Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Integr. Neurosci., 31 October 2013
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2013.00075

Executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease and timing deficits

  • 1Department of Neurology, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, IA, USA
  • 2Center for Mathematics, Computation and Cognition, Universidade Federal do ABC, Santo André, Brazil
  • 3Aging Mind and Brain Initiative, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, IA, USA

Patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) have deficits in perceptual timing, or the perception and estimation of time. PD patients can also have cognitive symptoms, including deficits in executive functions such as working memory, planning, and visuospatial attention. Here, we discuss how PD-related cognitive symptoms contribute to timing deficits. Timing is influenced by signaling of the neurotransmitter dopamine in the striatum. Timing also involves the frontal cortex, which is dysfunctional in PD. Frontal cortex impairments in PD may influence memory subsystems as well as decision processes during timing tasks. These data suggest that timing may be a type of executive function. As such, timing can be used to study the neural circuitry of cognitive symptoms of PD as they can be studied in animal models. Performance of timing tasks also maybe a useful clinical biomarker of frontal as well as striatal dysfunction in PD.

Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disease where midbrain dopamine neurons inexorably die. Cardinal manifestations of PD include bradykinesia, rigidity, resting tremor, and postural instability. PD patients also have non-motor manifestations such as constipation, salivation, sleep disorders, mood disorders, and cognitive deficits (Chaudhuri and Schapira, 2009; Zesiewicz et al., 2010). Of these, executive dysfunction, one major domain of cognitive deficits in PD (Gotham et al., 1988; Cooper et al., 1991; Aarsland et al., 2010), is associated with considerable morbidity (Williams-Gray et al., 2007) and predicts future mortality (Santangelo et al., 2007; Forsaa et al., 2010).

Executive dysfunction affects roughly 30% of PD patients (Williams-Gray et al., 2009; Aarsland and Kurz, 2010). Deficits in executive tasks may define a disease trajectory, as cognitive symptoms are prognosticators of dementia onset (Mahieux et al., 1998; Levy et al., 2002a,b; Janvin et al., 2005). Such deficits can occur early in the disease (Foltynie et al., 2004; Aarsland et al., 2009) and involve impaired coordination of a range of cognitive processes required to achieve complex, goal-oriented, and novel cognitive operations (Elliott, 2003; Jurado and Rosselli, 2007). Executive processes include working memory, planning, inhibition, attention, and decreased speed of processing (Uc et al., 2005). These processes typically involve the frontal lobe (Fuster, 2008). These impairments are revealed using classic tests of executive function (Van Spaendonck et al., 1996) including verbal fluency, abstract reasoning, picture completion, Stroop performance (Aarsland et al., 2011), and performance on the Tower-of-London task (Foltynie et al., 2004). Executive functions can include inhibitory control (Stuss et al., 2005; Picton et al., 2006) which may be impaired in PD (Wylie et al., 2010; Mirabella et al., 2013). Other executive processes such as action selection can be impaired in PD and correlate with dysfunctional frontostriatal networks (Hughes et al., 2013). Here, we are particularly interested in working memory aspects of executive function, as these appear to be involved in timing (Gibbon et al., 1984) and impaired in PD patients (Malapani et al., 1998).

Importantly, PD-related executive dysfunction is not directly correlated with motor dysfunction (Van Spaendonck et al., 1996), although it has been linked with gait-disturbance (Wylie et al., 2012). Dopaminergic therapy does not reliably improve executive dysfunction in high-functioning (Pascual-Sedano et al., 2008) or moderate PD patients (Morrison et al., 2004) and potentially can have detrimental effects (Cools et al., 2001; Cools and D’Esposito, 2011).

In addition to deficits in executive dysfunction, patients with PD consistently have impaired timing (e.g., Artieda et al., 1992; O’Boyle et al., 1996; Malapani et al., 1998, 2002; Merchant et al., 2008; Jahanshahi et al., 2010; Jones et al., 2011). While some consider timing an executive function (Fuster, 2008), it is not universally considered as such (Elliott, 2003; Jurado and Rosselli, 2007). Executive functions classically involve goal-directed behavior such as planning, flexibility, problem solving, and attentional control (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974; Norman and Shallice, 1986; Lezak et al., 2004) rather than timing. In this review, we discuss evidence that (1) timing tasks involve executive processing in the frontal cortex, and (2) frontal dysfunction may contribute to timing deficits in PD patients.

Perceptual Timing

Timing, i.e., the perception and estimation of time from seconds to minutes, is central in guiding a range of behaviors, from foraging and decision making to goal-directed behavior (Church, 1984). Perceptual timing at this scale can be measured using a variety of interval timing tasks. These tasks require subjects to make responses at precise times indicating their internal and subjective estimates of time. Interval timing is conserved across a wide range of species (Buhusi and Meck, 2005), and is distinct from other measures such as motor timing, implicit timing, ordinal timing, and rhythmic timing.

In order to understand perceptual timing, it is useful to construct a detailed model which accounts both for timing behavior and errors in timing. Understanding the neural basis of timing models can be used to illuminate the mechanism of brain diseases which produce timing errors. One prominent model of timing is the scalar timing theory or scalar expectancy theory, also referred to as the pacemaker–accumulator model of timing (Gibbon et al., 1984). This model assumes the existence of a pacemaker that emits pulses at a certain rate, an accumulator that stores these pulses, and a decision module that constantly compares the accumulated pulses with an example sampled from memory. According to this model (Figure 1), a discriminative signal triggers the accumulation of the pulses, which are regularly compared with a randomly sampled number of pulses from the reference memory related to that discriminative signal. When the comparison crosses a threshold, a response is triggered (Church, 1997, 2003). Each component in this model produces variability, and at least of one these must be scalar, producing linearly increasing variance (Gibbon et al., 1984). Of note, there are alternative interval timing theories that, arguing for a lack of biological validity, do not assume the existence of a pacemaker/accumulator-type internal clock (Buhusi and Oprisan, 2013; Laje and Buonomano, 2013); but see Simen et al. (2011) for a clock-like proposal of a neural model to estimate the passage of time). For instance, in a striatal beat–frequency model, striatal medium-spiny neurons serve as timers by detecting and integrating oscillatory states overtime (Matell et al., 2003; Meck et al., 2008; Allman and Meck, 2012). Of these, the scalar timing theory remains the most influential.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Scalar timing theory of interval timing based on that proposed by Church (1984). In this model, an external signal such as a sensory cue starts an internal clock, which compares the passage of time with a criterion stored in working memory. When the criterion approaches the actual clock time, a timed response is initiated. Extensive evidence has suggested that the striatum is involved in all aspects of interval timing (Merchant et al., 2013). Both dorsal and ventral striatum receive dopaminergic input from the substantia nigra via the nigrostriatal pathway, and this pathway can influence timing (Meck, 2006). However, areas in the medial frontal cortex may also be involved (Coull et al., 2011), and likely influence memory as well as decision processes (Mehanna and Jankovic, 2013). These areas receive dopaminergic input (in red) from mesocortical dopamine projections originating from the ventral tegmental area. These mesocortical dopamine projections may contribute to perceptual timing deficits in PD patients.

