Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 21 October 2015
Sec. Neurogenomics

Association between polymorphisms in NOS3 and KCNH2 and social memory

\r\nSusanne HenningssonSusanne Henningsson1Anna Zettergren,Anna Zettergren1,2Daniel HoveyDaniel Hovey1Lina JonssonLina Jonsson1Joakim SvrdJoakim Svärd3Diana S. CortesDiana S. Cortes4Jonas MelkeJonas Melke1Natalie C. Ebner,Natalie C. Ebner5,6Petri LaukkaPetri Laukka4Hkan Fischer,Håkan Fischer3,4Lars Westberg*Lars Westberg1*
  • 1Department of Pharmacology, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology at the Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology at the Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
  • 3Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
  • 4Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
  • 6Department of Aging and Geriatric Research, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA

Social memory, including the ability to recognize faces and voices, is essential for social relationships. It has a large heritable component, but the knowledge about the contributing genes is sparse. The genetic variation underlying inter-individual differences in social memory was investigated in an exploratory sample (n = 55), genotyped with a chip comprising approximately 200,000 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), and in a validation sample (n = 582), where 30 SNPs were targeted. In the exploratory study face identity recognition was measured. The validation study also measured vocal sound recognition, as well as recognition of faces and vocal sounds combined (multimodal condition). In the exploratory study, the 30 SNPs that were associated with face recognition at puncorrected < 0.001 and located in genes, were chosen for further study. In the validation study two of these SNPs showed significant associations with recognition of faces, vocal sounds, and multimodal stimuli: rs1800779 in the gene encoding nitric oxide synthase 3 (NOS3) and rs3807370 in the gene encoding the voltage-gated channel, subfamily H, member 2 (KCNH2), in strong linkage disequilibrium with each other. The uncommon alleles were associated with superior performance, and the effects were present for men only (p < 0.0002). The exploratory study also showed a weaker but significant association with (non-emotional) word recognition, an effect that was independent of the effect on face recognition. This study demonstrates evidence for an association between NOS3 and KCNH2 SNPs and social memory.

Introduction

Social memory refers to the ability to recognize the identity of previously encountered individuals, an ability essential for successful social interactions. While in rodents this skill is based on olfactory and pheromonal signals, in humans it is based mainly on the identification of faces and voices (Belin et al., 2011).

Face recognition ability, the most investigated facet of social memory in humans, varies considerably in the population (Kennerknecht et al., 2006; Russell et al., 2009) and has been reported to be highly heritable (Wilmer et al., 2010), indicating that some of the inter-individual variation in face recognition ability can be explained by genetic factors. For prosopagnosia—characterized by serious impairments in face recognition (Hecaen and Angelergues, 1962; Benton and Van Allen, 1972)—an autosomal dominant inheritance has even been suggested (Kennerknecht et al., 2006).

To date, the knowledge about which molecules are involved in social memory is sparse. Sex differences with regard to the ability to recognize faces and the mechanism for processing faces (Fischer et al., 2007) have provided some evidence for involvement of molecules related to sex differentiation. So far, rodent studies have put forward oxytocin (Ferguson et al., 2000, 2001), vasopressin (Le Moal et al., 1987), estrogen (Choleris et al., 2003), and nitric oxide (Mutlu et al., 2011) as crucial players for social memory, suggesting related genes to be candidates in humans (Skuse et al., 2014).

Domain-specificity has been suggested for recognition of faces, as compared to objects (Rezlescu et al., 2014; Weigelt et al., 2014). In line with this, face memory deficits have been shown often to be associated with a tendency to process faces as if they were any other object (Langdell, 1978; Boucher and Lewis, 1992; Rutherford et al., 2007; Spezio et al., 2007; Adolphs et al., 2008; Harms et al., 2010; McPartland et al., 2011; Arkush et al., 2013).

With the aim to identify common genetic variation that influences the mechanism of, and explain the differences in, social memory, we investigated two independent samples of healthy men and women. A first exploratory study (n = 55) measured performance in recognition of neutrally expressive faces. In this first study, we also had access to performance on a task measuring word memory, thus enabling analyses of whether associations with memory deficits were face-specific or rather due to an effect on general memory. A second, validation study (n = 582) measured performance in recognition of faces displaying both neutral and emotional expressions, as well as recognition of neutral and emotional vocal sounds and recognition in a multimodal condition where participants saw faces and heard the corresponding sounds simultaneously. Of the approximately 200,000 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) genotyped with a chip (the MetaboChip) in the exploratory study, 30 were filtered out as associated (p < 0.001) with the ability to recognize faces, and were subsequently targeted in the validation study.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Exploratory Study

Social memory performance and genetic data were available for a total of 55 participants, 29 women and 26 men (25 younger: 20–31 years, mean ± sd: 25.1 ± 3.4; 12 females; 30 older: 65–74 years, mean ± sd: 68.2 ± 2.5; 17 females; Ebner et al., 2012). Word memory data was available for 56 participants (29 women, 27 men). All participants were right-handed, native Swedish speakers, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, had no contraindications to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), had no history of stroke, heart disease or primary degenerative neurological disorder, no past or present neuropsychiatric diseases, diabetes or neurological disorders, and were free from blood-thinning medication, as assessed by self-reported medical history. For older adults, a radiologist screened a T1-weighted and a T2-weighted image and ruled out abnormal levels of atrophy or lesions. All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the regional ethical review board of Stockholm. All participants were Caucasian, as indicated by self-report.

