Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 23 July 2014
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic The functional organization of the auditory system. View all 23 articles

Auditory connections and functions of prefrontal cortex

  • Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, NY, USA

The functional auditory system extends from the ears to the frontal lobes with successively more complex functions occurring as one ascends the hierarchy of the nervous system. Several areas of the frontal lobe receive afferents from both early and late auditory processing regions within the temporal lobe. Afferents from the early part of the cortical auditory system, the auditory belt cortex, which are presumed to carry information regarding auditory features of sounds, project to only a few prefrontal regions and are most dense in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC). In contrast, projections from the parabelt and the rostral superior temporal gyrus (STG) most likely convey more complex information and target a larger, widespread region of the prefrontal cortex. Neuronal responses reflect these anatomical projections as some prefrontal neurons exhibit responses to features in acoustic stimuli, while other neurons display task-related responses. For example, recording studies in non-human primates indicate that VLPFC is responsive to complex sounds including vocalizations and that VLPFC neurons in area 12/47 respond to sounds with similar acoustic morphology. In contrast, neuronal responses during auditory working memory involve a wider region of the prefrontal cortex. In humans, the frontal lobe is involved in auditory detection, discrimination, and working memory. Past research suggests that dorsal and ventral subregions of the prefrontal cortex process different types of information with dorsal cortex processing spatial/visual information and ventral cortex processing non-spatial/auditory information. While this is apparent in the non-human primate and in some neuroimaging studies, most research in humans indicates that specific task conditions, stimuli or previous experience may bias the recruitment of specific prefrontal regions, suggesting a more flexible role for the frontal lobe during auditory cognition.

Introduction

Connections from the auditory cortex to the frontal lobes mediate a number of functions including language, object recognition and spatial localization. Discerning what types of auditory information reaches the frontal cortex, where that auditory input originates, and how information is utilized by the frontal lobes for complex behaviors, such as communication, is a fundamental question of neuroscience.

The frontal cortex is a heterogeneous region with multiple functional subdivisions, including the prefrontal cortex, which lies in the anterior frontal lobe and consists of medial, lateral, and orbital subdivisions. This review will focus on the lateral prefrontal cortex including the dorsolateral regions (DLPFC) (areas 8, 46, and 9) and the ventrolateral regions (VLPFC) (areas 12/47, 45, and 12 orbital) (Figure 1). Possible auditory functions and connections of frontal pole, medial and orbital areas of the frontal lobe are described elsewhere including Medalla and Barbas (2014).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Top panel are schematics of the lateral and frontal surfaces of the monkey (A) and human (B) brain from Petrides and Pandya (2002). Bottom panel are schematics illustrating area 12 vl (now referred to as 12/47) and 12 orb (C) and a coronal section (D) depicting these regions from Preuss and Goldman-Rakic (1991). Inset diagram is the lower part of arcuate sulcus to show cytoarchitectonic areas within the banks of the sulcus. Used/modified with permission from Petrides and Pandya (2002) and Preuss and Goldman-Rakic (1991).

The frontal lobe is well-known for its role in speech and language processes and executive functions that include working memory, planning, and decision making (Fuster, 2008). Early lesion studies indicated that lesions of prefrontal cortex caused impairments in delay response, delay spatial alternation, and delay object alternation tasks (Pribram et al., 1952; Mishkin and Pribram, 1955, 1956; Pribram and Mishkin, 1956; Mishkin et al., 1969). Later, more precise lesion studies implicated DLPFC in spatial and delay processes (Malmo, 1942; Mishkin, 1957; Passingham, 1975; Mishkin and Manning, 1978). In contrast, lesions of VLPFC resulted in impaired performance in non-spatial tasks and implicated VLPFC in object recognition (Mishkin and Manning, 1978). In the last two decades there have been a wealth of neuroimaging studies in human subject and single-unit recording studies in non-human primates, which confirm a role in working memory for the prefrontal cortex (Funahashi et al., 1993; Awh et al., 1996; McCarthy et al., 1996; Miller et al., 1996; Owen et al., 1996; Courtney et al., 1997; D'Esposito et al., 2000; Fuster et al., 2000; Bunge et al., 2003; Postle et al., 2003; Bor et al., 2004; Rowe et al., 2008). Unfortunately most neurophysiology studies utilize visual working memory paradigms. Therefore, while these studies have shed light on the neuronal mechanisms underlying prefrontal visual information processing and visual memory, there is much less known about prefrontal processing of auditory information. Fortunately, the past decade has seen several advances in our understanding of the organization of the primate auditory cortical system and how this system, is critical for speech, auditory attention, and multisensory integration. These advances have made it possible and necessary to investigate the pathways that bring auditory information to the prefrontal cortex and the neural mechanisms which underlie auditory cognition.

Auditory Connections of the Frontal Lobes

Historically, anatomical tract tracing and lesion degeneration studies provided evidence that presumptive auditory cortical regions send projections to prefrontal cortex. One general principal observed in these studies of prefrontal-auditory connections is the rostro-caudal topography (Pandya and Kuypers, 1969; Chavis and Pandya, 1976; Petrides and Pandya, 1988; Seltzer and Pandya, 1989; Barbas, 1992; Romanski et al., 1999a,b). Reciprocal connections are apparent between the caudal STG and caudal PFC, including caudal (dorsal) area 46, periarcuate area 8a and the inferior convexity, or ventral prefrontal cortex—areas 12 and 45 (Petrides and Pandya, 1988; Barbas, 1992). In addition, middle and rostral STG are reciprocally connected with rostral 46 and area 10 and orbito-frontal areas 11 and 12 (Pandya and Kuypers, 1969; Pandya et al., 1969; Chavis and Pandya, 1976). Furthermore, studies noted projections from the anterior temporal lobe to orbital and medial prefrontal cortex and the frontal pole (Petrides and Pandya, 1988; Barbas, 1993; Carmichael and Price, 1995; Hackett et al., 1999; Romanski et al., 1999a).

While these studies inform us of the existence of temporal prefrontal connectivity they do not indicate which of these connections carries acoustic information. To understand the flow of auditory information to the prefrontal cortex, it is necessary to know what parts of the temporal lobe are, in fact auditory responsive. Progress in defining the connections and areal organization of the auditory cortex was greatly accelerated by advancements in auditory cortical neurophysiology and neuroanatomy. First, Rauschecker and colleagues delineated the physiological boundaries of auditory cortical core and belt regions (Rauschecker et al., 1995, 1997). These studies provided the first electrophysiological evidence for three separate tonotopic regions in the non-primary lateral belt cortex (AL, ML, and CL) (antero-lateral belt, middle-lateral belt, caudal-lateral belt cortex respectively) with frequency reversals separating them. Compared with primary auditory cortical neurons, which readily respond to relatively simple acoustic elements, such as pure tones, neurons of the lateral belt association cortex prefer complex stimuli including band-passed noise and vocalizations (Rauschecker et al., 1995, 1997). Simultaneous advances in anatomical organization confirmed and extended these findings. Several groups showed that primary and non-primary auditory cortex could be distinguished on the basis of differential staining for the calcium binding protein parvalbumin along with cytoarchitectonic changes (Morel et al., 1993; Jones et al., 1995; Kosaki et al., 1997; Hackett et al., 1998). These combined physiological and anatomical studies made it possible to recognize individual boundaries of the auditory cortical system and showed its organization to consist of a primary, or core, region composed of potentially two areas, AI and R, surrounded by and connected to, a medial and lateral belt of secondary auditory association cortex with a lower density of parvalbumin staining (Morel et al., 1993; Jones et al., 1995; Kosaki et al., 1997; Hackett et al., 1998). A third zone lying adjacent to the lateral belt is the parabelt auditory cortex. Further distinctions between the core and belt, and the belt and parabelt have been based on myeloarchitectonic, and connectional differences. Recent neurophysiological studies have examined the more complex auditory and multisensory responses of the belt (Ghazanfar et al., 2005; Kuśmierek et al., 2012), the rostral superior temporal gyrus (STG) (Kikuchi et al., 2010; Tsunada et al., 2012; Scott et al., 2013, SFN; Perrodin et al., 2014), and the cortex of the superior temporal sulcus (STS) (Ghazanfar et al., 2008; Kikuchi et al., 2010).

