Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 29 October 2015
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 9 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00597

Structural and functional plasticity specific to musical training with wind instruments

  • 1Neuroscience Research Institute, Gachon University of Medicine and Science, Incheon, South Korea
  • 2Kansei Fukushi Research Institute, Tohoku Fukushi University, Sendai, Japan
  • 3Reid School of Music, Edinburgh College of Art, Institute for Music and Human Society Development, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK

Numerous neuroimaging studies have shown structural and functional changes resulting from musical training. Among these studies, changes in primary sensory areas are mostly related to motor functions. In this study, we looked for some similar functional and structural changes in other functional modalities, such as somatosensory function, by examining the effects of musical training with wind instruments. We found significant changes in two aspects of neuroplasticity, cortical thickness, and resting-state neuronal networks. A group of subjects with several years of continuous musical training and who are currently playing in university wind ensembles showed differences in cortical thickness in lip- and tongue-related brain areas vs. non-music playing subjects. Cortical thickness in lip-related brain areas was significantly thicker and that in tongue-related areas was significantly thinner in the music playing group compared with that in the non-music playing group. Association analysis of lip-related areas in the music playing group showed that the increase in cortical thickness was caused by musical training. In addition, seed-based correlation analysis showed differential activation in the precentral gyrus and supplementary motor areas (SMA) between the music and non-music playing groups. These results suggest that high-intensity training with specific musical instruments could induce structural changes in related anatomical areas and could also generate a new functional neuronal network in the brain.

Introduction

Structural and functional changes in the human brain resulting from musical training have been noninvasively studied using various imaging modalities, including magnetic resonance imaging (MRI; Schlaug, 2001; Münte et al., 2002; Schmithorst and Wilke, 2002; Schlaug et al., 2005, 2009; Hyde et al., 2009; Penhune, 2011; Steele et al., 2013). Two main approaches have been used in MRI studies: structural and functional. For structural approaches, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), cortical thickness analysis, and voxel-based morphometry (VBM) have been widely used.

Changes in the white matter (WM) of musicians have been demonstrated by DTI (Schlaug et al., 1995; Rüber et al., 2015). Musicians often show different fractional anisotropy (FA) maps in several brain areas compared with non-musicians (Schmithorst and Wilke, 2002; Bengtsson et al., 2005; Halwani et al., 2011; Engel et al., 2014; Moore et al., 2014). For example, they showed lower FA maps compared with non-musicians in the corticospinal tract, corona radiate, and internal capsules bilaterally (Schmithorst and Wilke, 2002; Imfeld et al., 2009), but higher FA maps in the right posterior limb of the internal capsule (Han et al., 2009) and corpus callosum (Schmithorst and Wilke, 2002; Steele et al., 2013). FA values are often used to study the strength of the WM fiber tract. Low FA values mean weak connectivity of the fiber tract, and high FA values mean strong connectivity of the fiber tract (Schmithorst and Wilke, 2002). Even within some groups of musicians, different FA maps can be found depending on whether or not they have absolute pitch (AP), which is the ability to detect a pitch without any tonal reference (Oechslin et al., 2010). Musicians with AP showed significantly higher FA maps compared with those without it in the subgyral WM of the right temporal lobe, particularly within the path of the inferior fronto-occipital and inferior longitudinal fasciculus (Dohn et al., 2015).

Changes in the gray matter (GM) have been demonstrated by cortical thickness analysis and VBM. It has been reported that musicians have greater cortical thickness in the superior temporal and dorsolateral frontal regions as a result of their training (Bermudez et al., 2009). Musicians who were trained at a younger age showed greater cortical thickness in the ventral premotor cortex compared with those who were trained at an older age (Bailey et al., 2014). Among the other brain areas that showed different cortical thickness, Heschl's and bilateral intraparietal sulci strongly correlated with musical performances in relative pitch tasks (Foster and Zatorre, 2010). Along with cortical thickness analysis, VBM is also a very useful tool for finding structural changes. Musicians have been shown to have a significant increase in the volume of GM in the right fusiform (James et al., 2014), right posterior and middle cingulate, right and left superior temporal (Schneider et al., 2002; Bermudez and Zatorre, 2005), and right inferior orbitofrontal gyri as well as in the Broca's area in the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) that is associated with music-related abilities (Sluming et al., 2002; Fauvel et al., 2014), premotor cortex (Gaser and Schlaug, 2003), cerebellum (Hutchinson et al., 2003), and hippocampus (Groussard et al., 2010). Musicians who were trained before the age of seven showed an increased volume of the vPMC compared with musicians who were trained at an older age (Bailey et al., 2014).