Validation Study

The validation study included 582 participants for whom both behavioral and genetic data were available, 223 men (age range: 18–36 years, mean ± sd: 23.4 ± 3.3) and 359 women (age range: 18–34 years, mean ± sd: 22.9 ± 3.2). All participants were right-handed, fluent in Swedish, healthy and had no past or present psychiatric diseases, or substance abuse. All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the regional ethical review board of Stockholm. Ethnicity was assessed by asking which country parents and grand-parents were born in. Eighty-seven percent (181 men, 309 women) of the participants were Caucasian.

Genotyping

Exploratory Study

The participants were genotyped using the Illumina iSelect MetaboChip. This chip includes nearly 200,000 SNPs selected from the results of genome-wide meta-analyses of several metabolism- and cardiovascular-relevant traits. It was designed to capture genetic variation coupled to type two diabetes, coronary artery disease, and myocardial infarction (Voight et al., 2012). Cardiovascular-relevant genes are often important for general brain function. The MetaboChip thus also covers genomic regions of interest for memory and face processing, including those involved in hormonal and neurotransmitter functions. Moreover, as the chip covers most of the genome, further polymorphisms relevant for social memory are covered in an indirect way by being in high linkage disequilibrium (LD) with those on the chip.

Validation Study

DNA was extracted from saliva samples using OraGene DNA self-collection kit (DNA Genotek, Inc., Ottawa, ON, Canada). The 30 SNPs that showed an association with face recognition at p < 0.001 in the exploratory study were genotyped with KASPar®, a competitive allele-specific polymerase chain reaction SNP genotyping system using FRET quencher cassette oligos (http://www.lgcgenomics.com). The genotyping success rate was >95% and all of the SNPs were found to be in Hardy Weinberg equilibrium. The association observed for the NOS3 polymorphism (see Results) prompted us to genotype two additional SNPs in this gene, rs2070744 and rs1799983.

Tasks in the Exploratory Study

Face Recognition Task

Face recognition was measured during functional MRI (fMRI) scanning (see Ebner et al., 2012 for an fMRI study on this sample). During incidental encoding (8.4 min), 48 photographs of neutral faces (unique face identities) were presented in pseudo-random order, and interspersed with 24 low-level null events (black crosses on gray background). Each stimulus was shown for 3.5 s. In between two faces, a fixation cross appeared on the screen in a jittered fashion (3–4 s). The pictures were taken from the FACES database (Ebner et al., 2010) and depicted equal numbers of men and women and younger and older faces. The instruction was to look at the pictures “as if you watched TV.” After a ten-min retention interval, during which anatomical images were taken, the two fMRI runs (each 8.4 min) of a surprise recognition task followed, during which the same 48 faces were presented at the same rate as at encoding (but in a different, pseudo-randomized order), together with 48 new distractor faces, randomly interspersed. The task was to indicate whether a face had been previously seen or not via button press (Gardiner and Richardson-Klavehn, 2000). D prime (d') was calculated based on participants' responses (MacMillan and Creelman, 2005).

Word Recognition Task

Word pair recognition memory was administered using the category-instance task (Dolan and Fletcher, 1997; Nyberg et al., 2009). Prior to scanning, participants were presented with pairs of words (n = 34) consisting of a category (e.g., “tree”) and an instance of the category (e.g., “pine”). Participants were instructed to memorize the pairs. Each pair was presented twice and for 2 s. During fMRI scanning, another list of word pairs was presented (2 s/item) with a jittered (1–6 s) inter-stimulus display of a fixation cross. A third of the pairs of this list had been presented before (old category, old instance; n = 17), another third were new (new category, new instance; n = 17), and the last third were categories that had been seen before but were paired with new instances (old category, new instance; n = 17). Participants again were instructed to memorize the pairs. After a 50 min retention interval, during which participants performed other tasks in the scanner, a cued recall test was administered outside the scanner. During this recall test, participants were presented with all categories seen during the task and were asked to pair each category with the instance presented during scanning. Based on these responses, the sum of correct responses was computed as a measure of word pair recognition memory.

