Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 24 June 2014
Sec. Speech and Language
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00460

Magnocellular-dorsal pathway and sub-lexical route in developmental dyslexia

Simone Gori1,2* Paolo Cecchini3 Anna Bigoni3 Massimo Molteni2 Andrea Facoetti1,2
  • 1Developmental and Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 2Developmental Neuropsychology Unit, Istituto Scientifico “E. Medea” di Bosisio Parini, Lecco, Italy
  • 3Ophthalmological Unit, Istituto Scientifico “E. Medea” di San Vito al Tagliamento, Pordenone, Italy

Although developmental dyslexia (DD) is frequently associate with a phonological deficit, the underlying neurobiological cause remains undetermined. Recently, a new model, called “temporal sampling framework” (TSF), provided an innovative prospect in the DD study. TSF suggests that deficits in syllabic perception at a specific temporal frequencies are the critical basis for the poor reading performance in DD. This approach was presented as a possible neurobiological substrate of the phonological deficit of DD but the TSF can also easily be applied to the visual modality deficits. The deficit in the magnocellular-dorsal (M-D) pathway - often found in individuals with DD - fits well with a temporal oscillatory deficit specifically related to this visual pathway. This study investigated the visual M-D and parvocellular-ventral (P-V) pathways in dyslexic and in chronological age and IQ-matched normally reading children by measuring temporal (frequency doubling illusion) and static stimuli sensitivity, respectively. A specific deficit in M-D temporal oscillation was found. Importantly, the M-D deficit was selectively shown in poor phonological decoders. M-D deficit appears to be frequent because 75% of poor pseudo-word readers were at least 1 SD below the mean of the controls. Finally, a replication study by using a new group of poor phonological decoders and reading level controls suggested a crucial role of M-D deficit in DD. These results showed that a M-D deficit might impair the sub-lexical mechanisms that are critical for reading development. The possible link between these findings and TSF is discussed.

Introduction

Developmental dyslexia (DD) is often defined as a deficit in reading acquisition despite normal intelligence and access to conventional instruction (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). According to the dual-route model (see Perry et al., 2007 for a review), written words can be processed either by the sub-lexical route or by the lexical route. The sub-lexical route is based on grapheme-to-phoneme correspondences and allows reading of unfamiliar words and pseudo-words. The lexical route is based on lexical unit correspondences and is crucial to read familiar and irregular words only. Both acquired and developmental disorders of reading have been generally discussed within this psycholinguistic framework (e.g., Castles and Coltheart, 1993). Phonological dyslexics show great difficulty in reading unfamiliar words and pseudo-words compared to known words, and this is thought to arise from damage to the sub-lexical route. In contrast, surface dyslexia is characterized by impaired reading of irregular words, and this is thought to arise from a damaged lexical route (e.g., Castles and Coltheart, 1993), potentially linked to an under-stimulation of the visual word recognition system resulting from low experience with literacy.

However, in shallow orthographies such as Italian, spelling-sound irregularity is limited to the supra-segmental level (i.e., to stress assignment). Thus, in Italian dyslexic children the increased weight of sub-lexical processing does not permit precise measurement of the efficiency of the lexical-route (see also Ruffino et al., 2014). It is crucial to note that –- regardless of spelling-sound regularity - for a beginner reader all words are at first pseudo-words because the lexical-orthographic representations still have to be developed. Accordingly, most longitudinal studies have shown that beginner readers primarily use the sub-lexical route (see Sprenger-Charolles et al., 2003 for a review).

Phonological decoding, which is typically measured by examining children’s pseudo-word reading performance, is one of the most critical skills for successful reading acquisition (e.g., Share, 1995). Interestingly, Ziegler et al. (2003) showed that dyslexics with regular (German-speaking children) and irregular (English-speaking children) spelling-to-sound correspondences present an extremely slow and serial phonological decoding mechanism. Thus, in learning to read it is of utmost importance to acquire an accurate and fluent use of the sub-lexical route (e.g., Goswami, 2000; see Vellutino et al., 2004 for a review).

Although there are a number of theories attempting to account for DD, two main views received major support. The first approach proposes that DD arises from deficits in systems that are specifically linguistic in nature. In particular, the phonological deficit theory suggests that DD arises from deficits in phonological processing (e.g., Snowling, 2000). In contrast, many authors suggest that deficits in underlying non-linguistic sensory mechanisms are the real core abnormality in DD (e.g., Stein and Walsh, 1997; Vidyasagar and Pammer, 2010 for visual deficits; Wright et al., 2000; Tallal, 2004 for auditory deficits). This theory, known as the temporal processing hypothesis is the multi-sensory (i.e., visual and auditory) version of the magnocellular dorsal (M-D) theory of DD, suggesting that children with DD have specific deficits in processing rapidly presented or brief sensory stimuli in either the visual or auditory modalities (see Farmer and Klein, 1995; Hari and Renvall, 2001 for reviews). Chiefly, the M-D temporal hypothesis explicitly claims that phonological decoding deficits in dyslexics could arise from impairments in sensory processing of visual and auditory dynamic-stimuli (e.g., Facoetti et al., 2010a,b). The well known M-D theory of DD is often referred specifically to the visual modality, and it is a comprehensive, albeit controversial account (e.g., Amitay et al., 2002; Sperling et al., 2005; Olulade et al., 2013). This theory stems from the observation that some reading disabled children are impaired in the specific visual M-D pathway (see Stein and Walsh, 1997; Boden and Giaschi, 2007; Vidyasagar and Pammer, 2010 for reviews). The M-D pathway originates in the ganglion cells of the retina, passes through the M-layer of the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), and finally reaches the occipital and parietal cortices (Maunsell and Newsome, 1987). The M-D stream is considered blind to colors, and responds optimally to contrast differences, low spatial frequencies, and to high temporal frequencies and motion (Livingstone and Hubel, 1987). The M-D stream seems to be impaired in individuals with DD, whereas the other major parallel pathway of the visual system, the parvocellular-ventral (P-V) stream, is intact (see Stein and Walsh, 1997; Boden and Giaschi, 2007; Vidyasagar and Pammer, 2010 for reviews). The P-V pathway is characterized by both lower temporal resolution and superior sensitivity to high spatial frequencies, and it is also sensitive to color changes (Livingstone and Hubel, 1987). Several studies showed the specificity of the M-D pathway deficit in individuals with DD in comparison to P-V processing in the normal range, suggesting the crucial role of the M-D pathway as the dominant visual stream for text reading (e.g., Lovegrove et al., 1986; Chouake et al., 2012). Dyslexics are less sensitive than typically reading controls to luminance patterns and motion displays with high temporal and low spatial frequencies (e.g., Eden et al., 1996), visual features that are primarily associated with the M-D pathway, but they perform normally on tasks primarily associated with the P-V pathway, such as those involving color and form (Merigan and Maunsell, 1993).