For functional studies, activation of brain areas and functional connectivity among them have been measured to examine neuronal differences between the musicians and non-musicians. A representative functional difference between the musicians and non-musicians in functional MRI (fMRI) studies showed strong activation in auditory and motor areas, which are known to play crucial roles in musical activities (Baumann et al., 2005). Other studies have reported that non-musicians show a significant decrease in the brain activation in the right superior temporal gyrus when listening to trained melodies (Chen et al., 2012). Among musicians, enhancement of functional activity was found in the left middle and superior temporal gyri, left IFG, and right ventromedial prefrontal cortex in response to pattern deviation (Habermeyer et al., 2009). While performing musical tasks, musicians showed increased audiovisual asynchrony responses and effective connectivities in their superior temporal sulcus-premotor-cerebellar circuitry (Lee and Noppeney, 2011). Musicians also showed different functional connectivities not only during the task but also at rest. Furthermore, musicians also showed strong functional connectivity among the cingulate, right prefrontal cortex, and left temporal pole (Fauvel et al., 2014).

Most previous studies investigating either structural or functional changes in the brain have focused on musicians who play keyboard (Jäncke et al., 2000; Krings et al., 2000; Itoh et al., 2001; Haslinger et al., 2004; Meister et al., 2004; Parsons et al., 2005; Bengtsson et al., 2005; Baumann et al., 2007; Han et al., 2009; Gärtner et al., 2013; Engel et al., 2014; Alves-Pinto et al., 2015) or string instruments (Kim et al., 2004; Norton et al., 2005; Bangert and Schlaug, 2006; Vaalto et al., 2013; Verrel et al., 2013; Vollmann et al., 2014; Rüber et al., 2015). The instruments in those studies require the use of hands, and brain areas that showed specific functional and structural changes in those studies were related to motor activities. These functional studies reported different brain activation associated with motor practice (Jäncke et al., 2000; Haslinger et al., 2004) and functional connectivity between auditory and motor areas (Krings et al., 2000). Structural changes were also shown in both WM (Bengtsson et al., 2005) and GM (Han et al., 2009).

While many previous music-related studies have been conducted, none has examined neuroplasticity relating to wind instruments, which mainly require the mouth (lips and tongue) to play, in contrast to instruments described above, which mainly use other body parts, such as hands. Although the mouth is also used for singing (Sundberg, 1975; Ladefoged, 1978; Sundberg and Rossing, 1990), mouth usage while singing is different from playing wind instruments because the mouth interacts with objects when playing the latter (Gallivan and Eitnier, 2006; Wolfe et al., 2009).

In the current study, we hypothesized that similar structural and functional changes would occur in other brain modalities, such as the somatosensory, and examined the effects of musical training with the subjects who play wind instruments (“music playing group”) compared with the subjects who do not play music (“non-music playing group”). Wind instrument players are expected to develop their lips and tongue touches. Therefore, we assumed that the structures of somatosensory areas of the brain that are associated with the lips and tongue would differ between the music and non-music playing groups. In addition, we performed a resting-state fMRI analysis to investigate the possibility of functional connectivity modifications due to musical training.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Fourteen music playing [all female; mean age ± standard deviation (SD), 20.35 ± 1.21 years] and 14 non-music playing subjects (all female; mean age ± SD, 20.14 ± 1.23 years) participated in this study. Those included in the music playing group played wind instruments (woodwinds and brass) for more than 7 years (mean ± SD, 7.93 ± 1.21 years) as members of junior and senior high school and university wind ensembles. In contrast, non-music playing subjects did not have any extra musical experience except for regular music classes in school.

None of the subjects had a history of neurological disease or any medical conditions (i.e., pregnancy, use of a cardiac pacemaker, or claustrophobia). After subjects were given a complete description of the study, written informed consent was obtained in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Tohoku Fukushi University (Japan).

MRI Data Acquisition

All subjects were scanned in two sessions that included structural (T1) and functional imaging (resting-state fMRI). Structural images were acquired using the following parameters: repetition time = 1900 ms, echo time = 2.52 ms, matrix size = 256 × 256, in-plane resolution = 1 × 1 mm2, slice thickness = 1 mm, and number of slices = 192. Resting-state fMRI data were acquired using the following parameters: repetition time = 2000 ms, echo time = 30 ms, matrix size = 64 × 64, in-plane resolution = 3.4 × 3.4 mm2, slice thickness = 3.4 mm, and number of volumes = 150. In the resting-state fMRI session, subjects were asked to lie on a bed and try to think about nothing about nothing with closed eyes.