Task in the Validation Study

Social Memory Task

The social memory task, measuring the recognition of the identity of faces, vocal sounds and their combination, consisted of an incidental encoding session and a recognition session. In contrast to the exploratory study, the validation tasks were not performed in a scanner but measured behaviorally only. During the encoding session, participants were presented with 24 photographs of faces, followed by 24 human vocal sounds, followed by 24 multimodal stimuli (photographs of faces presented together with vocal sounds). The order was randomized (within the modality) across subjects and no face or voice identity was presented more than once in each condition. In contrast to the first exploratory study, the faces and voices expressed anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, or no emotion (four stimuli for each condition). The timing for the presentation was self-paced and participants were asked to indicate with the mouse which emotion was conveyed by the stimulus in a forced-choice task. The response options were the same as the expressed emotions. MediaLab software (Jarvis, 2008) was used for stimulus presentation and recording of responses. The pictures were color photographs from the FACES database (Ebner et al., 2010) and the sounds were non-linguistic emotional vocalizations (e.g., crying, laughter, sighs, screams) from the VENEC database (Laukka et al., 2013). At recognition (6–10 min after encoding depending on condition and reaction times), the same stimuli, interspersed with the same number of new distractor stimuli (24 old, 24 new), were presented. The same identity always expressed the same emotion. Participants were asked to indicate (self-paced) whether they recognized a stimuli or not, using the remember/know-paradigm (Gardiner and Richardson-Klavehn, 2000). Pooling the correct answers (remember and know answers) and controlling hit frequency for false alarms (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999) provided four measures of recognition accuracy: d'faces, d'vocal, d'multimodal, and the average across presentation modalities, d'all.

Procedure

In the exploratory study, associations between nearly 200,000 SNPs and face recognition were explored. The most promising variants were further analyzed in the validation study with regard to a potential association with the d'all measure of social memory. Post-hoc analyses for significant SNPs included the specific measures d'faces, d'vocal, and d'multimodal. To determine whether the association with memory was specific for the social dimension, post-hoc analyses also included the word pair recognition task performed by the participants in the exploratory study.

Statistical Analyses

Linear regressions using SNP and Variation Suite v7.7 (Golden Helix, Inc., Bozeman, MT, www.goldenhelix.com) were used to determine the significance levels of the MetaboChip SNPs with MAF>5%. SPSS (IBM Corp., Version 22.0. Armonk, NY) was used for further examination of the NOS3 and KCNH2 associations in the exploratory study and for the 30 SNPs in the validation study. Linear regression models (additive model) were used, treating the heterozygote as the intermediate. Multiple testing was controlled for using Bonferroni correction. The significance level for the validation study was thus set to p = 0.00083 (0.05/(30 SNPs * 2 sexes)). Linkage disequilibrium (LD) between polymorphisms was assessed by Haploview 4.2 (Barrett et al., 2005).

Results

Exploration and Validation of Associations between SNPs and Social Memory

None of the 200,000 SNPs genotyped in the exploratory study displayed an association with face recognition memory that survived correction for multiple testing. Thirteen SNPs were significant at a threshold of p < 0.0001, 113 at a threshold of p < 0.001 and 1507 at a threshold of p < 0.01. Of the 113 SNPs showing suggestive evidence of association at a level of p < 0.001, those that had a minor allele frequency (MAF) > 5%, were in Hardy Weinberg Equilibrium, were located in genes, and were not in high LD (r2 > 0.8) with SNPs in the same gene, were filtered out. Table 1 shows these 30 SNPs, which were also analyzed in the validation study with regard to their association with the d'all measure. Two of the SNPs, the rs1800779 in NOS3 and the rs3807370 in KCNH2, in high LD (D' > 0.9, r2 > 0.9) with each other, displayed associations that survived correction for multiple testing (see information below). None of the other 28 SNPs showed associations that survived correction for multiple testing.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Promising SNPs (p < 0.001) identified in the exploratory study and selected for further analyses in the validation study.

KCNH2 and NOS3 SNPs in the Exploratory and Validation Studies

In the exploratory study, two of the 30 SNPs showing associations with face recognition (d'faces-n) at p < 0.001, were rs1800779 in NOS3 and rs3807370 in KCNH2 (NOS3: p = 0.0006, β = 0.45; KCNH2: p = 0.0009, β = 0.43): less common homozygotes (GG and AA, respectively), showed more accurate face recognition. The effects were similar for men and women (NOS3 men: p = 0.01, β = 0.49, women: p = 0.02, β = 0.42, Figure 1A; KCNH2 men: p = 0.02, β = 0.45, women: p = 0.02, β = 0.42). There was no significant difference in face recognition performance between younger and older participants [p = 0.09, t(56) = 1.7], nor between men and women [p = 0.48, t(56) = −0.7].

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Recognition memory for NOS3 genotypes. In the exploratory study, face (A) and word recognition (B) was superior in carriers of the G-allele of the NOS3 rs1800779 (M: nAA = 9, nAG = 14, nGG = 3 for faces and nAA = 9, nAG = 15, nGG = 3 for words. F: nAA = 11, nAG = 15, nGG = 3 for faces and nAA = 10, nAG = 15, nGG = 4 for words). The results were similar for the KCNH2 polymorphism. In the validation study, social recognition memory (C) was superior in male carriers of the G-allele of the NOS3 rs1800779 (M: nAA = 126, nAG = 84, nGG = 14. F: nAA = 166, nAG = 149, nGG = 44). The results were similar for the KCNH2 polymorphism and the NOS3 rs2070744 polymorphism. The figure displays mean ± sd. M, Male; F, Female. See the Results Section for p-values.