A link between interval timing and executive function is that classic tasks loading executive function impair interval timing (Brown, 2006). For instance, remembering items interfered with subjects’ ability to reproduce temporal epochs (Fortin and Rousseau, 1998). When children were asked to both estimate time and perform other executive processes, they were impaired (Rattat, 2010). These type impairments in dual-task experiments suggest that increased dual-task allocation to executive processing resulted in a tradeoff with perceptual timing performance (Brown et al., 2012). Interference seemed to be specifically constrained to the executive process of updating working memory buffers (Ogden et al., 2011). Taken together, these studies provide evidence that interval timing requires executive resources.

Perceptual timing has been reliably linked with dopamine signaling in the striatum (Buhusi and Meck, 2005; Jones and Jahanshahi, 2013). Interval timing can activate medial frontal regions (Hinton et al., 2004; Jahanshahi et al., 2010), including supplementary and pre-supplementary areas (Mita et al., 2009; Coull et al., 2011, 2012; Schneider and Ghose, 2012). Single neurons in medial prefrontal cortex are modulated by time (Niki and Watanabe, 1979; Meck et al., 2008; Genovesio et al., 2009; Narayanan and Laubach, 2009). Rodent studies using pharmacological and optogenetic methods have found that prefrontal D1 dopamine signaling is required for interval timing (Narayanan et al., 2012). A recent temporal discrimination task described that neuronal ensembles in rodent medial frontal cortex were modulated by the passage of time (Kim et al., 2013). These results suggest that interval timing and executive functions share similar neural networks.

Previous reviews have discussed the neuroanatomy of timing in great detail (Matell et al., 2003; Buhusi and Meck, 2005; Meck, 2006; Coull and Nobre, 2008; Coull et al., 2011). A central structure in interval timing is the striatum (Harrington et al., 1998; Jones and Jahanshahi, 2013), likely favoring dorsal striatum (Kurti and Matell, 2011). However, timing can involve diverse and distributed brain networks (Meck et al., 2008; Coull et al., 2011). In Figure 1, we have illustrated potential brain networks that underlie timing-related processes suggested by the scalar timing theory. For instance, it is clear that the striatum influences all aspects of perceptual timing, from clock functions to decision and memory mechanisms (Husárová et al., 2011, 2013; Coull et al., 2012; Merchant et al., 2013). While temporal memory is certainly likely to involve frontal networks (Shimamura et al., 1990), recent reviews noted that medial frontal and medial premotor networks were consistently activated by perceptual timing tasks (Coull and Nobre, 2008; Coull et al., 2011). These areas might provide “context” (Merchant et al., 2013) and serve as executive control processes. Many of the frontal areas consistently activated include motor, supplementary motor, and cingulate cortex, which we posit are involved in the response decisions. Systematic recording and disruption experiments from this network in animal models will clarify the role of frontal cortex in memory vs. decision functions during interval timing.

Dopamine and Interval Timing

The neurotransmitter dopamine is released from projections originating in the midbrain. Manipulations of dopaminergic signaling profoundly influence interval timing, leading to the hypothesis that dopamine influences internal pacemaker, or “clock,” activity (Maricq and Church, 1983; Buhusi and Meck, 2005, 2009; Lake and Meck, 2013). For instance, amphetamine, which increases concentrations of dopamine at the synaptic cleft (Maricq and Church, 1983; Zetterström et al., 1983) advances the start of responding during interval timing (Taylor et al., 2007), whereas antagonists of D2 type dopamine receptors typically slow timing (Drew et al., 2003; Lake and Meck, 2013). Animal work has indicated that manipulations of both nigrostriatal (Drew et al., 2003, 2007; Meck, 2006) and mesocortical dopamine signaling originating from the ventral tegmental area (Narayanan et al., 2012) can also strongly influence interval timing.

In PD, midbrain projection neurons containing dopamine die (Damier et al., 1999). PD patients are slow to initiate and execute movements (Evarts et al., 1981) and they also have impairments in interval timing. Early studies of PD patients off medication revealed a slowing of timing performance, which correlated with disease severity (Artieda et al., 1992; Pastor et al., 1992), and was consistent with bradykinesia observed with disease progression. Timing deficits were normalized by levodopa, and seem to be constrained to intervals on the scale of seconds (Koch et al., 2008). Notably, these patients had large variability in their estimates of time that was confirmed by a study in PD patients estimating two distinct intervals (Malapani et al., 1998). In this study, PD patients overestimated the short interval and underestimated the long interval; when they estimated the long interval only, they were slower. This effect, dubbed the “migration effect,” has been reliably found in PD patients (Koch et al., 2005). Timing deficits can be rescued by dopaminergic therapy (Malapani et al., 1998), suggesting that beyond clock speed, mnemonic representation for time is impaired in PD and that dopamine influences memory as well as clock subsystems.

Parkinson’s disease patients have timing impairments in related interval timing tasks. For instance, in a duration production and reproduction task with concurrent attentional loads, PD patients were more variable (Perbal et al., 2005). Both levodopa and stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus improved time reproduction (Torta et al., 2010). PD patients can also have deficits in processing temporal information at sub-second time scales (Harrington et al., 1998; Riesen and Schnider, 2001), but there is significant variability in timing at this scale (Merchant et al., 2008; Jones et al., 2011; Claassen et al., 2013). The consistency of PD timing deficits at seconds and inconsistency below milliseconds may be related to the memory and attentional load of longer time intervals (Koch et al., 2008). PD patients consistently tend to have slower, more variable timing over a scale of seconds, which implies that the loss of dopamine influences not only clock deficits in PD patients, but memory and decision processes as well (Mehanna and Jankovic, 2013; Figure 1).

Notably, in some studies levodopa normalizes timing (Malapani et al., 1998; Koch et al., 2008) but not other executive functions such as memory, reasoning, or flexible learning (Gotham et al., 1988; Cools et al., 2001; Pascual-Sedano et al., 2008). Dopamine signaling may be complex with non-linearities (Cools and D’Esposito, 2011). Levodopa’s effects may also be influenced by disease severity (Cools et al., 2001). Additionally, PD patients can be heterogeneous (Merchant et al., 2008; Aarsland et al., 2011) with respect to the disease.

