Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 23 September 2015
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 9 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00504

Neural correlates of reward processing in healthy siblings of patients with schizophrenia

Esther Hanssen1,2* Jorien van der Velde3 Paula M. Gromann1,2 Sukhi S. Shergill2 Lieuwe de Haan4 Richard Bruggeman5 Lydia Krabbendam1 André Aleman3 Nienke van Atteveldt1
  • 1Department of Educational Neuroscience and LEARN! Institute, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 2CSI Lab, Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
  • 3Neuroimaging Center, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Early Psychosis, Academic Psychiatric Centre, Amsterdam Medical Center, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 5Rob Giel Research Center, University Center for Psychiatry, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands

Deficits in motivational behavior and psychotic symptoms often observed in schizophrenia (SZ) may be driven by dysfunctional reward processing (RP). RP can be divided in two different stages; reward anticipation and reward consumption. Aberrant processing during reward anticipation seems to be related to SZ. Studies in patients with SZ have found less activation in the ventral striatum (VS) during anticipation of reward, but these findings do not provide information on effect of the genetic load on reward processing. Therefore, this study investigated RP in healthy first-degree relatives of SZ patients. The sample consisted of 94 healthy siblings of SZ patients and 57 healthy controls. Participants completed a classic RP task, the Monetary Incentive Delay task, during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). As expected, there were no behavioral differences between groups. In contrast to our expectations, we found no differences in any of the anticipatory reward related brain areas (region of interest analyses). Whole-brain analyses did reveal group differences during both reward anticipation and reward consumption; during reward anticipation siblings showed less deactivation in the insula, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and medial frontal gyrus (MFG) than controls. During reward consumption siblings showed less deactivation in the PCC and the right MFG compared to controls and activation in contrast to deactivation in controls in the precuneus and the left MFG. Exclusively in siblings, MFG activity correlated positively with subclinical negative symptoms. These regions are typically associated with the default mode network (DMN), which normally shows decreases in activation during task-related cognitive processes. Thus, in contrast to prior literature in patients with SZ, the results do not point to altered brain activity in classical RP brain areas, such as the VS. However, the weaker deactivation found outside the reward-related network in siblings could indicate reduced task-related suppression (i.e., hyperactivation) of the DMN. The presence of DMN hyperactivation during reward anticipation and reward consumption might indicate that siblings of patients with SZ have a higher baseline level of DMN activation and possible abnormal network functioning.

Introduction

Patients with schizophrenia often show motivational impairments and as a result fail to pursue goal-directed behavior, which particularly seems to be driven by aberrant DA functioning in reward-related brain areas (Kapur et al., 2005; Howes and Kapur, 2009; Hartmann et al., 2014). Despite apparently normal hedonic experiences, patients rarely engage in behavior that is directed to obtaining rewards and gaining pleasure (Strauss et al., 2014). This is suggestive of aberrant reward processing in schizophrenia. Rewards are desirable outcomes that serve to influence behavior and therefore reward processing is one of the most basic and important mechanisms for guiding human behavior and managing everyday life encounters successfully. Research in animals and in the healthy human population has established the crucial role for mesolimbic dopamine in reward processing (Schultz, 1997, 1998, 2006; Elliott et al., 2000; Bayer and Glimcher, 2005; Pizzagalli et al., 2008). Dopamine is essential in motivated learning, reinforcement learning, assigning salience, reward anticipation and the associated reward prediction error (Robbins and Everitt, 1996; Schultz, 1997; Bayer and Glimcher, 2005; Juckel et al., 2006b; Drew et al., 2007).

Dopaminergic projections in the mesolimbic system and the fronto-striatal network arise in the ventral tegmental region and project to the prefrontal cortex (PFC) via the dorsal and ventral striatum (VS) (Kapur, 2003; Drew et al., 2007). Projections from the ventral tegmental area/substantia nigra (VTA/SN) to the VS are central in the assignment of salience and reward (Zink et al., 2004; Berridge, 2007), which causes attentional and behavioral resources to be redirected. Aberrant cortical-striatal functions and interactions may be associated with dysfunctions in different stages of reward processing. Two stages can be distinguished: reward anticipation (i.e., expecting to receive a reward) and reward consumption (i.e., receiving the actual reward). Neuroimaging research in healthy individuals has implied that these stages are based on different brain regions: reward anticipation has been linked to activation in a number of limbic regions, in particular the VS (Knutson et al., 2001a; Bjork et al., 2010; Rademacher et al., 2010; Urban et al., 2012), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and VTA/SN, whereas frontal regions, more specifically the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), have been associated with reward consumption (Knutson et al., 2001b, 2003).