In the validation study, d'all was significantly associated with both polymorphisms in men (NOS3: p = 0.0001, β = 0.25, Figure 1C; KCNH2: p = 0.0002, β = 0.25) but not in women (NOS3: p = 0.8, β = 0.014, Figure 1C; KCNH2: p = 0.75, β = 0.017). These results did not change notably when participants of non-Caucasian origin were excluded from the analyses (men: NOS3: p = 0.0001, β = 0.28; KCNH2: p = 0.00008, β = 0.29; women: NOS3: p = 0.85, β = 0.01; KCNH2: p = 0.76, β = 0.02). The associations survived correction for multiple testing (p < 0.00083; see Table 1 for corrected p-values). Post-hoc tests showed this effect (in males) to be present for d'faces (NOS3: p = 0.002, β = 0.20; KCNH2: p = 0.02, β = 0.16), d'vocal (NOS3: p = 0.008, β = 0.18; KCNH2: p = 0.001, β = 0.22), and d'multimodal (NOS3: p = 0.007, β = 0.18; KCNH2: p = 0.02, β = 0.16).

To further investigate the importance of NOS3, two additional SNPs of known functional importance were genotyped. The NOS3 promoter SNP, rs2070744 (MAF = 0.30), showed effects very similar to the rs1800779, i.e., a significant association with d'all in men (p = 0.0002, β = 0.25), but not in women. As for rs1800779, post-hoc tests showed associations with d'faces (p = 0.002, β = 0.21), d'vocal (p = 0.008, β = 0.18) and d'multimodal (p = 0.01, β = 0.16) in men. Carriers of the uncommon CC genotype displayed better social recognition. The exon 8 rs1799983 (MAF = 0.27) showed a weak effect on the d'all measure in men (p = 0.04, β = 0.14) that did not survive correction for multiple testing. The rs2070744 showed high LD with the KCNH2 SNP and the NOS3 1800779 (D' > 0.9, r2 > 0.9), whereas the LD between rs1799983 and the other three polymorphisms was low (D' < 0.5, r2 < 0.2). T-tests revealed that there was no significant difference in any of the measures of social memory between men and women (p > 0.7).

NOS3 rs1800779 and KCNH2 rs3807370 polymorphisms were significantly associated with word pair recognition, but only for dominant models, i.e., when pooling the uncommon genotype with the heterozygote. Carriers of the uncommon allele had better memory than carriers of the common homozygous genotype [NOS3: p = 0.02, t(54) = 2.4, Figure 1B; KCNH2: p = 0.03, t(54) = 2.3], an effect driven by men [NOS3: p = 0.03, t(25) = 2.3; KCNH2: p = 0.04, t(25) = 2.1].

The two performance measures of face (d'faces-n) and word pair recognition in the exploratory study did not correlate significantly (all: p = 0.08, Pearson = 0.24; men: p = 0.07, Pearson = 0.37; women: p = 0.58, Pearson = 0.11). To explore whether the genetic effects on face and word memory were dependent, word recognition performance was added as covariate to the regression models with d'faces-n as the dependent variable. The addition did not change the results, neither for the NOS3 rs1800779 (p = 0.0006, β = 0.45 alone in the model; p = 0.0006, β = 0.45 with word memory performance in the model), nor for the KCNH2 polymorphism (p = 0.0009, β = 0.43 alone in the model, p = 0.0008, β = 0.44 with word memory performance in the model). Also, word pair recognition memory was not a significant predictor in the models.

Discussion

Advances in the field have proven that the heritability of complex traits such as social memory is explained by a large number of common genetic variants, all contributing with very small effects. The attempt to detect new variants and genes using genome-wide approaches requires very large samples to reach the genome-wide corrected significance level. We therefore investigated the possibility to validate the most promising associations between SNPs and behavior from an exploratory study of many SNPs in a small sample—not surviving correction for multiple testing—in a larger independent sample. By using this strategy we identified the NOS3 rs1800779 and KCNH2 rs380730 polymorphisms, in high LD with each other, as intriguing contributors to the inter-individual variation in social memory. The associations survived correction for multiple testing in the considerably larger validation sample. Furthermore, in the exploratory sample the polymorphisms associated with face recognition independently of sex, while, in the validation study they were associated with recognition of identity through faces as well as vocal sounds in men only. The smaller exploratory study provided evidence for a weaker association also with word pair memory, an effect that was independent of the effect of social memory.