Another possibility includes a role for non-dopaminergic signaling in PD-related cognitive dysfunction. Cortical and striatal areas are influenced by other broad neurotransmitter projection systems that are also impaired in PD, such as cholinergic projections originating from basal forebrain (Bigl et al., 1982). This area can be affected in PD patients (Arendt et al., 1983; Whitehouse et al., 1983; Fujita et al., 2006). Furthermore, clinical trials have demonstrated that cholinesterase inhibitor improves cognitive performance for PD patients with mild–moderate dementia (Emre et al., 2004; Poewe et al., 2006). Manipulations of cholinergic signaling (Meck and Church, 1987) and the basal forebrain (Olton et al., 1988) can strongly influence temporal memories. It is unclear if timing deficits are also improved in these patients.

Although PD also involves degeneration of many other brainstem projection nuclei (Scatton et al., 1983; Del Tredici et al., 2002; Jellinger, 2011) such as serotonin and norepinephrine (McCormick et al., 1985), the role of these systems in executive dysfunction and perceptual timing deficits is unclear. However, because levodopa does not reliably treat PD-related cognitive symptoms (Cools et al., 2001, 2002; Morrison et al., 2004; Pascual-Sedano et al., 2008), other non-dopaminergic mechanisms may be important for understanding cognition in PD. Future studies involving detailed correlation of neuropathology with clinical phenotypes as well as specific manipulations of non-dopaminergic systems in animal models will clarify this issue.

PD and Frontal Cortex

Deficits in interval timing have been uniformly interpreted as arising from deficits in nigrostriatal dopamine depletion affecting basal ganglia circuitry (Buhusi and Meck, 2005; Coull et al., 2011). However, we note that PD can profoundly influence the function of frontal cortex. The source of cortical dopamine projections in the medial nigral and ventral tegmental midbrain (Williams and Goldman-Rakic, 1998) degenerate in PD (Javoy-Agid and Agid, 1980; Javoy-Agid et al., 1981; Dymecki et al., 1996). These studies suggest that ventral tegmental dopamine neurons degenerate to a lesser extent than the nearby substantia nigra, which primarily projects to the striatum. To date, the functional consequence of ventral tegmental degeneration is unclear (Jellinger, 1999), although there is debate about the involvement of ventral tegmental area in PD (Fearnley and Lees, 1991) likely because there is much overlap between these areas.

Midbrain dopamine neurons encode errors in temporal predictions of reward (Hollerman and Schultz, 1998; Fiorillo et al., 2008; Kobayashi and Schultz, 2008) and can precisely encode temporal information (Lapish et al., 2007). PD involves a dysfunction and loss of these neurons, which could lead to abnormalities in temporal processing in synaptic projection targets in the cortex and striatum. Without this input, these areas can be temporally desynchronized. Depletion of dopamine in healthy volunteers impairs timing (Coull et al., 2012), while amphetamine releases synaptic dopamine and speeds up timing (Taylor et al., 2007). Animal models which record from both dopamine and downstream neurons or dynamically manipulate dopamine neurons while recording in downstream areas will identify the precise role that dopamine release has on temporal encoding in cortex and striatum.

Metabolic imaging demonstrates that PD patients have alterations in prefrontal dopamine signaling (Dubois and Pillon, 1995, 1997) which has been confirmed by positron emission tomography (PET with 18F-DOPA; Rakshi et al., 1999; Kaasinen et al., 2001). These studies have reported that in early PD, prefrontal dopamine uptake has been reported to be increased, presumably due to compensatory mechanisms. Dopamine influences executive function via mesocortical projections originating from the ventral tegmental area (Arnsten and Li, 2005; Cools and D’Esposito, 2011). Prefrontal dopamine release measured by microdialysis is correlated with working memory performance (Phillips et al., 2004). Blocking prefrontal D1 receptors degrades the ability of prefrontal neurons to represent items in working memory (Williams and Goldman-Rakic, 1995; Goldman-Rakic et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2004). In rodent models, prefrontal D1 receptors are associated specifically with interval timing (Narayanan et al., 2012) as well as memory (Seamans et al., 1998; Floresco and Phillips, 2001). In addition, prefrontal dopamine signaling has been linked with a variety of cognitive behaviors, such as reasoning (Takahashi et al., 2012), attentional set-shifting (Floresco et al., 2006), reversal learning (Kehagia et al., 2010), impulsivity (Loos et al., 2010), and decision-making (Floresco, 2013). Dopamine release in prefrontal cortex can modulate network state (Seamans and Yang, 2004). These studies suggest that abnormalities in prefrontal dopamine signaling in PD could impair executive processing (Miller and Cohen, 2001).

Cognitive symptoms of PD appear can be linked with dysfunction in prefrontal networks. Deactivation in medial prefrontal cortex is associated with cognitive dysfunction in PD in metabolic imaging studies (Huang et al., 2007). Brain imaging studies in PD patients have found aberrant prefrontal networks. For example, controls activated medial frontal networks more reliably than PD patients during a task of random numbers generation (Dirnberger et al., 2005). Levodopa restored prefrontal blood flow as measured by PET during performance on a Tower-of-London task (Cools et al., 2002). Decreased performance on attentional set-shifting by PD patients is correlated with less prefrontal metabolic activity (Sawada et al., 2012). This line of research suggests that there is executive dysfunction in PD that may involve the frontal cortex during timing tasks.

Several studies directly engage this question (Table 1). In a study of paced-finger tapping, less medial premotor as well as sensorimotor and cerebellar activity was observed in PD patients relative to controls (Elsinger et al., 2003). While on and off levodopa, Harrington et al. (2011) asked PD patients to perform a perceptual timing task, comparing two intervals of time while collecting functional magnetic resonance images (fMRI). This study found temporal impairments throughout frontostriatal and cerebellar networks, and found abnormal activations in medial frontal and parietal areas, which are typically associated with executive processes such as working memory. Activity in this network was abnormal while PD patients encoded time and absent during the decision phase; furthermore, in medial frontal areas such as the cingulate cortex, different temporal profiles were observed in concert with temporal alterations in the striatum. A study modeling effective connectivity found that in controls, medial supplementary motor areas had excitatory coupling with subcortical areas, while PD patients did not (Husárová et al., 2013). Some studies find no changes in this circuit in PD patients during timing tasks (Cerasa et al., 2006; Praamstra and Pope, 2007); of note, this may be related to both disease and pathological heterogeneities in PD patients (Merchant et al., 2008; Aarsland et al., 2011). Nonetheless, PD patients with timing-related dysfunction have abnormalities in medial frontal areas (including supplementary motor areas) in addition to the basal ganglia and cerebellum.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Studies that examine the neuroanatomical basis of timing deficits and PD.

These data are supported with neurophysiological studies from animal models. For instance, in a temporal judgment task, primate prefrontal neurons predominantly encoded temporal duration (Genovesio et al., 2009). These same neurons were robustly modulated during the response window when a judgment was made. Similar patterns have also been observed in rodent frontal cortex (Narayanan and Laubach, 2006, 2009; Kim et al., 2013), and suggest that medial frontal cortex plays an essential role in encoding and judging temporal information.

