Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 03 June 2014
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00391

Enhancing aesthetic appreciation by priming canvases with actions that match the artist's painting style

  • 1Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Épinière, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, Paris, France
  • 2Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Paris, France
  • 3Inserm, U 1127, Paris, France
  • 4CNRS, UMR 7225, Paris, France
  • 5Institut Jean Nicod, CNRS-EHESS-ENS UMR 8129, Paris, France
  • 6The Italian Society for Neuroaesthetics ‘Semir Zeki’, Trieste, Italy

The creation of an artwork requires motor activity. To what extent is art appreciation divorced from that activity and to what extent is it linked to it? That is the question which we set out to answer. We presented participants with pointillist-style paintings featuring discernible brushstrokes and asked them to rate their liking of each canvas when it was preceded by images priming a motor act either compatible or incompatible with the simulation of the artist's movements. We show that action priming, when congruent with the artist's painting style, enhanced aesthetic preference. These results support the hypothesis that involuntary covert painting simulation contributes to aesthetic appreciation during passive observation of artwork.

Introduction

Perceptual, cognitive, and affective evaluations contributes to the aesthetic experience of a work of art (Cela-Conde et al., 2004; Kawabata and Zeki, 2004; Vartanian and Goel, 2004). Although much research has focused on reward-related brain regions involved in artistic preference (above all the oribitofrontal cortex; Jacobsen et al., 2006; Ishizu and Zeki, 2011, 2013; see also Ticini and Omigie, 2013), the role of other brain structures has remained thus far poorly explored. Here, we investigate the contribution of motor areas to aesthetic experience, a topic of very wide interest (Freedberg and Gallese, 2007). Several neuroimaging experiments have shown that the perception of artworks elicits motor activity in the observers' brain without fully clarifying its role in aesthetic experience (Kawabata and Zeki, 2004; Cela-Conde et al., 2009; Cross et al., 2011; Ishizu and Zeki, 2011, 2013; Cross and Ticini, 2012; Umiltà et al., 2012; Sbriscia-Fioretti et al., 2013). Indeed, on the one hand, motor activity may simply be triggered by a covert approach or avoidance response related to the emotional nature of the artwork, as it has been shown for other types of stimuli (Hajcak et al., 2007). On the other, some have hypothesized that it may represent the covert and involuntary simulation of the artist's gestures when viewing a work of art, signs of which may be present on the canvas in the form of brushstrokes (Freedberg and Gallese, 2007). Whether the latter interpretation is correct and whether motor activity contributes to the aesthetic experience at all, is still unclear.

We recorded the preference of naïve individuals for 90 high quality reproductions of pointillist-style paintings presented under conditions specifically designed either to be compatible or not with the actions required to produce them (as established in associative training conducted beforehand, see Materials and Methods). Each painting was preceded by a supraliminal priming consisting of a static image depicting a hand either holding a paintbrush with a precision (Compatible) or a power grip (Incompatible). A hand resting palm down on a table was used as baseline (Control). We hypothesized that if action simulation is causally involved in the affective response to art, subjects would like the artwork in the Compatible condition more than in the other two conditions.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty naïve healthy right-handed individuals (13 females; mean = 24 years) participated in the study. They were all naïve to the purpose of the investigation and with normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

Stimuli

Stimuli consisted of 90 high quality color images of pointillist-style paintings (Table 1). Thirteen individuals (7 females; mean age = 27.9 years) who did not participate in the study pre-selected them among 200 canvases according to their style: pointillist-style, stroke-style, or otherwise. 90 images indicated as pointillist-style paintings by at least 10 out of 13 subjects were chosen for the experiment. Furthermore, three right gloved-hand images (holding a paintbrush with a power or a precision grip, or rested palm down) were used in the sensorimotor training (see Visuomotor Training) and as supraliminal priming images in the experiment (see Painting Observation and Liking Rates). All images were adjusted to the same size (470 × 351 pixels) using Adobe Photoshop and presented on a screen with a resolution of 1280 × 800 pixels, at 55 cm distance to subtend 12° horizontal and 9° vertical visual angles.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. List of the pointillist-style paintings used in the experiment.