NOS3 is situated just downstream of KCNH2 on chromosome 7q36. The two genes, both expressed in the brain (Judas et al., 1999; Huffaker et al., 2009), encode nitric oxide synthase 3, i.e., endothelial NOS, and the potassium voltage-gated channel, subfamily H, member 2, respectively. Endothelial NOS is responsible for the production of nitric oxide (NO) from L-arginine in the endothelium. NO is vasodilatory, and regulates cerebral blood flow (Ignarro, 1989; Quyyumi et al., 1995). It triggers multiple signal transduction pathways and influences synaptic function including transmitter release (Sagi et al., 2014). NO and NOS3-mediated NO signaling is known to be involved in hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) (Schuman and Madison, 1991; Dinerman et al., 1994; O'Dell et al., 1994) and has therefore been investigated in relation to learning and memory. Evidence for an involvement of NOS in social memory has been provided by rodent studies showing that NOS inhibitors impair olfactory memory in a social recognition test (Böhme et al., 1993; Mutlu et al., 2011), as well as in non-social memory (Böhme et al., 1993), an effect that required inhibition of both endothelial and neuronal NOS (NOS1) (Mutlu et al., 2011). The current finding thus provides intriguing evidence of a conserved role for NO in modulating social cognition in humans, comparable to the conserved role shown for oxytocin (Skuse et al., 2014).

Although the uncommon allele of the NOS3 promoter SNP rs1800779 has been associated with lower peripheric levels of NOS3 mRNA and protein (Aminuddin et al., 2013), its function is, as of yet, not established. However, the other two NOS3 polymorphisms genotyped in the validation study, i.e., the rs2070744 and rs1799983, are known to be functional, the promoter polymorphism rs2070744 (-786T/C) affecting the expression of the gene (Nakayama et al., 2000; Kittel-Schneider et al., 2015), and the exon 8 rs1799983 (894G/T) implicating an amino acid substitution, Glu298Asp, that causes a truncation of the protein (Tesauro et al., 2000). In the present study the effect for rs2070744 was equally strong as the effect for rs1800779. Therefore, it is plausible that this SNP, in high LD with the rs1800779, is responsible for the observed association. Functional studies have consistently shown the uncommon C-allele of rs2070744, here associated with superior social recognition, to be associated with less gene expression. It has been shown to reduce promoter activity (Nakayama et al., 2000), to be associated with lower mRNA (Venturelli et al., 2005; Kittel-Schneider et al., 2015) and NO metabolite levels in blood (Kittel-Schneider et al., 2015), and with a lack of shear stress-induced NOS3 expression (Cattaruzza et al., 2004). In line with our finding of enhanced social memory in carriers of the allele related to reduced NOS3 and thus NO production, elevated NO levels in blood have been negatively correlated with the performance on memory tests in humans (Talarowska et al., 2012).

To our knowledge, only one previous study has examined the relationship between NOS3 and memory in humans (Solé-Padullés et al., 2004). This previous study comprised participants with mild cognitive impairment. They showed that carriers of the uncommon Asp-allele of rs1799983, resulting in a truncation of the protein, had lower memory performance, which is consistent with the relationship between NOS inhibition and impaired memory in rodents. In contrast, in our sample, the Asp-allele was weakly (not surviving correction for multiple testing) associated with enhanced social memory.

The promoter polymorphisms in NOS3 are also in high LD with the intron 2 polymorphism in the neighboring gene KCNH2, which thus may be responsible for the associations reported. The rs380730 is located between two polymorphisms (rs3800779 and rs1036145) that have been associated with the expression of a truncated and brain-specific isoform of KCNH2, i.e., the ratio of KCNH2-3.1:KCNH2-1A (Huffaker et al., 2009). A higher degree of KCNH2-3.1 expression has been observed in patients with schizophrenia (Huffaker et al., 2009) and the rs3800779 SNP has been associated with schizophrenia, as well as with low IQ and working memory performance (Huffaker et al., 2009). A study of 191 Japanese individuals (Hashimoto et al., 2013) likewise showed an association between rs3800779 and working memory, as well as attention, but not with either verbal or visual memory (as assessed by the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning test and the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised), nor with social cognition as measured by the Facial Emotion Labeling Test (Hashimoto et al., 2013).

As mentioned in the introduction, face recognition memory involves a face-specific mechanism (Rezlescu et al., 2014; Weigelt et al., 2014). The potential influence of variation in NOS3 or KCNH2 on social memory appears, however, not to be specific to this dimension, but also to include word pair recognition memory, as suggested by the weaker, but significant association in the exploratory study. Recent studies show overlaps in development and function of face and word recognition (Bukowski et al., 2013; Dundas et al., 2013), such that those with difficulties in recognizing faces also show deficits for words and vice versa (Behrmann and Plaut, 2014). Face and word recognition memory did however not correlate in the exploratory sample, and the genetic effects on face recognition were independent of word recognition memory, as the regression coefficient did not change when the latter was included in the model.

Except for visual category, differences in the strength of the association for words and faces in the exploratory study could be due to other dissimilarities between these two tasks. Firstly, the words were not new to the participants in the same way as the faces were, because words are already represented in long-term memory from previous exposures. This is a point that other studies have solved by using famous faces (Nie et al., 2014). Second, the word pair recognition, but not the face recognition, test involved the component process of matching the two parts of the pair, the category and the instance.