Data Analysis

All data were analyzed with Brainvoyager QX software (Brain Innovation B.V., Maastricht, Netherlands). Structural images were spatially normalized through Talairach transformation with Brainvoyager QX. In the first part of the transformation, we detected the anterior and posterior commissure as landmarks for the y-axis of the Talairach coordinate system. In the anterior commissure, the x-axis runs from the left to the right hemisphere, while the z-axis runs from the inferior to the superior part of the brain. All boundaries were decided manually. After the anterior-posterior commissure transformation, the brain was separated by 12 sub-cuboids. These 12 sub-cuboids were expanded or shrunken linearly to correspond to the size standard Talairach brain sub-cuboids. These normalized T1 images were corrected by an intensity inhomogeneity correction (Sled et al., 1997; Hou et al., 2006). After this correction, non-cortical structures [other than GM, WM, and cerebral spinal fluid (CSF)] were removed by a brain extraction process. The extracted T1 images were segmented by several types of brain tissues (i.e., GM, WM, and CSF) with different ranges of intensities.

For cortical thickness measurements, we first defined boundary voxels, one at the WM–GM boundary and one at the GM–CSF boundary. The values of those boundary voxels were kept, and the intensities between the GM voxels were smoothed by Laplace's equation (Jones et al., 2000). From the smoothed values, the gradient value of each voxel was calculated and then streamlines with those values were made. To calculate cortical thickness, we checked the gradient and evaluated along the gradient direction. This process was repeated until we could approach other boundaries, such as WM or CSF. The sum of the process provides the cortical thickness. Cortical thickness measures were calculated by processes within Brainvoyager QX. Individual cortical thickness measures were aligned to each normalized cortex mesh for group analysis. Cortical thickness maps were calculated for all subjects.

Region of interest (ROI)-based analysis is known to be very useful in studies of specific brain areas (Gur et al., 2000; Singh et al., 2014; Kauttonen et al., 2015). Therefore, we performed a ROI-based analysis wherein the postcentral gyrus was defined as a ROI containing somatosensory areas (Biswal et al., 1995; Stippich et al., 1999; Ruben et al., 2001; Nguyen et al., 2004; Miyamoto et al., 2006). Cortical thickness maps of the two groups were analyzed by a two sample t-test in the ROI, and the result was corrected for multiple comparison at a cluster-level threshold of p < 0.005 by Brainvoyager QX (Forman et al., 1995; Goebel et al., 2006).

One subject in the non-music playing group was excluded from the analysis of resting-state functional images because of large head motion. The images of 25 subjects were preprocessed by slice scan time correction, 3D motion correction, and high pass temporal filtering (only signals with relatively high frequency >0.01 Hz). These functional images were smoothed with 6-mm FWHM and coregistered with each structural image. All resting-state functional images were analyzed by seed-based correlation analysis using Brainvoyager QX. In the analysis, the time course of seed ROIs were correlated with whole brain and correlation maps were created. Seed ROIs were those brain regions that showed different cortical thickness between the music and non-music playing groups. Individual correlation maps were made from seed ROIs. Correlation maps were analyzed by two sample t-test for two groups, and the resultant map was corrected for multiple comparison at a cluster-level threshold of p < 0.05.

Results

Cortical Thickness Analysis

Cortical thickness difference maps for music and non-music playing groups were evaluated using a two sample t-test (p < 0.005, corrected). Two brain areas, which showed significant differences between the two groups, were found in the postcentral gyrus of the right hemisphere (Figure 1). The music playing group showed a thicker cortex in an area of the anterior part of the postcentral gyrus corresponding to the lips (Breshears et al., 2015) [t-value = 3.41; p-value = 0.003; cluster size = 9.71 mm2; Brodmann's area (BA) 3; Talairach coordinates: x = 58, y = −14, z = 44] but a thinner cortex in an area of the posterior part of the postcentral gyrus corresponding to the tongue (Breshears et al., 2015) [t-value = − 3.51; p-value = 0.002; cluster size = 10.93 mm2; BA2; Talairach coordinates: x = 64, y = −14, z = 35] compared with the non-music playing group. Areas with significantly different cortical thickness were subjected to ROI-based correlation analysis of thickness and years of musical training (association analysis). In the lip-related area of the postcentral gyrus, three participants of the music playing group were found as outliers at the 99% confidence interval (Figure 2A). Therefore, these three data points were excluded, and the remaining 11 subjects' data were used for the correlation analysis, which revealed significant and positive high correlations between the thickness and years of musical training (r = 0.761, p = 0.006 by Pearson correlation; Figure 3A). This means that changes in cortical thickness resulted from musical training. In the same way, in the tongue-related area, three participants in the music playing group were also found as outliers at 99% confidence interval (Figure 2B). Therefore, these three data points were excluded, and the remaining 11 subjects' data were used for the correlation analysis. However, there was no significant correlation between the thickness and years of musical training in this area (r = 0.262, p = 0.437 by Pearson correlation; Figure 3B).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A surface atlas showing of the postcentral gyrus and a cortical thickness difference map. The orange represents the area in which the music playing group had greater cortical thickness than the non-music playing group. The blue represents the area in which the music playing group had less cortical thickness than the non-music playing group. Both thicker and thinner areas were statistically significant (p < 0.005, corrected).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Confidence interval (CI) for cortical thickness and musical training years in lip- (A) and tongue-related (B) brain areas.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Correlation between cortical thickness and musical training years in lip- (A) and tongue-related (B) brain areas.