Visuomotor Training

We first established an association between the participants' own movements and the creation of pointillist-style or stroke-style paintings. To achieve this, we presented the participants with one out of three right gloved-hand images (Figure 1A) displayed on a screen (in random order, for 10 s, 6 times each) that served as instruction for the subjects to perform the desired training with the right hand.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Visuomotor training. During the associative training, three images (A) depicting a right gloved-hand holding a paintbrush with a precision or a power grip (or rested palm down as control) instructed the participants to produce pointillist-style (B), and stroke-style (C), respectively.

The image of the hand holding a paintbrush with a precision grip instructed the participants to paint dots by executing stippling movements while holding the paintbrush with the precision grip (Figure 1B). The image depicting the hand holding a paintbrush with a power grip instructed the participants to paint strokes of about 10 cm by holding the paintbrush with a power grip (Figure 1C). The image depicting the hand rested palm down instructed the participants to position their hand palm down on the table. Task completion was supervised by the experimenter. The training was repeated before the first, third and sixth primed blocks (see below) for each grip (10 s each) to strengthen the visuomotor association.

Painting Observation and Liking Rates

After the visuomotor training, participants observed the 90 pointillist-style paintings preceded by one of the three images (700–1000 ms, randomly presented) depicting a right gloved-hand holding a paintbrush with a grip that supraliminally primed actions (for studies investigating how hand images prime actions see Borghi et al., 2007) that were either Compatible (precision grip) or Incompatible (power grip) with the drawing of pointillist-style paintings (Figure 2A). A palm down image served as Control. Each painting was presented three times, in nine randomized blocks (of 30 trials each) preceded by a different priming image. After 500 ms, the participants rated the paintings by moving a dot along a 9-point Likert-type scale displayed below the painting for 2500 ms (from “I like it very much” to “I do not like it at all,” direction counterbalanced across subjects) by left ring and index finger key-presses. Choices were confirmed by middle finger key-presses. A 1000 ms blank screen completed each trial. Due to the numerous unconfirmed ratings (≥10%) two participants were excluded from further analysis. In the remaining 18, a total of 3.25% of unconfirmed ratings was excluded.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Images of a gloved-hand holding a paintbrush were used as supraliminal priming before the display of each pointillist-style painting. The images consisted of either a precision or a power grip, or of a rested palm down hand and they created three conditions. Compatible (precision grip) or Incompatible (power grip) with the drawing of pointillist-style paintings. The palm down image served as Control. (B) The preference expressed when the paintings were preceded by priming images activating motor programs Compatible with the production of pointillist-style brushstrokes was higher than that expressed for the Incompatible (*p < 0.05) and the Control (marginally significant §p = 0.067) conditions. The liking ratings in the Incompatible and Control conditions did not differ from each other (p = 0.567). Mean liking ratings in the three conditions are depicted (error bars represent s.e.m.).

Familiarity

Upon completion of the experiment, the participants were debriefed to assess their familiarity with art by using an art questionnaire adapted from Chatterjee et al. (2010) by excluding questions 1–3 due to differences between the France and USA education systems. A median split (median of the Sums = 5.5) of the questions in Table 2 separated the participants into art-familiar and art-unfamiliar groups composed of nine participants each.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Art familiarity questionnaire.

Statistical Analysis

To asses whether covert painting simulation modulated the liking rating, we entered the ratings in a 3 (Condition: Compatible, Incompatible, Control; within subjects) × 2 (Group: art-familiar, art-unfamiliar; between subjects) ANOVA. A significance threshold of p < 0.05 was set for all statistical tests.