Notably, the frontal cortex and striatum function together as a circuit (Alexander et al., 1986), and several of the preceding studies involved PD-related abnormalities in the striatum as well as the frontal cortex (Elsinger et al., 2003; Harrington et al., 2011; Husárová et al., 2011) which could be modulated by dopamine (Jahanshahi et al., 2010). Effective connectivity analyses suggest that striatal and cortical areas work together to guide behavior during timing tasks (Husárová et al., 2013). However, investigating the precise mechanistic relationships among areas in the frontal cortex and striatum would require recording from both areas simultaneously or inactivating one area and examining neural activity in the other. Intraoperative recording during deep-brain stimulation surgery (Sheth et al., 2012) or recording from animal models (Narayanan and Laubach, 2006) could investigate this question in detail.

Several investigators have suggested that timing deficits in PD may involve cognitive processing well beyond motor timing such as memory (Malapani et al., 1998; Koch et al., 2008). Imaging studies have captured deficits in control processing originating from medial regions of the frontal cortex (Jahanshahi et al., 2010; Harrington et al., 2011). These data suggest that interval timing and executive functions share resources in the medial frontal cortex, and that dysfunctional timing in Parkinson’s patients may involve frontal as well striatal circuitry.

There are two clear implications supporting this idea. First, interval timing may be a useful clinical tool to assess the integrity of the frontostriatal system (Wild-Wall et al., 2008). One could imagine a specific interval timing paradigm to assess perceptual timing with a range of intervals spanning milliseconds to several seconds targeted at assaying both motor timing and higher order processing. As such, this task could provide early detection of timing deficits. Such a test would be simple, readily added to a neuropsychological battery or even administered remotely (Sternberg et al., 2013) to assist in diagnosis of disease, screen for executive dysfunction, track disease progression, and/or response to therapy.

Secondly, perceptual timing tasks can be readily trained in animals (Coull et al., 2011) such as rodents (Balci et al., 2008) or pigeons (Ludvig et al., 2011). This enables detailed, mechanistic investigation of neural circuits underlying the temporal organization of motivated behavior with tools such as transgenic mice (Drew et al., 2007), optogenetics (Narayanan et al., 2012), and neuronal ensemble recording (Matell et al., 2003; Kim et al., 2013). These circuits can be investigated in detail to understand how frontostriatal circuits are involved in perceptual timing.

In summary, we have suggested that perceptual timing is a type of executive function. Considering it as such implies that testing perceptual timing is a useful way of testing cognitive function in PD patients. Detailed mechanistic understanding of the neural circuits involved in perceptual timing in PD could lead to a greater understanding of the cognitive symptoms of PD and to targeted therapies for this difficult clinical problem.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

This work was supported by an NINDS K08 to Nandakumar S. Narayanan.

References

Aarsland, D., Brønnick, K., and Fladby, T. (2011). Mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 11, 371–378. doi: 10.1007/s11910-011-0203-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aarsland, D., Brønnick, K., Larsen, J. P., Tysnes, O. B., and Alves, G. (2009). Cognitive impairment in incident, untreated Parkinson disease: the Norwegian ParkWest study. Neurology 72, 1121–1126. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000338632.00552.cb

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aarsland, D., Bronnick, K., Williams-Gray, C., Weintraub, D., Marder, K., Kulisevsky, J., et al. (2010). Mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease: a multicenter pooled analysis. Neurology 75, 1062–1069. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f39d0e

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aarsland, D., and Kurz, M. W. (2010). The epidemiology of dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Brain Pathol. 20, 633–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2009.00369.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Alexander, G. E., DeLong, M. R., and Strick, P. L. (1986). Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 357–381. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Allman, M. J., and Meck, W. H. (2012). Pathophysiological distortions in time perception and timed performance. Brain 135, 656–677. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr210

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Arendt, T., Bigl, V., Arendt, A., and Tennstedt, A. (1983). Loss of neurons in the nucleus basalis of Meynert in Alzheimer’s disease, paralysis agitans and Korsakoff’s disease. Acta Neuropathol. 61, 101–108. doi: 10.1007/BF00697388

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Arnsten, A. F. T., and Li, B-M. (2005). Neurobiology of executive functions: catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical functions. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1377–1384. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Artieda, J., Pastor, M. A., Lacruz, F., and Obeso, J. A. (1992). Temporal discrimination is abnormal in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 115, 199–210. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.1.199

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baddeley, A., and Hitch, G. (1974). Working Memory. New York: Academic Press.

Balci, F., Papachristos, E. B., Gallistel, C. R., Brunner, D., Gibson, J., and Shumyatsky, G. P. (2008). Interval timing in genetically modified mice: a simple paradigm. Genes Brain Behav. 7, 373–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00348.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bigl, V., Woolf, N. J., and Butcher, L. L. (1982). Cholinergic projections from the basal forebrain to frontal, parietal, temporal, occipital, and cingulate cortices: a combined fluorescent tracer and acetylcholinesterase analysis. Brain Res. Bull. 8, 727–749. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(82)90101-0

CrossRef Full Text

Brown, S. W. (2006). Timing and executive function: bidirectional interference between concurrent temporal production and randomization tasks. Mem. Cogn. 34, 1464–1471. doi: 10.3758/BF03195911

CrossRef Full Text

Brown, S. W., Collier, S. A., and Night, J. C. (2012). Timing and executive resources: dual-task interference patterns between temporal production and shifting, updating, and inhibition tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 39, 947–963. doi: 10.1037/a0030484

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buhusi, C. V., and Meck, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 6, 755–765. doi: 10.1038/nrn1764

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buhusi, C. V., and Meck, W. H. (2009). Relativity theory and time perception: single or multiple clocks? PLoS ONE 4:e6268. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006268

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buhusi, C. V., and Oprisan, S. A. (2013). Time-scale invariance as an emergent property in a perceptron with realistic, noisy neurons. Behav. Processes 95, 60–70. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.02.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cerasa, A., Hagberg, G. E., Peppe, A., Bianciardi, M., Gioia, M. C., Costa, A., et al. (2006). Functional changes in the activity of cerebellum and frontostriatal regions during externally and internally timed movement in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res. Bull. 71, 259–269. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.09.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chaudhuri, K. R., and Schapira, A. H. V. (2009). Non-motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease: dopaminergic pathophysiology and treatment. Lancet Neurol. 8, 464–474. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70068-7

CrossRef Full Text

Church, R. (2003). “A concise introduction to scalar timing theory,” in Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing, ed. W. Meck (New York: CRC Press), 3–22.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Church, R. M. (1984). Properties of the internal clock. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 423, 566–582. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23459.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Church, R. M. (1997). Quantitative models of animal learning and cognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 23, 379–389. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.23.4.379