Resting-state fMRI Analysis

Seed-based correlation analysis of tongue-related areas of the brain did not show any significantly different maps between the music and non-music playing groups. Correlation analysis of lip-related areas revealed some significantly different maps between the two groups (higher correlation in the music playing group) (p < 0.05, corrected) in the precentral gyrus and supplementary motor areas (SMA) bilaterally (BA6; Figure 4). The correlation map located in the lower part of the precentral gyrus of the left hemisphere (BA6) was in the motor-related mouth region and corresponded to the somatosensory mouth region. Correlation maps located in the middle part of the precentral gyrus (BA4) of both hemispheres were in motor-related hand regions (the middle part was clearly separated from upper and lower parts in the left hemisphere but not separated from those in the right hemisphere). These results indicate that cortical thickness changes in somatosensory areas were related to new neuronal network developments in primary and supplemental motor areas.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correlation map differences between music and non-music playing groups from lip-related brain areas (higher correlation in the music playing group); left column, left hemisphere; right column, right hemisphere. Both brain hemispheres showed differences in the precentral gyrus and SMA bilaterally. All areas were corrected for multiple comparison at a cluster-level threshold of p < 0.05.

Discussion

The aim of our study was to determine how musical training with wind instruments causes structural and functional changes in the brain that could be attributed to specific ways of playing instruments. We mostly focused on somatosensory areas. Participants in this fMRI study were university students who received several years of training on wind instruments in junior and senior high school and are now members of reputable university wind ensembles. Participants chosen as non-music playing were university students with no special musical training. Cortical thickness analysis of structural plasticity in the brain showed significant differences in two areas of the postcentral gyrus associated with the lips and tongue according to previous reports (Pulvermüller et al., 2006; Meister et al., 2007; Brown et al., 2008; Venezia et al., 2012). In addition, correlation analysis in the lip-related area revealed a significant positive correlation between the thickness and years of musical training demonstrating that cortical thickness changes were longitudinal. In addition, seed-based correlation analysis by resting-state fMRI also revealed some areas that significantly differed between the music and non-music playing groups in the correlation map of lip-related areas, including those within the precentral gyrus and SMA, which are known to be related to learning mouth and motor skills and musical activity (Chen et al., 2012; Grabski et al., 2012; Gebel et al., 2013; Kim and Shin, 2014).

Along with changes in lip- and tongue-related brain areas, some reports suggest that playing wind instruments could result in physical changes to related body parts (i.e., the tongue and lips). For example, the muscles of the lips and tongue may change while playing a wind instrument (Methfessel, 1990). Also, lips are crucial to musicians who use only a single reed that physically produces sound on a wind instrument (Fuhrimann et al., 1987; Benade, 1990; Chen and Weinreich, 1996). Therefore, training with wind instruments can physically alter the lips and tongue, leading to changes in corresponding somatosensory brain areas (Draganski et al., 2006) as found in the current study. The positive correlation between the cortical thickness and training period in lip-related brain areas strongly suggests that changes in these areas were caused by musical training over several years.

The thickening of lip-associated brain areas can be explained in the same way as increased GM volume in certain areas associated with other types of training, such as juggling and piano playing. In contrast, the cause of cortical thinning in tongue-related brain areas is not easy to explain. Explanations for the cortical thickness decrease can be found in several previous studies. One explanation stems from previous studies showing sharpening in populations of neurons frequently recruited for a specific function (Desimone, 1996; Wiggs and Martin, 1998; Grill-Spector et al., 2006). Another possible explanation is that an increase in WM may cause a decrease in the ratio of GM in an imaging voxel as shown from some reports in which WM increased with musical training (Bengtsson et al., 2005; Zatorre, 2013). However, further study is needed to clarify the cause of this decrease as it relates to wind instrument training.

By resting-state fMRI, the music playing group showed stronger correlations with some areas in the precentral gyrus and SMA, and the relationship between precentral areas and SMAs with music has been found in previous studies. The precentral gyrus has been shown to be involved in musical instrument playing (Schieber, 1990; Amunts et al., 1997), musical training (Lin et al., 2002), and musical ability (Schlaug, 2001), while SMAs have been shown to be involved in musical training, especially in brain networks associated with rhythmic motor movements (Zatorre et al., 2007). In addition, the precentral gyrus and SMA are also known to interact closely for learning of musical instruments (Kim and Shin, 2014).