Results

The main factor Group [F(1,16) = 0.665, p = 0.427, η2p = 0.040] and the interaction Group × Condition [F(2, 32) = 2.577, p = 0.092, η2p = 0.139] were not significant. In other words, this result indicates that art familiarity did not influence the results. Instead, the factor Condition was significant [F(2, 32) = 3.355, p = 0.047, η2p = 0.173]. In particular, the aesthetic preference expressed for the paintings in the Compatible condition (4.974 ± 0.181; Mean ± s.e.m.) was significantly higher (p = 0.048, Newman-Keuls post-hoc test) than that in the Incompatible condition (4.877 ± 0.168), and marginally different (p = 0.067) from that in the Control condition (4.899 ± 0.176; Figure 2B). Instead, the liking rates did not differ between the Incompatible and Control conditions (p = 0.567).

Correlations between each condition and individuals' sum of experience ratings (see Table 2) were not significant (Pearson correlations rs < 0.236, ps > 0.346) thus ruling out any association between familiarity and liking scores.

Discussion

In this behavioral study we show that the aesthetic appreciation for pointillist-style paintings is enhanced by presenting supraliminal action priming images that are congruent (Compatible condition) with the style required to create those paintings. How can the priming modulate liking ratings of passively observed canvases? We believe that the congruent priming facilitated the covert simulation of the brushstrokes present in the paintings, thus yielding to higher ratings. This interpretation is consistent with the hypothesis that motor structures have a role in aesthetic and particularly that involuntary painting simulation contributes to aesthetic appreciation (Freedberg and Gallese, 2007; Leder et al., 2012; Umiltà et al., 2012). In agreement with previous work (Umiltà et al., 2012), our results also suggest that this effect is independent of familiarity with art. Nonetheless, since all participants were not actively engaged in creating artwork (see Question 3 in the Art familiarity questionnaire, Table 2) we cannot rule out the possibility that the results would be different for artists.

What is the mechanisms involved in simulating brushstrokes? The concept of covert action simulation has acquired a new interest with the work conducted on the mirror neuron mechanism in the non-human and human primate brain (Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2010). Through this mechanism, other agents' actions are mirrored in one's own motor system thus, it is thought, helping to understand others' motor acts from “within.” Action of other agents can be mirrored or covertly simulated when they are directly observed as well as when they are represented as static pictures (i.e., images depicting body movements, see Mado-Proverbio et al., 2009; Urgesi et al., 2010), and when they are hidden from view and only their sound (Ticini et al., 2012) or their traces (Longcamp et al., 2003) are perceived. For instance, there is evidence that observation of hand written letters triggers activity in motor areas involved in writings (Longcamp et al., 2003; see also Ticini, 2013), and particularly that learning to write facilitates the visual recognition of letters through the participation of brain areas known to be activated by the execution, imagery and observation of actions (Longcamp et al., 2008). Our result is supported by these and more recent behavioral findings reporting that the direction of observed brushstrokes affects participants' response speed in reaction time experiments (Taylor et al., 2012) and that active execution of movements increases (or decreases) the viewer's liking ratings when they match (or not) the style of the painting (Leder et al., 2012).

These results could be also explained by alternative mechanisms not necessarily involving painting simulation. For instance, it is plausible that the implicit knowledge about the correct action needed to manipulate the paintbrush (see Buxbaum and Kalenine, 2010) may have facilitated the most functional and effortless motor program to grasp a brush in order to create pointillist-like paintings. This would be in accordance with the idea that fluency in stimulus processing can influence aesthetic responses, as well (Reber et al., 2004). Moreover, unlike in Leder et al. (2012), we cannot exclude that self-observation of one own's hands during the training may have strengthened visuo-visual (instead of visuo-motor) associations between the hand grip and the painting style. We also cannot exclude that an intrinsic affective value of the action primes may have biased the preference ratings (e.g., the precision grip could have been perceived as more positive than the power grip). In this regard, a recent article from Flexas et al. showed differences in liking for abstract artwork when they were preceded by facial primes showing happiness, disgust or no emotion (Flexas et al., 2013). In particular, paintings preceded by happiness primes were liked more than those preceded by disgust primes. If it were the case in our experimental setup, our results would extend previous research on how the affective transfer elicited by priming may influence evaluative judgments (e.g., Murphy and Zajonc, 1993; Rotteveel et al., 2001) to the domain of aesthetic experience. Finally, we cannot exclude that the prior training alone could be sufficient to enhance the ratings as a result of an exposure effect, without the need of priming images presented before each painting.