Most of the evidence for the visual M-D deficit theory has derived from studies of coherent dot motion perception (see Stein, 2001 for a review), which taps the cortical portion of the M-D pathway.

However, the coherent dot motion deficit is rarely found in all individuals in a dyslexic sample (e.g., Talcott et al., 2013). Children or adult poor readers may be specifically impaired in motion perception only in the presence of high external noise, but not in the presence of low external noise or when the signal is clearly defined (Sperling et al., 2006), weakening the strongest evidence for the more dorsal portion of the M-D pathway deficit in DD. In addition, pure M-D deficits have rarely been documented in dyslexic subjects (e.g., Amitay et al., 2002; Sperling et al., 2005). Disabled readers show impaired performance in non-M-D tasks requiring fine frequency discrimination, and the stimuli used in those tasks were neither modulated in time nor briefly presented (e.g., Amitay et al., 2002). Dyslexic children had difficulties detecting both gratings with high temporal frequency and low spatial frequency (i.e., M-D stimuli) and gratings with low temporal frequency and high spatial frequency (i.e., P-V stimuli) when the grating were embedded in external noise (Sperling et al., 2005). Nevertheless, these results did not falsify the evidence obtained from a large population of studies demonstrating significant and replicable differences between dyslexic and control groups and longitudinal studies in the coherent motion perception task (e.g., Cornelissen et al., 1995; Talcott et al., 2000, 2002, 2013; Boets et al., 2011). In addition, it has been reported that up to 75% of dyslexic individuals show visual temporal processing deficits (Lovegrove et al., 1986). Important literature supports the transient subsystem deficit hypothesis in DD which suggests a dissociation in sensitivity between low spatial, high temporal versus high spatial, low temporal grating stimuli (e.g., Martin and Lovegrove, 1987; Keen and Lovegrove, 2000). These grating stimuli tap into the receptive field characteristics of the M system at a retino-cortical level providing the most relevant support for the lower portion of the M-D theory deficit. Moreover, a post mortem study, in a small sample, showed that in the brain of individuals with DD the M neurons of the LGN were noticeably smaller than those found in normal readers’ brains, while the P neurons did not differ (Livingstone et al., 1991).

It should be noted that the M-D pathway terminates mainly in the posterior parietal cortex (Mishkin and Ungerleider, 1982; Merigan and Maunsell, 1993), which is the cortical region controlling selective attention in humans (Facoetti and Molteni, 2000; see Corbetta and Shulman, 2002, 2011 for reviews). Thus, a weakened or abnormal M-D input to the dorsal-stream could result in a spatial and temporal attention deficit in dyslexic children and adults (e.g., Brannan and Williams, 1987; Williams et al., 1987; Valdois et al., 1995; Cestnick and Coltheart, 1999; Hari et al., 1999, 2001; Vidyasagar and Pammer, 1999; Facoetti et al., 2000, 2001, 2005, 2006, 2008, 2010a,b; Iles et al., 2000; Buchholz and McKone, 2004; Bosse et al., 2007; Buchholz and Aimola Davies, 2007; Roach and Hogben, 2007; Ruffino et al., 2010; see Vidyasagar, 1999; Hari and Renvall, 2001; Boden and Giaschi, 2007; Vidyasagar and Pammer, 2010; Gori and Facoetti, 2014 for reviews) and specifically in dyslexics, a more severe poor pseudo-word reading ability in comparison to word reading skills (Buchholz and McKone, 2004; Facoetti et al., 2006, 2010a; Roach and Hogben, 2007; Jones et al., 2008; Ruffino et al., 2010, 2014). Children with autism spectrum disorder have shown attentional zooming-out and dorsal pathway disorders (e.g., Ronconi et al., 2012, 2013 a,b, 2014), the visual attentional deficit is now recognized as a core feature of DD (Franceschini et al., 2012, 2013; Zorzi et al., 2012; see Gori and Facoetti, 2014 for a review). The sub-lexical route is crucial for reading pseudo-words or new words during reading acquisition in all alphabetic languages, and it specifically requires serial attentional graphemic parsing (Facoetti et al., 2010a).

Recently, a new model, labeled “temporal sampling framework” (TSF), was proposed by Goswami (2011) providing a new, different and, intriguing prospect in the DD study. TSF integrates the data on the auditory processing deficit with the findings on neural oscillatory mechanisms related to the temporal sampling of speech. In short, the innovative proposal by Goswami (2011) suggests that deficits in syllabic perception at relatively low temporal frequencies (inside of the range of the delta/theta, i.e., <10 Hz) are the critical basis for the reading disability in DD (Power et al., 2013). This hypothesis is supported by the findings that show the role of neuronal oscillations in speech perception (Luo and Poeppel, 2007). Although this approach was presented as a possible neurophysiological substrate of the phonological deficit of DD the TSF it is not only limited to that but also, can easily be applied to all the stages of processing within the visual system (Vidyasagar, 2013). TSF also has the potential to integrate several low level deficits already associated with DD (Vidyasagar, 2013; Pammer, 2014). As suggested by Vidyasagar (2013) the D-stream deficit could also be integrated in the TSF theory because the TSF fits well in describing the attentional feedback within the D-stream. What is expected is a deficit in neural oscillation at higher temporal frequency than in the auditory modality because the M-D visual stream process relatively high temporal frequency (Vidyasagar, 2013).