Unfortunately, the current study is limited in its design. Because the present study was designed to be cross-sectional, we cannot exclude the possibility that the music playing group members' abilities come from certain anatomical advantages over those of the non-music playing group rather than functional changes resulting from musical training. However, the positive correlation between the cortical thickness and years of musical training in lip-related brain areas suggests otherwise. This longitudinal evidence and the appearance of the new functional neuronal network in the music playing group support our interpretation that cortical thickness changes reflect changes in neuronal plasticity by musical training.

In conclusion, we found structural and functional changes in lip- and tongue-related areas in the postcentral gyrus related to longitudinal musical training with wind instruments. These results indicate that high-intensity musical training can change the structure and function of the brain. Furthermore, the current findings support the idea of altered neuroplasticity by musical training.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by MEXT-Supported Program for the Strategic Research Foundation at Private Universities 2014–2018 and the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) Grant funded by Korea Government (MSIP) (no. NRF-2014M3C7033998).

References

Alves-Pinto, A., Turova, V., Blumenstein, T., Thienel, A., Wohlschläger, A., and Lampe, R. (2015). fMRI assessment of neuroplasticity in youths with neurodevelopmental-associated motor disorders after piano training. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 19, 15–28. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2014.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amunts, K., Schlaug, G., Jäncke, L., Steinmetz, H., Schleicher, A., Dabringhaus, A., et al. (1997). Motor cortex and hand motor skills: structural compliance in the human brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 5, 206–215.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bailey, J. A., Zatorre, R. J., and Penhune, V. B. (2014). Early musical training is linked to gray matter structure in the ventral premotor cortex and auditory-motor rhythm synchronization performance. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 755–767. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bangert, M., and Schlaug, G. (2006). Specialization of the specialized in features of external human brain morphology. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 1832–1834. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05031.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumann, S., Koeneke, S., Meyer, M., Lutz, K., and Jäncke, L. (2005). A network for sensory-motor integration. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1060, 186–188. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumann, S., Koeneke, S., Schmidt, C. F., Meyer, M., Lutz, K., and Jancke, L. (2007). A network for audio–motor coordination in skilled pianists and non-musicians. Brain Res. 1161, 65–78. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.05.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benade, A. H. (1990). Fundamentals of Musical Acoustics: Second, Revised Edition, 2nd Edn, Revised Edition. New York, NY: Dover Publications.

