Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Integr. Neurosci., 20 October 2010
Volume 4 - 2010 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2010.00126

Nucleus accumbens core and shell are necessary for reinforcer devaluation effects on Pavlovian conditioned responding

  • 1 Program in Neuroscience, University of Maryland, School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
  • 2 Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, University of Maryland, School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
  • 3 Department of Psychiatry, University of Maryland, School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA

The nucleus accumbens (NA) has been hypothesized to be part of a circuit in which cue-evoked information about expected outcomes is mobilized to guide behavior. Here we tested this hypothesis using a Pavlovian reinforcer devaluation task, previously applied to assess outcome-guided behavior after damage to regions such as the orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala that send projections to NA. Rats with sham lesions or neurotoxic lesions of either the core or shell subdivision of NA were trained to associate a 10-s CS+ with delivery of three food pellets. After training, half of the rats in each lesion group received food paired with illness induced by LiCl injections; the remaining rats received food and illness unpaired. Subsequently, responding to the CS+ was assessed in an extinction probe test. Both sham and lesioned rats conditioned to the CS+ and formed a conditioned taste aversion. However only sham rats reduced their conditioned responding as a result of reinforcer devaluation; devalued rats with lesions of either core or shell showed levels of responding that were similar to lesioned, non-devalued rats. This impairment was not due to the loss of motivational salience conferred to the CS+ in lesioned rats as both groups responded similarly for the cue in conditioned reinforcement testing. These data suggest that NA core and shell are part of a circuit necessary for the use of cue-evoked information about expected outcomes to guide behavior.

Introduction

A critical feature of adaptive behavior is the ability to use expectations of outcomes to appropriately guide behavior. Experiments in rats, monkeys, and humans using reinforcer devaluation have implicated the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and basolateral amygdala (ABL) in the ability to use information about expected outcomes to guide Pavlovian behavior (Hatfield et al., 1996; Malkova et al., 1997; Gallagher et al., 1999; Gottfried et al., 2003; Izquierdo et al., 2004; Machado and Bachevalier, 2007).

ABL and OFC – defined broadly as including ventrolateral and lateral orbital regions as well as parts of insular cortex (Ongur and Price, 2000; Schoenbaum et al., 2002) – send projections to the nucleus accumbens (NA) (McDonald, 1991; Berendse et al., 1992; Brog et al., 1993; Voorn et al., 2004; Schilman et al., 2008). The NA has long been implicated in a variety of Pavlovian behaviors (Mogenson et al., 1980; Cardinal et al., 2002a). Neurons in the NA fire to cues in a manner that appears to signal their associative significance (Setlow et al., 2003; Nicola et al., 2004; Peoples et al., 2004; German and Fields, 2007; Hollander and Carelli, 2007; Kimchi and Laubach, 2009; Roesch et al., 2009; van der Meer and Redish, 2009), and this region is critical to Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer and other aspects of Pavlovian responding (Parkinson et al., 1999a,b, 2000; Corbit et al., 2001; Hall et al., 2001; Cardinal et al., 2002b; de Borchgrave et al., 2002; Balleine and Corbit, 2005).

These data suggest that NA, along with ABL and afferent regions in lateral OFC, might be part of a circuit critical for the use of cue-evoked information about expected outcomes to guide behavior. We tested this hypothesis by using a Pavlovian reinforcer devaluation task. In the critical extinction probe test, rats must use a representation of the outcome evoked by the CS+. To adaptively guide behavior, this CS+ evoked representation must be updated to reflect the current value of the outcome. Previous evidence has demonstrated that OFC and ABL are essential to updating and using the current value of the outcome to guide behavior. Here, we tested the role of NA core and shell in the Pavlovian reinforcer devaluation task and found pre-training lesions disrupted the ability of the animal to alter conditioned responding based on the current value of the outcome.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Fifty-six Male Long–Evans rats (Charles River Laboratories, Wilmington, MA, USA), weighing between 275 to 325 g on arrival, were individually housed and were given ad libitum access to food and water, except during testing. Beginning 5 days before testing and continuing until testing ended, rats were food deprived to 85% of their baseline body weight. The animals were placed on a 12-h light/dark cycle and tested only during the light cycle. Rats were tested at the University of Maryland, School of Medicine in accordance with University of Maryland and NIH guidelines.