Assuming that the M-D pathway deficit is the neurobiological basis of visual selective attention disorders in DD, we predict that the M-D deficit should be found mainly in poor phonological decoders. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to investigate the efficiency of the visual M-D pathway inside the TSF approach in dyslexic and typically reading children (age- and reading-matched) using the frequency doubling (FD) illusion. The FD illusion is a visual illusion that was first described by Kelly (1966). Measuring a visual illusion, even if it sounds counterintuitive, can be done in a very accurate way (e.g., Gori et al., 2006, 2008, 2011; Yazdanbakhsh and Gori, 2008; Giora and Gori, 2010; Gori and Spillmann, 2010; Ito, 2012). The FD illusion appears to be dependent on the spatial and temporal frequency of a flickering grating. When a grating with a spatial frequency of 0.1–4 c/deg is flickering faster than 15 Hz, the viewer perceives a grating with double the physical spatial frequency. The FD was later explained by Kelly (1981) in terms of the full wave rectification carried out by the visual system. Such rectification is found in M(y)-cells of the primate retina (Benardete et al., 1992) and LGN (Kaplan and Shapley, 1982; Marrocco et al., 1982). It is therefore suggested, that responses from the M(y)-cells underlie perception of the FD illusion (see Maddess et al., 1992 for a detailed discussion regarding the relationship between M(y)-cells and frequency doubling). A previous study (Pammer and Wheatley, 2001) showed that individuals with DD are less sensitive to the FD illusion than normal readers, supporting a low-level deficit in the M-D pathway. Kevan and Pammer (2008) demonstrated that children at risk of DD already show a higher threshold for the FD illusion even at the pre-reading stage. Importantly, the threshold for the FD illusion at the pre-reading stage predicts future reading skills (Kevan and Pammer, 2009). The FD illusion is, therefore, a consolidated M-D index which taps the lower portion of the M-D pathway and can be difficultly described in terms of signal-to-noise exclusion (e.g., Sperling et al., 2005, 2006; Olulade et al., 2013). Interestingly, the FD illusion was never previously tested in children with DD in shallow languages as Italian. More importantly, the FD illusion is a temporal stimulus that fit well with the opportunity to measure the M-D pathway functionality inside the context of the TSF. What is expected, indeed, is that if a neural oscillation deficit is present also in the visual system of children with DD and specifically in their M-D stream (Vidyasagar, 2013), the children with DD will need more contrast to perceive the flickering stimulus at the same oscillation frequency in comparison with the chronological-age control group.

In addition, we studied the efficiency of the P-V pathway in the same children to rule out the alternative explanation that perceptual processing is generally inefficient in dyslexic children (e.g., because of poor perceptual noise exclusion). The task employed was High-Pass Resolution (HPR) perimetry which measures the detection threshold for fixed ring-shaped stimuli of different sizes. HPR perimetry is commonly adopted for selective analysis of the lower portion of the P-V pathway.

A crucial aim of this study was to investigate if a specific subgroup of children with DD, i.e., poor phonological decoders, were affected by the M-D deficit. In the Experiment 1 we selected a poor phonological decoder subgroup and we compared them with the chronological-age control group. In the Experiment 2 we collected a new poor phonological decoder group in order to carry out a replication study. Stringently, we contrast the new group with a reading-level (RL) control group. The RL children (see Goswami, 2003) were never included in previous studies using FD illusion. The inclusion of the RL group is particularly important to address the issue of the causal link between FD illusion perception and poor phonological decoding.

Experiment 1

Methods

Participants

Seventeen dyslexic children (mean age 11 years, SD = 2), were selected from a sample of children referred to the Neuropsychiatric Unit of the scientific Hospital “E. Medea” of San Vito al Tagliamento, Pordenone, Italy, because of specific reading disability. These children had been diagnosed as dyslexic based on standard criteria (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). Their performance reading aloud had to be two standard deviations below the norm in one reading subtest or 1 standard deviation below the norm in at least two reading subtests according to the Italian age-standardized tests (Sartori et al., 1995). The ability to read aloud was measured using a clinical standardized Italian test composed of 112 words (separated into four lists; word reading task, Sartori et al., 1995) and phonological decoding ability was measured using three standardized clinical lists of 48 Italian pseudo-words (pseudo-word Reading task, Sartori et al., 1995). Finally, reading fluency and errors in age-standardized prose passages from Italian clinical tests were used to measure ecological-context reading (Sartori et al., 1995).

The children with DD were selected on the basis of:

(1) a full-scale IQ greater than, or equal to, 85, as measured by the Wechsler intelligence scale for children-revised (WISC-R; Wechsler, 1986);

(2) normal or corrected-to-normal vision and hearing;

(3) absence of neurological and/or psychiatric disorders;

(4) absence of specific language impairments (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994); and

(5) absence of attention deficit disorders with or without hyperactivity (ADHD and ADD; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). Several recent studies have shown that sustained attention deficits are significant covariates in group studies using dyslexics and controls pointing out the relevance of this exclusion criterion (e.g., Rochelle et al., 2009).

The children with DD were divided into two subgroups (i.e., poor and non-impaired phonological decoders) according to their performance on the pseudo-word reading test (Sartori et al., 1995). Children were considered poor phonological decoders (PPD, n = 12 dyslexic children) if their performance, in terms of mean between accuracy and speed of pseudo-word reading, was below two standard deviation from the norm. The remaining children were assigned to the non-impaired phonological decoders subgroup (NPD, n = 5 dyslexic children). Note that pseudo-word reading efficiency is the most appropriate measure of phonological decoding skills. A performance well below the normative data implies that the child is a poor phonological decoder (e.g., Facoetti et al., 2006, 2010a; Ruffino et al., 2010, 2014). In dyslexic children with a regular spelling-to-sound correspondence, like Italian, it is practically impossible to apply the classical English sub-typing (i.e., phonological and surface DD; e.g., Castles and Coltheart, 1993; Talcott et al., 2013) because the English language presents an higher number of irregular words. Importantly, the two dyslexic groups did not differ in word reading [t(14) = 0.68, p > 0.05] nor in text reading [t(14) = -0.01, p > 0.05] abilities, excluding that our PPDs were simply more severely impaired dyslexic children.

Twenty four chronological, age- and IQ-matched typically reading children (mean age 10 years, SD = 2) were randomly selected from the same primary school. They were of average or above average intelligence on three WISC-R (Wechsler, 1986) sub-tests (i.e., Block Design and Comprehension).

All participants’ parents gave informed consent.

Table 1 shows the mean and SD of age, Block Design, Comprehension and text reading tests for the control and dyslexic groups. Controls and dyslexics were comparable to chronological age and IQ. In contrast, controls and dyslexics were significantly different on accuracy and speed of word and text reading. In Table 2 we showed that PPD and NPD groups differed only for the pseudo-word reading mean (accuracy and speed).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Mean (M) and standard deviation (SD) of age, Comprehension, Block Design sub-test (WISC-R; Wechsler, 1986), text reading errors and speed in the control and dyslexic groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Mean (M) and standard deviation (SD) of Comprehension, Block Design sub-test (WISC-R; Wechsler, 1986), text reading mean (errors and speed), word reading mean (errors and speed), pseudo-word reading mean (errors and speed) in the two dyslexic subgroups: the poor phonological decoders (PPD) and the non-impaired phonological decoders (NPD) groups.