Bengtsson, S. L., Nagy, Z., Skare, S., Forsman, L., Forssberg, H., and Ullén, F. (2005). Extensive piano practicing has regionally specific effects on white matter development. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1148–1150. doi: 10.1038/nn1516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bermudez, P., Lerch, J. P., Evans, A. C., and Zatorre, R. J. (2009). Neuroanatomical correlates of musicianship as revealed by cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1583–1596. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bermudez, P., and Zatorre, R. J. (2005). Differences in gray matter between musicians and nonmusicians. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1060, 395–399. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breshears, J. D., Molinaro, A. M., and Chang, E. F. (2015). A probabilistic map of the human ventral sensorimotor cortex using electrical stimulation. J. Neurosurg. 123, 340–349. doi: 10.3171/2014.11.JNS14889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, S., Ngan, E., and Liotti, M. (2008). A larynx area in the human motor cortex. Cereb. Cortex 18, 837–845. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F.-C., and Weinreich, G. (1996). Nature of the lip reed. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 99, 1227–1233. doi: 10.1121/1.414604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J. L., Rae, C., and Watkins, K. E. (2012). Learning to play a melody: an fMRI study examining the formation of auditory-motor associations. Neuroimage 59, 1200–1208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 13494–13499. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dohn, A., Garza-Villarreal, E. A., Chakravarty, M. M., Hansen, M., Lerch, J. P., and Vuust, P. (2015). Gray- and White-matter anatomy of absolute pitch possessors. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1379–1388. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H. G., Winkler, J., Büchel, C., et al. (2006). Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 26, 6314–6317. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4628-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, A., Hijmans, B. S., Cerliani, L., Bangert, M., Nanetti, L., Keller, P. E., et al. (2014). Inter-individual differences in audio-motor learning of piano melodies and white matter fiber tract architecture. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 2483–2497. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fauvel, B., Groussard, M., Chételat, G., Fouquet, M., Landeau, B., Eustache, F., et al. (2014). Morphological brain plasticity induced by musical expertise is accompanied by modulation of functional connectivity at rest. Neuroimage 90, 179–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forman, S. D., Cohen, J. D., Fitzgerald, M., Eddy, W. F., Mintun, M. A., and Noll, D. C. (1995). Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn. Reson. Med. Off. J. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. 33, 636–647. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910330508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, N. E. V., and Zatorre, R. J. (2010). Cortical structure predicts success in performing musical transformation judgments. Neuroimage 53, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuhrimann, S., Schüpbach, A., Thüer, U., and Ingervall, B. (1987). Natural lip function in wind instrument players. Eur. J. Orthod. 9, 216–223. doi: 10.1093/ejo/9.1.216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallivan, G. J., and Eitnier, C. M. (2006). Vocal fold polyp in a professional brass/wind instrumentalist and singer. J. Voice 20, 157–164. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2004.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gärtner, H., Minnerop, M., Pieperhoff, P., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., Altenmüller, E., et al. (2013). Brain morphometry shows effects of long-term musical practice in middle-aged keyboard players. Audit. Cogn. Neurosci. 4:636. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Gaser, C., and Schlaug, G. (2003). Gray matter differences between musicians and nonmusicians. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 999, 514–517. doi: 10.1196/annals.1284.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gebel, B., Braun, C., Kaza, E., Altenmüller, E., and Lotze, M. (2013). Instrument specific brain activation in sensorimotor and auditory representation in musicians. Neuroimage 74, 37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goebel, R., Esposito, F., and Formisano, E. (2006). Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with brainvoyager QX: from single-subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self-organizing group independent component analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 392–401. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabski, K., Lamalle, L., Vilain, C., Schwartz, J.-L., Vallée, N., Tropres, I., et al. (2012). Functional MRI assessment of orofacial articulators: neural correlates of lip, jaw, larynx, and tongue movements. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 2306–2321. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grill-Spector, K., Henson, R., and Martin, A. (2006). Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groussard, M., La Joie, R., Rauchs, G., Landeau, B., Chételat, G., Viader, F., et al. (2010). When music and long-term memory interact: effects of musical expertise on functional and structural plasticity in the hippocampus. PLoS ONE 5:e13225. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gur, R. C., Alsop, D., Glahn, D., Petty, R., Swanson, C. L., Maldjian, J. A., et al. (2000). An fMRI study of sex differences in regional activation to a verbal and a spatial task. Brain Lang. 74, 157–170. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Habermeyer, B., Herdener, M., Esposito, F., Hilti, C. C., Klarhöfer, M., di Salle, F., et al. (2009). Neural correlates of pre-attentive processing of pattern deviance in professional musicians. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3736–3747. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halwani, G. F., Loui, P., Rüber, T., and Schlaug, G. (2011). Effects of practice and experience on the arcuate fasciculus: comparing singers, instrumentalists, and non-musicians. Front. Psychol. 2:156. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, Y., Yang, H., Lv, Y.-T., Zhu, C.-Z., He, Y., Tang, H.-H., et al. (2009). Gray matter density and white matter integrity in pianists' brain: a combined structural and diffusion tensor MRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 459, 3–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haslinger, B., Erhard, P., Altenmüller, E., Hennenlotter, A., Schwaiger, M., Gräfin von Einsiedel, H., et al. (2004). Reduced recruitment of motor association areas during bimanual coordination in concert pianists. Hum. Brain Mapp. 22, 206–215. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hou, Z., Huang, S., Hu, Q., and Nowinski, W. L. (2006). “A fast and automatic method to correct intensity inhomogeneity in MR brain images,” in Medical Image Computing and Computer-Assisted Intervention – MICCAI 2006 Lecture Notes in Computer Science, eds R. Larsen, M. Nielsen, and J. Sporring (Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer), 324–331. Available online at: http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/11866763_40 (Accessed December 1, 2014).

Hutchinson, S., Lee, L. H.-L., Gaab, N., and Schlaug, G. (2003). Cerebellar volume of musicians. Cereb. Cortex 13, 943–949. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.9.943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A. C., et al. (2009). Musical training shapes structural brain development. J. Neurosci. 29, 3019–3025. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5118-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imfeld, A., Oechslin, M. S., Meyer, M., Loenneker, T., and Jancke, L. (2009). White matter plasticity in the corticospinal tract of musicians: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuroimage 46, 600–607. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itoh, K., Fujii, Y., Suzuki, K., and Nakada, T. (2001). Asymmetry of parietal lobe activation during piano performance: a high field functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci. Lett. 309, 41–44. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02024-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, C. E., Oechslin, M. S., Van De Ville, D., Hauert, C.-A., Descloux, C., and Lazeyras, F. (2014). Musical training intensity yields opposite effects on grey matter density in cognitive versus sensorimotor networks. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 353–366. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0504-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jäncke, L., Shah, N. J., and Peters, M. (2000). Cortical activations in primary and secondary motor areas for complex bimanual movements in professional pianists. Cogn. Brain Res. 10, 177–183. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00028-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, S. E., Buchbinder, B. R., and Aharon, I. (2000). Three-dimensional mapping of cortical thickness using Laplace's Equation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 11, 12–32. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200009)11:1<12::AID-HBM20>3.0.CO;2-K