Two relevant studies followed on the heels of this revised characterization of auditory core and belt regions and described prefrontal-auditory connections in the context of these defined core, belt and parabelt regions (Hackett et al., 1999; Romanski et al., 1999a). A series of >15 tracer injections into discrete cytoarchitectonic regions of the prefrontal cortex showed that the rostral, orbital and ventrolateral areas of the prefrontal cortex are reciprocally connected with the rostral STG, the rostral belt (areas AL and anterior ML) and the rostral parabelt, whereas caudal principalis and some dorsolateral regions (46, 8, 9) of the prefrontal cortex are reciprocally connected with the caudal belt (caudal ML and CL) and caudal parabelt (Romanski et al., 1999a). Importantly, projections to the PFC from higher order cortical auditory regions such as parabelt and STS were more robust than the early auditory cortical regions such as the lateral belt, suggesting a cascade of lighter to stronger projections to the prefrontal cortex from early to late auditory processing regions (Figure 2), (Hackett et al., 1999; Romanski et al., 1999a). Furthermore, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) was shown to have a very dense reciprocal connection with the dorsal bank of the STS including areas TPO (temporal parieto-occipital junction), and TAa (temporal area a) (Romanski et al., 1999a; Figures 2, 3).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Connections of VLPFC with auditory cortex. An injection of WGA-HRP shown previously (Romanski et al., 1999a) illustrates the density of connections of VLPFC with auditory cortical regions in the belt, parabelt and superior temporal sulcus as well as the rostral temporal lobe. Coronal sections through the temporal lobe are shown in (A–C) with black dots illustrating the location of retrogradely labeled cells. Labeling is heaviest in the superior temporal sulcus regions TPO and TAa, moderate in the parabelt and lighter in the lateral belt. (D) A photomicrograph from a temporal lobe section adjacent to that shown in (B). (E) Is a photomicrograph of the prefrontal cortex section containing the injection site for this injection which was located in the ventral part of area 12/47. (F) Portrays the location of the injection site in VLPFC and the locations of the coronal sections from A–C on a lateral schematic of the macaque brain. Adapted from Romanski et al. (1999a).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. A circuit diagram summary of auditory inputs from temporal lobe areas to the PFC, from anatomical studies (including: Petrides and Pandya, 1988; Seltzer and Pandya, 1989; Hackett et al., 1999; Romanski et al., 1999a,b) discussed in this review. Thicker lines represents stronger connections. R, Rostral; C, Caudal; STS, Superior Temporal Sulcus; STG, Superior Temporal Gyrus; AL, Antero-lateral; ML, Middle-lateral; CL, Caudal-lateral; TPO, temporal parieto-occiptal area; TAa, Temporal area.

While these anatomical studies suggest that the PFC receives auditory information, since afferents from the auditory belt and parabelt terminate in PFC, more direct evidence that projections are carrying acoustic information is obtained when anatomical and physiological methods are combined. In one such study, Romanski et al. (1999b) recorded auditory responses from lateral belt auditory areas AL, ML, and CL and placed injections of anatomical tracers into these physiologically defined regions. These connections were topographically organized such that projections from AL typically involved the frontal pole (area 10), the rostral principal sulcus (area 46), the inferior convexity (areas 12/47 and 45) and the lateral orbital cortex (areas 11, 12o). In contrast, projections from area CL targeted the dorsal periarcuate cortex (area 8a, frontal eye fields) and the caudal principal sulcus (area 46), and a small connection with caudal inferior convexity (areas 12/47 and 45) and, in two cases, premotor cortex (area 6d). These highly specific rostrocaudal topographical frontal-temporal connections suggest the existence of separate streams of auditory information that targeted previously identified visual domains in the prefrontal cortex. One pathway, originating in CL, targets caudal DLPFC; the other pathway, originating in AL, targets rostral prefrontal cortex and VLPFC. Previous studies have designated these regions in the frontal lobe as being involved in visuo-spatial (DLPFC) and visual object (VLPFC) processing based on physiological responses to visual stimuli (Wilson et al., 1993; O'Scalaidhe et al., 1997). Thus, it is possible the pathways originating from anterior and posterior auditory belt and parabelt cortices are analogous to the “what” and “where” streams of the visual system and that auditory functions in VLPFC and DLPFC could also be object and spatially based, respectively.

Further exploration of prefrontal auditory connections has focused on the VLPFC following the discovery of auditory responsive neurons in VLPFC, (Romanski and Goldman-Rakic, 2002). Anatomical connections of VLPFC regions with auditory belt, parabelt and rostral STG have been confirmed in other anatomical studies (Gerbella et al., 2010; Saleem et al., 2014) though clarification on whether area 45 or 12/47 receives greater auditory inputs is still needed (Romanski, 2012). Previous examination of responses in area 45 and the gradation of visual responses from the frontal eye fields located just dorsal to it argue in favor of stronger visual inputs to area 45 (Webster et al., 1994; Bullier et al., 1996; O'Scalaidhe et al., 1997). Previous cytoarchitectonic studies of VLPFC in M. Mulatta differ with the recent studies cited by Gerbella et al. (2010) and Saleem et al. (2014). Our organization of VLPFC is based on parcellations mainly by Preuss and Goldman-Rakic (1991) with additional studies by Carmichael and Price (1995), Medalla and Barbas (2014), Price (2008), Barbas (1988), and Saleem et al. (2008). Furthermore, we maintain that characterization of VLPFC must be accomplished with both anatomical and physiological data as stated above. Cytoarchitectonic boundaries vary across the different the studies we have referenced. Preuss and Goldman-Rakic (1991) show a much smaller boundary for area 45 while Saleem et al. (2014) shows it to be much larger. Gerbella et al. (2010) and Petrides and Pandya (2002) show differences in their parcellation of area 12. These differences confirm that additional studies combining neurophysiology and anatomical methods are needed to understand the organization of the frontal lobe in general, and VLPFC specifically.

One principle that has emerged from anatomical studies is that a cascade of afferents reaches the VLPFC (Figure 4). The densest projections to VLPFC originate from the STS and as-yet-uncharacterized regions of the rostral STG, while the parabelt provides a moderate innervation of rostral and ventrolateral regions (area 12/47 and area 12o). In contrast, the anterior and middle auditory belt cortex provides only a modest input to VLPFC (Hackett et al., 1999; Romanski et al., 1999a,b; Figures 3, 4), though their input may arrive earliest due to fewer synaptic junctions. This is in agreement with the notion that our association cortical regions receive highly processed information about a sensory stimulus after it has undergone transformations through earlier sensory cortical regions.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Schematic diagram illustrating the flow of information from the auditory cortex to the VLPFC. Thick and dark gray arrows illustrate dense projections from STS, with less dense projections arriving from parabelt and lateral belt regions. AL, Antero-lateral; R, Rostral; STS, Superior Temporal Sulcus; STG, Superior Temporal Gyrus; TPO, temporal parieto-occiptal area; TAa, Temporal area.

Physiological Responses of Neurons in PFC

Prior to 2000, responses to acoustic stimuli of a non-spatial nature were sporadically noted across a widespread region of the frontal lobe in Old and New World primates (Newman and Lindsley, 1976; Benevento et al., 1977; Wollberg and Sela, 1980; Tanila et al., 1992, 1993; Watanabe, 1992; Bodner et al., 1996). Several of these studies used auditory stimuli in combination with visual stimuli as task elements but did not systematically explore the selectivity of auditory responsive cells (Ito, 1982; Vaadia et al., 1986, 1989; Watanabe, 1992). Despite reports of responses to complex stimuli including clicks, environmental sounds and vocalizations, the prior neurophysiological recordings in the frontal lobe of non-human primates failed to demonstrate a discrete clustering of auditory cells indicative of an auditory responsive domain (Newman and Lindsley, 1976; Tanila et al., 1992, 1993). Building on the connectional studies which predicted an auditory-responsive region in VLPFC (Romanski et al., 1999a,b), neurophysiological studies investigated the responses of lateral PFC neurons. Romanski and Goldman-Rakic (2002), described a discrete auditory responsive region in the macaque prefrontal cortex in which a region of VLPFC had neurons which responded to a variety of complex acoustic stimuli including species-specific vocalizations. The auditory responsive region was small (4 × 4 mm) and was localized to the VLPFC, mostly area 12/47 and potentially area 45 (Romanski and Goldman-Rakic, 2002). Further analysis showed that prefrontal neurons typically responded to stimuli that were acoustically similar (Romanski et al., 2005). Specifically neurons responded to species-specific vocalizations that had a similar acoustic morphology and not a similar behavioral referent, (Romanski et al., 2005). Analysis of the classification of the vocalizations with a hidden Markov model (HMM), showed that the HMM was more effective at discriminating among the call classes than previous methods, reaching a classification performance of almost 75% correct. Furthermore the complex responses of prefrontal neurons to these sounds could be predicted as linear functions of the probabilistic output of the HMM (Averbeck and Romanski, 2006).