All participants underwent a complete ophthalmological evaluation, consisting of “Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study” (ETDRS) chart (standardized eye charts and visual acuity test), orthoptic examination, anterior segment slit lamp examination, cycloplegic refraction, and indirect ophthalmoscopy.

Apparatus and stimuli

Frequency doubling perimetry. The FD perimetry relies on the frequency doubling illusion described in the Introduction. The settings resembled the ones adopted, in a previous study, by Pammer and Wheatley (2001). A low spatial frequency grating displayed in counter-phase flicker mode at a high temporal frequency is perceived as if it had twice its actual spatial frequency. The Humphrey Matrix perimeter was the presentation tool used with the program threshold set to 30-2. The threshold is expressed as contrast attenuation in decibels (dB) and it is calculated by a staircase algorithm built into the Humphrey Matrix perimeter tool. Thresholds ranged from 0 to 38 db. The stimulus was presented at 69 locations in the 30 central degrees of the visual field. The background luminance was 100 cd/m2. The pattern consisted of a sinusoidal grating presented at different contrast levels, arranged in 5° × 5° square stimuli, and a circular macular stimulus of 2.5° radius. The spatial frequency of the bar target was 0.50 cy/deg, the counter-phase flickered at 18 Hz and was presented for 300 ms. The FD provides selective stimulation of the M-retinal ganglion cells and M LGN neurons. Due to the design of the target, no P-cell activity should be stimulated. Visual fields with 20% or less false positive or false negative responses, and 30% or less fixation errors were considered acceptable.

High-pass resolution (ophthimus) perimetry. The Ophthimus system HPR perimetry uses ring-shaped stimuli, consisting of dark borders and a lighter core. Fourteen different sized targets are available (ranged from 1.26 to 17.64 dB). The target contrast was held constant while the size varied in steps of 1.26 dB. The background luminance was 20 cd/m2. The luminance of the ring borders was 15 cd/m2 and the luminance of the ring core was 25 cd/m2. The target was “high-pass spatial frequency filtered”. The participants either detected and resolved it, or it was invisible to them. The perimeter assesses resolution thresholds as the smallest stimulus size seen in the 50 locations over the central 30° of the visual field. The blindspot is not mapped. The high-pass spatial frequency filter allows for selective analysis of the P-cells of the retina and probably of the LGN. Due to the software characteristics, fixation errors were not tested for the HPR perimetry.

Procedures

All participants performed the FD and the HPR perimetry in random order, beginning with one of the two eyes. On another day, they performed the two visual field tests again, beginning with the other eye, in order to avoid fatigue and any learning effects. The children were verbally instructed on how to perform the two tests and were given the opportunity to practice. Two pauses were given throughout each test, and a 5-min pause was permitted between testing of the first and second eye.

Each child was seated comfortably with their face against the eyepiece. For the FD testing the child was given a description of the display, and instructed to press the response button each time she/he saw a pattern against the homogeneous background. For the HPR task each child had to report whenever they saw a circle in any tested position of the visual field.

Results

FD results

All groups and subgroups were normally distributed as showed by a non-significant Shapiro-Wilk test of normality (all ps > .05). The mean FD thresholds (averaged for all positions and the two eyes) for the children with DD (n = 17) differed significantly [t-test: t(39) = 2.697, p < 0.05] in comparison to the normal reader age- and IQ-matched controls (n = 24), showing that the dyslexic group was less sensitive to the FD illusion at 18 Hz of temporal frequency of (see the Figure 1 and the plot of the individual data in Figure 6A). An univariate ANCOVA (omnibus test) was run in which the independent variable was group (chronological age- and IQ-matched controls, PPDs and NPDs) and the dependent variable was the mean FD threshold, co-varied for the participant’s age [F(3,37) = 11.999, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.493; see Figure 2 and the plot of the individual data in Figure 6A]. A planned comparison (univariate ANCOVA) was then run where the independent variable was group (chronological age- and IQ-matched controls vs. PPDs) and the dependent variable was the mean FD threshold, co-varied for the participants’ ages [F(2,33) = 15.139, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.478]. Finally another planned comparison (univariate ANCOVA), where the independent variable was the group (age- and IQ-matched controls vs. NPDs) and the dependent variable was the mean FD threshold, co-varied for the participants’ ages [F(1,26) = 1.899, p > 0.05, ηp2 = 0.068]. In summary, only the PPDs were significantly worse than the controls in their mean FD threshold.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The difference between the chronological age- and IQ-matched (CA) controls and dyslexic children in the FD task measuring the M-D pathway functionality (i.e., M threshold) in the Experiment 1. *p < 0.05.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. The difference between chronological-age and IQ matched (CA) controls and the subtypes of children with DD (i.e., poor phonological decoders, hereafter PPD and non-impaired phonological decoders, hereafter NPD) in the FD task (Experiment 1). *p < 0.05.

Although PPD children showed a significantly worse performance in the FD task at the group level, in comparison with the controls, it is important to establish how reliable is this abnormal pattern at the individual level. In the PPD group, 75% of them were at least 1 SD below the mean of the controls (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. The scatter-plot shows that at individual level the 75% of the PPDs (the red triangle) are at least 1 SD under the mean of the CA controls (the green circle) in the FD task (Experiment 1).

Based on these results indicating a specific relationship between M-D pathway and the spelling to sound translation process we concentrate our Experiment 2 only on the PPDs.

HPR results

All groups and subgroups were normally distributed as showed by a non-significant Shapiro-Wilk test of normality (all ps > 0.05). The mean HPR thresholds (averaged for all positions and the two eyes) for the children with DD (n = 17) and the normal reader age- and IQ-matched controls (n = 24) did not differ significantly [t-test: t(39) = 0.432, p > 0.05] showing that the dyslexic and normal reader groups were similar in their P-V pathway performance (see Figure 4 and the plot of the individual data in Figure 6B).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. No difference was found between age and IQ matched (CA) controls and the children with DD in the HPR task measuring the P-V pathway functionality (i.e., P threshold) in the Experiment 1.

The relationship between M-D functioning and reading text ability

Partial correlation between M-D pathway functioning (indexed by FD threshold) and the text reading efficiency (the mean between speed and errors z-scores) in the entire sample (n = 40; a child with DD score is missing), controlling for chronological age, IQ (block design and similarities), and the P-V functioning, was significant (r = 0.43, p < 0.01).