In conclusion, we here provide empirical evidence that, beyond other factors such as upbringing, historical context and nature of the artistic stimuli, covert painting simulation may influence affective responses to art (Freedberg and Gallese, 2007). Although we cannot fully rule out alternative explanations, we suggest that the contribution of motor areas may be fundamental for the attribution of the hedonic value to some objects of art. Since simulation appears pivotal for understanding the actions and emotions of others, one important area of future research will be to characterize its influence on affective centers beyond the domain of artistic preference. Obtaining a better understanding of the contribution of action simulation in affective states is likely to shed light not just on how the brain encodes affective stimuli but also may enrich our perspective on the neural mechanisms involved in some social and communicative deficits associated with action simulation, such as autism spectrum disorder (Oberman and Ramachandran, 2007).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank A. Berthoz, H. Leder, M. Nadal, and the reviewers for their insightful comments. This work was supported by the French National Research Agency (ANR-10-CREA-005).

References

Borghi, A. M., Bonfiglioli, C., Lugli, L., Ricciardelli, P., Rubichi, S., and Nicoletti, R. (2007). Are visual stimuli sufficient to evoke motor information? Studies with hand primes. Neurosci. Lett. 411, 17–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.10.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buxbaum, L. J., and Kalenine, S. (2010). Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1191, 201–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05447.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cela-Conde, C. J., Ayala, F. J., Munar, E., Maestú, F., Nadal, M., Capó, M. A., et al. (2009). Sex-related similarities and differences in the neural correlates of beauty. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 3847–3852. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900304106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cela-Conde, C. J., Marty, G., Maestú, F., Ortiz, T., Munar, E., Fernández, A., et al. (2004). Activation of the prefrontal cortex in the human visual aesthetic perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 6321–6325. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0401427101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chatterjee, A., Widick, P., Sternschein, R., Smith, W. B., and Bromberger, B. (2010). The assessment of art attributes. Empir. Stud. Arts 28, 207–222. doi: 10.2190/EM.28.2.f

CrossRef Full Text

Cross, E. S., Kirsch, L., Ticini, L. F., and Schuetz-Bosbach, S. (2011). The impact of aesthetic evaluation and physical ability on dance perception. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:102. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cross, E. S., and Ticini, L. F. (2012). Neuroaesthetics and beyond: new horizons in applying the science of the brain to the art of dance. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 11, 5–16. doi: 10.1007/s11097-010-9190-y

CrossRef Full Text

Flexas, A., Rosselló, J., Christensen, J. F., Nadal, M., Olivera La Rosa, A., and Munar, E. (2013). Affective priming using facial expressions modulates liking for abstract art. PLoS ONE 8:e80154. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080154

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Freedberg, D., and Gallese, V. (2007). Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 197–203. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hajcak, G., Molnar, C., George, M. S., Bolger, K., Koola, J., and Nahas, Z. (2007). Emotion facilitates action: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study of motor cortex excitability during picture viewing. Psychophysiology 44, 91–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00487.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ishizu, T., and Zeki, S. (2011). Toward a brain-based theory of beauty. PLoS ONE 6:e21852. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021852

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ishizu, T., and Zeki, S. (2013). The brain's specialized systems for aesthetic and perceptual judgment. Eur. J. Neurosci. 37, 1413–1420. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12135

