral questionnaires

To test the group manipulation, participants answered a questionnaire at the end of the experiment. This measured the level of identification (“I feel connected to the blue/yellow/purple team”), trust (“I trust people from the blue/yellow/purple team”), and the degree of positive associations (“I have positive associations with the blue/yellow/purple team”) toward other participants. Responses ranged from 1 (not true at all) to 7 (very true).

Conformity

Conformity was assessed by calculating the percentage of trials on which participants gave the same judgments as the in-group or out-group member.

Brain imaging analyses

Data were preprocessed and analyzed using a standard software package (SPM8, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology London). The first 5 images of each participant's EPI sequence were discarded to allow for longitudinal relaxation time. The remaining images were realigned to the first imaging volume. Functional images were corrected for motion and differences in slice time acquisition. Next, images were normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template using parameters defined from the normalization of the anatomical scan to the MNI template, and images were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full-width at half-maximum to reduce noise. Motion parameters were stored and used as nuisance variables in the general linear model (GLM) analysis. A random-effects analysis within the framework of the GLM was applied to model event-related responses (Friston et al., 1995).

Four regressors were defined for each participant based on the onsets of the relevant trials: “Conformity to In-group,” “Conformity to Out-group,” “Non-Conformity to In-group,” and “Non Conformity to Out-group.” Brain responses were time-locked to the presentation of the estimate of either the in-group or out-group member. Regressors were modeled with a canonical hemodynamic response function and a GLM analysis was used to create contrast images summarizing differences in brain activity across the Conformity to In-group and Non-Conformity to In-group trials, as well as differences in brain activity across the Conformity to Out-group and Non-Conformity to Out-group trials. To test hypotheses regarding brain areas that were uniquely involved in conformity to an in-group member, we masked the brain activity present in the In-group contrast map (Conformity to In-group > Non-Conformity to In-group) with the Out-group contrast map (Conformity to Out-group > Non-Conformity to Out-group) (p < 0.05 uncorrected) (e.g., Pochon et al., 2002; Uncapher et al., 2006; Enzi et al., 2009). This exclusive masking procedure revealed activity in the In-group contrast map that did not overlap with the brain areas involved in the Out-group contrast map (p < 0.001, uncorrected, 10-voxel minimum). To assess whether there was a relationship between brain activity underlying conformity and the self-report measures assessed, we correlated individual beta values of the reported brain activity with participants' scores on the scales measuring identification, positive associations, and trust toward in-group and out-group members.

Results

Manipulation Check

In line with the group manipulation, participants identified more strongly with in-group members (M = 4.7, SD = 1.0) than with out-group members (M = 3.2, SD = 1.0), t(23) = 5.4, p < 0.001 (paired t-test). There were no differences in identification between the two out-groups, that is, participants identified equally with out-group members that were classified as background perceivers (M = 3.3, SD = 1.1) or that were not classified (M = 3.2, SD = 1.2), t(23) = 0.6, ns. Consistent with an in-group preference, participants had more positive associations with in-group members (M = 5.8, SD = 0.6) than with out-group members (M = 4.9, SD = 0.9), t(23) = 4.3, p < 0.001, and participants reported greater trust in in-group members (M = 4.9, SD = 1.1) than in out-group members (M = 4.1, SD = 1.0), t(23) = 3.3, p < 0.005.

Behavioral Conformity

Participants conformed more often to in-group judgments than to out-group judgments. The percentage of trials in which participants' judgment matched the estimate of the group member was higher when seeing the estimate of an in-group member (M = 67.8%, SD = 9.4%) than an out-group member (M = 65.4%, SD = 9.2%), t(23) = 2.8, p < 0.01. In-group conformity did not differ between easy (≤11 dots), and difficult trials, t(23) = 0.5, n.s.