To determine the predictive relationships between M-D pathway functioning and ecological reading skills in a more stringent way, we computed a three-step fixed-entry multiple regression analysis in which the dependent variable was text reading efficiency. To control for the effects of chronological age, verbal and nonverbal IQ, and P-V pathway functioning, the predictors entered at the three steps were as follows: (1) age, block design and similarities, (2) P-V pathway threshold, and (3) M-D pathway threshold. The ANOVA regression model was significant [F(5,34) = 2805, p < 0.05] explaining the 29% of the text reading quote of variance. Only the M-D pathway measure, entered last, accounted for a significant quote of unique variance in text reading efficiency (r2 change = 0.16, p < 0.01).

Experiment 2

Methods

Participants

In a replication study, we selected a new sample of 8 PPDs (mean age 11 years, SD = 2) and a RL matched control group (10 younger children well matched to the dyslexics for reading ability and IQ, mean age 7 years, SD = 1). In order to find an RL group in Italian speaking language population, it is necessary to search for younger children than would be use in countries with deeper orthographic-to-phonological mapping than in Italian. All the inclusion criteria were the same for the Experiment 1. For details of the new two groups check Table 3. All participants’ parents gave informed consent.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Mean (M) and standard deviation (SD) of chronological age, Block Design sub-test (WISC-R; Wechsler, 1986), phonological decoding speed and errors in the reading-level (RL) controls and poor phonological decoders (PPD) of the Experiment 2.

Apparatus and stimuli

Frequency doubling perimetry. The procedure was exactly the same for the Experiment 1. The HPR perimetry task was not performed because the results were not be discriminative in the Experiment 1.

Results

The mean FD thresholds (averaged for all positions and the two eyes) for PPDs (n = 8) and the typical readers IQ- and RL matched controls (n = 10) differed significantly [t-test: t(16) = 2.962, p < 0.05] showing that the PPDs were less sensitive to the FD illusion at 18 Hz of temporal frequency even compared to younger normal readers with the same reading abilities (see Figure 5 and the plot of the individual data in Figure 6A). The two control groups (CA and RL) did not differ in the FD illusion threshold (p > 0.05). It is not surprising that age did not affect that task because the M-D pathway should be completely operative much before the age of 7 years (e.g., Gori et al., in press).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. The difference between the new sample of PPDs and reading-level and IQ-matched (RL) controls in the FD task (Experiment 2). *p < 0.05.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. (A) The scatter-plot of the individual data showing the difference between the children without DD (i.e., CA and RL controls) and the subtypes of children with DD (NPD and PPD) in the FD task measuring the M-D pathway functionality (Experiment 1 and 2). The findings did not change running the ANCOVA without the outliers. Error bars represent one standard deviation of the mean. (B) The plot of the individual data showing the difference between the children without (i.e., CA controls) and with DD in the HPR task (Experiment 1). Error bars represent one standard deviation of the mean.

Although the study was designed as two independent experiments the PPDs of the Experiment 1 and 2 were very similar in age and they did not differ in other relevant variables. Additionally, we found it could be of interest to merge the two groups of PPDs and contrast them with the CA and the RL controls. We applied an univariate ANOVA with the FD threshold as the independent variable and groups (PPD, n = 25, CA, n = 24 and RL, n = 10) as the between subjects factor. The Group main effect was significant [F(2,56) = 7.33; p < 0.05] and PPD group differed from CA and from RL (Bonferroni multiple comparison with ps < 0.05). This result confirmed the results obtained separately in Experiment 1 and 2.

Discussion

These results provide strong support for an M-D deficit in DD that has its origins at the sub-cortical level of the pathway (i.e., at the LGN). Notably, this deficit characterized only a subgroup of dyslexics, namely the poor phonological decoders.

The absence of differences in the P-V task between groups emphasizes the selectivity of the visual deficit that seems to be associated with the reading disability in the sub-lexical route. It is therefore impossible to attribute the difference found in the FD task to poor testing endurance, as the children with DD performed as well as the typically reading children on the HPR task. The absence of a difference between the two groups in the P-V task cannot be attributed to a ceiling or a floor effect that could mask a poor performance of children with DD as shown by the individual data shown in Figure 6B. The graph shows that the data of both groups were not clustered on the limits of the stimulation range. Consequently, even if the M-D and the P-V task could be different in difficulty, both tasks should have enough sensitivity to show a possible difference between groups.

Moreover, the poor phonological decoders with DD not only demonstrated less sensitivity to the FD illusion compared to the typical reading age- and IQ-matched group (supporting Pammer and Wheatley, 2001), but also to a reading-level and IQ-matched control group. The latter result excludes that the M-D deficit is simply an effect of DD.

This is the first study to show that the M-D deficit in children with DD is present also when compared with younger children with the same reading performance, challenging the idea that the M-D deficit is a simple effect of reading deprivation and not a cause, as recently suggested by Olulade et al. (2013). Importantly, the results obtained in the between-group analyses cannot be attributed to the presence of a few (and perhaps peculiar) dyslexic children in the PPD group but were fully confirmed by the analyses at the level of individual cases. M-D deficits were present in a large part of the PPDs (75%) in comparison to CA. Moreover, none of the NPDs presents a deficit in the FD task. Thus, the FD task allowed us to accurately discriminate between poor phonological decoders and CA controls. Regardless of whether children in the NPD group constitute a specific subtype in shallow orthographies (e.g., Wimmer, 1993) or they have partly compensated for their reading deficit, M-D pathway functioning seems to play an important role in phonological decoding. In general, the results from the current study provide evidence for an M-D stream involvement in DD characterized by poor phonological decoding (the most frequent pattern even in Italian individuals with DD; see Facoetti et al., 2006). This conclusion was supported by the finding that individual differences in the M-D pathway were predictive of reading performance even after controlling for age, IQ, and P-V pathway functioning. The FD task allowed us to show an M-D deficit that seems to be in the lower portion of this visual stream (the sub-cortical component and/or the primary visual cortex) and that cannot be interpreted as a signal-to-noise exclusion deficit (which could be consider the weak point of the coherent dot motion task) or as an effect of reading level (as clearly highlighted by the difference with the RL group). The present results could be interpreted inside the framework of the TSF (Goswami, 2011). A multi-sensory (auditory and visual) temporal sampling disorder of neural oscillations could include the M-D deficit theory as one of several possible causes of DD. While an auditory deficit in low temporal frequency is observed in DD (e.g., Goswami, 2011; Power et al., 2013), in the visual modality a higher temporal frequency processing seems to be damaged in children with DD which is in agreement with the Vidyasagar (2013) prediction of a temporal oscillation deficit in the M-D pathway. However, the spatial and temporal sampling of the orthographic information could also be considered as a proxy deficit for auditory modality deficits in phonological decoding (e.g., Vidyasagar and Pammer, 2010). Moreover, as recently suggested (Vidyasagar, 2013; Pammer, 2014) the TSF seems to be very appropriate to model not only for the auditory deficits of DD but also the visual deficits that appear to be common in DD. Our results, based on temporal illusion sensitivity seem to be the first experimental test of the TSF in visual modality. The fact that children with DD need more contrast to see the FD pattern at 18 hz of temporal frequency, in comparison with both control groups supports the hypothesis of a neural oscillation deficit in the M-D pathway of children with DD. This neural oscillation defict seems to be selective for the M-D stream as theorized by Vidyasagar (2013).