The auditory responsive region in VLPFC lies adjacent to a region where visually responsive neurons, face cells and face-responsive patches have been localized (O'Scalaidhe et al., 1997, 1999; Tsao et al., 2008). Thus, the idea that VLPFC neurons might be responsive to both vocalization and faces is hardly surprising. VLPFC, as mentioned previously, receives afferents from both auditory and visual portions of the temporal lobe as well as a robust innervation from the multisensory area TPO in the dorsal bank of the STS (Barbas, 1988; Romanski et al., 1999a,b). A study by Benevento et al. (1977) found neurons in VLPFC (area 12o) that were responsive to simple auditory and visual stimuli (clicks and light flashes), and, as demonstrated with intracellular recordings, at least some of these interactions were due to convergence on single cortical cells. Using species-specific vocalizations and their accompanying facial gestures, Romanski and colleagues demonstrated multisensory responses to simultaneously presented faces and vocalizations in VLPFC neurons (Sugihara et al., 2006). Sugihara et al. (2006) further characterized multisensory responses as enhanced or suppressed. Multisensory neurons accounted for about half the recorded population with ~4% unimodal auditory responses and ~50% unimodal visual responses, suggesting that a large proportion of VLPFC neurons are likely to be multisensory if tested properly. Since the region of VLPFC where multisensory neurons are located overlaps extensively with the location of previously characterized auditory responses, it is probable that previous studies which examined either unimodal auditory or unimodal visual functions included multisensory cells in their populations.

The anatomical studies described above have shown that the auditory responsive regions in VLPFC receives very dense innervation from areas TPO and TAa multisensory zones on the dorsal bank of the STS (Romanski et al., 1999a; Figures 2, 4), with moderate projections from the rostral STG and parabelt and lighter inputs from the anterior and middle belt (AL and ML) to VLPFC. Thus, VLPFC neurons may receive acoustic afferents from early (belt) or late (TPO/rostral STG) regions of the auditory cortical hierarchy. It is possible that the specific pattern of afferent input may dictate the types of neurophysiological responses found in VLPFC. The fact that neurons in VLPFC exhibit a wide range of response latencies to auditory stimuli (30–330 ms) also supports this concept of heterogeneous afferents (Romanski and Hwang, 2012). For example, a small number of auditory responsive neurons have extremely fast latency responses, these cells could be receiving inputs from early auditory cortical areas (Romanski et al., 2005; Romanski and Hwang, 2012) with narrow selectivity and phasic onsets to acoustic stimuli. It is possible that these feature-sensitive, rapid onset responses could arise from early auditory cortex such as the anterior belt region AL which is known to project sparsely to this region and would arrive first. In contrast, neurons which respond to combinations of complex acoustic features, or more generally to task variables may be more likely to receive afferents from parabelt and rostral STG which would be several synapses away from VLPFC and would presumably take longer and provide more highly processed information about an auditory object. Finally, multisensory responses in VLPFC could arise as a de novo integration of inputs from auditory belt, parabelt or rostral STG and extrastriate visual cortical areas such as TE. Alternatively multisensory VLPFC responses could originate from multisensory cells of TPO or TAa on the dorsal bank of the STS, which send dense projections to VLPFC. Multisensory responses in VLPFC have longer latencies than unimodal auditory response latencies measured in the same cells (multisensory response range 50–490 ms; Romanski and Hwang, 2012).

Localization of Auditory Function in DLPFC and VLPFC: Animal Studies

As reviewed above the frontal cortex receives afferents from early and late auditory cortical processing stations allowing frontal lobe neurons to detect and discriminate auditory stimuli (Ito, 1982; Watanabe, 1992; Romanski and Goldman-Rakic, 2002; Poremba et al., 2004), or to be remembered during auditory working memory processes (Plakke et al., 2013a). Divergent processing pathways conforming to ventral and dorsal “what” and “where” streams, respectively, originate in the belt and parabelt auditory cortex and terminate in VLPFC and DLPFC regions as described above. DLPFC receives information from caudal auditory regions, which have been shown to preferentially process auditory location information and VLPFC receives input from rostral auditory regions that show a greater preference for type of stimuli (Romanski et al., 1999b; Rauschecker and Tian, 2000; Tian et al., 2001; Kuśmierek et al., 2012). Based on these anatomical connections it has been proposed that DLPFC is primarily involved in spatial processing while VLPFC may be preferentially involved in object processing.

This traditional division of labor between dorsal and ventral prefrontal regions is supported by some neurophysiology studies. Early studies demonstrated that DLPFC neurons were preferentially responsive when acoustic stimuli were presented from specific directions (Azuma and Suzuki, 1984) or when animal subjects localized auditory or visual stimuli (Vaadia et al., 1989). In latter studies which focused on working memory processes, neurons in DLPFC were active during the mnemonic processing of auditory and visual location (Kikuchi-Yorioka and Sawaguchi, 2000; Artchakov et al., 2007). In both studies, a portion of DLPFC neurons were spatially selective during the delay for both auditory and visual cues.

However, other neurophysiological studies demonstrated that DLPFC neurons were active during non-spatial tasks. Studies by Watanabe (1992) showed that prefrontal neurons responded when tones were predictive of juice reward and Bodner et al. (1996) described auditory working memory cells in DLPFC during a task where a tone was paired with a color to predict reward. More recently, recordings during a non-spatial auditory delayed match-to-sample task demonstrated task related activity in neurons in both dorsal and ventral PFC (Plakke et al., 2013a). Prefrontal neurons responded to sound cues during both the sample and match/nonmatch presentations, and also during the delay, response, and reward periods of the task (Plakke et al., 2013a), (Figure 5). During this task, cells in this region appeared to be responsive to tracking when a relative stimulus is needed to be remembered or responded too. The general task responses of these neurons suggests that the role of the DLPFC in auditory working memory may be for rule representation or response control, as previously suggested in studies of visual working memory (Fuster et al., 1982; Miller et al., 1996; Iba and Sawaguchi, 2002; Warden and Miller, 2007). Together these studies suggest that the role of DLPFC in auditory memory may relate more to task and cognitive requirements than to acoustic stimulus encoding.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Example cells with activity occurring during the presentation of the auditory sample, match/nonmatch and during the decision period of an auditory delayed match-to-sample task. (A) An example cell with increased activity during the auditory cues, wait time and response periods for correct trials. (B) An example cell with increased firing rated during auditory cue and wait time periods for correct trials. Y-axis label is frequency (imp/s); bin = 100 ms; asterisk signifies significant change in firing rate from baseline.

In contrast to the task related processes in DLPFC, neurophysiology in non-human primates suggest that VLPFC may perform both stimulus and task related processes. As described above, VLPFC contains neurons that are responsive to complex sounds including, species-specific vocalizations and human vocalizations (Romanski and Goldman-Rakic, 2002; Romanski et al., 2005), suggesting a role for VLPFC in auditory object processing. VLPFC involvement in auditory feature processing is supported by studies showing single-units that encode categories of vocalization call types (Averbeck and Romanski, 2004, 2006; Plakke et al., 2013b). Moreover, evidence that VLPFC cells are multisensory and respond to the simultaneous presentation of faces and their corresponding vocalizations strongly suggests a role in recognition and identity processing, a ventral stream function (Sugihara et al., 2006).