CrossRef Full Text

Claassen, D. O., Jones, C. R. G., Yu, M., Dirnberger, G., Malone, T., Parkinson, M., et al. (2013). Deciphering the impact of cerebellar and basal ganglia dysfunction in accuracy and variability of motor timing. Neuropsychologia 51, 267–274. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cools, R., Barker, R. A., Sahakian, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2001). Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. Cereb. Cortex 11, 1136–1143. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.12.1136

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cools, R., and D’Esposito, M. (2011). Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol. Psychiatry 69, e113–e125. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cools, R., Stefanova, E., Barker, R. A., Robbins, T. W., and Owen, A. M. (2002). Dopaminergic modulation of high-level cognition in Parkinson’s disease: the role of the prefrontal cortex revealed by PET. Brain 125, 584–594. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf052

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cooper, J. A., Sagar, H. J., Jordan, N., Harvey, N. S., and Sullivan, E. V. (1991). Cognitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson’s disease and its relationship to motor disability. Brain 114(Pt 5), 2095–2122. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.5.2095

CrossRef Full Text

Coull, J., and Nobre, A. (2008). Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 18, 137–144. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.07.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coull, J. T., Cheng, R.-K., and Meck, W. H. (2011). Neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates of timing. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 3–25. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.113

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coull, J. T., Hwang, H. J., Leyton, M., and Dagher, A. (2012). Dopamine precursor depletion impairs timing in healthy volunteers by attenuating activity in putamen and supplementary motor area. J. Neurosci. 32, 16704–16715. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1258-12.2012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Damier, P., Hirsch, E. C., Agid, Y., and Graybiel, A. M. (1999). The substantia nigra of the human brain II. Patterns of loss of dopamine-containing neurons in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 122, 1437–1448. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.8.1437

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Del Tredici, K., Rüb, U., De Vos, R. A. I., Bohl, J. R. E., and Braak, H. (2002). Where does Parkinson disease pathology begin in the brain? J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 61, 413–426.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Dirnberger, G., Frith, C. D., and Jahanshahi, M. (2005). Executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease is associated with altered pallidal-frontal processing. Neuroimage 25, 588–599. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Drew, M. R., Fairhurst, S., Malapani, C., Horvitz, J. C., and Balsam, P. D. (2003). Effects of dopamine antagonists on the timing of two intervals. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 75, 9–15. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(03)00036-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Drew, M. R., Simpson, E. H., Kellendonk, C., Herzberg, W. G., Lipatova, O., Fairhurst, S., et al. (2007). Transient overexpression of striatal D2 receptors impairs operant motivation and interval timing. J. Neurosci. 27, 7731–7739. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1736-07.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dubois, B., and Pillon, B. (1995). Do cognitive changes of Parkinson’s disease result from dopamine depletion? J. Neural Transm. Suppl. 45, 27–34.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Dubois, B., and Pillon, B. (1997). Cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. 244, 2–8. doi: 10.1007/PL00007725

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dymecki, J., Lechowicz, W., Bertrand, E., and Szpak, G. M. (1996). Changes in dopaminergic neurons of the mesocorticolimbic system in Parkinson’s disease. Folia Neuropathol. 34, 102–106.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Elliott, R. (2003). Executive functions and their disorders. Br. Med. Bull. 65, 49–59. doi: 10.1093/bmb/65.1.49

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Elsinger, C. L., Rao, S. M., Zimbelman, J. L., Reynolds, N. C., Blindauer, K. A., and Hoffmann, R. G. (2003). Neural basis for impaired time reproduction in Parkinson’s disease: an fMRI study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 9, 1088–1098. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703970123

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Emre, M., Aarsland, D., Albanese, A., Byrne, E. J., Deuschl, G., De Deyn, P. P., et al. (2004). Rivastigmine for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 351, 2509–2518. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa041470

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Evarts, E. V., Teräväinen, H., and Calne, D. B. (1981). Reaction time in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 104, 167–186. doi: 10.1093/brain/104.1.167

CrossRef Full Text

Fearnley, J. M., and Lees, A. J. (1991). Ageing and Parkinson’s disease: substantia nigra regional selectivity. Brain 114, 2283–2301. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.5.2283

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fiorillo, C. D., Newsome, W. T., and Schultz, W. (2008). The temporal precision of reward prediction in dopamine neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 966–973. doi: 10.1038/nn.2159

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Floresco, S. B. (2013). Prefrontal dopamine and behavioral flexibility: shifting from an “inverted-U” toward a family of functions. Front. Neurosci. 7:62. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00062

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Floresco, S. B., Magyar, O., Ghods-Sharifi, S., Vexelman, C., and Tse, M. T. L. (2006). Multiple dopamine receptor subtypes in the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat regulate set-shifting. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 297–309. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300825

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Floresco, S. B., and Phillips, A. G. (2001). Delay-dependent modulation of memory retrieval by infusion of a dopamine D1 agonist into the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 934–939. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.4.934

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Foltynie, T., Brayne, C. E. G., Robbins, T. W., and Barker, R. A. (2004). The cognitive ability of an incident cohort of Parkinson’s patients in the UK. The CamPaIGN study. Brain 127, 550–560. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh067

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Forsaa, E. B., Larsen, J. P., Wentzel-Larsen, T., and Alves, G. (2010). What predicts mortality in Parkinson disease? A prospective population-based long-term study. Neurology 75, 1270–1276. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f61311

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fortin, C., and Rousseau, R. (1998). Interference from short-term memory processing on encoding and reproducing brief durations. Psychol. Res. 61, 269–276. doi: 10.1007/s004260050031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fujita, M., Ichise, M., Zoghbi, S. S., Liow, J.-S., Ghose, S., Vines, D. C., et al. (2006). Widespread decrease of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in Parkinson’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 59, 174–177. doi: 10.1002/ana.20688

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fuster, J. (2008). The Prefrontal Cortex, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Genovesio, A., Tsujimoto, S., and Wise, S. P. (2009). Feature- and order-based timing representations in the frontal cortex. Neuron 63, 254–266. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., and Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 423, 52–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23417.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goldman-Rakic, P. S., Castner, S. A., Svensson, T. H., Siever, L. J., and Williams, G. V. (2004). Targeting the dopamine D1 receptor in schizophrenia: insights for cognitive dysfunction. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 174, 3–16. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1793-y

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gotham, A. M., Brown, R. G., and Marsden, C. D. (1988). “Frontal” cognitive function in patients with Parkinson’s disease “on” and “off” levodopa. Brain 111(Pt 2), 299–321. doi: 10.1093/brain/111.2.299