Neural Correlates of in-Group Conformity (Table 1)

When examining brain areas exclusively involved in conformity to the in-group, we found a significant increase in activity in right caudate, subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (subACC), right hippocampus, and in the intersection of the right posterior insula and the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) (Figure 2). Analyses of the In-group contrast (Conformity to In-group > Non-Conformity to In-group) and Out-group contrast (Conformity to Out-group > Non-Conformity to Out-group) directly did not reveal any significant activation patterns. Next, we calculated whether there were any correlations between the neural activity underlying in-group conformity and participants' self-reports on in-group trust and associations. Correlation analyses were conducted for each brain region found to be involved in in-group conformity, and corrected for multiple comparisons accordingly (Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.0125). We found that the activity in the posterior insula/pSTS positively correlated with participants' scores on the trustworthiness of in-group members (r = 0.53, p < 0.01). Thus, the more trustworthy participants' judged their in-group, the higher the activity in this region. No other significant correlations were found.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Significant areas of activation associated with conformity toward in-group members.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Brain regions involved in in-group conformity, p < 0.001 uncorrected. (A) subgenual ACC, x = 0; (B) pSTS/insula (circled in red) and hippocampus (circled in yellow), x = 40; (C) pSTS/insula (circled in red) and caudate (circled in green), z = 4.

Discussion

To examine the basic processes that mediate in-group conformity, we explored the neural mechanisms underlying this effect. Activity in the caudate was selectively enhanced when participants conformed to the in-group, supporting the hypothesis that the striatum plays an important role in social influence (Klucharev et al., 2009; Campbell-Meiklejohn et al., 2010; Zaki et al., 2011). The striatum, located in the center of the brain, is a major input station for midbrain dopamine neurons and plays a primary role in the processing of rewards, including primary rewards such as liquids, foods, and sexual stimuli (Redouté et al., 2000; Berns et al., 2001; O'Doherty et al., 2003), as well as to money (Knutson et al., 2000) and more abstract rewards such as reputation or status (Izuma et al., 2008; Zink et al., 2008). The finding that the striatum is involved in in-group conformity, in conjunction with conformity-related activations in other low-level processing areas such as the subACC, an area implicated in the experience of affective states (Drevets et al., 1997), and the hippocampus, an area important for the retrieval of spatial memories (such as the dot display) (e.g., Eldridge et al., 2000), suggests that in-group conformity is mediated by fundamental value signals in the brain. Importantly, involvement of the subACC suggests that affective signals may be related to the positive experience of social inclusion in particular, as this brain region has been implicated in social acceptance (Somerville et al., 2006), and also shown to be more active for individuals low in rejection sensitivity (Burklund et al., 2007). Taken together, these findings suggest that people conform more to in-group members than to out-group members because the behavior of in-group members is more strongly associated with the experience of positive affect and reward.

Greater activity for in-group conformity was also found in a region bordering the pSTS and the posterior insula, with peak activity in the posterior insula but extending further into pSTS. The pSTS is an area often implicated in the cognitive capacity of perspective taking, typically termed Theory of Mind (Frith and Frith, 2006). The concept of Theory of Mind is defined as the understanding that others have their own individual perspective on the world, which may differ from your own. Finding that the BOLD response in the pSTS is selectively enhanced for in-group conformity is interesting, as this could imply that participants took the perspective of the other more when the other was an in-group member than an out-group member. This hypothesis supports previous work suggesting that people mentalize more about in-group than out-group members (Harris and Fiske, 2006; Freeman et al., 2010; Heatherton, 2011). Moreover, activity in the pSTS correlated with participants' self-report measures on the perceived trustworthiness of the in-group, again indicating that those who reported strong feelings of trust toward their in-group were more in-tune with the mental state of their in-group member.

The present findings complement behavioral studies (e.g., Asch, 1951; Deutsch and Gerard, 1955; Cialdini and Goldstein, 2004; Jetten et al., 2004) and recent pharmacological work (Stallen et al., 2012) on group influence, and expand on investigations of the neural bases of both conformity (Berns et al., 2001, 2010; Klucharev et al., 2009, 2011; Mason et al., 2009; Campbell-Meiklejohn et al., 2010; Zaki et al., 2011; Berns and Moore, 2012), and in-group influence (Volz et al., 2009; Hein et al., 2010). Furthermore, our results provide potential relevant insights for the design of social influence programs. We show that conformity to the in-group is presumably mediated by both positive affect as well as perspective taking. This suggests that social influence programs may benefit by emphasizing the positive aspects associated with in-group membership rather than, for instance, stressing the negative feelings associated with social exclusion. Additionally, our data suggest that social influence-programs will work more effectively when the target is stimulated to imagine the state of mind of the in-group and “puts himself in the others” shoes', thereby facilitating perspective-taking processes which may result in more trust directed toward in-group information.