Several studies from Cohen and colleagues have examined neuronal responses in VLPFC during non-spatial auditory performance tasks (Cohen et al., 2006, 2007; Russ et al., 2008a; Tsunada et al., 2011). For example, VLPFC neurons were modulated during non-spatial auditory discrimination but showed no modulation during spatial auditory discrimination (Cohen et al., 2009). Further recordings over a large region of PFC which Cohen termed “vPFC” during categorization and decision making paradigms, demonstrate that prefrontal neuronal activity is correlated with behavioral choices (Russ et al., 2008b; Lee et al., 2009), although the location of these prefrontal neurons does not appear to overlap entirely with the ventrolateral PFC regions previously shown to be auditory responsive (Romanski et al., 2005). Nonetheless, inactivation studies are needed to determine whether VLPFC is essential in the performance of working memory or decision making tasks. Toward this end, a recent study by Plakke et al., (2013c, SFN) shows that transient inactivation of VLPFC impairs performance in an audiovisual working memory task and suggests an essential role in mnemonic processing when acoustic stimuli are involved. Thus, processing of auditory information in DLPFC may relate more to the task demands, while processing of auditory information in VLPFC is clearly related to auditory features and task demands.

Processing of Auditory Information in the Human Dorsal and Ventral PFC

The anatomical and neurophysiological studies performed in nonhuman primates delineate somewhat separable roles for dorsal and ventral frontal lobe regions. How these functional streams in nonhuman primates map onto auditory function in the human brain is, as yet, not completely clear. Although it is well known that speech and language functions rely on the cortex within the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) neuroimaging studies have provided evidence that the human frontal lobe is also active during auditory discrimination (Zatorre et al., 1994), auditory detection (Benedict et al., 1998, 2002), auditory attention/oddball tasks (Stevens et al., 2000), auditory judgments (Zatorre et al., 1998), and auditory working memory (Anurova et al., 2003, 2001; Grady et al., 2008; Protzner and McIntosh, 2009). These studies have described discrete activations in DLPFC and VLPFC that are related to the type of information processed. For example, several imaging studies have described activations in DLPFC (superior frontal gyrus, superior frontal sulcus) during auditory spatial localization (Griffiths et al., 1998; Martinkauppi et al., 2000; Weeks et al., 2000; Lipschutz et al., 2002; Lutzenberger et al., 2002; Zatorre et al., 2002; Gaab et al., 2003; Leiberg et al., 2006). Conversely, VLPFC activation (IFG; BA 45,47), has been noted during auditory non-spatial processes, such as listening to melodies, attending pitch/rhythm, determining sound length, word/voice discrimination and auditory working memory (Zatorre et al., 1994, 1998; Platel et al., 1997; Linden et al., 1999; Pedersen et al., 2000; Alain et al., 2001; Kiehl et al., 2001; Muller et al., 2001; Kaiser et al., 2003; Maddock et al., 2003; Arnott et al., 2004; Rämä et al., 2004; Rämä and Courtney, 2005; Kaiser et al., 2009; Koelsch et al., 2009).

In addition, activation of DLPFC (Brodman's area 46/9) occurs during various complex working memory paradigms. For instance there were increases in activity in DLPFC when participants listened to numbers and made self-ordered choices (Petrides et al., 1993). Dorsolateral activity is also increased during studies of divided auditory attention (Benedict et al., 1998) as well as encoding of nonverbal sounds (Opitz et al., 2000). Taken together these studies suggest DLPFC may be recruited more frequently based on cognitive demands including the type of process that is necessary such as monitoring information in memory, encoding auditory information, as well as manipulation of spatial information.

In contrast, the IFG and related VLPFC regions are activated during phonological processing (Klein et al., 1995; Buchanan et al., 2000; Strand et al., 2008), semantic processing (Caplan et al., 2000; Burton et al., 2003), syntactic operations (Waters et al., 2003), naming objects (Tranel et al., 2003), word discrimination (Buchanan et al., 2000; Vaden et al., 2013), and directed auditory attention (Hill and Miller, 2010) reinforcing the connection of this region with language and auditory feature processing. Interestingly, there has also been activation within the IFG during nonverbal auditory stimulus detection (Linden et al., 1999; Kiehl et al., 2001; Maeder et al., 2001), nonverbal auditory discrimination (Zatorre et al., 1994; Muller et al., 2001), and auditory working memory (Kaiser et al., 2003). The activation of the more anterior regions of the IFG (areas 47 and 45) during nonverbal auditory sound detection, discrimination and auditory feature detection (Zatorre et al., 2004; Fecteau et al., 2005) suggests these areas may play a more fundamental role in auditory processing, paralleling the auditory responsive region that has been described in non-human primates (Romanski and Goldman-Rakic, 2002; Romanski et al., 2005). The role of VLPFC in general sound discrimination is also supported by its activation when listening to rhymes (Burton et al., 2003) and by the case of a patient with an inferior frontal lesion that was impaired on detecting modulated sounds (Griffiths et al., 2000).

Verbal vs. Non-Verbal Stimuli and Cognitive Requirements

Localization of auditory cognition to discrete networks in the human brain is complicated by the potential activation of language networks when verbal stimuli are used as memoranda in cognitive tasks. Comparing studies when verbal and nonverbal stimuli have been used has revealed activation in both DLPFC and VLPFC including the middle frontal gyrus and the anterior and posterior portions of the IFG. As might be predicted, VLPFC is active for language related functions but VLPFC activation also occurs for simple nonverbal auditory target detection/discrimination with tones (Linden et al., 1999; Kiehl et al., 2001; Muller and Basho, 2004; Huang et al., 2012), animal cries (Maeder et al., 2001), and melodies (Zatorre et al., 1994). Conversely, verbal discrimination has activated DLPFC (middle frontal gyrus) (Pedersen et al., 2000). This suggests that the prefrontal cortex is not simply dividing the processing of auditory information based solely on verbal information (Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Sagittal and horizontal view of human brain on which activations have been plotted across several fMRI studies involving processing of information during tasks that utilized stimuli that were Non-verbal/Spatial (dark green square), Non-verbal/Non-spatial (light green triangle), Verbal/Spatial (dark blue square), and Verbal/Non-spatial (light blue triangle). fMRI coordinates are plotted in Talairach space. Sagittal (x = +29 mm) and horizontal (z = −4 mm), Talairach images for reference from Talairach and Tournoux (1988). For a list of the studies plotted see (Supplemental Table 1).

In order to examine auditory function independent of language circuits, noise bursts were used for both a spatial (localization) and non-spatial (pitch discrimination) auditory task (Alain et al., 2001). As predicted by the dorsal/ventral streams model, pitch processing evoked greater activation in the IFG while localization evoked greater activity in the superior frontal gyrus (Alain et al., 2001). The use of identical auditory stimuli under different demands, which led to diverse activation patterns, indicates cognitive load can recruit specialized areas within the frontal cortex (Alain et al., 2001). A similar pattern of results emerged in Du et al. (2013). In this study, subjects were trained to discriminate simultaneously presented vowel sounds. Vowels were presented with different frequencies or from different locations; this information was irrelevant for correct performance, but served as implicit information. After training, participants were exposed to both spatial and pitch differences while making vowel judgments and improved accuracy of vowel discrimination was observed when the pair of vowels presented matched their previous training (frequency or location). In addition, magnetoencephalography (MEG) activity was localized to the anterior ventral frontal regions for the group exposed to frequency changes, while MEG changes were more frequent in dorsal frontal regions for the group exposed to location changes (Du et al., 2013). Thus, even when participants did not make any explicit frequency or location choices the short term exposure to implicit spatial and object information segregated the dorsal and ventral prefrontal cortex respectively. This demonstrates that the activation of a particular neural network can be biased based on subtle cognitive demands.

In general, a division of labor for spatial and non-spatial information may exist (Ahveninen et al., 2013), and in non-human primates that do not possess language functions, may be most prominent. However, it is the underlying cognitive contingencies of a task that may ultimately recruit specific regions of frontal cortex. For example, pitch discrimination/detection and auditory attention have been found to activate both DLPFC (Griffiths et al., 1998; Linden et al., 1999; Muller et al., 2001; Gaab et al., 2003; Seydell-Greenwald et al., 2013) and VLPFC Linden et al., 1999; Alain et al., 2001; Gaab et al., 2003; Seydell-Greenwald et al., 2013. Moreover, attention may bias which auditory network is recruited. Lipschutz et al. (2002) demonstrated that during dichotic listening when attention was divided both the lateral middle frontal gyrus and the IFG were activated although, the middle frontal gyrus was active when participants were told to selectively attend. Therefore, examining only whether a task has a spatial component is insufficient to determine which prefrontal regions will be recruited. Performing diverse types of cognitive processes such as making a pitch discrimination or dividing auditory attention may rely on different or overlapping auditory networks.