Surgical Procedures

We performed aseptic surgeries to make bilateral neurotoxic lesions of the NA core and NA shell under isoflurane anesthesia. All surgeries were conducted prior to behavioral testing, and the animals were given 1 week to recover. Bilateral neurotoxic lesions of the core (n = 20) were made with a 2-μl Hamilton syringe using 0.09 M quinolinic acid, pH ∼7.4 (Sigma, St. Louis, MO, USA) in Dulbecco’s PBS (Sigma, St. Louis, MO, USA). Quinolinic acid (0.3 μl) was delivered at 0.1 μl/min at: AP: + 1.9; ML ± 1.9; DV −7.3. Bilateral shell lesions (n = 16) were made using 0.09 M NMDA (Sigma, St. Louis, MO, USA) in Dulbecco’s PBS (Sigma, St. Louis, MO, USA). NMDA was delivered at 0.1 μl/min at AP: 1.6, ML: ± 0.9 at three DV sites: DV −7.8 (0.2 μl), DV −7.2 (0.1 μl) and DV −6.5 (0.1 μl). These coordinates and neurotoxic agents have been demonstrated to selectively lesion either NA shell or NA core with little to no damage to surrounding regions (Ito et al., 2008). Sham surgeries (n = 20) were performed by lowering the Hamilton syringe without infusing neurotoxin.

Apparatus

Sixteen standard behavior boxes (12′ × 10′ × 12′) in sound attenuating cubicles were used for testing (Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA, USA). A recessed food cup was placed in the center of the right wall approximately 2 cm above the floor. A feeder, mounted outside of the behavioral box, contained 45 mg sucrose pellets (Bio-Serv., Frenchtown, NJ, USA) and was connected to the food cup. The house light was mounted above the food cup in the center panel.

Pavlovian Conditioning

All behavioral training procedures are outlined in Table 1. After rats reached 85% of their baseline body weight, they were trained to eat from the food cup over two days during daily 64-min shaping sessions that each included 16 deliveries of three 45 mg grain pellets (Bio-Serv, Frenchtown, NJ, USA). No additional stimuli were present during food cup shaping. Following food cup shaping, animals underwent 10 days of Pavlovian conditioning. Rats received 16 10-s presentations of the house light (CS+) followed by delivery of three sucrose pellets. Intertrial intervals varied from 3 to 5 min. The 10-s pre-CS period was used to calculate baseline responding.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Outline of experimental procedures.

Reinforcer Devaluation

Following conditioning, rats were matched for performance and divided into devalued and non-devalued groups. On days 1 and 3, non-devalued groups were given 10-min access to a ceramic bowl containing 100 sucrose pellets. On days 2 and 4, devalued groups were given 10-min access to a ceramic bowl containing 100 sucrose pellets, and immediately following this consumption period, they were given an (intraperitoneal) i.p. injection of 0.3 M LiCl (Hatfield et al., 1996; Pickens et al., 2003, 2005; Johnson et al., 2009). Non-devalued animals also received i.p. injections of 0.3 M LiCl on days 2 and 4 but this was not contiguously paired with sucrose pellets. In Figure 2B, pellet consumption is depicted as a function of trial. Thus, trial 1 indicates the first exposure to pellets, when no learning has taken place. Trial 2 reflects consumption after rats have experienced one pairing of either food and illness or the LiCl injection alone. Finally trial 3 reflects the final consumption test, which reflects learning from two pairings of food and illness, or illness alone.

Probe Test

Following reinforcer devaluation (but prior to the final consumption test), rats were given a probe test that was exactly the same as Pavlovian conditioning outlined above, except that this test was run under extinction conditions (i.e., at the end of CS+ presentation, no pellets were delivered).

Response Measures

We measured percent time spent in the food cup with an infrared photo beam positioned at the front of the food cup. For purposes of analysis, we examined the last 5 s of the CS+. Previous reports have demonstrated that responses are confined to this segment of the CS+ (Pickens et al., 2003). All of the reported behavior is conditioned responding and thus by definition occurs prior to food pellet delivery. Conditioned behavior was measured as the percent of time during the CS during which the photobeam was broken, indicating presence in the food cup. This would sum across multiple entries; historically this measure correlates closely with rate of responding and also latency to respond during the CS (Holland, 1977).