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harrington, D. L., Castillo, G. N., Greenberg, P. A., Song, D. D., Lessig, S., Lee, R. R., et al. (2011). Neurobehavioral mechanisms of temporal processing deficits in Parkinson’s disease. PLoS ONE 6:e17461. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017461

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harrington, D. L., Haaland, K. Y., and Hermanowicz, N. (1998). Temporal processing in the basal ganglia. Neuropsychology 12, 3–12. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.12.1.3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hinton, S. C., Harrington, D. L., Binder, J. R., Durgerian, S., and Rao, S. M. (2004). Neural systems supporting timing and chronometric counting: an FMRI study. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 21, 183–192. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.04.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hollerman, J. R., and Schultz, W. (1998). Dopamine neurons report an error in the temporal prediction of reward during learning. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 304–309. doi: 10.1038/1124

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Huang, C., Mattis, P., Tang, C., Perrine, K., Carbon, M., and Eidelberg, D. (2007). Metabolic brain networks associated with cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimage 34, 714–723. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hughes, L. E., Altena, E., Barker, R. A., and Rowe, J. B. (2013). Perseveration and choice in Parkinson’s disease: the impact of progressive frontostriatal dysfunction on action decisions. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1572–1581. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs144

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Husárová, I., Lungu, O. V., Mareček, R., Mikl, M., Gescheidt, T., Krupa, P., et al. (2011). Functional imaging of the cerebellum and basal ganglia during predictive motor timing in early Parkinson’s disease. J. Neuroimaging doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2011.00663.x [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Husárová, I., Mikl, M., Lungu, O. V., Mareček, R., Vaníček, J., and Bareš, M. (2013). Similar circuits but different connectivity patterns between the cerebellum, basal ganglia, and supplementary motor area in early Parkinson’s disease patients and controls during predictive motor timing. J. Neuroimaging doi: 10.1111/jon.12030 [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jahanshahi, M., Jones, C. R. G., Zijlmans, J., Katzenschlager, R., Lee, L., Quinn, N., et al. (2010). Dopaminergic modulation of striato-frontal connectivity during motor timing in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 133, 727–745. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Janvin, C. C., Aarsland, D., and Larsen, J. P. (2005). Cognitive predictors of dementia in Parkinson’s disease: a community-based, 4-year longitudinal study. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 18, 149–154. doi: 10.1177/0891988705277540

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Javoy-Agid, F., and Agid, Y. (1980). Is the mesocortical dopaminergic system involved in Parkinson disease? Neurology 30, 1326–1330. doi: 10.1212/WNL.30.12.1326

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Javoy-Agid, F., Ploska, A., and Agid, Y. (1981). Microtopography of tyrosine hydroxylase, glutamic acid decarboxylase, and choline acetyltransferase in the substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area of control and Parkinsonian brains. J. Neurochem. 37, 1218–1227. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1981.tb04672.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jellinger, K. A. (1999). Post mortem studies in Parkinson’s disease: is it possible to detect brain areas for specific symptoms? J. Neural Transm. Suppl. 56, 1–29. doi: 10.1007/978-3-7091-6360-3_1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jellinger, K. A. (2011). Synuclein deposition and non-motor symptoms in Parkinson disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 310, 107–111. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.04.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jones, C. R., and Jahanshahi, M. (2013). Contributions of the basal ganglia to temporal processing: evidence from Parkinson’s disease. Timing Time Percept. 1, 1–41.

Jones, C. R. G., Claassen, D. O., Yu, M., Spies, J. R., Malone, T., Dirnberger, G., et al. (2011). Modeling accuracy and variability of motor timing in treated and untreated Parkinson’s disease and healthy controls. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 5:81. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00081

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jurado, M. B., and Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol. Rev. 17, 213–233. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kaasinen, V., Nurmi, E., Brück, A., Eskola, O., Bergman, J., Solin, O., et al. (2001). Increased frontal [(18)F]fluorodopa uptake in early Parkinson’s disease: sex differences in the prefrontal cortex. Brain 124, 1125–1130. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.6.1125

CrossRef Full Text

Kehagia, A. A., Murray, G. K., and Robbins, T. W. (2010). Learning and cognitive flexibility: frontostriatal function and monoaminergic modulation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 199–204. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.01.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, J., Ghim, J.-W., Lee, J. H., and Jung, M. W. (2013). Neural correlates of interval timing in rodent prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 33, 13834–13847. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1443-13.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kobayashi, S., and Schultz, W. (2008). Influence of reward delays on responses of dopamine neurons. J. Neurosci. 28, 7837–7846. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1600-08.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koch, G., Brusa, L., Oliveri, M., Stanzione, P., and Caltagirone, C. (2005). Memory for time intervals is impaired in left hemi-Parkinson patients. Neuropsychologia 43, 1163–1167. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koch, G., Costa, A., Brusa, L., Peppe, A., Gatto, I., Torriero, S., et al. (2008). Impaired reproduction of second but not millisecond time intervals in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 46, 1305–1313. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kurti, A. N., and Matell, M. S. (2011). Nucleus accumbens dopamine modulates response rate but not response timing in an interval timing task. Behav. Neurosci. 125, 215–225. doi: 10.1037/a0022892

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laje, R., and Buonomano, D. V. (2013). Robust timing and motor patterns by taming chaos in recurrent neural networks. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 925–933. doi: 10.1038/nn.3405

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lake, J. I., and Meck, W. H. (2013). Differential effects of amphetamine and haloperidol on temporal reproduction: dopaminergic regulation of attention and clock speed. Neuropsychologia 51, 284–292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lapish, C. C., Kroener, S., Durstewitz, D., Lavin, A., and Seamans, J. K. (2007). The ability of the mesocortical dopamine system to operate in distinct temporal modes. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 191, 609–625. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0527-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levy, G., Jacobs, D. M., Tang, M.-X., Côté, L. J., Louis, E. D., Alfaro, B., et al. (2002a). Memory and executive function impairment predict dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 17, 1221–1226. doi: 10.1002/mds.10280

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levy, G., Tang, M. -X., Louis, E. D., Côté, L. J., Alfaro, B., Mejia, H., et al. (2002b). The association of incident dementia with mortality in PD. Neurology 59, 1708–1713. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000036610.36834.E0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., and Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Loos, M., Pattij, T., Janssen, M. C. W., Counotte, D. S., Schoffelmeer, A. N. M., Smit, A. B., et al. (2010). Dopamine receptor D1/D5 gene expression in the medial prefrontal cortex predicts impulsive choice in rats. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1064–1070. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp167

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ludvig, E. A., Balci, F., and Spetch, M. L. (2011). Reward magnitude and timing in pigeons. Behav. Processes 86, 359–363. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2011.01.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mahieux, F., Fénelon, G., Flahault, A., Manifacier, M. J., Michelet, D., and Boller, F. (1998). Neuropsychological prediction of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 64, 178–183. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.64.2.178