It has been questioned whether frontal lobe activity is related to cognitive demands or the stimulus properties within the task. Surprisingly, when verbal working memory is required, more dorsal regions (area 46/9) are recruited (Petrides et al., 1993; Petrides, 1996; Crottaz-Herbette et al., 2004). Whereas ventral regions (BA 47/12; 45) are utilized during active retrieval (Petrides, 1996; Kostopoulos and Petrides, 2008). In addition, areas of activation in frontal cortex can be shared by different auditory working memory demands (Arnott et al., 2005). It has also been suggested that within the auditory domain, DLPFC is more important for heavier memory loads, while VLPFC is necessary for dealing with attentional interference (Huang et al., 2013). Postle (2006) has reviewed the role of the prefrontal cortex with respect to information encoding, segregation, and manipulation of information, for visual working memory. Similar treatment needs to be given to the processing of auditory information and how dorsal and ventral prefrontal areas contribute to its encoding, manipulation, and short-term storage.

Summary

The prefrontal cortex is involved in auditory cognition and receives information from a wide array of auditory regions including multisensory (STS) and unimodal auditory cortical regions. Understanding how that information is processed by the PFC and utilized during auditory cognition is an ongoing investigation. In the non-human primate, single-unit studies have indicated VLPFC has a specialized region for processing auditory stimuli but is also multisensory (Sugihara et al., 2006; Romanski, 2012) and involved in some aspects of higher auditory function (Cohen et al., 2007; Bizley and Cohen, 2013). In contrast, the DLPFC may have auditory responsive units but activity has mainly been observed during tasks requiring cognitive processes or localization of sound (Bodner et al., 1996; Kikuchi-Yorioka and Sawaguchi, 2000; Artchakov et al., 2007; Plakke et al., 2013a). Research in non-human primates suggests a functional division between DLPFC and VLPFC, with DLPFC utilized for spatial and auditory task requirements, while VLPFC is recruited for non-spatial and auditory feature processing as well as some cognitive operations. In humans, more cortical regions and cognitive ability complicate the picture. The spatial and non-spatial divide is somewhat supported; but new research suggests a more nuanced view is necessary and that different neural areas are recruited under various stimulus and cognitive demands. These recent neuroimaging studies provide support for a role of the prefrontal cortex in complex auditory cognition and demonstrate that attentional demands can shift which prefrontal network is activated. Overall, research from both humans and non-human primates suggests that the frontal cortex is essential in auditory cognition. Determining which specific cortical networks and prefrontal regions are critical in various aspects of auditory cognition is necessary for comprehending and treating communication disorders.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health grant (DC: 04845) and the Training and Hearing Balance and Spatial Orientation grant (DC: 009974). The authors thank Lia Soneson for graphic design assistance on Figures 3, 4.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fnins.2014.00199/abstract

References

Ahveninen, J., Huang, S., Nummenmaa, A., Belliveau, J. W., Hung, A. Y., Jaaskelainen, I. P., et al. (2013). Evidence for distinct human auditory cortex regions for sound location versus identity processing. Nat. Commun. 4:2585. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3585

CrossRef Full Text

Alain, C., Arnott, S. R., Hevenor, S., Graham, S., and Grady, C. L. (2001). “What” and “where” in the human auditory system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 12301–12306. doi: 10.1073/pnas.211209098

CrossRef Full Text

Anurova, I., Artchakov, D., Korvenoja, A., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Aronen, H. J., and Carlson, S. (2003). Differences between auditory evoked responses recorded during spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks. Neuroimage 20, 1181–1192. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00353-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Anurova, I., Nikouline, V. V., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Hotta, J., Aronen, H. J., and Carlson, S. (2001). Evidence for dissociation of spatial and nonspatial auditory information processing. Neuroimage 14, 1268–1277. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0903

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Arnott, S. R., Binns, M. A., Grady, C. L., and Alain, C. (2004). Assessing the auditory dual-pathway model in humans. Neuroimage 22, 401–408. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Arnott, S. R., Grady, C. L., Hevenor, S. J., Graham, S., and Alain, C. (2005). The functional organization of auditory working memory as revealed by fMRI. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 819–831. doi: 10.1162/0898929053747612

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Artchakov, D., Tikhonravov, D., Vuontela, V., Linnankoski, I., Korvenoja, A., and Carlson, S. (2007). Processing of auditory and visual location information in the monkey prefrontal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 180, 469–479. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0873-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Averbeck, B. B., and Romanski, L. M. (2004). Principal and independent components of macaque vocalizations: constructing stimuli to probe high-level sensory processing. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 2897–2909. doi: 10.1152/jn.01103.2003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Averbeck, B. B., and Romanski, L. M. (2006). Probabilistic encoding of vocalizations in macaque ventral lateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 26, 11023–11033. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3466-06.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Awh, E., Smith, E. E., and Jonides, J. (1996). Human rehearsal processes and the frontal lobes: PET evidence. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 769, 97–117.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Azuma, M., and Suzuki, H. (1984). Properties and distribution of auditory neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the alert monkey. Brain Res. 298, 343–346. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(84)91434-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barbas, H. (1988). Anatomic organization of basoventral and mediodorsal visual recipient prefrontal regions in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 276, 313–342.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Barbas, H. (1992). Architecture and cortical connections of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. [Review]. Adv. Neurol. 57, 91–115.

Barbas, H. (1993). Organization of cortical afferent input to orbitofrontal areas in the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience 56, 841–864. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(93)90132-Y

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Benedict, R. H., Lockwood, A., Shucard, J. L., Shucard, D. W., Wack, D., and Murphy, B. W. (1998). Functional neuroimaging of attention in the auditory modality. Neuroreport 9, 121–126. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199801050-00024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Benedict, R. H., Shucard, D. W., Santa Maria, M. P., Shucard, J. L., Abara, J. P., Coad, M. L., et al. (2002). Covert auditory attention generates activation in the rostral/dorsal anterior cingulate cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 637–645. doi: 10.1162/08989290260045765

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Benevento, L. A., Fallon, J., Davis, B. J., and Rezak, M. (1977). Auditory-visual interaction in single cells in the cortex of the superior temporal sulcus and the orbital frontal cortex of the macaque monkey. Exp. Neurol. 57,849–872. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(77)90112-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bizley, J. K., and Cohen, Y. E. (2013). The what, where and how of auditory-object perception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 693–707. doi: 10.1038/nrn3565

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bodner, M., Kroger, J., and Fuster, J. M. (1996). Auditory memory cells in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroreport 7, 1905–1908. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199608120-00006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bor, D., Cumming, N., Scott, C. E. L., and Owen, A. M. (2004). Prefrontal cortical involvement in verbal encoding strategies. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 3365–3370. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03438.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buchanan, T. W., Lutz, K., Mirzazade, S., Specht, K., Shah, N. J., Zilles, K., et al. (2000). Recognition of emotional prosody and verbal components of spoken language: an fMRI study. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 9, 227–238. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00060-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bullier, J., Schall, J. D., and Morel, A. (1996). Functional streams in occipito-frontal connections in the monkey. Behav. Brain Res. 76, 89–97. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00182-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bunge, S. A., Kahn, I., Wallis, J. D., Miller, E. K., and Wagner, A. D. (2003). Neural circuits subserving the retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 3419–3428. doi: 10.1152/jn.00910.2002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burton, H., Diamond, J. B., and McDermott, K. B. (2003). Dissociating cortical regions activated by semantic and phonological tasks: a FMRI study in blind and sighted people. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 1965–1982. doi: 10.1152/jn.00279.2003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Caplan, D., Alpert, N., Waters, G., and Olivieri, A. (2000). Activation of Broca's area by syntactic processing under conditions of concurrent articulation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 9, 65–71. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200002)9:2<65::AID-HBM1>3.0.CO;2-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carmichael, S. T., and Price, J. L. (1995). Sensory and premotor connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 363, 642–664. doi: 10.1002/cne.903630409

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chavis, D. A., and Pandya, D. N. (1976). Further observations on corticofrontal connections in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res. 117, 369–386. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90089-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, Y. E., Hauser, M. D., and Russ, B. E. (2006). Spontaneous processing of abstract categorical information in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Biol. Lett. 2, 261–265. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0436