Conditioned Reinforcement

Conditioned reinforcement testing took place over two consecutive days, beginning two days after the probe test. Each day consisted of a single 30-min session. For each session, two levers were inserted into the behavioral box, one on either side of the food cup. Responding on one lever resulted in a 1-s presentation of the CS+ (the same CS used prior conditioning); responding on the other had no programmed consequences. Cues were presented on an FR2 schedule, and lever–cue associations were counterbalanced across animals.

Histology

After conditioned reinforcement testing, the rats were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane and perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA). Brains were extracted and kept in 4% PFA for 24 h and then transferred to a 30% sucrose solution for 24–48 h. Finally, 40-μm sections were made, mounted on slides, and Thionin stained to verify lesions.

Data Analysis

Data was collected using Graphic State 2 software from Coulbourn Instruments (Allentown, PA, USA). Then, the data was processed in Matlab to extract response rates and percent time spent in the food cup during CS presentations. Finally, the data was analyzed using Statistica, version 9. Post hoc comparisons were either planned comparisons or Tukey’s honestly significant difference (HSD) tests. Planned comparisons assume that one group (i.e., devalued) will be different than another (i.e., non-devalued) and calculate a suitable p value.

Results

Rats with sham (n = 20) or neurotoxic lesions of the nucleus accumbens shell (n = 16) or core (n = 20) were trained in a Pavlovian devaluation task. An experimental timeline is shown in Table 1. Histology after testing showed that 17 core lesioned rats and 16 shell lesioned rats had acceptable (>85%) bilateral damage to the respective region with limited damage to surrounding regions (Figure 1). The remaining rats were excluded from the analysis.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Neurotoxic lesion of the NA core (A) or shell (B). Traces showing minimum, maximum, and representative lesions.

Training began with 10 days of conditioning. As illustrated in Figure 2A, conditioned responding in the last 5 s of the CS+ increased in both groups across sessions. A three-factor ANOVA (lesion × session × cue/pre) revealed significant main effects of session (F9,387 = 58.63, p < 0.001) and cue/pre (F1,43 = 150.46, p < 0.0001), and a significant day × cue/pre interaction (F9,387 = 41.44, p < 0.001). In addition to these effects, there was also a significant overall effect of lesion (F2,43 = 4.05, p < 0.05). Notably, while post hoc testing revealed that sham rats responded significantly more during the pre-CS baseline period than core and shell lesioned rats (Tukey’s HSD, p < 0.05, df = 43), on the final day of conditioning there were no significant differences in responding during the CS + in any group. Moreover, shell and core lesioned rats responded similarly to sham rats in the 5 s after CS termination (when the US is delivered), indicating that all groups were consuming pellets in the food cup and that they had similar motivation to retrieve the food reward (Fs < 1, ps > 0.1).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Effect of NA core and shell lesions on Pavlovian conditioning (A), reinforcer devaluation (B), conditioned responding after devaluation (C), and conditioned reinforcement (D). Bars indicate SEMs; *p < 0.05.

Following Pavlovian conditioning, rats in each group were divided into devalued and non-devalued groups and underwent reinforcer devaluation. As illustrated in Figure 2B, pairing of the food with illness resulted in a decline in food consumption in both groups. A three-factor ANOVA (lesion × trial × pairing) revealed a significant main effect of trial (F2,80 = 82.07, p < 0.0001), lesion (F2,40 = 5.01, p < 0.05) and pairing (F1,40 = 323.18, p < 0.0001) and a significant trial × pairing interaction (F2,80 = 137.91, p < 0.0001). Tukey’s HSD post hoc tests revealed that on average, shell lesioned rats ate fewer pellets than core lesioned rats (Tukey’s HSD, p < 0.05, df = 40); however, paired rats in each group ate significantly fewer pellets than unpaired rats (p < 0.001).

After reinforcer devaluation (and prior to the final consumption test, which occurred after completion of all testing), all rats underwent a probe test in which they were exposed to the CS+ again under extinction conditions. As illustrated in Figure 2C, reinforcer devaluation had a significant effect on conditioned responding in sham but not core or shell lesioned rats. A two-factor ANOVA (lesion × pairing) revealed a significant lesion × pairing interaction (F1,29 = 4.83, p < 0.05). Planned comparisons demonstrated that sham-paired rats responded significantly less than sham-unpaired rats (p < 0.05), whereas both shell and core lesioned rats in the paired and unpaired groups responded similarly (p > 0.2).