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kauttonen, J., Hlushchuk, Y., and Tikka, P. (2015). Optimizing methods for linking cinematic features to fMRI data. Neuroimage 110, 136–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, D.-E., Shin, M.-J., Lee, K.-M., Chu, K., Woo, S. H., Kim, Y. R., et al. (2004). Musical training-induced functional reorganization of the adult brain: functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation study on amateur string players. Hum. Brain Mapp. 23, 188–199. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. K., and Shin, S. H. (2014). Comparison of effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area in motor skill learning (randomized, cross over study). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:937. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krings, T., Töpper, R., Foltys, H., Erberich, S., Sparing, R., Willmes, K., et al. (2000). Cortical activation patterns during complex motor tasks in piano players and control subjects. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci. Lett. 278, 189–193. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00930-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladefoged, P. (1978). Physiology of speech production: an introduction for speech scientists by W. J. Hardcastle (review). Language 54, 757–758. doi: 10.1353/lan.1978.0013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H., and Noppeney, U. (2011). Long-term music training tunes how the brain temporally binds signals from multiple senses. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, E1441–E1450. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1115267108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, K.-L., Kobayashi, M., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2002). Effects of musical training on speech-induced modulation in corticospinal excitability. Neuroreport 13, 899–902. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200205070-00033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meister, I. G., Krings, T., Foltys, H., Boroojerdi, B., Müller, M., Töpper, R., et al. (2004). Playing piano in the mind—an fMRI study on music imagery and performance in pianists. Cogn. Brain Res. 19, 219–228. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meister, I. G., Wilson, S. M., Deblieck, C., Wu, A. D., and Iacoboni, M. (2007). The essential role of premotor cortex in speech perception. Curr. Biol. 17, 1692–1696. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Methfessel, G. (1990). [Myofunction in players of wind instruments]. Dtsch. Zahnärztl. Z. 45, S48–50.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Miyamoto, J. J., Honda, M., Saito, D. N., Okada, T., Ono, T., Ohyama, K., et al. (2006). The representation of the human oral area in the somatosensory cortex: a functional MRI study. Cereb. Cortex 16, 669–675. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, E., Schaefer, R. S., Bastin, M. E., Roberts, N., and Overy, K. (2014). Can musical training influence brain connectivity? Evidence from diffusion tensor MRI. Brain Sci. 4, 405–427. doi: 10.3390/brainsci4020405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Münte, T. F., Altenmüller, E., and Jäncke, L. (2002). The musician's brain as a model of neuroplasticity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 473–478. doi: 10.1038/nrn843

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, B. T., Tran, T. D., Hoshiyama, M., Inui, K., and Kakigi, R. (2004). Face representation in the human primary somatosensory cortex. Neurosci. Res. 50, 227–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2004.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norton, A., Winner, E., Cronin, K., Overy, K., Lee, D. J., and Schlaug, G. (2005). Are there pre-existing neural, cognitive, or motoric markers for musical ability? Brain Cogn. 59, 124–134. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oechslin, M. S., Imfeld, A., Loenneker, T., Meyer, M., Jäncke, L., Oechslin, M. S., et al. (2010). The plasticity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus as a function of musical expertise: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 3:76. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.076.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, L. M., Sergent, J., Hodges, D. A., and Fox, P. T. (2005). The brain basis of piano performance. Neuropsychologia 43, 199–215. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penhune, V. B. (2011). Sensitive periods in human development: evidence from musical training. Cortex 47, 1126–1137. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F., Huss, M., Kherif, F., Martin, F. M. del P., Hauk, O., and Shtyrov, Y. (2006). Motor cortex maps articulatory features of speech sounds. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 7865–7870. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509989103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruben, J., Schwiemann, J., Deuchert, M., Meyer, R., Krause, T., Curio, G., et al. (2001). Somatotopic organization of human secondary somatosensory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 11, 463–473. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.5.463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rüber, T., Lindenberg, R., and Schlaug, G. (2015). Differential adaptation of descending motor tracts in musicians. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1490–1498. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schieber, M. H. (1990). How might the motor cortex individuate movements? Trends Neurosci. 13, 440–445. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90093-P