Further research is now necessary to better understand the role of the cortical component of the M-D pathway in DD. In order to pursue that goal a sensitivity task employing specific motion illusions (i.e., Gori and Hamburger, 2006; Gori et al., 2006, 2010b, 2011; Gori and Yazdanbakhsh, 2008; Yazdanbakhsh and Gori, 2008, 2011; Hamburger, 2012) could be devised given the fact that this kind of illusory motion is processed by V5/MT (Ruzzoli et al., 2011). Potential differences in this task could not be related to a general perceptual noise exclusion deficit.

In conclusion, sensitivity to the FD illusion could provide a simple and powerful diagnostic tool for the evaluation and identification of the risk of DD, even at the pre-reading stage (Kevan and Pammer, 2008, 2009) and the results obtained with the RL group strongly point in the direction of a causal role of a neural oscillatory deficit in the M-D pathway of individuals with DD.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

We would like to thank Luca Ronconi and Julia R. Duggan for their helpful comment on the manuscript.

References

American Psychiatric Association (APA). (1994). Task Force on DSM-IV. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 886.

Amitay, S., Ben-Yehudah, G., Banai, K., and Ahissar, M. (2002). Disabled readers suffer from visual and auditory impairments but not from a specific magnocellular deficit. Brain 125, 2272–2285. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf231

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Benardete, E. A., Kaplan, E., and Knight, B. W. (1992). Contrast gain control in the primate retina: P cells are not X-like, some M cells are. Vis. Neurosci. 8, 483–486. doi: 10.1017/S0952523800004995

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boden, C., and Giaschi, D. (2007). M-stream deficits and reading-related visual processes in developmental dyslexia. Psychol. Bull. 133, 346–366. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.346

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boets, B., Vandermosten, M., Cornelissen, P., Wouters, J., and Ghesquière, P. (2011). Coherent motion sensitivity and reading development: changing relations in the transition from pre-reading to reading stage. Child Dev. 82, 854–869. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01527.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bosse, M., Tainturier, M. J., and Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: the visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition 104, 198–230. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.05.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brannan, J. R., and Williams, M. C. (1987). Allocation of visual attention in good and poor readers. Percept. Psychophys. 41, 23–28. doi: 10.3758/BF03208209

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buchholz, J., and Aimola Davies, A. (2007). Attentional blink deficits observed in dyslexia depend on task demands. Vision Res. 47, 1292–1302. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.11.028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buchholz, J., and McKone, E. (2004). Adults with dyslexia show deficits on spatial frequency doubling and visual attention tasks. Dyslexia 10, 24–43. doi: 10.1002/dys.263

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Castles, A., and Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition 47, 149–180. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90003-E

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cestnick, L., and Coltheart, M. (1999). The relationship between language-processing and visual-processing deficits in developmental dyslexia. Cognition 71, 231–255. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00023-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chouake, T., Levy, T., Javitt, D. C., and Lavidor, M. (2012). Magnocellular training improves visual word recognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:6–14. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2011). Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 569–599. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cornelissen, P., Richardson, A., Mason, A., Fowler, S., and Stein, J. (1995). Contrast sensitivity and coherent motion detection measured at photopic luminance levels in dyslexics and controls. Vision Res. 35, 1483–1494. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)98728-R

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eden, G. F., VanMeter, J. W., Rumsey, J. M., Maisog, J. M., Woods, R. P., and Zeffiro, T. A. (1996). Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging. Nature 382, 66–69. doi: 10.1038/382066a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Facoetti, A., Lorusso, M. L., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., and Molteni, M. (2005). Visual and auditory attentional capture are both sluggish in children with developmental dyslexia. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 65, 61–72.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Facoetti, A., and Molteni, M. (2000). Is attentional focusing an inhibitory process at distractor location? Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 1–2, 185–188. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00031-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Facoetti, A., Paganoni, P., Turatto, M., Marzola, V., and Mascetti, G. G. (2000). Visual-spatial attention in developmental dyslexia. Cortex 36,109–123. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70840-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Facoetti, A., Ruffino, M., Peru, A., Paganoni, P., and Chelazzi, L. (2008). Sluggish engagement and disengagement of non-spatial attention in dyslexic children. Cortex 44, 1221–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.10.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Ruffino, M., Lorusso, M. L., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., et al. (2010a). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1011–1025. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21232

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Facoetti, A., Corradi, N., Ruffino, M., Gori, S., and Zorzi, M. (2010b). Visual spatial attention and speech segmentation are both impaired in preschoolers at familial risk for developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 16, 226–239. doi: 10.1002/dys.413

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Facoetti, A., Turatto, M., Lorusso, M. L., and Mascetti, G. G. (2001). Orienting of visual attention in dyslexia: evidence for asymmetric hemispheric control of attention. Exp. Brain Res. 138, 46–53. doi: 10.1007/s002210100700

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Facoetti, A., Zorzi, M., Cestnick, L., Lorusso, M. L., Molteni, M., Paganoni, P., et al. (2006). The relationship between visuo-spatial attention and nonword reading in developmental dyslexia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 23, 841–855. doi: 10.1080/02643290500483090

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Farmer, M. E., and Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: a review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2, 460–493. doi: 10.3758/BF03210983

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Pedrolli, K., and Facoetti, A. (2012). A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Curr. Biol. 22, 814–819. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Viola, S., Molteni, M., and Facoetti, A. (2013). Action video games make dyslexic children read better. Curr. Biol. 23, 462–466. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.044

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Giora, E., and Gori, S. (2010). The perceptual expansion of a filled area depends on textural characteristics. Vision Res. 50, 2466–2475. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.08.033

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gori, S., and Facoetti, A. (2014). Perceptual learning as a possible new approach for remediation and prevention of developmental dyslexia. Vision Res. 99, 78–87. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.11.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gori, S., Giora, E., and Agostini, T. (2010a). Measuring the Breathing Light Illusion by means of induced simultaneous contrast. Perception 39, 5–12. doi: 10.1068/p6489

