Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pharmacol., 26 April 2016
Sec. Ethnopharmacology

A Phaseolus vulgaris Extract Reduces Cue-Induced Reinstatement of Chocolate Seeking in Rats

\r\nIrene LorraiIrene Lorrai1Valentina PigaValentina Piga1Mauro A. M. CaraiMauro A. M. Carai2Antonella RivaAntonella Riva3Paolo MorazzoniPaolo Morazzoni3Gian Luigi GessaGian Luigi Gessa1Giancarlo Colombo*Giancarlo Colombo1*Paola MaccioniPaola Maccioni1
  • 1Neuroscience Institute, National Research Council of Italy, Section of Cagliari, Cagliari, Italy
  • 2Cagliari Pharmacological Research s.r.l., Cagliari, Italy
  • 3Indena S.p.A, Milan, Italy

Previous evidence has suggested that treatment with a standardized dry extract of Phaseolus vulgaris reduced intake and operant self-administration of highly palatable foods and fluids in rats and mice. The present study was designed to assess whether such extract was also effective in reducing seeking behavior for a highly hedonic chocolate-flavored beverage, using a “reinstatement” procedure adopted from the drug addiction research field and modeling relapse behavior. Rats were initially trained to lever-respond for the chocolate-flavored beverage under the Fixed Ratio (FR) 10 schedule of reinforcement. Subsequently, rats were exposed to an extinction responding phase, during which lever-responding – being unreinforced – diminished progressively up to extinction. Lever-responding was then powerfully reinstated by the non-contingent presentation of a complex of gustatory, olfactory, auditory, and visual stimuli previously associated to the availability of the chocolate-flavored beverage. Acute, intragastric administration of P. vulgaris dry extract (100 and 500 mg/kg) reduced lever-responding by 40–45%, in comparison to vehicle condition. These results indicate the ability of P. vulgaris dry extract to reduce seeking behavior for a highly palatable nourishment in an experimental model of relapse into disordered eating of palatable foods. The unavailability of the chocolate-flavored beverage in the reinstatement session tends to exclude that the observed effect of the P. vulgaris dry extract was secondary to any inhibition of carbohydrate metabolism; conversely, it is the likely consequence on a central action on the rewarding and hedonic properties of food.

Introduction

The Phaseolus vulgaris genus includes all species of legume seeds typically referred to as common beans. Nowadays, P. vulgaris is considered a major functional food: extracts from P. vulgaris have indeed been repeatedly reported to reduce (i) food intake, body weight, lipid deposit, and glycemia in a series of validated animal models of overeating, obesity, diabetes, and metabolic syndrome (e.g., Grant et al., 1995; Pusztai et al., 1995, 1998, 2008; Bardocz et al., 1996; Tormo et al., 2004; Carai et al., 2009, 2011; Fantini et al., 2009; Loi et al., 2013) and (ii) appetite, food intake, body weight, waist circumference, and glycemia in healthy, overweight, or obese individuals (e.g., Layer et al., 1985; Boivin et al., 1987; Udani et al., 2004; Celleno et al., 2007; Udani and Singh, 2007; Rondanelli et al., 2011; Spadafranca et al., 2013). Together, these results suggest that P. vulgaris extracts may constitute a valuable source for novel and potentially effective therapies for obesity and related disorders (see Obiro et al., 2008; Astell et al., 2013; however, see also Onakpoya et al., 2011).

Recent studies from this lab have suggested that extracts from P. vulgaris may preferentially suppress several behaviors motivated by highly palatable nourishments in rodents: administration of standardized extracts of P. vulgaris indeed suppressed intake of butter cookies, condensed milk, and chocolate-flavored beverages in rats (Carai et al., 2009; Fantini et al., 2009) and mice (Loi et al., 2013) as well as operant self-administration of a chocolate-flavored beverage in rats (Maccioni et al., 2010). When the study design envisaged concurrent availability of the highly palatable food and regular food pellets, P. vulgaris extracts were found to be more potent and effective in suppressing intake of the palatable over regular food (Fantini et al., 2009), suggesting that P. vulgaris extracts may have a greater impact on the hedonic, rather than nutritive, properties of food.