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, Y. E., Russ, B. E., Davis, S. J., Baker, A. E., Ackelson, A. L., and Nitecki, R. (2009). A functional role for the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in non-spatial auditory cognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 20045–20050. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907248106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, Y. E., Theunissen, F., Russ, B. E., and Gill, P. (2007). Acoustic features of rhesus vocalizations and their representation in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 1470–1484. doi: 10.1152/jn.00769.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Courtney, S. M., Ungerleider, L. G., Keil, K., and Haxby, J. V. (1997). Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory. Nature 386, 608–611. doi: 10.1038/386608a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Crottaz-Herbette, S., Anagnoson, R. T., and Menon, V. (2004). Modality effects in verbal working memory: differential prefrontal and parietal responses to auditory and visual stimuli. Neuroimage 21, 340–351. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

D'Esposito, M., Postle, B. R., and Rympa, B. (2000). Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 3–11. doi: 10.1007/s002210000395

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Du, Y., He, Y., Arnott, S. R., Ross, B., Wu, X., Li, L., et al. (2013). Rapid tuning of auditory “what” and “where” pathways by training. Cereb. Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht251. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fecteau, S., Armony, J. L., Joanette, Y., and Belin, P. (2005). Sensitivity to voice in human prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 2251–2254. doi: 10.1152/jn.00329.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. J., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1993). Dorsolateral prefrontal lesions and oculomotor delayed-response performance: evidence for mnemonic “scotomas”. J. Neurosci. 13, 1479–1497.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fuster, J. M. (2008). The Prefrontal Cortex. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Fuster, J. M., Bauer, R. H., and Jervey, J. P. (1982). Cellular discharge in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the monkey in cognitive tasks. Exp. Neurol. 77, 679–694. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(82)90238-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fuster, J. M., Bodner, M., and Kroger, J. K. (2000). Cross-modal and cross-temporal association in neurons of frontal cortex. Nature 405, 347–351. doi: 10.1038/35012613

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaab, N., Gaser, C., Zaehle, T., Jancke, L., and Schlaug, G. (2003). Functional anatomy of pitch memory—an fMRI study with sparse temporal sampling. Neuroimage 19, 1417–1426. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00224-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gerbella, M., Belmalih, A., Borra, E., Rozzi, S., and Luppino, G. (2010). Cortical connections of the macaque caudal ventrolateral prefrontal areas 45A and 45B. Cereb. Cortex 20, 141–168. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ghazanfar, A. A., Chandrasekaran, C., and Logothetis, N. K. (2008). Interactions between the superior temporal sulcus and auditory cortex mediate dynamic face/voice integration in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 28, 4457–4469. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0541-08.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ghazanfar, A. A., Maier, J. X., Hoffman, K. L., and Logothetis, N. K. (2005). Multisensory integration of dynamic faces and voices in rhesus monkey auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 25, 5004–5012. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0799-05.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grady, C. L., Yu, H., and Alain, C. (2008). Age-related differences in brain activity underlying working memory for spatial and nonspatial auditory information. Cereb. Cortex 18, 189–199. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm045

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Griffiths, T. D., Penhune, V. B., Peretz, I., Dean, J. L., Patterson, R. D., and Green, G. G. R. (2000). Frontal processing and auditory perception. Neuroreport 11, 919–922. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200004070-00004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Griffiths, T. D., Rees, G., Rees, A., Green, G. G., Witton, C., Rowe, D., et al. (1998). Right parietal cortex is involved in the perception of sound movement in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 74–79. doi: 10.1038/276

CrossRef Full Text

Hackett, T. A., Stepniewska, I., and Kaas, J. H. (1998). Subdivisions of auditory cortex and ipsilateral cortical connections of the parabelt auditory cortex in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 394, 475–495.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hackett, T. A., Stepniewska, I., and Kaas, J. H. (1999). Prefrontal connections of the parabelt auditory cortex in macaque monkeys. Brain Res. 817, 45–58. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)01182-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hill, K. T., and Miller, L. M. (2010). Auditory attentional control and selection during cocktail party listening. Cereb. Cortex 20, 583–590. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp124

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Huang, S., Belliveau, J. W., Tengshe, C., and Ahveninen, J. (2012). Brain networks of novelty-driven involuntary and cued voluntary auditory attention shifting. PLoS ONE 7:e44062. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044062

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Huang, S., Seidman, L. J., Rossi, S., and Ahveninen, J. (2013). Distinct cortical networks activated by auditory attention and working memory load. Neuroimage 83, 1098–1108. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.074

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Iba, M., and Sawaguchi, T. (2002). Neuronal activity representing visuospatial mnemonic processes associated with target selection in the monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neurosci.Res. 43, 9–22. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(02)00013-5

CrossRef Full Text

Ito, S. I. (1982). Prefrontal unit activity of macaque monkeys during auditory and visual reaction time tasks. Brain Res. 247, 39–47.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Jones, E. G., Dell'Anna, M. E., Molinari, M., Rausell, E., and Hashikawa, T. (1995). Subdivisions of macaque monkey auditory cortex revealed by calcium-binding protein immunoreactivity. J. Comp. Neurol. 362, 153–170. doi: 10.1002/cne.903620202

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kaiser, J., Lutzenberger, W., Decker, C., Wibral, M., and Rahm, B. (2009). Task- and performance-related modulation of domain-specific auditory short-term memory representations in the gamma-band. Neuroimage 46, 1127–1136. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kaiser, J., Ripper, B., Birbaumer, N., and Lutzenberger, W. (2003). Dynamics of gamma-band activity in human magnetoencephalogram during auditory pattern working memory. Neuroimage 20, 816–827. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00350-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kiehl, K. A., Laurens, K. R., Duty, T. L., Forster, B. B., and Liddle, P. F. (2001). Neural sources involved in auditory target detection and novelty processing: an event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology 38, 133–142. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3810133

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kikuchi, Y., Horwitz, B., and Mishkin, M. (2010). Hierarchical auditory processing directed rostrally along the monkey's supratemporal plane. J. Neurosci. 30, 13021–13030. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2267-10.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kikuchi-Yorioka, Y., and Sawaguchi, T. (2000). Parallel visuospatial and audiospatial working memory processes in the monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1075–1076. doi: 10.1038/80581

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klein, D., Milner, B., Zatorre, R. J., Meyer, E., and Evans, A. C. (1995). The neural substrates underlying word generation: a bilingual functional-imaging study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 2899–2903. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.7.2899

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koelsch, S., Schulze, K., Sammler, D., Fritz, T., Muller, K., and Gruber, O. (2009). Functional architecture of verbal and tonal working memory: an FMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 859–873. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20550

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kosaki, H., Hashikawa, T., He, J., and Jones, E. G. (1997). Tonotopic organization of auditory cortical fields delineated by parvalbumin immunoreactivity in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 386, 304–316.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kostopoulos, P., and Petrides, M. (2008). Left mid-ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: underlying principles of function. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 1037–1049. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06066.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kuśmierek, P., Ortiz, M., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2012). Sound-identity processing in early areas of the auditory ventral stream in the macaque. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 1123–1141. doi: 10.1152/jn.00793.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lee, J. H., Russ, B. E., Orr, L. E., and Cohen, Y. E. (2009). Prefrontal activity predicts monkeys' decisions during an auditory category task. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 3:16. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.016.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leiberg, S., Kaiser, J., and Lutzenberger, W. (2006). Gamma-band activity dissociates between matching and nonmatching stimulus pairs in an auditory delayed matching-to-sample-task. Neuroimage 30, 1357–1364. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Linden, J. F., Grunewald, A., and Andersen, R. A. (1999). Responses to auditory stimuli in macaque lateral intraparietal area. II. Behavioral modulation. J. Neurophysiol. 82, 343–358.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Lipschutz, B., Kolinsky, R., Damhaut, P., Wikler, D., and Goldman, S. (2002). Attention-dependent changes of activation and connectivity in dichotic listening. Neuroimage 17, 643–656. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1184

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lutzenberger, W., Ripper, B., Busse, L., Birbaumer, N., and Kaiser, J. (2002). Dynamics of gamma-band activity during an audiospatial working memory task in humans. J. Neurosci. 22, 5630–5638.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Maddock, R. J., Garrett, A. S., and Buonocore, M. H. (2003). Posterior cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 18, 30–41. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10075

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maeder, P. P., Meuli, R. A., Adriani, M., Bellmann, A., Fornari, E., Thiran, J. P., et al. (2001). Distinct pathways involved in sound recognition and localization: a human fMRI study. Neuroimage 14, 802–816. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0888

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Malmo, R. B. (1942). Interference factors in delayed response in monkeys after removal of frontal lobes. J. Neurophysiol. 5, 295–308.