A number of studies have been conducted to investigate reward processing in schizophrenia, but the findings up to now are inconclusive. Performance on reward processing tasks sometimes indicates that patients are slower, demonstrated by reduced reaction times in patients compared to controls (Schlagenhauf et al., 2008; Nielsen et al., 2012b). Several neuroimaging studies indicate that unmedicated patients with schizophrenia and patients on atypical antipsychotics show less striatal activity during reward anticipation in a widely used reward processing task, the Monetary Incentive Delay task, than healthy controls (Juckel et al., 2006a,b; Kirsch et al., 2007; Schlagenhauf et al., 2008, 2009; Nielsen et al., 2012a,b). In contrast, several studies have shown a normal neural reward anticipation response in patients with schizophrenia on atypical antipsychotics (Juckel et al., 2006a,b; Schlagenhauf et al., 2008; Walter et al., 2009; Simon et al., 2010; Nielsen et al., 2012a). Studies that investigated the reward consumption phase found no differences between healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia (Simon et al., 2010; Nielsen et al., 2012b). This suggests that in particular reward anticipation, i.e., “wanting,” seems to be impaired in schizophrenia rather than the consumption of reward, i.e., “liking” (Gard et al., 2007; Kirsch et al., 2007; Nielsen et al., 2012b). Moreover, neural correlates of reward processing seem to be associated with symptom severity in patients with schizophrenia. A negative correlation between VS activation and positive symptoms was found by Nielsen et al. (2012b) and negative symptoms were shown to be negatively correlated with VS activation (Robbins and Everitt, 1996; Juckel et al., 2006a,b; Schlagenhauf et al., 2008; Simon et al., 2010). More specifically, anhedonia is one of the symptoms that seems to be related to a decrease in activation in the striatum (Juckel et al., 2006a,b). Previous research indicates that negative symptoms may predict future onset of schizophrenia and illness outcome (Gooding et al., 2005).

Although research on patients is vital and yielded substantial knowledge so far, these results cannot clarify the contribution of the genetic risk to abnormal reward processing in schizophrenia. The strongest known predictor of a disorder in the schizophrenia spectrum is the presence of an affected first-degree relative. This genetic risk accounts for 8–12 times increased risk (Kendler and Diehl, 1993; Stone et al., 2005; MacDonald et al., 2009). Moreover, behavioral and especially neuroimaging findings from patients are difficult to interpret, due to D2 blocking effects of antipsychotic medication, which changes striatal activation (Pessiglione et al., 2006), chronic hospitalization, and the potential neurotoxic effects of schizophrenia (MacDonald et al., 2009). Only two recent studies have investigated reward processing in relatives of patients so far, reporting blunted VS activity in siblings (de Leeuw et al., 2015) and other first-degree relatives (Grimm et al., 2014), in the absence of behavioral differences. The present study includes a larger homogeneous first-degree relatives (siblings) sample using a neutral modified version of the MID task.

The aim of the present study was to examine the underlying neural mechanisms of reward processing in siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Insight into neural correlates of reward processing in healthy siblings of schizophrenia patients may shed light on the possible underlying vulnerability and provide more insight on reward processing throughout the spectrum of the disorder. We expected to find: (a) no behavioral differences between the groups in terms of accuracy and reaction times on the reward processing task; (b) reduced activation of the VS, ACC, and VTA/SN during reward anticipation in siblings compared to controls; (c) no differences in the mPFC and the right dlPFC during the consumption of reward in siblings vs. controls, (d) blunted fMRI responses in the anticipation phase are expected to be negatively correlated with subclinical symptom severity (in particular negative symptomatology). To investigate this, we acquired functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data on 94 healthy siblings of patients with schizophrenia and 57 healthy controls while participating in a monetary incentive delay (MID) task tapping into reward anticipation and reward consumption.

Methods

Subjects

The subjects were 94 healthy siblings of patients with schizophrenia, and 57 healthy control subjects. The sample (N = 151) was recruited from a multi-center (Amsterdam, N = 81; Groningen, N = 70) add-on study from the Dutch Genetic Risk and Outcome in Psychosis (GROUP) project (https://www.group-project.nl) and had an age range between 18–60 years. Inclusion criteria for all subjects were: able and willing to give an informed consent and a good command of the Dutch language. The main exclusion criterion for the control group, and not for the sibling group, was: a personal or family history of a psychotic disorder. For a more detailed overview of participant inclusion for the GROUP study see Korver et al. (2012). Further exclusion criteria consisted of MRI contraindications such as metal implants, prostheses, pregnancy, and history of claustrophobia or epilepsy. The study was approved by the local ethics committees in Amsterdam and Groningen and conducted with strict compliance to ethical standards.

Experimental Design

Subjects completed a modified version of the Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) task (Knutson et al., 2001b). In the current version, three levels of reward anticipation were visually cued: large reward, small reward, or no reward (control trial). The participants' reaction time of a button press to a target determined whether the reward outcome was successful (within the target presentation time) in each trial. The two phases of interest are the anticipatory phase (during the cue presentation); and the outcome phase (trial outcome presentation). Event durations were randomly assigned to the trials in each run, but equal for each participant, and summing up to a fixed total trial duration of 18.5 s (semi-random jittered). Each trial (Figure 1) consisted of (1) reward cue (semi-random jittered duration: 2000–7750 ms): a large green arrow pointing up indicating a potential reward of € 5 (large reward), a small green arrow pointing up indicating potential reward of € 0.5 (small reward), or a small green arrow pointing up combined with a red one pointing down (control trial, no reward), (2) target: presentation of a brief “target” square (duration initially 250 ms, then adjusted by an algorithm to ensure 66% success), (3) fixation 1: a delay of 2000–7750 ms (semi-random jittered), (4) reward outcome: presentation of the outcome of the trial (semi-random jittered 2000–7750 ms), (5) fixation 2 (semi-random jittered 2000–7750 ms).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Monetary Incentive Delay task: an example of a successful large reward trial displaying all events and durations in the trial.

All participants performed three runs of 31, 32, and 33 trials each, in randomized order. Half of the 96 trials were control trials (48) and the other trials were equally divided between large and small reward (24 each). One run lasted approximately 8 min, depending on how many trials there were in the run. Instructions were given to the participants about the different reward symbols and they were requested to respond as fast as possible to the target in all trials by pressing a button. To increase their motivation, participants were told that if they performed above average, they could earn a bonus of € 5. For ethical reasons, all participants received this bonus at the end of the session. In addition to this bonus, participants were given a voucher of € 25 for their participation regardless of their performance.