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlaug, G. (2001). The brain of musicians. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 930, 281–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05739.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlaug, G., Forgeard, M., Zhu, L., Norton, A., Norton, A., and Winner, E. (2009). Training-induced neuroplasticity in young children. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1169, 205–208. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04842.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlaug, G., Jäncke, L., Huang, Y., Staiger, J. F., and Steinmetz, H. (1995). Increased corpus callosum size in musicians. Neuropsychologia 33, 1047–1055. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00045-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlaug, G., Norton, A., Overy, K., and Winner, E. (2005). Effects of music training on the child's brain and cognitive development. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1060, 219–230. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmithorst, V. J., and Wilke, M. (2002). Differences in white matter architecture between musicians and non-musicians: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neurosci. Lett. 321, 57–60. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00054-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, P., Scherg, M., Dosch, H. G., Specht, H. J., Gutschalk, A., and Rupp, A. (2002). Morphology of Heschl's gyrus reflects enhanced activation in the auditory cortex of musicians. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 688–694. doi: 10.1038/nn871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, M. K., Chang, K. D., Kelley, R. G., Saggar, M., Reiss, A. L., and Gotlib, I. H. (2014). Early signs of anomalous neural functional connectivity in healthy offspring of parents with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 16, 678–689. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sled, J. G., Zijdenbos, A. P., and Evans, A. C. (1997). “A comparison of retrospective intensity non-uniformity correction methods for MRI,” in Information Processing in Medical Imaging Lecture Notes in Computer Science, eds J. Duncan and G. Gindi (Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer), 459–464. Available online at: http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/3-540-63046-5_43 (Accessed December 1, 2014).

Sluming, V., Barrick, T., Howard, M., Cezayirli, E., Mayes, A., and Roberts, N. (2002). Voxel-based morphometry reveals increased gray matter density in broca's area in male symphony orchestra musicians. Neuroimage 17, 1613–1622. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, C. J., Bailey, J. A., Zatorre, R. J., and Penhune, V. B. (2013). Early musical training and white-matter plasticity in the corpus callosum: evidence for a sensitive period. J. Neurosci. 33, 1282–1290. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3578-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stippich, C., Hofmann, R., Kapfer, D., Hempel, E., Heiland, S., Jansen, O., et al. (1999). Somatotopic mapping of the human primary somatosensory cortex by fully automated tactile stimulation using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci. Lett. 277, 25–28. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00835-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundberg, J. (1975). Formant technique in a professional female singer. Acta Acust. United Acust. 32, 89–96.

Google Scholar

Sundberg, J., and Rossing, T. (1990). The Science of singing voice. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 87, 462–463. doi: 10.1121/1.399243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaalto, S., Julkunen, P., Säisänen, L., Könönen, M., Määttä, S., and Karhu, J. (2013). Long-term plasticity may be manifested as reduction or expansion of cortical representations of actively used muscles in motor skill specialists: Neuroreport 24, 596–600. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283628636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venezia, J. H., Saberi, K., Chubb, C., and Hickock, G. (2012). Response bias modulates the speech motor system during syllable discrimination. Audit. Cogn. Neurosci. 3:157. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verrel, J., Pologe, S., Manselle, W., Lindenberger, U., and Woollacott, M. (2013). Coordination of degrees of freedom and stabilization of task variables in a complex motor skill: expertise-related differences in cello bowing. Exp. Brain Res. 224, 323–334. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3314-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vollmann, H., Ragert, P., Conde, V., Villringer, A., Classen, J., Witte, O. W., et al. (2014). Instrument specific use-dependent plasticity shapes the anatomical properties of the corpus callosum: a comparison between musicians and non-musicians. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:245. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiggs, C. L., and Martin, A. (1998). Properties and mechanisms of perceptual priming. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8, 227–233. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80144-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfe, J., Garnier, M., and Smith, J. (2009). Vocal tract resonances in speech, singing, and playing musical instruments. HFSP J. 3, 6–23. doi: 10.2976/1.2998482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Predispositions and plasticity in music and speech learning: neural correlates and implications. Science 342, 585–589. doi: 10.1126/science.1238414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., and Penhune, V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music: auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 547–558. doi: 10.1038/nrn2152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cortical thickness, resting-state network, wind instruments, musical training, neuronal plasticity

Citation: Choi U-S, Sung Y-W, Hong S, Chung J-Y and Ogawa S (2015) Structural and functional plasticity specific to musical training with wind instruments. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:597. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00597

Received: 07 July 2015; Accepted: 14 October 2015;
Published: 29 October 2015.

Edited by:

Yong He, Beijing Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Jürgen Hänggi, University of Zurich, Switzerland
Xin Di, New Jersey Institute of Technology, USA

Copyright © 2015 Choi, Sung, Hong, Chung and Ogawa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yul-Wan Sung, sung@tfu-mail.tfu.ac.jp; sungstone@gmail.com

Download