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gori, S., Giora, E., and Stubbs, D. A. (2010b). Perceptual compromise between apparent and veridical motion indices: the unchained-dots illusion. Perception 39, 863–866. doi: 10.1068/p6678

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gori, S., Giora, E., and Pedersini, R. (2008). Perceptual multistability in figure-ground segregation using motion stimuli. Acta Psychol. 129, 399–409. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.09.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gori, S., Giora, E., Yazdanbakhsh, A., and Mingolla, E. (2011). A new motion illusion based on competition between two kinds of motion processing units: the accordion grating. Neural Netw. 24, 1082–1092. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2011.06.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gori, S., and Hamburger, K. (2006). A new motion illusion: the rotating-tilted-lines illusion. Perception 35, 853–857. doi: 10.1068/p5531

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gori, S., Hamburger, K., and Spillmann, L. (2006). Reversal of apparent rotation in the Enigma-figure with and without motion adaptation and the effect of T-junctions. Vision Res. 46, 3267–3273. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.03.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gori, S., Mascheretti, S., Giora, E., Ronconi, L., Ruffino, M., Quadrelli, E., et al. (in press). The DCDC2 intron 2 deletion impairs illusory motion perception unveiling the selective role of magnocellular-dorsal stream in reading (dis)ability. Cereb. Cortex.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gori, S., and Spillmann, L. (2010). Detection vs. grouping thresholds for elements differing in spacing, size and luminance. An alternative approach towards the psychophysics of Gestalten. Vision Res. 50, 1194–1202. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gori, S., and Yazdanbakhsh, A. (2008). The riddle of the rotating-tilted-lines illusion. Perception 37, 631–635. doi: 10.1068/p5770

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goswami, U. (2003). Why theories about developmental dyslexia require developmental designs. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 534–540. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goswami, U. (2000). Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia: towards a cross-linguistic theoretical framework. Dyslexia 6, 133–151. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0909(200004/06)6:2<133::AID-DYS160>3.0.CO;2-A

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goswami, U. (2011). A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hamburger, K. (2012). Still motion? Motion illusions and luminance contrast. Perception 41, 113–116. doi: 10.1068/p7005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hari, R., and Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 525–532. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01801-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hari, R., Renvall, H., and Tanskanen, T. (2001). Left minineglect in dyslexic adults. Brain 124, 1373–1380. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.7.1373

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hari, R., Valta, M., and Uutela, K. (1999). Prolonged attentional dwell time in dyslexic adults. Neurosci. Lett. 271, 202–204. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00547-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Iles, J., Walsh, V., and Richardson, A. (2000). Visual search performance in dyslexia. Dyslexia 6, 163–177. doi: 10.1002/1099-0909(200007/09)6:3<163::AID-DYS150>3.0.CO;2-U

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ito, H. (2012). Illusory object motion in the centre of a radial pattern: the pursuit–pursuing illusion. Iperception 3, 59–87. doi: 10.1068/i0430

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jones, M. W., Branigan, H. P., and Kelly, M. L. (2008). Visual deficits in developmental dyslexia: relationships between non-linguistic visual tasks and their contribution to components of reading. Dyslexia 14, 95–115. doi: 10.1002/dys.345

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kaplan, E., and Shapley, R. (1982). X and Y cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque monkeys. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 330, 125–143.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Keen A. G., and Lovegrove W. J. (2000).Transient deficit hypothesis and dyslexia: examination of whole-parts relationship, retinal sensitivity, and spatial and temporal frequencies. Vision Res. 40, 705–715. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00208-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kelly, D. (1966). Frequency doubling in visual responses. JOSA 56, 1628–1632. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.56.001628

CrossRef Full Text

Kelly, D. (1981). Nonlinear visual responses to flickering sinusoidal gratings. JOSA 71, 1051–1055. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.71.001051

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kevan, A., and Pammer, K. (2008). Visual deficits in pre-readers at familial risk for dyslexia. Vision Res. 48, 2835–2839. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.09.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kevan, A., and Pammer, K. (2009). Predicting early reading skills from pre-reading measures of dorsal stream functioning. Neuropsychologia 47, 3174–3181. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Livingstone, M. S., and Hubel, D. H. (1987). Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. J. Neurosci. 7, 3416–3468.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Livingstone, M. S., Rosen, G. D., Drislane, F. W., and Galaburda, A. M. (1991). Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 88, 7943–7947. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.18.7943

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lovegrove, W., Martin, F., and Slaghuis, W. (1986). A theoretical and experimental case for a visual deficit in specific reading disability. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 3, 225–267. doi: 10.1080/02643298608252677

CrossRef Full Text

Luo, H., and Poeppel, D. (2007). Phase patterns of neuronal responses reliably discriminate speech in human auditory cortex. Neuron 54, 1001–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maddess, T., Hemmi, J. M., and James, A. C. (1992). Evidence for spatial aliasing effects in the Y-like cells of the magnocellular visual pathway. Vision Res. 38, 1843–1859. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00344-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marrocco, R. T., McClurkin J. W., and Young R. A. (1982). Spatial summation and conduction latency classification of cells of the lateral geniculate nucleus of macaques. J. Neurosci. 2, 1275–1291.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Martin, F., and Lovegrove, W. (1987). Flicker contrast sensitivity in normal and specifically disabled readers. Perception 16, 215–221. doi: 10.1068/p160215

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maunsell, J. H. R., and Newsome, W. T. (1987). Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 363–401. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.10.030187.002051

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Merigan, W. H., and Maunsell, J. H. (1993). How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 369–402. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.002101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mishkin, M., and Ungerleider, L. G. (1982). Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys. Behav. Brain Res. 6, 57–77. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(82)90081-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olulade, O. A., Napoliello, E. M., and Eden, G. F. (2013). Abnormal visual motion processing is not a cause of dyslexia. Neuron 79, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pammer, K. (2014). Temporal sampling in vision and the implication in dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:1–18. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00933

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pammer, K., and Wheatley, C. (2001). Isolating the M (y)-cell response in dyslexia using the spatial frequency doubling illusion. Vision Res. 41, 2139–2147. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00092-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., and Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP model of reading aloud. Psychol. Rev. 114, 273–315. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.273

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Power, A. J., Mead, N., Barnes, L., and Goswami U. (2013). Neural entrainment to rhythmic speech in children with developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:777. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00777