Finally, the rats were tested to see if the CS+ would support the acquisition of a new instrumental response. As illustrated in Figure 2D, all groups learned to respond selectively on a lever that activated the CS+. A two-factor ANOVA (lesion × lever) demonstrated a significant main effect of lever (F1,30 = 13.14, p < 0.01) but no significant main effect nor any interaction with group (F < 1.2, p > 0.1). Planned comparisons revealed that rats in both the sham and the core lesioned groups responded significantly more on the lever which led to the CS+ (ps < 0.05); rats with shell lesions showed the same pattern, although the direct comparison did not reach significance (p = 0.098). Comparison of lever pressing in the current experiment to lever pressing by controls in previous studies in which we have tested conditioned reinforcement immediately following the final Pavlovian conditioning session (Burke et al., 2007, 2008) revealed no significant differences in lever pressing (Fs < 2.0, ps > 0.1). Thus the imposition of a probe test had no effect on conditioned reinforcement. This is consistent with reports that extinction does not impact underlying CS–US associations that form the basis of conditioned reinforcement (Rescorla, 1996).

Discussion

Here we show that NA lesions impair changes in Pavlovian conditioned responding after reinforcer devaluation, in a task that is sensitive to damage to upstream regions of the OFC and ABL. Neurotoxic lesions of either core or shell prior to behavioral testing had little effect on conditioning or the formation of a conditioned taste aversion; however rats with lesions of either NA core or shell failed to show the normal reduction in conditioned responding in the probe test after devaluation. This suggests that lesions of either region affect the ability of rats to utilize the cue to evoke a representation of the expected outcome; because rats were given pre-training lesions, this deficit could be in the formation of the original associations (i.e., in the ability of the CS to evoke a representation of the expected outcome including its value), updating of the associated outcome representation during devaluation (i.e., linking the cue-evoked representation to the new value of the US after devaluation), or in the subsequent use of this information in the probe test.

Importantly the deficit cannot be attributed to the inability of the CS to gain motivational salience in NA lesioned rats, because lesioned rats responded like control rats in conditioned reinforcement. Conditioned reinforcement conducted using similar procedures has been shown to be insensitive to reinforcer devaluation (Parkinson et al., 2005; Burke et al., 2007). Thus the normal performance here by lesioned rats is consistent with preservation of these devaluation-insensitive or general affective representations.

Similarly, while there was a non-specific effect of lesions on behavior, evident in attenuated baseline and conditioned responding during training and in the probe test, the lack of an effect of devaluation was not easily attributable to this general effect. Lesioned rats clearly conditioned to the cue, and conditioned responding to the cue in the probe test, though reduced relative to controls, was roughly double baseline responding during conditioning. Thus there was not a floor to prevent an effect of devaluation on responding in lesioned rats. Nevertheless there was no effect of devaluation on responding of rats in either lesioned group; in both cases, lesioned rats that had received food–illness pairings (and stopped eating the food) responded the same as lesioned rats that had received food and illness on alternate days (and continued eating the food). While it has been suggested to us that this pattern of results could reflect a generalized effect of illness on responding, this seems unlikely given that illness was experienced days removed from training and in the rats’ home cages. Further rats showed no lingering effects of illness, and those that received food and illness on alternate days continued to consume the food pellets like controls. Thus we would suggest the much more parsimonious explanation of these data, which is that lesions of NA – either core or shell – had two somewhat independent effects: a generalized effect on behavior and a more specific impairment in outcome-guided responding, due to effects on acquisition, updating or the use of the specific CS–US association.

Although the single-reinforcer Pavlovian reinforcer devaluation paradigm used here has been extensively applied to understand the functions of OFC and ABL, to the best of our knowledge it has not been applied previously to study NA. Instead prior studies using devaluation to explore NA function have typically focused on tasks with significant instrumental components; these studies have led to conflicting evidence (Corbit et al., 2001; de Borchgrave et al., 2002). One notable exception to this is a recent study examining the effects of dopamine depletion in NA core on changes in Pavlovian and instrumental responding after satiation (Lex and Hauber, 2010). Rats were trained that different responses or cues predicted either a food or sucrose reinforcer. Subsequently rats were satiated on one reinforcer prior to an extinction test, similar to the probe test employed here. Dopamine depletion within NA core had no effect on instrumental behavior, but it generally reduced Pavlovian responding and also abolished the normal affect of devaluation induced by satiation. Our results are consistent with this prior report and extend it to show that both core and shell are necessary for normal changes in conditioned responding following reinforcer devaluation, even if training involves only a single reinforcer and devaluation is induced by pairing with illness.