Subclinical Symptom Assessment

The Community Assessment of Psychic Experience (CAPE) is a questionnaire based on self-report (http://www.cape42.homestead.com). It measures the lifetime prevalence of psychotic-like experiences in the general population. Results from previous research indicates that these self-reported psychotic-like experiences are stable, valid, and reliable (Konings et al., 2006). The questionnaire consists of 42 items within three dimensions: positive (20 items), negative (14 items) and depressive symptoms (8 items). We focused on the positive and negative scale in our study. The CAPE answers are rated on two 4-point Likert scales; one to indicate the frequency of symptoms and one to indicate the distress the symptoms inflict; in our study we focused on the frequency scores and not the subjective distress scores. The CAPE scores provide a total score per dimension by adding the score of each item in these dimensions (item scores range from 1 to 4), divided by the amount of items filled in. In this study, the Dutch translation of the CAPE was used.

Scanning Parameters

Imaging data were acquired on two 3.0 Tesla whole body scanners (Philips Intera, Best, NL) located at the University Medical Center Groningen and at the Academic Medical Centre in Amsterdam. Both systems were equipped with an 8-channel SENSE head coil and the scanning parameters were set in an identical way. Foam padding was placed around the subject's head in the coil to minimize head movement. The functional images were acquired by a T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging sequence scanning 40 axial slices of 2.4 mm thick with 1 mm gap, providing complete brain coverage. The in-plane resolution was 2.8 × 2.8 mm (FOV 224 × 224), TR = 2.00 s; TE = 25 ms. There were 258 volumes per run. For anatomical reference, a T1-weighted image (170 slices; isotropic voxels of 1 mm; TR 9 ms; TE 3.54 ms; α 8°; FOV 256 mm) was acquired in the bicommissural plane, covering the whole brain. Physiological data (ECG) during the task were also measured, but these data are not included in the current analyses as they are beyond the scope of this report.

Statistical Analyses

The demographical and behavioral data were analyzed using SPSS, version 20 (SPSS, IBM, Chicago, Illinois). Differences in group characteristics were examined with independent samples t-tests. Since the subclinical symptom scores have a non-normal distribution they were examined with Mann-Whitney U-tests. Accuracy (i.e., percentage of successful, on time button presses) and reaction time (RT, i.e., time between target presentation and button press, for all responses) were used as dependent variables, and analyzed by the ANCOVA module, with group (siblings, controls) as fixed factor and scanning site as covariate. Separate analyses were run for large and small reward trials. In addition, we used paired-sample t-tests to see whether the task showed an effect of reward on reaction times and accuracy. Linear regression analyses with group, age and scan site as predictors were performed to examine the effect of age on the behavioral measures.

The imaging data was analyzed using Brainvoyager QX, version 2.8 (Brain Innovation, Maastricht, The Netherlands). Preprocessing of the functional time-series consisted of slice scan-time correction, 3D motion correction, temporal highpass filtering (0.01 Hz) including linear trend removal, and modest temporal Gaussian smoothing (FWHM = 3 s). Finally, spatial smoothing using a 3D Gaussian kernel (FWHM = 6 mm) was performed. The preprocessed functional data were then coregistered to each individual anatomical scan, and resampled to 3 × 3 × 3 mm resolution in Talairach space resulting in normalized 4D volume time-course data. For each run of each subject, a stimulation protocol was created defining the onsets and offsets of the events. Using these protocols, design matrices were computed by convolving each event with a standard hemodynamic response function. The final design matrices contained 6 predictors of interest: reward cue (3x: control, small, large), indicating the anticipation of reward, and reward outcome + fixation 2 (3x: win, no win, control), indicating the consumption of reward. Additionally, there were 6 predictors-of-no-interest: target (3x: control, small, large) and fixation 1 (3x: control, small, large).

Whole-brain Analyses

To increase power and to look at overall patterns of reward processing, large and small reward trials were combined for the whole-brain analyses. A voxel-wise, random-effects, whole-brain General Linear Model (GLM) was run to (1) identify the general activation pattern evoked by reward processing in the used task, by contrasting the reward anticipation (small + large reward combined) and reward consumption (small + large reward combined, where subjects actually won) events with the control trials (Bonferroni corrected at p < 0.001) and (2) to explore group differences in reward processing. To further examine group effects, a second-level analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was run on the reward anticipation and consumption predictors (small + large combined; for analyses investigating trial differences see Supplementary Material), with group as between factor, and scanning site as covariate. To correct for multiple comparisons, a cluster-extent threshold (alpha < 0.05) determined by Monte Carlo simulations with 1000 iterations was applied to the resulting maps (Forman et al., 1995) based on an initial threshold of p < 0.01 (Goebel et al., 2006) across the whole-brain volume. For the group*anticipation map, the resulting spatial cluster threshold was 31 voxels/812 mm3. For the group*outcome map, the resulting spatial cluster threshold was 18 voxels/460 mm3. For each active cluster remaining after applying this threshold, individual beta weights (separately for small reward and large reward during the anticipation phase, and separately for win trials during the outcome phase) were extracted for visualization of the direction of the group differences, the differences between large and small reward and to investigate correlations with the symptom scores.