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Malapani, C., Deweer, B., and Gibbon, J. (2002). Separating storage from retrieval dysfunction of temporal memory in Parkinson’s disease. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 311–322. doi: 10.1162/089892902317236920

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Malapani, C., Rakitin, B., Levy, R., Meck, W. H., Deweer, B., Dubois, B., et al. (1998). Coupled temporal memories in Parkinson’s disease: a dopamine-related dysfunction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 316–331. doi: 10.1162/089892998562762

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maricq, A. V., and Church, R. M. (1983). The differential effects of haloperidol and methamphetamine on time estimation in the rat. Psychopharmacology 79, 10–15. doi: 10.1007/BF00433008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matell, M. S., Meck, W. H., and Nicolelis, M. A. L. (2003). Interval timing and the encoding of signal duration by ensembles of cortical and striatal neurons. Behav. Neurosci. 117, 760–773. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.4.760

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McCormick, D. A., Steinmetz, J. E., and Thompson, R. F. (1985). Lesions of the inferior olivary complex cause extinction of the classically conditioned eyeblink response. Brain Res. 359, 120–130. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)91419-2

CrossRef Full Text

Meck, W. H. (2006). Neuroanatomical localization of an internal clock: a functional link between mesolimbic, nigrostriatal, and mesocortical dopaminergic systems. Brain Res. 1109, 93–107. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meck, W. H., and Church, R. M. (1987). Cholinergic modulation of the content of temporal memory. Behav. Neurosci. 101, 457–464. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.101.4.457

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meck, W. H., Penney, T. B., and Pouthas, V. (2008). Cortico-striatal representation of time in animals and humans. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 18, 145–152. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.08.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mehanna, R., and Jankovic, J. (2013). Movement disorders in cerebrovascular disease. Lancet Neurol. 12, 597–608. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70057-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Merchant, H., Harrington, D. L., and Meck, W. H. (2013). Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 36, 313–336. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170349

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Merchant, H., Luciana, M., Hooper, C., Majestic, S., and Tuite, P. (2008). Interval timing and Parkinson’s disease: heterogeneity in temporal performance. Exp. Brain Res. 184, 233–248. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1097-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mirabella, G., Iaconelli, S., Modugno, N., Giannini, G., Lena, F., and Cantore, G. (2013). Stimulation of subthalamic nuclei restores a near normal planning strategy in Parkinson’s patients. PLoS ONE 8:e62793. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062793

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mita, A., Mushiake, H., Shima, K., Matsuzaka, Y., and Tanji, J. (2009). Interval time coding by neurons in the presupplementary and supplementary motor areas. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 502–507. doi: 10.1038/nn.2272

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morrison, C. E., Borod, J. C., Brin, M. F., Hälbig, T. D., and Olanow, C. W. (2004). Effects of levodopa on cognitive functioning in moderate-to-severe Parkinson’s disease (MSPD). J. Neural Transm. 111, 1333–1341. doi: 10.1007/s00702-004-0145-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Narayanan, N. S., Land, B. B., Solder, J. E., Deisseroth, K., and Dileone, R. J. (2012). Prefrontal D1 dopamine signaling is required for temporal control. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211258109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Narayanan, N. S., and Laubach, M. (2006). Top-down control of motor cortex ensembles by dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron 52, 921–931. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Narayanan, N. S., and Laubach, M. (2009). Delay activity in rodent frontal cortex during a simple reaction time task. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 2859–2871. doi: 10.1152/jn.90615.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Niki, H., and Watanabe, M. (1979). Prefrontal and cingulate unit activity during timing behavior in the monkey. Brain Res. 171, 213–224. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90328-7

CrossRef Full Text

Norman, T., and Shallice, D. (1986). “Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behavior,” in Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research and Theory, eds R. J. Davidson, G. E. Shwartz, and D. Shapiro (New York: Plenum), 1–18.

O’Boyle, D. J., Freeman, J. S., and Cody, F. W. (1996). The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with Parkinson’s disease. Brain 119(Pt 1), 51–70. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.1.51

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ogden, R. S., Salominaite, E., Jones, L. A., Fisk, J. E., and Montgomery, C. (2011). The role of executive functions in human prospective interval timing. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 137, 352–358. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.04.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olton, D. S., Wenk, G. L., Church, R. M., and Meck, W. H. (1988). Attention and the frontal cortex as examined by simultaneous temporal processing. Neuropsychologia 26, 307–318. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(88)90083-8

CrossRef Full Text

Pascual-Sedano, B., Kulisevsky, J., Barbanoj, M., García-Sánchez, C., Campolongo, A., Gironell, A., et al. (2008). Levodopa and executive performance in Parkinson’s disease: a randomized study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 14, 832–841. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708081010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pastor, M. A., Artieda, J., Jahanshahi, M., and Obeso, J. A. (1992). Time estimation and reproduction is abnormal in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 115(Pt 1), 211–225. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.1.211

CrossRef Full Text

Perbal, S., Deweer, B., Pillon, B., Vidailhet, M., Dubois, B., and Pouthas, V. (2005). Effects of internal clock and memory disorders on duration reproductions and duration productions in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain Cogn. 58, 35–48. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Phillips, A. G., Ahn, S., and Floresco, S. B. (2004). Magnitude of dopamine release in medial prefrontal cortex predicts accuracy of memory on a delayed response task. J. Neurosci. 24, 547–553. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4653-03.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Picton, T. W., Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Shallice, T., Binns, M. A., and Gillingham, S. (2006). Effects of focal frontal lesions on response inhibition. Cereb. Cortex 17, 826–838. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhk031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Poewe, W., Wolters, E., Emre, M., Onofrj, M., Hsu, C., Tekin, S., et al. (2006). Long-term benefits of rivastigmine in dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease: an active treatment extension study. Mov. Disord. 21, 456–461. doi: 10.1002/mds.20700

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Praamstra, P., and Pope, P. (2007). Slow brain potential and oscillatory EEG manifestations of impaired temporal preparation in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 2848–2857. doi: 10.1152/jn.00224.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rakshi, J. S., Uema, T., Ito, K., Bailey, D. L., Morrish, P. K., Ashburner, J., et al. (1999). Frontal, midbrain and striatal dopaminergic function in early and advanced Parkinson’s disease A 3D [(18)F]dopa-PET study. Brain 122(Pt 9), 1637–1650. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.9.1637

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rattat, A. C. (2010). Bidirectional interference between timing and concurrent memory processing in children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 106, 145–162. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.02.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Riesen, J. M., and Schnider, A. (2001). Time estimation in Parkinson’s disease: normal long duration estimation despite impaired short duration discrimination. J. Neurol. 248, 27–35. doi: 10.1007/s004150170266