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jacobsen, T., Schubotz, R. I., Höfel, L., and Cramon, D. Y. V. (2006). Brain correlates of aesthetic judgment of beauty. Neuroimage 29, 276–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kawabata, H., and Zeki, S. (2004). Neural correlates of beauty. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 1699–1705. doi: 10.1152/jn.00696.2003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leder, H., Bär, S., and Topolinski, S. (2012). Covert painting simulations influence aesthetic appreciation of artworks. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1479–1481. doi: 10.1177/0956797612452866

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Longcamp, M., Anton, J.-L., Roth, M., and Velay, J.-L. (2003). Visual presentation of single letters activates a premotor area involved in writing. Neuroimage 19, 1492–1500. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00088-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Longcamp, M., Boucard, C., Gilhodes, J.-C., Anton, J.-L., Roth, M., Nazarian, B., et al. (2008). Learning through hand- or typewriting influences visual recognition of new graphic shapes: behavioral and functional imaging evidence. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 802–815. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20504

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mado-Proverbio, A. M., Riva, F., and Zani, A. (2009). Observation of static pictures of dynamic actions enhances the activity of movement-related brain areas. PLoS ONE 4:e5389. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005389

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Murphy, S. T., and Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 64, 723–739. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.5.723

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oberman, L. M., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The simulating social mind: the role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychol. Bull. 133, 310–327. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.310

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reber, R., Schwarz, N., and Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: is beauty in the perceiver's processing experience? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8, 364–382. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., and Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 264–274. doi: 10.1038/nrn2805

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rotteveel, M., de Groot, P., Geutskens, A., and Phaf, R. H. (2001). Stronger suboptimal than optimal affective priming? Emotion 1, 348–364. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.1.4.348

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sbriscia-Fioretti, B., Berchio, C., Freedberg, D., Gallese, V., and Umiltà, M. A. (2013). ERP Modulation during observation of abstract paintings by Franz Kline. PLoS ONE 8:e75241. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075241

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Taylor, J. E., Witt, J. K., and Grimaldi, P. J. (2012). Uncovering the connection between artist and audience: viewing painted brushstrokes evokes corresponding action representations in the observer. Cognition 125, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ticini, L. F. (2013). Does the motor cortex differentiate between linguistic symbols and scribbles? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:715. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00715

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ticini, L. F., and Omigie, D. (2013). Why do we like what we like? When information flow matters. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:731. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00731

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ticini, L. F., Schuetz-Bosbach, S., Weiss, C., Casile, A., and Waszak, F. (2012). When sounds become actions: higher-order representation of newly learned action sounds in the human motor system. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 464–474. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00134

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Umiltà, M. A., Berchio, C., Sestito, M., Freedberg, D., and Gallese, V. (2012). Abstract art and cortical motor activation: an EEG study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:311. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00311

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Urgesi, C., Maieron, M., Avenanti, A., Tidoni, E., Fabbro, F., and Aglioti, S. M. (2010). Simulating the future of actions in the human corticospinal system. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2511–2521. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp292

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vartanian, O., and Goel, V. (2004). Neuroanatomical correlates of aesthetic preference for paintings. Neuroreport 15, 893–897. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200404090-00032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: action, priming, simulation, aesthetic appreciation, art, mirror neurons

Citation: Ticini LF, Rachman L, Pelletier J and Dubal S (2014) Enhancing aesthetic appreciation by priming canvases with actions that match the artist's painting style. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:391. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00391

Received: 16 December 2013; Accepted: 16 May 2014;
Published online: 03 June 2014.

Edited by:

Silvio Ionta, University Hospital Center (CHUV) and University of Lausanne (UNIL), Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Alissa Fourkas, National Institutes of Health, USA
Luis Carlo Bulnes, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Belgium

Copyright © 2014 Ticini, Rachman, Pelletier and Dubal. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Luca F. Ticini, School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Manchester, Zochonis Building, Brunswick Street, Manchester M13 9PL, UK e-mail: luca.ticini@gmail.com

Download