ROI-based Analyses

We complemented the whole-brain analyses with analyses using a priori defined regions-of-interest (ROI's; Table 1 in Supplementary Material), based on the Talairach coordinates from previous research (Nielsen et al., 2012b). Different regions were used as ROI's for the reward anticipation and reward consumption phases, due to the finding that these two phases recruited distinct brain regions in previous research (Knutson et al., 2003). For the reward anticipation phase, four regions—the VTA/SN, right and left ventral striatum (VS), and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)—were defined in line with their known role during reward anticipation. For the reward consumption phase, two regions—the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)—were defined, due to their association with evaluation of rewarding outcomes. The ROI's for the smaller brain regions (i.e., left and right VS, and VTA/SN) were created with a 5 mm sphere centered around the published coordinates. For the larger brain regions (i.e., ACC, mPFC, and DLPFC) 10 mm spheres were used. ANCOVA analyses with group as between-subjects factor and scanning site as a covariate were run, on the averaged time-courses per ROI, using the individual design matrices. The results of the ROI-based analyses were corrected at alpha levels of 0.0125 per test (0.05/4; Bonferroni corrected) for regions tested during reward anticipation (4 ROI's) and 0.025 per test (0.05/2; Bonferroni corrected) for regions tested during reward consumption (2 ROI's). For each ROI, individual beta weights were extracted to investigate correlations with symptom scores.

Results

Demographics

The demographic group characteristics are displayed in Table 1. There were no significant differences between siblings and controls in terms of gender [t(149) = 1.49, p = 0.137], education [t(149) = −1.02, p = 0.312], and handedness [t(149) = 0.23, p = 0.816]. There was a significant group difference for age [t(149) = 2.77, p = 0.006], with controls having a higher mean age than siblings (respectively being 36 years and 32 years). There were no significant group differences between CAPE scores; positive dimension (z = −0.837, p = 0.402, with a mean rank of 65.52 for siblings and 71.28 for controls), negative dimension (z = −0.263, p = 0.792, with a mean rank of 66.86 for siblings and 68.72 for controls).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics.

Behavioral Results

The percentage of hits (accuracy) and the RTs are displayed in Table 1. There were no significant differences between siblings and controls for either the large reward trials, in terms of accuracy [F(1, 148) = 0.001, p = 0.975] or RT [F(1, 148) = 0.02, p = 0.897], small reward trials [F(1, 148) = 0.369, p = 0.544; F(1, 148) = 0.77, p = 0.381] or control trials [F(1, 146) = 0.154, p = 0.695; F(1, 146) < 0.001, p = 0.993], respectively. A linear regression model with age, group and scan site as predictors showed no significant effect of age on any of the behavioral measures (see Supplementary Material for statistical tests). There was a significant difference between control trials and reward trials on reaction times in siblings and controls [control trials > small reward trials: respectively, t(92) = 9.054, p < 0.001, t(55) = 7.529, p < 0.001; and control trials > large reward trials: t(92) = 9.311, p < 0.001, t(55) = 7.177, p < 0.001].

fMRI Results

Whole-brain Analyses: Overall Task Effect

First, we evaluated the overall task effect to see if the task produced the expected activation patterns during reward processing. In Figure 2A the regions active for the whole sample during the anticipation of reward (any kind of reward; small or large reward), contrasted with the control trials without reward, are displayed. We observed robust activation in important reward-related areas; a substantial region in and near the caudate, including the VS, a sizable region at the red nucleus containing part of the SN, and the mid/anterior cingulate cortex (Table 2 in the Supplementary Material). For the outcome phase, areas relevant in reward consumption showed activation, including a wide range of other frontal brain areas and, more importantly, including reward-related areas such as parts of the mPFC and the right dlPFC (Figure 2B, and Table 3 in the Supplementary Material).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Brain activation patterns for the whole-brain overall task effect (reward trials > control trials; whole-brain GLM; N = 151) shown on an anatomical scan of one of the control participants (A) in the reward anticipation phase for overall reward (small and large reward together), (B) in the reward consumption phase (win trials > controls trials; N = 151). VS, ventral striatum; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; VTA, ventral tegmental area; SN, substantia nigra; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Whole-brain Analysis: Group Effects

The whole-brain ANCOVA showed an effect of group during the reward anticipation phase in several brain regions: the insula, posterior cingulate, medial frontal gyrus, and the paracentral lobule (Table 2, Figure 3). The extracted beta-weights indicate that the anticipation of reward was associated with less deactivation in siblings compared to controls in the insula, posterior cingulate and MFG, and a positive activation compared to deactivation in the paracentral lobule (bar graphs in Figure 3). Within these brain regions percent signal change for small and large reward are displayed separately, but they were not different in any of the regions (see Supplementary Material for statistical tests). The whole-brain ANCOVA showed an effect of group during the reward consumption phase within the bilateral medial frontal gyrus, posterior cingulate and the right precuneus (Table 3, Figure 4), again with siblings showing less deactivation than controls in the time period when subjects received their rewards.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Brain areas with a significant main effect of group during anticipation of reward.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Inner panel: Group differences during anticipation of reward (ANCOVA; N = 151) depicted on an anatomical scan of one of the control participants (p < 0.05, cluster-level corrected). Outer panel: The difference in percent signal change between small and large rewards in these regions for siblings (N = 94) and controls (N = 57). MFG, medial frontal gyrus; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Brain areas with a significant main effect of group during the consumption of reward.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Upper panel: The whole-brain group effects during win trials in the reward consumption phase (ANCOVA; N = 151) displayed on an anatomical scan of one of the control participants (p < 0.05, cluster-level corrected). Lower panel: Group differences (% signal change) between siblings (N = 94) and controls (N = 57) during consumption of reward in the regions with a whole-brain group effect. MFG, medial frontal gyrus; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex.