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Santangelo, G., Trojano, L., Vitale, C., Ianniciello, M., Amboni, M., Grossi, D., et al. (2007). A neuropsychological longitudinal study in Parkinson’s patients with and without hallucinations. Mov. Disord. 22, 2418–2425. doi: 10.1002/mds.21746

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sawada, Y., Nishio, Y., Suzuki, K., Hirayama, K., Takeda, A., Hosokai, Y., et al. (2012). Attentional set-shifting deficit in Parkinson’s disease is associated with prefrontal dysfunction: an FDG-PET study. PLoS ONE 7:e38498. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038498

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Scatton, B., Javoy-Agid, F., Rouquier, L., Dubois, B., and Agid, Y. (1983). Reduction of cortical dopamine, noradrenaline, serotonin and their metabolites in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res. 275, 321–328. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90993-9

CrossRef Full Text

Schneider, B. A., and Ghose, G. M. (2012). Temporal production signals in parietal cortex. PLoS Biol. 10:e1001413. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001413

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Seamans, J. K., Floresco, S. B., and Phillips, A. G. (1998). D1 receptor modulation of hippocampal–prefrontal cortical circuits integrating spatial memory with executive functions in the rat. J. Neurosci. 18, 1613–1621.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Seamans, J. K., and Yang, C. R. (2004). The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Prog. Neurobiol. 74, 1–58. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.05.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sheth, S. A., Mian, M. K., Patel, S. R., Asaad, W. F., Williams, Z. M., Dougherty, D. D., et al. (2012). Human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex neurons mediate ongoing behavioural adaptation. Nature 488, 218–221. doi: 10.1038/nature11239

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shimamura, A. P., Janowsky, J. S., and Squire, L. R. (1990). Memory for the temporal order of events in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia 28, 803–813. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90004-8

CrossRef Full Text

Simen, P., Balci, F., de Souza, L., Cohen, J. D., and Holmes, P. (2011). A model of interval timing by neural integration. J. Neurosci. 31, 9238–9253. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3121-10.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sternberg, D. A., Ballard, K., Hardy, J. L., Katz, B., Doraiswamy, P. M., and Scanlon, M. (2013). The largest human cognitive performance dataset reveals insights into the effects of lifestyle factors and aging. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:292. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00292

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Shallice, T., Picton, T. W., Binns, M. A., Macdonald, R., et al. (2005). Multiple frontal systems controlling response speed. Neuropsychologia 43, 396–417. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Takahashi, H., Yamada, M., and Suhara, T. (2012). Functional significance of central D1 receptors in cognition: beyond working memory. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 32, 1248–1258. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2011.194

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Taylor, K. M., Horvitz, J. C., and Balsam, P. D. (2007). Amphetamine affects the start of responding in the peak interval timing task. Behav. Processes 74, 168–175. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2006.11.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Torta, D. M. E., Castelli, L., Latini-Corazzini, L., Banche, A., Lopiano, L., and Geminiani, G. (2010). Dissociation between time reproduction of actions and of intervals in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. 257, 1356–1361. doi: 10.1007/s00415-010-5532-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Uc, E. Y., Rizzo, M., Anderson, S. W., Qian, S., Rodnitzky, R. L., and Dawson, J. D. (2005). Visual dysfunction in Parkinson disease without dementia. Neurology 65, 1907–1913. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000191565.11065.11

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Spaendonck, K. P., Berger, H. J., Horstink, M. W., Buytenhuijs, E. L., and Cools, A. R. (1996). Executive functions and disease characteristics in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 34, 617–626. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00159-X

CrossRef Full Text

Wang, M., Vijayraghavan, S., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2004). Selective D2 receptor actions on the functional circuitry of working memory. Science 303, 853–856. doi: 10.1126/science.1091162

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Whitehouse, P. J., Hedreen, J. C., White, C. L., and Price, D. L. (1983). Basal forebrain neurons in the dementia of Parkinson disease. Ann. Neurol. 13, 243–248. doi: 10.1002/ana.410130304

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wild-Wall, N., Willemssen, R., Falkenstein, M., and Beste, C. (2008). Time estimation in healthy ageing and neurodegenerative basal ganglia disorders. Neurosci. Lett. 442, 34–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.06.069

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Williams, G. V., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1995). Modulation of memory fields by dopamine D1 receptors in prefrontal cortex. Nature 376, 572–575. doi: 10.1038/376572a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Williams, S. M., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1998). Widespread origin of the primate mesofrontal dopamine system. Cereb. Cortex 8, 321–345. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.4.321

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Williams-Gray, C. H., Evans, J. R., Goris, A., Foltynie, T., Ban, M., Robbins, T. W., et al. (2009). The distinct cognitive syndromes of Parkinson’s disease: 5 year follow-up of the CamPaIGN cohort. Brain 132, 2958–2969. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp245

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Williams-Gray, C. H., Foltynie, T., Brayne, C. E. G., Robbins, T. W., and Barker, R. A. (2007). Evolution of cognitive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson’s disease cohort. Brain 130, 1787–1798. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm111

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wylie, S. A., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Elias, W. J., Frysinger, R. C., Bashore, T. R., Downs, K. E., et al. (2010). Subthalamic nucleus stimulation influences expression and suppression of impulsive behaviour in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 133, 3611–3624. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq239

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wylie, S. A., van den Wildenberg, W., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Claassen, D. O., Wooten, G. F., and Manning, C. A. (2012). Differential susceptibility to motor impulsivity among functional subtypes of Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 83, 1149–1154. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-303056

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zesiewicz, T. A., Sullivan, K. L., Arnulf, I., Chaudhuri, K. R., Morgan, J. C., Gronseth, G. S., et al. (2010). Practice parameter: treatment of nonmotor symptoms of Parkinson disease: report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 74, 924–931. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181d55f24

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zetterström, T., Sharp, T., Marsden, C. A., and Ungerstedt, U. (1983). In vivo measurement of dopamine and its metabolites by intracerebral dialysis: changes after d-amphetamine. J. Neurochem. 41, 1769–1773. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1983.tb00893.x

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: temporal processing, executive function, cognitive impairment, Parkinson’s disease, interval timing

Citation: Parker KL, Lamichhane D, Caetano MS and Narayanan NS (2013) Executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease and timing deficits. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:75. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00075

Received: 30 July 2013; Accepted: 14 October 2013;
Published online: 31 October 2013.

Edited by:

Warren H. Meck, Duke University, USA

Reviewed by:

Vinay V. Parikh, Temple University, USA
Daniel Claassen, Vanderbilt University, USA

Copyright © 2013 Parker, Lamichhane, Caetano and Narayanan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nandakumar S. Narayanan, Department of Neurology, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, 200 Hawkins Drive, Iowa City, IA 52243, USA e-mail: nandakumar-narayanan@uiowa.edu

Download