The involvement of NA in outcome-guided responding is consistent with anatomical studies showing that NA receives input from the region we would define as rat OFC (Ongur and Price, 2000; Schoenbaum et al., 2002) – particularly the most lateral agranular insular regions – and ABL (McDonald, 1991; Berendse et al., 1992; Brog et al., 1993; Voorn et al., 2004; Schilman et al., 2008). Experiments using pre- and post-training lesions of these areas in the same behavioral paradigm applied here demonstrate that OFC is critical for using outcome–expectancies to guide responding (Gallagher et al., 1999; Pickens et al., 2003, 2005). ABL also seems to be critically involved in this setting, particularly for the acquisition of cue–outcome associations in the present task (Hatfield et al., 1996). Pre-training ABL lesions abolish the devaluation effect while rats with post-training lesions do not (Pickens et al., 2003), unless multiple reinforcers are used (Wellmann et al., 2005; Johnson et al., 2009). Based on these results, it is tempting to speculate that these upstream regions interact with NA to drive changes in Pavlovian responding after reinforcer devaluation.

Notably this hypothesis was addressed recently in a study of reward-based decision-making in primates. In this study (Izquierdo and Murray, 2010), monkeys were allowed to make choices between visual objects that had been paired with different food rewards. Choices were made before and after the monkeys were fed to satiety on one of the foods. Prior work using this task has shown that normal monkeys bias their choices away from objects associated with the food devalued by satiation, whereas monkeys with damage to the orbitofrontal–amygdalar circuit show no effect of devaluation on choice behavior (Malkova et al., 1997; Baxter and Murray, 2000; Izquierdo and Murray, 2004; Izquierdo et al., 2004). In this follow-up study, monkeys that had exhibited deficits after ipsilateral damage to the orbitofrontal–amygdalar circuit 26 months earlier (Izquierdo and Murray, 2004) were retested before and after lesions of contralateral NA (both core and shell, thereby disconnecting the orbitofrontal–amygdalar regions from NA). These monkeys, who had recovered function during the time between the two studies, had no difficulty shifting their choice performance away from the objects paired with the devalued food after removal of contralateral NA. These results are contrary to the hypothesis that the role of OFC and amygdala in devaluation involves input to NA.

The apparent discrepancy between this study and the implications of the current results is striking and may be due to a number of factors. One possibility is the use of multiple reinforcers, though this seems unlikely since upstream regions are, if anything, more important when representations become more complex (Wellmann et al., 2005; Johnson et al., 2009), and the study mentioned earlier using 6-OHDA lesions of accumbens core found results comparable to ours using two different reinforcers (Lex and Hauber, 2010). A second possibility is the presence of both instrumental contingencies and Pavlovian associations in the choice task (Izquierdo and Murray, 2010); this might make possible more complex strategies for solving the devaluation problem not available to rats in the current study. A third, related possibility is that the recovery of function shown by the monkeys between the initial study, when the ipsilateral orbital and amygdalar lesions were made (Izquierdo and Murray, 2004), and the later study when NA was removed (Izquierdo and Murray, 2010) may have allowed them to sustain performance in the face of an insult that would have impaired devaluation in a naïve animal. For example, these monkeys may have come to rely more heavily on mediodorsal thalamic circuits; mediodorsal thalamus is implicated in reinforcer devaluation (Mitchell et al., 2007), and contralateral damage to this region in monkeys with prior orbitofrontal–amygdalar lesions resulted in a return to impaired performance (Izquierdo and Murray, 2010).

Lastly it may be that lesions of both core and shell of NA are less effective at impairing devaluation than partial damage to one or the other subdivision. This might occur if the impairment caused by damage to one region is mediated by abnormal processing in the remaining area. We have found something analogous to this in studying the role of amygdala in mediating OFC-dependent reversal deficits, where removal of basolateral amygdala actually mitigated impairment of reversal learning caused by OFC damage (Stalnaker et al., 2007). Notably there is some basis for such speculation; Balleine and colleagues have reported that lesions of NA restricted to the core impair changes in instrumental responding after reinforcer devaluation (Corbit et al., 2001), whereas lesions that encompass both core and shell have no effect (de Borchgrave et al., 2002).