Future research could productively test these hypotheses, as the present effects are small and the interpretations here are based on previous research linking activity in specific brain regions to basic cognitive functioning. In general, the ability to assess with certainty the cognitive processing reflected by specific brain activity is challenging due to the multiple functions brain regions typically engage in (Poldrack, 2006). Follow-up behavioral and neuroimaging studies can reveal how the basic mechanisms of in-group conformity reported here are modulated by different contexts, in particular participant population and decision-making domain. For instance, the conformity effect reported here is quite small, likely due to the minimal conditions under which in-group conformity was tested. However, when using natural groups, such as friends or sports teams, and when measuring conformity in a decision-making domain more closely related to identity formation, such as consumption choice for clothing, music, hairstyle, or food (Bearden and Etzel, 1982; Berger and Heath, 2007, 2008), the present in-group conformity bias would likely be stronger. In-group conformity in contexts more relevant to identity formation may not only be mediated by mechanisms of positive affect and perspective taking as reported here, but by the activation of social identity processes as well. A candidate brain region for these processes is the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, as previous research has found this area to be implicated in the activation of self and group identity and to correlate with a behavioral in-group bias (Volz et al., 2009). In addition, our findings encourage the study of in-group conformity across different age ranges. We found in-group conformity to be mediated by increased activity in the subACC, an area known to be involved in social inclusion (Somerville et al., 2006; Burklund et al., 2007) and positive affect (Kim et al., 2003; Sharot et al., 2007) in adults. However, in adolescents the subACC has been found to correlate with social exclusion (Masten et al., 2009). This may predict that while in-group conformity in adults is primarily driven by the positive affect associated with social inclusion, in-group conformity in adolescents might be driven more by the negative affect associated with social exclusion. Social influence campaigns targeted at adolescents may therefore be more effective when emphasizing the negative aspects of social exclusion than the positive affect associated with social inclusion.

Conclusion

The present findings complement recent work on the physiological bases of both conformity (Breiter et al., 2001; Klucharev et al., 2009, 2011; Mason et al., 2009; Berns et al., 2010; Campbell-Meiklejohn et al., 2010; Zaki et al., 2011), and in-group influence (Volz et al., 2009; Hein et al., 2010; Stallen et al., 2012). The current study is a first step toward understanding the nature of actual in-group conformity behavior, and provides a first insight into what mechanisms may drive this effect. Our data indicate that both positive associations linked to in-group members, as well as the ability to take the perspective of the in-group, likely play an important role in in-group conformity. Understanding why group membership has such a profound influence on decision-making provides a window into one of the basic motivations underlying people's behavior, and may help in developing effective campaigns based on a social influence approach.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Asch, S. (ed.). (1951). Effects of Group Pressure Upon the Modification and Distortion of Judgments. Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Press.

Baer, J. S., Stacy, A., and Larimer, M. (1991). Biases in the perception of drinking norms among college-students. J. Stud. Alcohol 52, 580–586.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bearden, W. O., and Etzel, M. J. (1982). Reference group influence on product and brand purchase decisions. J. Consum. Res. 9, 183–194.

Beer, J. S., Stallen, M., Lombardo, M. V., Gonsalkorale, K., Cunningham, W. A., and Sherman, J. W. (2008). The Quadruple Process model approach to examining the neural underpinnings of prejudice. Neuroimage 43, 775–783.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berger, J., and Heath, C. (2007). Where consumers diverge from others: identity signaling and product domains. J. Consum. Res. 34, 121–134.

Berger, J., and Heath, C. (2008). Who drives divergence? Identity signaling, outgroup dissimilarity, and the abandonment of cultural tastes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 593–607.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berkowitz, A. D. (2010). “Fostering healthy norms to prevent violence and abuse: the social norms approach,” in The Prevention of Sexual Violence: A Practitioner's Sourcebook, ed K. Kaufman (Holyoke, MA: NEARI Press), 147–172.

Berns, G. S., Capra, C. M., Moore, S., and Noussair, C. (2010). Neural mechanisms of the influence of popularity on adolescent ratings of music. Neuroimage 49, 2687–2696.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berns, G. S., Chappelow, J., Zink, C. F., Pagnoni, G., Martin-Skurski, M. E., and Richards, J. (2005). Neurobiological correlates of social conformity and independence during mental rotation. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 245–253.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berns, G. S., McClure, S. M., Pagnoni, G., and Montague, P. R. (2001). Predictability modulates human brain response to reward. J. Neurosci. 21, 2793–2798.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Berns, G. S., and Moore, S. E. (2012). A neural predictor of cultural popularity. J. Consum. Psychol. 22, 154–160.