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roach, N. W., and Hogben, J. H. (2007). Impaired filtering of behaviourally irrelevant visual information in dyslexia. Brain 130, 771–785. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl353

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rochelle, K. S., Witton, C., and Talcott, J. B. (2009). Symptoms of hyperactivity and inattention can mediate deficits of postural stability in developmental dyslexia. Exp. Brain Res. 192, 627–633. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1568-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ronconi, L., Basso, D., Gori, S., and Facoetti, A. (2014). TMS on right frontal eye fields induces an inflexible focus of attention. Cereb. Cortex 24, 396–402. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs319

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ronconi, L., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Franceschini, S., Urbani, B., Molteni, M., et al. (2012). Decreased coherent motion discrimination in autism spectrum disorder: the role of attentional zoom-out deficit. PLoS ONE 7:e49019. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ronconi, L., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Molteni, M., and Facoetti, A. (2013a). Zoom-out attentional impairment in children with autism spectrum disorder. Cortex 49, 1025–1033. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.03.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ronconi, L., Gori, S., Giora, E., Ruffino, M., Molteni, M., and Facoetti, A. (2013b). Deeper attentional masking by lateral objects in children with autism. Brain Cogn. 82, 213–218. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.04.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ruffino, M., Gori, S., Boccardi D., Molteni, M., and Facoetti, A. (2014). Spatial and temporal attention are both sluggish in poor phonological decoders with developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00331

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ruzzoli, M., Gori, S., Pavan, A., Pirulli, C., Marzi, C. A., and Miniussi, C. (2011). The neural basis of the Enigma illusion: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuropsychologia 49, 3648–3655. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ruffino, M., Trussardi, A. N., Gori, S., Finzi, A., Giovagnoli, S., Menghini, D., et al. (2010). Attentional engagement deficits in dyslexic children. Neuropsychologia 8, 3793–3801. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sartori, G., Job, R., and Tressoldi, P. E. (1995). Batteria per la Valutazione Della Dislessia e Della Disortografia Evolutiva. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.

Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 55, 151–218. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Snowling M. (2000). Dyslexia. Oxford: Blackwell.

Sperling, A. J., Lu, Z., Manis, F. R., and Seidenberg, M. S. (2005). Deficits in perceptual noise exclusion in developmental dyslexia. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 862–863. doi: 10.1038/nn1474

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sperling, A. J., Lu, Z. L., Manis, F. R., and Seidenberg, M. S. (2006). Motion-perception deficits and reading impairment: it’s the noise, not the motion. Psychol. Sci. 17, 1047–1053. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01825.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S., Béchennec, D., and Serniclaes, W. (2003). Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading, and in spelling: a four-year longitudinal study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 84, 194–217. doi: 10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00024-9

CrossRef Full Text

Stein, J. (2001). The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 7, 12–36. doi: 10.1002/dys.186

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stein, J., and Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends Neurosci. 20, 147–152. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)01005-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Talcott, J. B., Witton, C., Hebb, G. S., Stoodley, C. J., Westwood, E. A., France, S. J., et al. (2002). On the relationship between dynamic visual and auditory processing and literacy skills; results from a large primary-school study. Dyslexia 8, 204–225. doi: 10.1002/dys.224

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Talcott, J. B., Witton, C., McLean, M. F., Hansen, P. C., Rees, A., Green, G. G., et al. (2000). Dynamic sensory sensitivity and children’s word decoding skills. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 2952–2957. doi: 10.1073/pnas.040546597

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Talcott, J. B., Witton, C., and Stein, J. F. (2013). Probing the neurocognitive trajectories of children’s reading skills. Neuropsychologia 51, 472–481. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.11.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tallal, P. (2004). Improving language and literacy is a matter of time. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 721–728. doi: 10.1038/nrn1499

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Valdois, S., Gérard, C., Vanault, P., and Dugas, M. (1995). Peripheral developmental dyslexia: a visual attentional account? Cogn. Neuropsychol. 12, 31–67. doi: 10.1080/02643299508251991

CrossRef Full Text

Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., and Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 2–40. doi: 10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vidyasagar, T. R. (1999). A neuronal model of attentional spotlight: parietal guiding the temporal. Brain Res. Rev. 30, 66–76. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00005-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vidyasagar, T. R. (2013). Reading in to neuronal oscillations in the visual system: implications for developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:811. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00811

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vidyasagar, T. R., and Pammer, K. (1999). Impaired visual search in dyslexia relates to the role of the magnocellular pathway in attention. Neuroreport 10, 1283–1287. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199904260-00024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vidyasagar, T. R., and Pammer, K. (2010). Dyslexia: a deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wechsler, D. (1986). WISC-R: Scala di Intelligenza Wechsler per Bambini Riveduta. Firenze: Organizazioni Speciali.

Williams, M., Brannan, J., and Lartigue, E. (1987). Visual search in good and poor readers. Clin. Vis. Sci. 1, 367–371.

Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Appl. Psycholinguist. 14, 1–33. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400010122

CrossRef Full Text

Wright, B. A., Bowen, R. W., and Zecker, S. G. (2000). Nonlinguistic perceptual deficits associated with reading and language disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10, 482–486. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00119-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Yazdanbakhsh, A., and Gori, S. (2008). A new psychophysical estimation of the receptive field size. Neurosci. Lett. 438, 246–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.04.040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yazdanbakhsh, A., and Gori, S. (2011). Mathematical analysis of the accordion grating illusion: a differential geometry approach to introduce the 3D aperture problem. Neural Netw. 24, 1093–1101. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2011.06.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., and Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: language-specific or universal? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 86, 169–193. doi: 10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00139-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zorzi, M., Barbiero, C., Facoetti, A., Lonciari, I., Carrozzi, M., Montico, M., et al. (2012). Extra-large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 11455–11459. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1205566109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: transient system, reading acquisition, phonological decoding, reading disability, visual disorder, dorsal stream

Citation: Gori S, Cecchini P, Bigoni A, Molteni M and Facoetti A (2014) Magnocellular-dorsal pathway and sub-lexical route in developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:460. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00460

Received: 30 June 2013; Accepted: 05 June 2014;
Published online: 24 June 2014.

Edited by:

Usha Goswami, University of Cambridge, UK

Reviewed by:

Usha Goswami, University of Cambridge, UK
John Frederick Stein, University of Oxford, UK

Copyright © 2014 Gori, Cecchini, Bigoni, Molteni and Facoetti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Simone Gori, Developmental and Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università degli Studi di Padova, Via Venezia, 8 35131 Padova, Italy e-mail: simone.gori@unipd.it

Download