Of course, even if one accepts the involvement of NA in Pavlovian reinforcer devaluation, the exact nature of that involvement remains to be determined using more detailed parametric manipulations. For example, here we made lesions prior to any training and used only a single outcome; prior work in ABL and OFC have shown that lesions (or inactivation) later in training may have effects that differ from pre-training lesions (Pickens et al., 2003; Wellmann et al., 2005; Johnson et al., 2009). Likewise, the use of multiple outcomes can reveal dissociations not evident using the procedures applied here (Pickens et al., 2003; Wellmann et al., 2005; Johnson et al., 2009). Based on its anatomical position and prior evidence from second-order conditioning work (Setlow et al., 2002a,b), it seems likely that NA functions to integrate prefrontal and amygdalar input. This would be consistent with long-standing notions that NA functions as the limbic motor interface (Mogenson et al., 1980) and with more recent proposals that ventral striatum generally functions as a critical final processing station for computing expected value in a common currency (Joel et al., 2002).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

This work was supported by grants from NIDA to Geoffrey Schoenbaum.

References

Balleine, B. W., and Corbit, L. H. (2005). Double dissociation of nucleus accumbens core and shell on the general and outcome-specific forms of pavlovian-instrumental transfer. Soc. Neurosci. Abstracts 71.16.

Baxter, M. G., and Murray, E. A. (2000). “Reinterpreting the behavioral effects of amygdala lesions in nonhuman primates,” in The Amygdala: A Functional Analysis, ed. J. P. Aggleton (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 545–568.

Berendse, H. W., Galis-de Graaf, Y., and Groenewegen, H. J. (1992). Topographical organization and relationship with ventral striatal compartments of prefrontal corticostriatal projections in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 316, 314–347.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brog, J. S., Salyapongse, A., Deutch, A. Y., and Zahm, D. S. (1993). The patterns of afferent innervation of the core and shell in the “accumbens” part of the rat ventral striatum: immunohistochemical detection of retrogradely transported fluoro-gold. J. Comp. Neurol. 338, 255–278.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burke, K. A., Franz, T. M., Miller, D. N., and Schoenbaum, G. (2007). Conditioned reinforcement can be mediated by either outcome-specific or general affective representations. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 1, 2. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07/002.2007.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burke, K. A., Franz, T. M., Miller, D. N., and Schoenbaum, G. (2008). The role of orbitofrontal cortex in the pursuit of happiness and more specific rewards. Nature 454, 340–344.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cardinal, R. N., Parkinson, J. A., Hall, G., and Everitt, B. J. (2002a). Emotion and motivation: the role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 321–352.

CrossRef Full Text

Cardinal, R. N., Parkinson, J. A., Lachenal, G., Halkerston, K. M., Rudarakanchana, N., Hall, J., Morrison, C. H., Howes, S. R., Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (2002b). Effects of selective excitotoxic lesions of the nucleus accumbens core, anterior cingulate cortex, and the central nucleus of the amygdala on autoshaping performance in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 116, 553–567.

CrossRef Full Text

Corbit, L. H., Muir, J. L., and Balleine, B. W. (2001). The role of the nucleus accumbens in instrumental conditioning: evidence of a functional dissociation between accumbens core and shell. J. Neurosci. 21, 3251–3260.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

de Borchgrave, R., Rawlins, J. N. P., Dickinson, A., and Balleine, B. W. (2002). Effects of cytotoxic nucleus accumbens lesions on instrumental conditioning in rats. Exp. Brain Res. 144, 50–68.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gallagher, M., McMahan, R. W., and Schoenbaum, G. (1999). Orbitofrontal cortex and representation of incentive value in associative learning. J. Neurosci. 19, 6610–6614.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

German, P. W., and Fields, H. L. (2007). Rat nucleus accumbens neurons persistently encode locations associated with morphine reward. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 2094–2106.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gottfried, J. A., O’Doherty, J., and Dolan, R. J. (2003). Encoding predictive reward value in human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Science 301, 1104–1107.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hall, J., Parkinson, J. A., Connor, T. M., Dickinson, A., and Everitt, B. J. (2001). Involvement of the central nucleus of the amygdala and nucleus accumbens core in mediating Pavlovian influences on instrumental behavior. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 1984–1992.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hatfield, T., Han, J. S., Conley, M., Gallagher, M., and Holland, P. (1996). Neurotoxic lesions of basolateral, but not central, amygdala interfere with Pavlovian second-order conditioning and reinforcer devaluation effects. J. Neurosci. 16, 5256–5265.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Holland, P. C. (1977). Conditioned stimulus as a determinant of the form of Pavlovian conditioned response. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Proc. 3, 77–104.