Breiter, H. C., Aharon, I., Kahneman, D., Dale, A., and Shizgal, P. (2001). Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience of monetary gains and losses. Neuron 30, 619–639.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burklund, L. J., Eisenberger, N. I., and Lieberman, M. D. (2007). The face of rejection: rejection sensitivity moderates dorsal anterior cingulate activity to disapproving facial expressions. Soc. Neurosci. 2, 238–253.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Campbell-Meiklejohn, D. K., Bach, D. R., Roepstorff, A., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2010). How the opinion of others affects our valuation of objects. Curr. Biol. 20, 1165–1170.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cialdini, R. B., and Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: compliance and conformity. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55, 591–621.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Clapp, J. D., Lange, J. E., Russell, C., Shillington, A., and Voas, R. (2003). A failed norms social marketing campaign. J. Stud. Alcohol 64, 409–414.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Deutsch, M., and Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and social influences upon individual judgments. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 51, 629–636.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Donaldson, S. I., Graham, J. W., and Hansen, W. B. (1994). Testing the generalizability of intervening mechanism theories: understanding the effects of adolescent drug use prevention interventions. J. Behav. Med. 17, 195–216.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Drevets, W. C., Price, J. L., Simpson, J. R., Todd, R. D., Reich, T., Vannier, M., et al. (1997). Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders. Nature 386, 824–827.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eldridge, L. L., Knowlton, B. J., Furmanski, C. S., Bookheimer, S. Y., and Engel, S. A. (2000). Remembering episodes: a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1149–1152.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Enzi, B., de Greck, M., Prösch, U., Tempelmann, C., and Northoff, G. (2009). Is our self nothing but reward? Neuronal overlap and distinction between reward and personal relevance and its relation to human personality. PLoS ONE 4:e8429. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008429

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Freeman, J. B., Schiller, D., Rule, N. O., and Ambady, N. (2010). The neural origins of superficial and individuated judgments about ingroup and outgroup members. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 150–159.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S. J., and Turner, R. (1995). Characterizing dynamic brain responses with fMRI– a multivariate approach. Neuroimage 2, 166–172.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frith, C. D., and Frith, U. (2006). How we predict what other people are going to do. Brain Res. 1079, 36–46.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B., and Griskevicius, V. (2008). A room with a viewpoint: using social norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. J. Consum. Res. 35, 472–482.

Gonsalkorale, K., Sherman, J. W., Allen, T. J., Klauer, K. C., and Amodio, D. M. (2011). Accounting for successful control of implicit racial bias: the roles of association activation, response monitoring, and overcoming bias. Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 37, 1534–1545.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Granfield, R. (2005). Alcohol use in college: limitations on the transformation of social norms. Addict. Res. Theory 13, 281–292.

Harris, L. T., and Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low. Psychol. Sci. 17, 847–853.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 363–390.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hein, G., Silani, G., Preuschoff, K., Batson, C. D., and Singer, T. (2010). Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members' suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron 68, 149–160.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Izuma, K., Saito, D. N., and Sadato, N. (2008). Processing of social and monetary rewards in the human striatum. Neuron 58, 284–294.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jetten, J., Spears, R., and Postmes, T. (2004). Intergroup distinctiveness and differentiation: a meta-analytic integration. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 86, 862–879.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, M. J., Chey, J., Chung, A., Bae, S., Khang, H., Ham, B., et al. (2003). Diminished rostral anterior cingulate activity in response to threat-related events in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 268–277.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klucharev, V., Hytonen, K., Rijpkema, M., Smidts, A., and Fernandez, G. (2009). Reinforcement learning signal predicts social conformity. Neuron 61, 140–151.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klucharev, V., Munneke, M. A. M., Smidts, A., and Fernandez, G. (2011). Downregulation of the posterior medial frontal cortex prevents social conformity. J. Neurosci. 31, 11934–11940.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klucharev, V., Smidts, A., and Fernandez, G. (2008). Brain mechanisms of persuasion: how ‘expert power’ modulates memory and attitudes. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 353–366.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Knutson, B., Westdorp, A., Kaiser, E., and Hommer, D. (2000). FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. Neuroimage 12, 20–27.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mason, M. F., Dyer, R., and Norton, M. I. (2009). Neural mechanisms of social influence. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 110, 152–159.