ROI Analyses

A priori defined ROI's were analyzed separately for the reward anticipation phase and the consumption phase. For the reward anticipation, there were no significant group differences for the left VS: for small reward F(1, 148) = 2.09, p = 0.151, and large reward F(1, 148) = 1.88, p = 0.172, right VS: for small reward F(1, 148) = 0.08, p = 0.772, and large reward F(1, 148) = 0.79, p = 0.377, ACC: small reward F(1, 148) = 0.37, p = 0.542, and large reward F(1, 148) = 0.80, p = 0.372, VTA/SN: small reward F(1, 148) = 1.16, p = 0.283, and large reward F(1, 148) = 0.82, p = 0.368. For the win trials in the outcome phase, no significant group difference were found in the mPFC F(1, 148) = 1.95, p = 0.165, and the dlPFC F(1, 148) = 0.21, p = 0.647.

Correlations with Symptoms

In the regions that showed an effect of group in the whole-brain analyses (see Figures 3, 4), we calculated the correlation between the fMRI response strength (beta estimates) and the CAPE scores (Figure 5). We eliminated extreme outliers (mean ± 2 sd) in beta values to ensure that possible correlations were not driven by outliers. Due to non-normal distribution of the subclinical symptom scales, we performed Spearman's rank correlations. To correct for multiple comparisons in the whole-brain areas, correlations were corrected at alpha levels of 0.00625 per test (0.05/8, Bonferroni corrected). In siblings, there was a positive correlation between MFG activation (large reward trials), where a group effect of reward anticipation was found, and negative symptoms (r = 0.368, p < 0.001).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Correlation (Spearman's rank) between fMRI response strength and subclinical negative symptoms in siblings (N = 88) in the medial frontal gyrus (MFG) during the anticipation phase (large reward).

Likewise, we calculated correlations between subclinical symptoms and fMRI response strength in the anticipation phase in the a priori defined ROIs (corrected at alpha levels of 0.0125, Bonferroni corrected; 0.05/4). For siblings and for controls, no correlations between brain activity and symptom scores were found in the a priori defined ROIs.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate the underlying neural mechanisms of reward processing in siblings of patients with schizophrenia. First, we confirmed the behavioral effect of reward in the MID task and the absence of behavioral differences between groups, as was in line with previous research (Grimm et al., 2014; de Leeuw et al., 2015). In addition, we confirmed that the task activated the intended reward-related brain regions during anticipation and consumption of reward.

In the reward anticipation phase, we did not find the expected blunted response in the VS in siblings or in the other a priori defined ROIs (VTA/SN and ACC). This is in contrast with earlier findings in siblings and other first-degree relatives (Grimm et al., 2014; de Leeuw et al., 2015), these different findings could be due to group differences, as we had a relatively healthy homogeneous sibling sample, and slight task differences, as our task was a neutral modified version of the MID task. The whole-brain analyses, however, revealed differences between groups during the anticipation of reward in the insula, the PCC, the MFG and the paracentral lobule. Looking closer at these activations, siblings apparently showed less deactivation compared to healthy controls in the insula, the PCC, the MFG. This lack of deactivation in the PCC and the medial frontal region may be related to abnormalities in the default mode network (DMN), which includes these areas (Raichle et al., 2001). Research in healthy subjects on the DMN indicates that the mPFC is activated during construction of mental simulations and receives input from the medial temporal lobe. Information from these two regions may subsequently be integrated by the PCC (Buckner et al., 2008). This network has been regarded as an underlying network in mental mind wandering/mental time travel, theory of mind and perspective taking; all constructions of complex self-referential stimuli (Buckner and Carroll, 2007; Mason et al., 2007; Molnar-Szakacs and Arzy, 2009). Normally, the DMN demonstrates decreases in activation during task-related cognitive processes (Buckner et al., 2008). In patients with schizophrenia, aberrations in the DMN have been reported in previous studies: i.e., an overall heightened activation of DMN structures during cognitive tasks was observed (Pomarol-Clotet et al., 2008; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2009). DMN abnormalities have been demonstrated in siblings and other first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia in previous studies as well; reduced task-related suppression (Garrity et al., 2007), abnormally high functional connectivity (Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2009) or reduced functional connectivity (Jang et al., 2011). Possibly, less deactivation in the MFG and the PCC during reward processing may signal a failure to fully disengage from the default-mode and thus engage neural resources underlying reward-related behavior. Furthermore, these results may suggest that reward processing in siblings may be more internally-focused, since the DMN is more active during internally-focused tasks (Buckner et al., 2008).

The failure to disengage from the DMN in siblings is related to the amount of subclinical negative symptoms; we found a positive correlation between negative symptom scores and BOLD signal change in the MFG. Siblings with higher subclinical negative symptoms demonstrate even lower task-related supression during a reward processing task. This association could mean that default mode is associated with the self-referential aspect of negative symptoms. This could suggest an impaired ability to suppress attention to internal states, which might lead to a disturbance during cognitive processes. More specifically, regions of the DMN may be involved in attentional deficits in schizophrenia and symptoms such as distractibility and impaired focus on irrelevant internal and external stimuli and could interfere with motivational behavior, thinking and functioning. Heightened activation of the default mode brain regions has the potential to interfere with the effectiveness of goal-directed task performance (Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos, 2007). Since we found this association between negative symptoms and the medial frontal part of the DMN in a sample of siblings of patients with schizophrenia, who do not show behavioral symptoms on a clinical level and no behavioral impairments on reward processing, we suggest that hyperactivation of the default mode could be a endophenotype in the formation of symptoms in schizophrenia.