CrossRef Full Text

Hollander, J. A., and Carelli, R. M. (2007). Cocaine-associated stimuli increase cocaine seeking and activate accumbens core neurons after abstinence. J. Neurosci. 27, 3535–3539.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ito, R., Robbins, T. W., Pennartz, C. M., and Everitt, B. J. (2008). Functional interaction between the hippocampus and nucleus accumbens shell is necessary for the acquisition of appetitive spatial context conditioning. J. Neurosci. 28, 6950–6959.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Izquierdo, A., and Murray, E. A. (2010). Functional interaction of medial mediodorsal thalamic nucleus but not nucleus accumbens with amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex is essential for adaptive response selection after reinforcer devaluation. J. Neurosci. 30, 661–669.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Izquierdo, A. D., and Murray, E. A. (2004). Combined unilateral lesions of the amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex impair affective processing in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 2033–2039.

CrossRef Full Text

Izquierdo, A. D., Suda, R. K., and Murray, E. A. (2004). Bilateral orbital prefrontal cortex lesions in rhesus monkeys disrupt choices guided by both reward value and reward contingency. J. Neurosci. 24, 7540–7548.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Joel, D., Niv, Y., and Ruppin, E. (2002). Actor-critic models of the basal ganglia: New anatomical and computational perspectives. Neural Netw. 15, 535–547.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Johnson, A. W., Gallagher, M., and Holland, P. C. (2009). The basolateral amygdala is critical to the expression of Pavlovian and instrumental outcome-specific reinforcer devaluation effects. J. Neurosci. 29, 696–704.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kimchi, E. Y., and Laubach, M. (2009). Dynamic encoding of action selection by the medial striatum. J. Neurosci. 29, 3148–3159.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lex, B., and Hauber, W. (2010). The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in outcome encoding in instrumental and Pavlovian conditioning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 93, 283–290.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Machado, C. J., and Bachevalier, J. (2007). The effects of selective amygdala, orbital frontal cortex or hippocampal formation lesions on reward assessment in nonhuman primates. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 2885–2904.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Malkova, L., Gaffan, D., and Murray, E. A. (1997). Excitotoxic lesions of the amygdala fail to produce impairment in visual learning for auditory secondary reinforcement but interfere with reinforcer devaluation effects in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 17, 6011–6020.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

McDonald, A. J. (1991). Organization of the amygdaloid projections to the prefrontal cortex and associated striatum in the rat. Neuroscience 44, 1–14.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mitchell, A. S., Browning, P. G., and Baxter, M. G. (2007). Neurotoxic lesions of the medial mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus disrupt reinforcer devaluation effects in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 27, 11289–11295.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mogenson, G. J., Jones, D. L., and Yim, C. Y. (1980). From motivation to action: functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Prog. Neurobiol. 14, 69–97.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nicola, S. M., Yun, I. A., Wakabayashi, K. T., and Fields, H. L. (2004). Cue-evoked firing of nucleus accumbens neurons encodes motivational significance during a discriminative task. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 1840–1865.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ongur, D., and Price, J. L. (2000). The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb. Cortex 10, 206–219.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parkinson, J. A., Olmstead, M. C., Burns, L. H., Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (1999a). Dissociation of effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on appetitive Pavlovian approach behavior and the potentiation of conditioned reinforcement and locomotor activity by d-amphetamine. J. Neurosci. 19, 2401–2411.

Parkinson, J. A., Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (1999b). Selective excitotoxic lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell differentially effect aversive Pavlovian conditioning to discrete and contextual cues. Psychobiology 27, 256–266.

Parkinson, J. A., Roberts, A. C., Everitt, B. J., and Di Ciano, P. (2005). Acquisition of instrumental conditioned reinforcement is resistant to the devaluation of the unconditioned stimulus. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 58, 19–30.