Masten, C. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Borofsky, L. A., Pfeifer, J. H., McNealy, K., Mazziotta, J. C., et al. (2009). Neural correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: understanding the distress of peer rejection. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 143–157.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neighbors, C., Larimer, M. E., and Lewis, M. A. (2004). Targeting misperceptions of descriptive drinking norms: efficacy of a computer-delivered personalized normative feedback intervention. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 72, 434–447.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Friston, K., Critchley, H., and Dolan, R. J. (2003). Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain. Neuron 38, 329–337.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Peeler, C. M., Far, J., Miller, J., and Brigham, T. A. (2000). An analysis of the effects of a program to reduce heavy drinking among college students. J. Alcohol Drug Educ. 45, 39–54.

Pochon, J. B., Levy, R., Fossati, P., Lehericy, S., Poline, J. B., Pillon, B., et al. (2002). The neural system that bridges reward and cognition in humans: an fMRI study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 5669–5674.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 59–63.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Redouté, J., Stoléru, S., Grégoire, M. C., Costes, N., Cinotti, L., Lavenne, F., et al. (2000). Brain processing of visual sexual stimuli in human males. Hum. Brain Mapp. 11, 162–177.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Schultz, P. (1999). Changing behavior with normative feedback interventions: a field experiment on curbside recycling. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 21, 25–36.

Schultz, P. W., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., and Griskevicius, V. (2007). The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychol. Sci. 18, 429–434.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M., and Phelps, E. A. (2007). Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature 450, 102–105.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Somerville, L. H., Heatherton, T. F., and Kelley, W. M. (2006). Anterior cingulate cortex responds differentially to expectancy violation and social rejection. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1007–1008.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stallen, M., de Dreu, C. K. W., Shalvi, S., Smidts, A., and Sanfey, A. G. (2012). The herding hormone: oxytocin stimulates in-group conformity. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1288–1292.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stallen, M., Smidts, A., Rijpkema, M., Smit, G., Klucharev, V., and Fernandez, G. (2010). Celebrities and shoes on the female brain: the neural correlates of product evaluation in the context of fame. J. Econ. Psychol. 31, 802–811.

Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., and Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1, 149–178.

Tajfel, H., and Turner, J. C. (1986). “The social identity theory of intergroup behavior,” in Psychology of Intergroup Relations, eds S. Worchel and W. G. Austing (Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall), 7–24.

Uncapher, M. R., Otten, L. J., and Rugg, M. D. (2006). Episodic encoding is more than the sum of its parts: an fMRI investigation of multifeatural contextual encoding. Neuron 52, 547–556.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Bavel, J. J., Packer, D. J., and Cunningham, W. A. (2008). The neural substrates of in-group bias. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1131–1139.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Volz, K. G., Kessler, T., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2009). In-group as part of the self: in-group favoritism is mediated by medial prefrontal cortex activation. Soc. Neurosci. 4, 244–260.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wechsler, H., Nelson, T., Lee, J. E., Seiberg, M., Lewis, C., and Keeling, R. (2003). Perception and reality: a national evaluation of social norms marketing interventions to reduce college students' heavy alcohol use. Q. J. Stud. Alcohol 64, 484–494.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zaki, J., Schirmer, J., and Mitchell, J. P. (2011). Social influence modulates the neural computation of value. Psychol. Sci. 22, 894–900.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zink, C. F., Tong, Y., Chen, Q., Bassett, D. S., Stein, J. L., and Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2008). Know your place: neural processing of social hierarchy in humans. Neuron 58, 273–283.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: conformity, in-group bias, MRI imaging, judgment and decision-making, social influence

Citation: Stallen M, Smidts A and Sanfey AG (2013) Peer influence: neural mechanisms underlying in-group conformity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:50. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00050

Received: 30 September 2012; Paper pending published: 06 November 2012;
Accepted: 06 February 2013; Published online: 08 March 2013.

Edited by:

Leonie Koban, University of Colorado Boulder, USA

Reviewed by:

Vasily Klucharev, University of Basel, Switzerland
Grit Hein, University of Zurich, Switzerland

Copyright © 2013 Stallen, Smidts and Sanfey. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Mirre Stallen, Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Room 1.40, PO Box 9101, 6500 HB Nijmegen, Netherlands. e-mail: m.stallen@donders.ru.nl

Download