In addition to deactivation in DMN regions, we found deactivation in siblings compared to controls in the right insula. This area is part of the cognitive task control or salience network (Dosenbach et al., 2007; Seeley et al., 2007). This network is formed by the insula and ACC and is thought to play a pivotal role in the disengagement of the DMN and engagement of task-related networks (Menon and Uddin, 2010). Moreover, it has been hypothesized that this network may not function normally in patients with schizophrenia (White et al., 2010; Palaniyappan and Liddle, 2012). Reward processing abnormalities in schizophrenia may be a consequence of abnormal interactions between the salience network—insula and ACC—and the brain's reward processing network. Furthermore, Gradin et al. (2013) found reduced functional connectivity in patients with schizophrenia between dopaminergic midbrain regions and the right insula, which was associated with more severe positive, psychotic symptoms (Gradin et al., 2013). In a study including individuals who experience occasional mild psychotic symptoms, but are otherwise healthy, the authors found a positive correlation between connectivity in the cingulo-opercular (CO) network and positive symptoms. The cingulo-opercular network is composed of anterior insula, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, and thalamus (Orr et al., 2014). We did not find such a correlation in healthy siblings. Our results contrast the results found in previous research; the insula was found to show decreased activity compared to healthy controls instead of less deactivation in siblings (de Leeuw et al., 2015), patients (Morris et al., 2011), and individuals with a high clinical risk (Juckel et al., 2012). These differences can possibly be explained by different methodology and the fact that our sample of siblings was particularly healthy and was especially low on subclinical positive symptoms.

As hypothesized, we did not find group differences in the predefined regions of interest in the reward consumption phase; the mPFC and the dlPFC. However, in the whole-brain analyses, we found four brain regions where siblings showed more activation than controls. Specifically, less deactivation was observed in the bilateral MFG and the PCC. Again, these regions are part of the DMN and, similar to deactivation in the anticipation phase, the findings may reflect less task-related suppression of brain activation in these areas. These results indicate that, for siblings, reduced task-related suppression of these brain areas during reward processing is present at the time of reward consumption as well as reward anticipation. In addition, siblings showed more activation in the precuneus compared to controls, this is in contrast to the findings of a previous study in patients, where patients showed increased deactivation (Garrity et al., 2007).

Taken together, our findings do not point to deficits in classical reward-related brain areas in siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Nonetheless, the results do indicate that abnormal neural responses are present during reward processing in siblings, which may suggest reward processing deficits could be partly caused by the genetic load in schizophrenia. The lack of disengagement of the DMN may reflect decreased task-related suppression during reward-related tasks for siblings, even though this is not manifest at the behavioral level. Also, since we found the same result of DMN hyperactivation during reward anticipation and reward consumption this might indicate that siblings of patients have an overall heightened DMN activity, resulting in a higher baseline level of DMN activation. Overall heightened activation of default mode structures has been reported before in patients with schizophrenia (Pomarol-Clotet et al., 2008; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2009). Abnormal functioning of the DMN in schizophrenia has intriguing theoretical possibilities. As this network normally shows decreases in activation during task performance, the failure to deactivate the default mode could be relevant to disturbed cognitive processes (Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos, 2007) and might play a role in the development of symptoms in schizophrenia. Our results also point to the probability of a spectrum of abnormal network (reward regions, DMN, salience network) functioning and aberrant connectivity underlying the psychosis continuum. The results further support the relevance of the study of first-degree relatives of patients to enhance our understanding of genetic and pathogenic processes.

Future studies should shed more light on the relation between the different networks that seem disturbed in schizophrenia and relatives, such as the reward network, the DMN and the salience network. Specifically, communication between reward-related areas and other networks might be the underlying endophenotype of reward-related symptoms in schizophrenia. In addition, investigating the DMN in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia may further highlight the importance of the DMN in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia and DMN abnormalities as a possible endophenotype in schizophrenia.

Funding

LK supported by a NWO VICI grant 453-11-005. SS supported by a Medical Research Council New Investigator Award Project grant 93641 and an ERC Consolidator Award. AA was supported by a NWO VICI grant 453-11-004.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00504