CrossRef Full Text

Parkinson, J. A., Willoughby, P. J., Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (2000). Disconnection of the anterior cingulate cortex and nucleus accumbens core impairs Pavlovian approach behavior: further evidence for limbic cortical - ventral striatopallidal systems. Behav. Neurosci. 114, 42–63.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Peoples, L. L., Lynch, K. G., Lesnock, J., and Gangadhar, N. (2004). Accumbal neural responses during the initiation and maintenance of intravenous cocaine self-administration. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 314–323.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pickens, C. L., Saddoris, M. P., Gallagher, M., and Holland, P. C. (2005). Orbitofrontal lesions impair use of cue-outcome associations in a devaluation task. Behav. Neurosci. 119, 317–322.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pickens, C. L., Setlow, B., Saddoris, M. P., Gallagher, M., Holland, P. C., and Schoenbaum, G. (2003). Different roles for orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in a reinforcer devaluation task. J. Neurosci. 23, 11078–11084.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rescorla, R. A. (1996). Preservation of pavlovian associations through extinction. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 49B, 245–258.

Roesch, M. R., Singh, T., Brown, P. L., Mullins, S. E., and Schoenbaum, G. (2009). Ventral striatal neurons encode the value of the chosen action in rats deciding between differently delayed or sized rewards. J. Neurosci. 29, 13365–13376.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schilman, E. A., Uylings, H. B. M., Galis-de Graaf, Y., Joel, D., and Groenewegen, H. J. (2008). The orbital cortex in rats topographically projects to central parts of the caudate-putamen complex. Neurosci. Lett. 432, 40–45.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schoenbaum, G., Setlow, B., and Gallagher, M. (2002). “Orbitofrontal cortex: modeling prefrontal function in rats,” in The Neuropsychology of Memory, eds L. Squire and D. Schacter (New York: Guilford Press), 463–477.

Setlow, B., Gallagher, M., and Holland, P. (2002a). The basolateral complex of the amygdala is necessary for acquisition but not expression of CS motivational value in appetitive Pavlovian second-order conditioning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 1841–1853.

CrossRef Full Text

Setlow, B., Holland, P. C., and Gallagher, M. (2002b). Disconnection of the basolateral amygdala complex and nucleus accumbens impairs appetitive Pavlovian second-order conditioned responses. Behav. Neurosci. 116, 267–275.

CrossRef Full Text

Setlow, B., Schoenbaum, G., and Gallagher, M. (2003). Neural encoding in ventral striatum during olfactory discrimination learning. Neuron 38, 625–636.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stalnaker, T. A., Franz, T. M., Singh, T., and Schoenbaum, G. (2007). Basolateral amygdala lesions abolish orbitofrontal-dependent reversal impairments. Neuron 54, 51–58.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van der Meer, M. A., and Redish, A. D. (2009). Covert expectation of reward in rat ventral striatum at decision points. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 3, 1. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.001.2009.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Voorn, P., Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J., Groenewegen, H. J., Robbins, T. W., and Pennartz, C. M. A. (2004). Putting a spin on the dorsal-ventral divide of the striatum. Trends Neurosci. 27, 468–474.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wellmann, L. L., Gale, K., and Malkova, L. (2005). GABAA-mediated inhibition of basolateral amygdala blocks reward devaluation in macaques. J. Neurosci. 25, 4577–4586.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: nucleus accumbens, Pavlovian conditioning, reinforcement, reward, rat, motivation

Citation: Singh T, McDannald MA, Haney RZ and Cerri DH and Schoenbaum G (2010) Nucleus accumbens core and shell are necessary for reinforcer devaluation effects on Pavlovian conditioned responding. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 4:126. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2010.00126

Received: 31 August 2010; Paper pending published: 24 September 2010;
Accepted: 01 October 2010; Published online: 20 October 2010

Edited by:

Alan Carleton, University of Geneva, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Mark Laubach, The John B. Pierce Laboratory, USA
Patricia H Janak, University of California at San Francisco, USA

Copyright: © 2010 Singh, McDannald, Haney, Cerri and Schoenbaum. This is an open-access article subject to an exclusive license agreement between the authors and the Frontiers Research Foundation, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original authors and source are credited.

*Correspondence: Geoffrey Schoenbaum, School of Medicine, Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, University of Maryland, 20 Penn Street, HSF-2 S251, Baltimore, MD 21201, USA. e-mail: schoenbg@schoenbaumlab.org

Download