In the exploratory study, the association between the NOS3 and KCNH2 polymorphisms and face recognition held in both men and women, whereas the larger validation study only revealed an effect in men. Differences in the characteristics of the social memory tasks used in the two studies may possibly explain this discrepancy. The exploratory study included the recognition of the identity of faces of neutral face expressions, whereas the validation study included faces of several different emotional expressions. However, a previous study of face recognition in children showed that emotional expressions did not influence the recognition of face identity (Krebs et al., 2011). A possible mechanism explaining the sex differences is suggested by the finding that estrogen induces NO production via NOS activation in endothelial cells (Nevzati et al., 2015), and that NOS3 polymorphisms hence may influence the degree of estrogen-induced production differently in men and women. Although there was no significant difference in face recognition performance between men and women in either of the present samples, sex differences have been repeatedly reported for both face recognition and its underlying neural mechanisms (Fischer et al., 2007).

In conclusion, by using one exploratory sample to isolate a number of promising polymorphisms and then examining them in a targeted manner in a validation sample, we have demonstrated an association between polymorphisms in the KCNH2 and NOS3 genes and social memory.

Author Contributions

Study concept and design: SH, DH, LW. Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors. Drafting of the manuscript: SH. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors. Final approval of the version to be published: All authors. Agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved: All authors.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Christina Lindahl for data collection to the validation study. Funding for the study was provided by grants from the Swedish Research Council (PL, HF, LW). The funding bodies had no role in the design and conduct of the study, preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript, and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

References

Adolphs, R., Spezio, M. L., Parlier, M., and Piven, J. (2008). Distinct face-processing strategies in parents of autistic children. Curr. Biol. 18, 1090–1093. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aminuddin, F., Hackett, T.-L., Stefanowicz, D., Saferali, A., Paré, P. D., Gulsvik, A., et al. (2013). Nitric oxide synthase polymorphisms, gene expression and lung function in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. BMC Pulm. Med. 13:64. doi: 10.1186/1471-2466-13-64

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arkush, L., Smith-Collins, A. P. R., Fiorentini, C., and Skuse, D. H. (2013). Recognition of face and non-face stimuli in autistic spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 6, 550–560. doi: 10.1002/aur.1318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, J. C., Fry, B., Maller, J., and Daly, M. J. (2005). Haploview: analysis and visualization of LD and haplotype maps. Bioinformatics 21, 263–265. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bth457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrmann, M., and Plaut, D. C. (2014). Bilateral hemispheric processing of words and faces: evidence from word impairments in prosopagnosia and face impairments in pure alexia. Cereb. Cortex 24, 1102–1118. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belin, P., Bestelmeyer, P. E. G., Latinus, M., and Watson, R. (2011). Understanding voice perception. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 711–725. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02041.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benton, A. L., and Van Allen, M. W. (1972). Prosopagnosia and facial discrimination. J. Neurol. Sci. 15, 167–172. doi: 10.1016/0022-510X(72)90004-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Böhme, G. A., Bon, C., Lemaire, M., Reibaud, M., Piot, O., Stutzmann, J. M., et al. (1993). Altered synaptic plasticity and memory formation in nitric oxide synthase inhibitor-treated rats. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 90, 9191–9194. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.19.9191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boucher, J., and Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 33, 843–859. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb01960.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bukowski, H., Dricot, L., Hanseeuw, B., and Rossion, B. (2013). Cerebral lateralization of face-sensitive areas in left-handers: only the FFA does not get it right. Cortex 49, 2583–2589. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cattaruzza, M., Guzik, T. J., Słodowski, W., Pelvan, A., Becker, J., Halle, M., et al. (2004). Shear stress insensitivity of endothelial nitric oxide synthase expression as a genetic risk factor for coronary heart disease. Circ. Res. 95, 841–847. doi: 10.1161/01.RES.0000145359.47708.2f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choleris, E., Gustafsson, J.-A., Korach, K. S., Muglia, L. J., Pfaff, D. W., and Ogawa, S. (2003). An estrogen-dependent four-gene micronet regulating social recognition: a study with oxytocin and estrogen receptor-alpha and -beta knockout mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 6192–6197. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0631699100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinerman, J. L., Dawson, T. M., Schell, M. J., Snowman, A., and Snyder, S. H. (1994). Endothelial nitric oxide synthase localized to hippocampal pyramidal cells: implications for synaptic plasticity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91, 4214–4218. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.10.4214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolan, R. J., and Fletcher, P. C. (1997). Dissociating prefrontal and hippocampal function in episodic memory encoding. Nature 388, 582–585. doi: 10.1038/41561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dundas, E. M., Plaut, D. C., and Behrmann, M. (2013). The joint development of hemispheric lateralization for words and faces. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 348–358. doi: 10.1037/a0029503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebner, N. C., Johnson, M. K., and Fischer, H. (2012). Neural mechanisms of reading facial emotions in young and older adults. Front. Psychol. 3:223. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebner, N. C., Riediger, M., and Lindenberger, U. (2010). FACES–a database of facial expressions in young, middle-aged, and older women and men: development and validation. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 351–362. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferguson, J. N., Aldag, J. M., Insel, T. R., and Young, L. J. (2001). Oxytocin in the medial amygdala is essential for social recognition in the mouse. J. Neurosci. 21, 8278–8285. Available online at: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/21/20/8278.long

PubMed Abstract

Ferguson, J. N., Young, L. J., Hearn, E. F., Matzuk, M. M., Insel, T. R., and Winslow, J. T. (2000). Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin gene. Nat. Genet. 25, 284–288. doi: 10.1038/77040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, H., Sandblom, J., Nyberg, L., Herlitz, A., and Bäckman, L. (2007). Brain activation while forming memories of fearful and neutral faces in women and men. Emotion 7, 767–773. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardiner, J. M., and Richardson-Klavehn, A. (2000). “Remembering and Knowing,” in The Oxford Handbook of Memory, eds E. Tulving and F. Craik (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 229–244.