References

Bayer, H. M., and Glimcher, P. W. (2005). Midbrain dopamine neurons encode a quantitative reward prediction error signal. Neuron 47, 129–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C. (2007). The debate over dopamine's role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology 191, 391–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjork, J. M., Smith, A. R., Chen, G., and Hommer, D. W. (2010). Adolescents, adults and rewards: comparing motivational neurocircuitry recruitment using fMRI. PLoS ONE 5:e11440. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain's default network. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., and Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Leeuw, M., Kahn, R. S., and Vink, M. (2015). Fronto-striatal dysfunction during reward processing in unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr. Bull. 41, 94–103. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dosenbach, N. U., Fair, D. A., Miezin, F. M., Cohen, A. L., Wenger, K. K., Dosenbach, R. A., et al. (2007). Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 11073–11078. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704320104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drew, M. R., Simpson, E. H., Kellendonk, C., Herzberg, W. G., Lipatova, O., Fairhurst, S., et al. (2007). Transient overexpression of striatal D2 receptors impairs operant motivation and interval timing. J. Neurosci. 27, 7731–7739. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1736-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliott, R., Friston, K. J., and Dolan, R. J. (2000). Dissociable neural responses in human reward systems. J. Neurosci. 20, 6159–6165.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Forman, S. D., Cohen, J. D., Fitzgerald, M., Eddy, W. F., Mintun, M. A., and Noll, D. C. (1995). Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster−size threshold. Magn. Reson. Med. 33, 636–647. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910330508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gard, D. E., Kring, A. M., Gard, M. G., Horan, W. P., and Green, M. F. (2007). Anhedonia in schizophrenia: distinctions between anticipatory and consummatory pleasure. Schizophr. Res. 93, 253–260. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrity, A. G., Pearlson, G. D., Mckiernan, K., Lloyd, D., Kiehl, K. A., and Calhoun, V. D. (2007). Aberrant “default mode” functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 450–457. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.3.450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goebel, R., Esposito, F., and Formisano, E. (2006). Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with brainvoyager QX: from single−subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self−organizing group independent component analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 392–401. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gooding, D. C., Tallent, K. A., and Matts, C. W. (2005). Clinical status of at-risk individuals 5 years later: further validation of the psychometric high-risk strategy. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114:170. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gradin, V. B., Waiter, G., O'Connor, A., Romaniuk, L., Stickle, C., Matthews, K., et al. (2013). Salience network-midbrain dysconnectivity and blunted reward signals in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 211, 104–111. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grimm, O., Heinz, A., Walter, H., Kirsch, P., Erk, S., Haddad, L., et al. (2014). Striatal response to reward anticipation: evidence for a systems-level intermediate phenotype for schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 531–539. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartmann, M. N., Hager, O. M., Reimann, A. V., Chumbley, J. R., Kirschner, M., Seifritz, E., et al. (2014). Apathy but not diminished expression in schizophrenia is associated with discounting of monetary rewards by physical effort. Schizophr. Bull. 41, 503–512. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(14)70444-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howes, O. D., and Kapur, S. (2009). The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: version III—the final common pathway. Schizophr. Bull. 35, 549–562. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jang, J. H., Jung, W. H., Choi, J. S., Choi, C. H., Kang, D. H., Shin, N. Y., et al. (2011). Reduced prefrontal functional connectivity in the default mode network is related to greater psychopathology in subjects with high genetic loading for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 127, 58–65. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juckel, G., Friedel, E., Koslowski, M., Witthaus, H., Ozgürdal, S., Gudlowski, Y., et al. (2012). Ventral striatal activation during reward processing in subjects with ultra-high risk for schizophrenia. Neuropsychobiology 66, 50–56. doi: 10.1159/000337130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juckel, G., Schlagenhauf, F., Koslowski, M., Filonov, D., Wüstenberg, T., Villringer, A., et al. (2006a). Dysfunction of ventral striatal reward prediction in schizophrenic patients treated with typical, not atypical, neuroleptics. Psychopharmacology 187, 222–228. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0405-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juckel, G., Schlagenhauf, F., Koslowski, M., Wüstenberg, T., Villringer, A., Knutson, B., et al. (2006b). Dysfunction of ventral striatal reward prediction in schizophrenia. Neuroimage 29, 409–416. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kapur, S. (2003). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 13–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kapur, S., Mizrahi, R., and Li, M. (2005). From dopamine to salience to psychosis—linking biology, pharmacology and phenomenology of psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 79, 59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendler, K. S., and Diehl, S. R. (1993). The genetics of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 19, 261–285. doi: 10.1093/schbul/19.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirsch, P., Ronshausen, S., Mier, D., and Gallhofer, B. (2007). The influence of antipsychotic treatment on brain reward system reactivity in schizophrenia patients. Pharmacopsychiatry 40, 196–198. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-984463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W., and Hommer, D. (2001a). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci. 21, RC159.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Knutson, B., Fong, G. W., Adams, C. M., Varner, J. L., and Hommer, D. (2001b). Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI. Neuroreport 12, 3683–3687. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200112040-00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutson, B., Fong, G. W., Bennett, S. M., Adams, C. M., and Hommer, D. (2003). A region of mesial prefrontal cortex tracks monetarily rewarding outcomes: characterization with rapid event-related fMRI. Neuroimage 18, 263–272. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00057-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konings, M., Bak, M., Hanssen, M., van Os, J., and Krabbendam, L. (2006). Validity and reliability of the CAPE: a self−report instrument for the measurement of psychotic experiences in the general population. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 114, 55–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00741.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korver, N., Quee, P. J., Boos, H., Simons, C. J., and Haan, L. (2012). Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP), a multi site longitudinal cohort study focused on gene–environment interaction: objectives, sample characteristics, recruitment and assessment methods. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 21, 205–221. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, A. W. III. Thermenos, H. W., Barch, D. M., and Seidman, L. J. (2009). Imaging genetic liability to schizophrenia: systematic review of FMRI studies of patients' nonpsychotic relatives. Schizophr. Bull. 35, 1142–1162. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., and Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315, 393–395. doi: 10.1126/science.1131295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V., and Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 655–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molnar-Szakacs, I., and Arzy, S. (2009). Searching for an integrated self-representation. Commun. Integr. Biol. 2, 1–3. doi: 10.4161/cib.2.4.8290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, R., Vercammen, A., Lenroot, R., Moore, L., Langton, J., Short, B., et al. (2011). Disambiguating ventral striatum fMRI-related bold signal during reward prediction in schizophrenia. Mol. Psychiatry 17, 280–289. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.75