Martinkauppi, S., Rämä, P., Aronen, H. J., Korvenoja, A., and Carlson, S. (2000). Working memory of auditory localization. Cereb. Cortex 10, 889–898. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.9.889

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Constable, R. T., Krystal, J. H., Gore, J. C., and Goldman-Rakic, P. (1996). Activation of human prefrontal cortex during spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks measured by functional MRI. Cereb. Cortex 6, 600–611. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.4.600

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Medalla, M., and Barbas, H. (2014). Specialized prefrontal “auditory fields”: organization of primate prefrontal-temporal pathways. Front. Neurosci. 8:77. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00077

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miller, E. K., Erickson, C. A., and Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque. J. Neurosci. 16, 5154–5167.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mishkin, M. (1957). Effects of small frontal lesions on delayed alternation in monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 20, 615–622.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mishkin, M., and Manning, F. J. (1978). Non-spatial memory after selective prefrontal lesions in monkeys. Brain Res. 143, 313–323. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90571-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mishkin, M., and Pribram, K. H. (1955). Analysis of the effects of frontal lesions in monkeys: I. Variations of delayed alternations. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 48, 492–495. doi: 10.1037/h0040318

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mishkin, M., and Pribram, K. H. (1956). Analysis of the effects of frontal lesions in monkey: II. Variations of delayed response. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 49, 36–40. doi: 10.1037/h0040592

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mishkin, M., Vest, B., Waxler, M., and Rosvold, H. E. (1969). A re-examination of the effects of frontal lesions on object alternation. Neuropsychologia 7, 357–363. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(69)90060-8

CrossRef Full Text

Morel, A., Garraghty, P. E., and Kaas, J. H. (1993). Tonotopic organization, architectonic fields, and connections of auditory cortex in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 335, 437–459. doi: 10.1002/cne.903350312

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muller, R. A., and Basho, S. (2004). Are nonlinguistic functions in “Broca's area” prerequisites for language acquisition? FMRI findings from an ontogenetic viewpoint. Brain Lang. 89, 329–336. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00346-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muller, R. A., Kleinhans, N., and Courchesne, E. (2001). Broca's area and the discrimination of frequency transitions: a functional MRI study. Brain Lang. 76, 70–76. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2398

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Newman, J. D., and Lindsley, D. F. (1976). Single unit analysis of auditory processing in squirrel monkey frontal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 25, 169–181.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Opitz, B., Mecklinger, A., and Friederici, A. D. (2000). Functional asymmetry of human prefrontal cortex: encoding and retrieval of verbally and nonverbally coded information. Learn. Mem. 7, 85–96. doi: 10.1101/lm.7.2.85

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Scalaidhe, S. P. O., Wilson, F. A. W., and Goldman-Rakic, P. G. R. (1999). Face-selective neurons during passive viewing and working memory performance of rhesus monkeys: evidence for intrinsic specialization of neuronal coding. Cereb. Cortex 9, 459–475. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.5.459

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Scalaidhe, S. P., Wilson, F. A., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1997). Areal segregation of face-processing neurons in prefrontal cortex. Science 278, 1135–1138. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5340.1135

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Owen, A. M., Evans, A. C., and Petrides, M. (1996). Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing with the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. Cereb. Cortex 6, 31–38. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.1.31

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pandya, D. N., Hallett, M., and Kmukherjee, S. K. (1969). Intra- and interhemispheric connections of the neocortical auditory system in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res. 14, 49–65.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Pandya, D. N., and Kuypers, H. G. (1969). Cortico-cortical connections in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res. 13, 13–36. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(69)90141-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Passingham, R. (1975). Delayed matching after selective prefrontal lesions in monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Brain Res. 92, 89–102. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90529-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pedersen, C. B., Mirz, F., Ovesen, T., Ishizu, K., Johannsen, P., Madsen, S., et al. (2000). Cortical centres underlying auditory temporal processing in humans: a PET study. Audiology 39, 30–37. doi: 10.3109/00206090009073052

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Perrodin, C., Kayser, C., Logothetis, N. K., and Petkov, C. I. (2014). Auditory and visual modulation of temporal lobe neurons in voice-sensitive and association cortices. J Neurosci. 34, 2524–2537. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2805-13.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Petrides, M. (1996). Specialized systems for the processing of mnemonic information within the primate frontal cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 351, 1455–1461; discussion: 1461–1462.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Petrides, M., Alivisatos, B., Meyer, E., and Evans, A. C. (1993). Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 90, 878–882. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.3.878

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Petrides, M., and Pandya, D. N. (1988). Association fiber pathways to the frontal cortex from the superior temporal region in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 273, 52–66. doi: 10.1002/cne.902730106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Petrides, M., and Pandya, D. N. (2002). Comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis of the human and the macaque ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and corticocortical connection patterns in the monkey. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16, 291–310. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2001.02090.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plakke, B., Diltz, M. D., and Romanski, L. M. (2013b). Coding of vocalizations by single neurons in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Hear. Res. 305, 135–143. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2013.07.011

CrossRef Full Text

Plakke, B., Hwang, J., Diltz, M. D., and Romanski, L. M. (2013c). The role of ventral prefrontal cortex in auditory, visual and audiovisual working memory. Soc. Neurosci. 574.15.

Plakke, B., Ng, C. W., and Poremba, A. (2013a). Neural correlates of auditory recognition memory in primate lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 244, 64–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.04.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Platel, H., Price, C., Baron, J. C., Wise, R., Lambert, J., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (1997). The structural components of music perception. A functional anatomical study. Brain 120(Pt 2), 229–243.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Poremba, A., Malloy, M., Saunders, R. C., Carson, R. E., Herscovitch, P., and Mishkin, M. (2004). Species-specific calls evoke asymmetric activity in the monkey's temporal poles. Nature 427, 448–451. doi: 10.1038/nature02268

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience 139, 23–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Postle, B. R., Druzgal, T. J., and D'Esposito, M. D. (2003). Seeking the neural substrates of visual working memory storage. Cortex 39, 927–946. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70871-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Preuss, T. M., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1991). Myelo- and Cytoarchitecture of the granular frontal cortex and surrounding regions in the strepsirhine primate galago and the anthropoid primate macaca. J. Comp. Neurol. 310, 429–474. doi: 10.1002/cne.903100402

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pribram, K. H., and Mishkin, M. (1956). Analysis of the effects of frontal lesions in monkeys. III. Object alternation. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 49, 41–45. doi: 10.1037/h0046248

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pribram, K. H., Mishkin, M., Rosvold, H. E., and Kaplan, S. J. (1952). Effects on delayed-response performance of lesions of dorsolateral and ventromedial frontal cortex of baboon. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 47, 14–20.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Price, J. L. (2008). Multisensory convergence in the orbital and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Chem. Percept. 1,103–109. doi: 10.1007/s12078-008-9013-5

CrossRef Full Text

Protzner, A. B., and McIntosh, A. R. (2009). Modulation of ventral prefrontal cortex functional connections reflects the inteplay of cognitive processes and stimulus characteristics. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1042–1054. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn146

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rämä, P., and Courtney, S. M. (2005). Functional topography of working memory for face or voice identity. Neuroimage 24, 224–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.024

CrossRef Full Text

Rämä, P., Poremba, A., Sala, J. B., Yee, L., Malloy, M., Mishkin, M., et al. (2004). Dissociable functional cortical topographies for working memory maintenance of voice identity and location. Cereb. Cortex 14, 768–780. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P., and Tian, B. (2000). Mechanisms and streams for processing of “what” and “where” in auditory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 11800–11806. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.22.11800

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P., Tian, B., and Hauser, M. (1995). Processing of complex sounds in the macaque nonprimary auditory cortex. Science 268, 111–114. doi: 10.1126/science.7701330