Harms, M. B., Martin, A., and Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychol. Rev. 20, 290–322. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9138-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hashimoto, R., Ohi, K., Yasuda, Y., Fukumoto, M., Yamamori, H., Kamino, K., et al. (2013). The KCNH2 gene is associated with neurocognition and the risk of schizophrenia. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 14, 114–120. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2011.604350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hecaen, H., and Angelergues, R. (1962). Agnosia for faces (prosopagnosia). Arch. Neurol. 7, 92–100. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1962.04210020014002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huffaker, S. J., Chen, J., Nicodemus, K. K., Sambataro, F., Yang, F., Mattay, V., et al. (2009). A primate-specific, brain isoform of KCNH2 affects cortical physiology, cognition, neuronal repolarization and risk of schizophrenia. Nat. Med. 15, 509–518. doi: 10.1038/nm.1962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ignarro, L. J. (1989). Biological actions and properties of endothelium-derived nitric oxide formed and released from artery and vein. Circ. Res. 65, 1–21. doi: 10.1161/01.RES.65.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarvis B. G. (2008). MediaLab, v. 2008 [Computer program]. New York, NY: Empirisoft Corporation.

Judas, M., Sestan, N., and Kostović, I. (1999). Nitrinergic neurons in the developing and adult human telencephalon: transient and permanent patterns of expression in comparison to other mammals. Microsc. Res. Tech. 45, 401–419.

PubMed Abstract

Kennerknecht, I., Grueter, T., Welling, B., Wentzek, S., Horst, J., Edwards, S., et al. (2006). First report of prevalence of non-syndromic hereditary prosopagnosia (HPA). Am. J. Med. Genet. A 140, 1617–1622. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.31343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kittel-Schneider, S., Reuß, M., Meyer, A., Weber, H., Gessner, A., Leistner, C., et al. (2015). Multi-level biomarker analysis of nitric oxide synthase isoforms in bipolar disorder and adult ADHD. J. Psychopharmacol. 29, 31–38. doi: 10.1177/0269881114555251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krebs, J. F., Biswas, A., Pascalis, O., Kamp-Becker, I., Remschmidt, H., and Schwarzer, G. (2011). Face processing in children with autism spectrum disorder: independent or interactive processing of facial identity and facial expression? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 796–804. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1098-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: an approach to the study of autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 19, 255–268. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1978.tb00468.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laukka, P., Elfenbein, H. A., Söder, N., Nordström, H., Althoff, J., Chui, W., et al. (2013). Cross-cultural decoding of positive and negative non-linguistic emotion vocalizations. Front. Psychol. 4:353. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Moal, M., Dantzer, R., Michaud, B., and Koob, G. F. (1987). Centrally injected arginine vasopressin (AVP) facilitates social memory in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 77, 353–359. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(87)90527-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacMillan, N., and Creelman, D. (2005). Detection Theory: A User's Guide. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

McPartland, J. C., Webb, S. J., Keehn, B., and Dawson, G. (2011). Patterns of visual attention to faces and objects in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 148–157. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1033-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mutlu, O., Ulak, G., and Belzung, C. (2011). Effects of nitric oxide synthase inhibitors 1-(2-trifluoromethylphenyl)–imidazole (TRIM) and 7-nitroindazole (7-NI) on learning and memory in mice. Fundam. Clin. Pharmacol. 25, 368–377. doi: 10.1111/j.1472-8206.2010.00851.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakayama, M., Yasue, H., Yoshimura, M., Shimasaki, Y., Ogawa, H., Kugiyama, K., et al. (2000). T(-786)–> C mutation in the 5'-flanking region of the endothelial nitric oxide synthase gene is associated with myocardial infarction, especially without coronary organic stenosis. Am. J. Cardiol. 86, 628–634. doi: 10.1016/S0002-9149(00)01041-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nevzati, E., Shafighi, M., Bakhtian, K. D., Treiber, H., Fandino, J., and Fathi, A. R. (2015). Estrogen induces nitric oxide production via nitric oxide synthase activation in endothelial cells. Acta Neurochir. Suppl. 120, 141–145. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-04981-6_24