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, M. O., Rostrup, E., Wulff, S., Bak, N., Broberg, B. V., Lublin, H., et al. (2012a). Improvement of brain reward abnormalities by antipsychotic monotherapy in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 1195–1204. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, M. Ø., Rostrup, E., Wulff, S., Bak, N., Lublin, H., Kapur, S., et al. (2012b). Alterations of the brain reward system in antipsychotic naive schizophrenia patients. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 898–905. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orr, J. M., Turner, J. A., and Mittal, V. A. (2014). Widespread brain dysconnectivity associated with psychotic-like experiences in the general population. NeuroImage 4, 343–351. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palaniyappan, L., and Liddle, P. F. (2012). Does the salience network play a cardinal role in psychosis? An emerging hypothesis of insular dysfunction. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 37, 17. doi: 10.1503/jpn.100176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessiglione, M., Seymour, B., Flandin, G., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2006). Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature 442, 1042–1045. doi: 10.1038/nature05051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pizzagalli, D. A., Evins, A. E., Schetter, E. C., Frank, M. J., Pajtas, P. E., Santesso, D. L., et al. (2008). Single dose of a dopamine agonist impairs reinforcement learning in humans: behavioral evidence from a laboratory-based measure of reward responsiveness. Psychopharmacology 196, 221–232. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0957-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pomarol-Clotet, E., Salvador, R., Sarró, S., Gomar, J., Vila, F., Martinez, A., et al. (2008). Failure to deactivate in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: dysfunction of the default mode network? Psychol. Med. 38, 1185–1193. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rademacher, L., Krach, S., Kohls, G., Irmak, A., Gründer, G., and Spreckelmeyer, K. N. (2010). Dissociation of neural networks for anticipation and consumption of monetary and social rewards. Neuroimage 49, 3276–3285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., Macleod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (1996). Neurobehavioural mechanisms of reward and motivation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6, 228–236. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80077-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlagenhauf, F., Juckel, G., Koslowski, M., Kahnt, T., Knutson, B., Dembler, T., et al. (2008). Reward system activation in schizophrenic patients switched from typical neuroleptics to olanzapine. Psychopharmacology 196, 673–684. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-1016-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlagenhauf, F., Sterzer, P., Schmack, K., Ballmaier, M., Rapp, M., Wrase, J., et al. (2009). Reward feedback alterations in unmedicated schizophrenia patients: relevance for delusions. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 1032–1039. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W. (1997). Dopamine neurons and their role in reward mechanisms. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 191–197. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80007-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 1–27.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schultz, W. (2006). Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology of reward. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 87–115. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, J. J., Biller, A., Walther, S., Roesch-Ely, D., Stippich, C., Weisbrod, M., et al. (2010). Neural correlates of reward processing in schizophrenia—relationship to apathy and depression. Schizophr. Res. 118, 154–161. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J., and Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: a neurobiological hypothesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 977–986. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, W. S., Faraone, S. V., Seidman, L. J., Olson, E. A., and Tsuang, M. T. (2005). Searching for the liability to schizophrenia: concepts and methods underlying genetic high-risk studies of adolescents. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 15, 403–417. doi: 10.1089/cap.2005.15.403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, G. P., Waltz, J. A., and Gold, J. M. (2014). A review of reward processing and motivational impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 40, S107–S116. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urban, N. B., Slifstein, M., Meda, S., Xu, X., Ayoub, R., Medina, O., et al. (2012). Imaging human reward processing with positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychopharmacology 221, 67–77. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2543-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walter, H., Kammerer, H., Frasch, K., Spitzer, M., and Abler, B. (2009). Altered reward functions in patients on atypical antipsychotic medication in line with the revised dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology 206, 121–132. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1586-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, T. P., Joseph, V., Francis, S. T., and Liddle, P. F. (2010). Aberrant salience network (bilateral insula and anterior cingulate cortex) connectivity during information processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 123, 105–115. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Thermenos, H. W., Milanovic, S., Tsuang, M. T., Faraone, S. V., McCarley, R. W., et al. (2009). Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default network in schizophrenia and in first-degree relatives of persons with schizophrenia. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 106, 1279–1284. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809141106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zink, C. F., Pagnoni, G., Martin-Skurski, M. E., Chappelow, J. C., and Berns, G. S. (2004). Human striatal responses to monetary reward depend on saliency. Neuron 42, 509–517. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00183-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: schizophrenia, siblings, reward processing, fMRI, default mode network (DMN)

Citation: Hanssen E, van der Velde J, Gromann PM, Shergill SS, de Haan L, Bruggeman R, Krabbendam L, Aleman A and van Atteveldt N (2015) Neural correlates of reward processing in healthy siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:504. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00504

Received: 30 January 2015; Accepted: 31 August 2015;
Published: 23 September 2015.

Edited by:

Megan Ann Boudewyn, University of California, Davis, USA

Reviewed by:

Arun Bokde, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
Laura Magdalen Tully, University of California, Davis Medical Center, USA

Copyright © 2015 Hanssen, van der Velde, Gromann, Shergill, de Haan, Bruggeman, Krabbendam, Aleman and van Atteveldt. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Esther Hanssen, Department of Educational Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University Amsterdam, Van der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT Amsterdam, Netherlands, esther.hanssen@vu.nl

Download