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P., Tian, B., Pons, T., and Mishkin, M. (1997). Serial and parallel processing in rhesus monkey auditory cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 382, 89–103.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Romanski, L. M. (2012). Integration of faces and vocalizations in ventral prefrontal cortex: implications for the evolution of audiovisual speech. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109(Suppl. 1), 10717–10724. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1204335109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Romanski, L. M., Averbeck, B. B., and Diltz, M. (2005). Neural representation of vocalizations in the primate ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 734–747. doi: 10.1152/jn.00675.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Romanski, L. M., Bates, J. F., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1999a). Auditory belt and parabelt projections to the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 403, 141–157.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Romanski, L. M., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2002). An auditory domain in primate prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 15–16. doi: 10.1038/nn781

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Romanski, L. M., and Hwang, J. (2012). Timing of audiovisual inputs to the prefrontal cortex and multisensory integration. Neuroscience 214, 36–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.03.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Romanski, L. M., Tian, B., Fritz, J., Mishkin, M., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., and Rauschecker, J. P. (1999b). Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 1131–1136.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rowe, J., Hughes, L., Eckstein, D., and Owen, A. M. (2008). Rule-selection and action-selection have a shared neuroanatomical basis in the human prefrontal and parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2275–2285. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm249

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Russ, B. E., Ackelson, A. L., Baker, A. E., and Cohen, Y. E. (2008a). Coding of auditory-stimulus identity in the auditory non-spatial processing stream. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 87–95. doi: 10.1152/jn.01069.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Russ, B. E., Orr, L. E., and Cohen, Y. E. (2008b). Prefrontal neurons predict choices during an auditory same-different task. Curr. Biol. 18, 1483–1488. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.054

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saleem, K. S., Kondo, H., and Price, J. L. (2008). Complementary circuits connection the orbital and medial prefrontal networks with the temporal, insular, and opercular cortex in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 506, 659–693. doi: 10.1002/cne.21577

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saleem, K. S., Miller, B., and Price, J. L. (2014). Subdivisions and connectional networks of the lateral prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 522, 1641–1690. doi: 10.1002/cne.23498

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Scott, B. H., Yin, P., and Mishkin, M. (2013). Activity of auditory cortical neurons in monkeys performing a short-term memory task. Soc. Neurosci. 549.16.

Seltzer, B., and Pandya, D. N. (1989). Frontal lobe connections of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 281, 97–113. doi: 10.1002/cne.902810108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Seydell-Greenwald, A., Greenberg, A. S., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2013). Are you listening? Brain activation associated with sustained nonspatial auditory attention in the presence and absence of stimulation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 2233–2252. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22323

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stevens, A. A., Skudlarski, P., Gatenby, J. C., and Gore, J. C. (2000). Event-related fMRI of auditory and visual oddball tasks. Magn. Reson. Imaging 18, 495–502. doi: 10.1016/S0730-725X(00)00128-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Strand, F., Forssberg, H., Klingberg, T., and Norrelgen, F. (2008). Phonological working memory with auditory presentation of pseudo-words – an event related fMRI Study. Brain Res. 1212, 48–54. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.097

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sugihara, T., Diltz, M. D., Averbeck, B. B., and Romanski, L. M. (2006). Integration of auditory and visual communication information in the primate ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 26, 11138–11147. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3550-06.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Talairach, J., and Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. New York, NY: Thieme Medical Publishers.

Tanila, H., Carlson, S., Linnankoski, I., and Kahila, H. (1993). Regional distribution of functions in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the monkey. Behav. Brain Res. 53, 63–71. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80266-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tanila, H., Carlson, S., Linnankoski, I., Lindroos, F., and Kahila, H. (1992). Functional properties of dorsolateral prefrontal cortical neurons in awake monkey. Behav. Brain Res. 47, 169–180. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80123-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tian, B., Reser, D., Durham, A., Kustov, A., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2001). Functional specialization in rhesus monkey auditory cortex. Science 292, 290–293.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Eichhorn, G. R., Grabowski, T., Ponto, L. L., and Hichwa, R. D. (2003). Neural correlates of naming animals from their characteristic sounds. Neuropsychologia 41, 847–854. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00223-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tsao, D. Y., Schweers, N., Moeller, S., and Freiwald, W. A. (2008). Patches of face-selective cortex in the macaque frontal lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 877–879. doi: 10.1038/nn.2158

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tsunada, J., Baker, A. E., Christison-Lagay, K. L., Davis, S. J., and Cohen, Y. E. (2011). Modulation of cross-frequency coupling by novel and repeated stimuli in the primate ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Front. Psychol. 2:217. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00217

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tsunada, J., Lee, J. H., and Cohen, Y. E. (2012). Differential representation of auditory categories between cell classes in primate auditory cortex. J. Physiol. 590, 3129–3139. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.232892

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vaadia, E., Benson, D. A., Hienz, R. D., and Goldstein, M. H. Jr. (1986). Unit study of monkey frontal cortex: active localization of auditory and of visual stimuli. J. Neurophysiol. 56, 934–952.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Vaadia, E., Bergman, H., and Abeles, M. (1989). Neuronal activities related to higher brain functions–theoretical and experimental implications. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 36, 25–35. doi: 10.1109/10.16446

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vaden, K. I. Jr. Kuchinsky, S. E., Cute, S. L., Ahlstrom, J. B., Dubno, J. R., and Eckert, M. A. (2013). The cingulo-opercular network provides word-recognition benefit. J Neurosci. 33, 18979–18986. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1417-13.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Warden, M. R., and Miller, E. K. (2007). The representaion of multiple objects in prefrontal neuronal delay activity. Cereb. Cortex 17, i41–i50. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm070

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watanabe, M. (1992). Frontal units of the monkey coding the associative significance of visual and auditory stimuli. Exp. Brain Res. 89, 233–247.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Waters, G., Caplan, D., Alpert, N., and Stanczak, L. (2003). Individual differences in rCBF correlates of syntactic processing in sentence comprehension: effects of working memory and speed of processing. Neuroimage 19, 101–112. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00007-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Webster, M. J., Bachevalier, J., and Ungerleider, L. G. (1994). Connections of inferior temporal areas TEO and TE with parietal and frontal cortex in macaque monkeys. Cereb. Cortex 4, 470–483. doi: 10.1093/cercor/4.5.470

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weeks, R., Horwitz, B., Aziz-Sultan, A., Tian, B., Wessinger, C. M., Cohen, L. G., et al. (2000). A positron emission tomographic study of auditory localization in the congenitally blind. J. Neurosci. 20, 2664–2672.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Wilson, F. A., O'Scalaidhe, S. P., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1993). Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. Science 260, 1955–1958. doi: 10.1126/science.8316836

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wollberg, Z., and Sela, J. (1980). Frontal cortex of the awake squirrel monkey: responses of single cells to visual and auditory stimuli. Brain Res. 198, 216–220. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(80)90358-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zatorre, R. J., Bouffard, M., Ahad, P., and Belin, P. (2002). Where is ‘where’ in the human auditory cortex? Nat. Neurosci. 5, 905–909. doi: 10.1038/nn904

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zatorre, R. J., Bouffard, M., and Belin, P. (2004). Sensitivity to auditory object features in human temporal neocortex. J. Neurosci. 24, 3637–3642. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5458-03.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zatorre, R. J., Evans, A. C., and Meyer, E. (1994). Neural mechanisms underlying melodic perception and memory for pitch. J. Neurosci. 14, 1908–1919.

Zatorre, R. J., Perry, D. W., Beckett, C. A., Westbury, C. F., and Evans, A. C. (1998). Functional anatomy of musical processing in listeners with absolute pitch and relative pitch. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 5, 3172–3177. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3172

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: monkey, working memory, acoustic, frontal lobe

Citation: Plakke B and Romanski LM (2014) Auditory connections and functions of prefrontal cortex. Front. Neurosci. 8:199. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00199

Received: 31 March 2014; Accepted: 26 June 2014;
Published online: 23 July 2014.

Edited by:

Yukiko Kikuchi, Newcastle University Medical School, UK

Reviewed by:

Kadharbatcha S. Saleem, National Institutes of Health, USA
Christopher I. Petkov, Newcastle University, UK

Copyright © 2014 Plakke and Romanski. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bethany Plakke, Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, 601 Elmwood Ave., Box 603, Rochester, NY 14642, USA e-mail: bethany_plakke@urmc.rochester.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.