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nie, A., Griffin, M., Keinath, A., Walsh, M., Dittmann, A., and Reder, L. (2014). ERP profiles for face and word recognition are based on their status in semantic memory not their stimulus category. Brain Res. 1557, 66–73. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyberg, L., Andersson, M., Forsgren, L., Jakobsson-Mo, S., Larsson, A., Marklund, P., et al. (2009). Striatal dopamine D2 binding is related to frontal BOLD response during updating of long-term memory representations. Neuroimage 46, 1194–1199. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Dell, T. J., Huang, P. L., Dawson, T. M., Dinerman, J. L., Snyder, S. H., Kandel, E. R., et al. (1994). Endothelial NOS and the blockade of LTP by NOS inhibitors in mice lacking neuronal NOS. Science 265, 542–546.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Quyyumi, A. A., Dakak, N., Andrews, N. P., Gilligan, D. M., Panza, J. A., and Cannon, R. O. (1995). Contribution of nitric oxide to metabolic coronary vasodilation in the human heart. Circulation 92, 320–326. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.92.3.320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezlescu, C., Susilo, T., Barton, J. J. S., and Duchaine, B. (2014). Normal social evaluations of faces in acquired prosopagnosia. Cortex 50, 200–203. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, R., Duchaine, B., and Nakayama, K. (2009). Super-recognizers: people with extraordinary face recognition ability. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, 252–257. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutherford, M. D., Clements, K. A., and Sekuler, A. B. (2007). Differences in discrimination of eye and mouth displacement in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Res. 47, 2099–2110. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagi, Y., Heiman, M., Peterson, J. D., Musatov, S., Scarduzio, M., Logan, S. M., et al. (2014). Nitric oxide regulates synaptic transmission between spiny projection neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 17636–17641. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1420162111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuman, E. M., and Madison, D. V. (1991). A requirement for the intercellular messenger nitric oxide in long-term potentiation. Science 254, 1503–1506. doi: 10.1126/science.1720572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skuse, D. H., Lori, A., Cubells, J. F., Lee, I., Conneely, K. N., Puura, K., et al. (2014). Common polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is associated with human social recognition skills. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 1987–1992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302985111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solé-Padullés, C., Bartrés-Faz, D., Junqué, C., Via, M., Matarín, M., González-Pérez, E., et al. (2004). Poorer cognitive performance in humans with mild cognitive impairment carrying the T variant of the Glu/Asp NOS3 polymorphism. Neurosci. Lett. 358, 5–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.12.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spezio, M. L., Adolphs, R., Hurley, R. S. E., and Piven, J. (2007). Abnormal use of facial information in high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 929–939. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0232-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanislaw, H., and Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav. Res. Methods. Instrum. Comput. 31, 137–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03207704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talarowska, M., Gałecki, P., Maes, M., Orzechowska, A., Chamielec, M., Bartosz, G., et al. (2012). Nitric oxide plasma concentration associated with cognitive impairment in patients with recurrent depressive disorder. Neurosci. Lett. 510, 127–131. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tesauro, M., Thompson, W. C., Rogliani, P., Qi, L., Chaudhary, P. P., and Moss, J. (2000). Intracellular processing of endothelial nitric oxide synthase isoforms associated with differences in severity of cardiopulmonary diseases: cleavage of proteins with aspartate vs. glutamate at position 298. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 2832–2835. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.6.2832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venturelli, E., Galimberti, D., Lovati, C., Fenoglio, C., Scalabrini, D., Mariani, C., et al. (2005). The T-786C NOS3 polymorphism in Alzheimer's disease: association and influence on gene expression. Neurosci. Lett. 382, 300–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voight, B. F., Kang, H. M., Ding, J., Palmer, C. D., Sidore, C., Chines, P. S., et al. (2012). The metabochip, a custom genotyping array for genetic studies of metabolic, cardiovascular, and anthropometric traits. PLoS Genet. 8:e1002793. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weigelt, S., Koldewyn, K., Dilks, D. D., Balas, B., McKone, E., and Kanwisher, N. (2014). Domain-specific development of face memory but not face perception. Dev. Sci. 17, 47–58. doi: 10.1111/desc.12089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilmer, J. B., Germine, L., Chabris, C. F., Chatterjee, G., Williams, M., Loken, E., et al. (2010). Human face recognition ability is specific and highly heritable. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 5238–5241. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913053107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: face recognition, social, memory, nitric oxide, NOS3, KCNH2

Citation: Henningsson S, Zettergren A, Hovey D, Jonsson L, Svärd J, Cortes DS, Melke J, Ebner NC, Laukka P, Fischer H and Westberg L (2015) Association between polymorphisms in NOS3 and KCNH2 and social memory. Front. Neurosci. 9:393. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00393

Received: 23 July 2015; Accepted: 06 October 2015;
Published: 21 October 2015.

Edited by:

Jessica J. Connelly, University of Virginia, USA

Reviewed by:

James Morris, University of Virginia, USA
Steven C. Munger, The Jackson Laboratory, USA

Copyright © 2015 Henningsson, Zettergren, Hovey, Jonsson, Svärd, Cortes, Melke, Ebner, Laukka, Fischer and Westberg. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lars Westberg, lars.westberg@pharm.gu.se

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.