With the aim of characterizing more in-depth the pharmacological profile of P. vulgaris extracts, the present study was designed to investigate the effect of a P. vulgaris extract on seeking behavior for a highly palatable food in rats. To this end, we employed (i) the same, standardized dry extract of P. vulgaris tested in all previous studies with different palatable nourishments (Carai et al., 2009; Fantini et al., 2009; Maccioni et al., 2010; Loi et al., 2013) and (ii) a chocolate-flavored beverage possessing high hedonic, reinforcing, and motivational properties in rats (see Maccioni and Colombo, 2016); additionally, we adapted to the chocolate-flavored beverage an experimental procedure, named “reinstatement of seeking behavior,” largely used in the drug addiction field to model relapse episodes and loss of control over substances of abuse (see Markou et al., 1993; Bossert et al., 2013) and, in much fewer instances, palatable foods (Martín-García et al., 2011; Calu et al., 2014). In this procedure, laboratory animals are initially trained to lever-respond for a given substance under standard schedules of reinforcement. Once lever-responding has stabilized, animals are exposed to an extinction responding phase, during which lever-responding – being unreinforced – diminishes progressively up to virtually complete extinction. After extinction, lever-responding (or seeking for the substance) is resumed, or reinstated, by different stimuli, including (i) limited, non-contingent availability of the substance, (ii) olfactory or visual cues previously associated to the substance (predictive of its availability), (iii) exposure to stressful stimuli, and/or (iv) injection of specific drugs. Concurrence of these stimuli or events is expected to trigger an intense reinstatement of lever-responding (still unreinforced), proposed to mimic human craving and seeking for the specific substance (see Markou et al., 1993; Bossert et al., 2013; Calu et al., 2014).

The procedure of reinstatement of seeking for a chocolate-flavored beverage, recently set up in this lab (see Maccioni and Colombo, 2016), is based on the ability of a complex of gustatory, olfactory, auditory, and visual stimuli to promptly and effectively resume previously extinguished lever-responding for the chocolate-flavored beverage in rats. With relevance to the aims of the present study, reinstatement of seeking behavior for the chocolate-flavored beverage was found to be pharmacologically manipulable: for instance, the cannabinoid CB1 receptor antagonist/inverse agonist, rimonabant, known to suppress appetite and food intake in laboratory animals and humans (see Carai et al., 2006; Leite et al., 2009; Scherma et al., 2014), suppressed the reinstatement of seeking behavior for the chocolate-flavored beverage in rats (Maccioni and Colombo, 2016).

Materials and Methods

The experimental procedure employed in the present study was in accordance with the Italian Law on the “Protection of animals used for scientific reasons.”

Animals

Adult, male Wistar rats (n = 30; Harlan Laboratories, San Pietro al Natisone, Italy), weighing approximately 300 g at the start of the study, were used. Rats were housed 3 per cage. All cages had wood chip bedding. The animal facility was under an inverted 12:12 h light-dark cycle (lights on at 8:00 p.m.), constant temperature of 22 ± 2°C, and relative humidity of approximately 60%. Standard rat chow and tap water were always available in the homecage, except as noted below. Rats were extensively habituated to handling and intragastric infusion (by means of a stainless steel gavage).

Extract Preparation

Preparation procedure of P. vulgaris dry extract (Beanblock®) has been described in detail elsewhere (Fantini et al., 2009). Briefly, P. vulgaris dry extract was prepared by means of aqueous extraction and alcoholic precipitation from the common kidney bean. Bean extract was obtained by extraction with citrate buffer and precipitation with ethanol. The obtained extract was characterized by a standardized composition in: (i) 8.5% (w/w) α-amylase inhibitor, with inhibiting activity of 1,400 U/mg; (ii) phytohaemoagglutinin (haemoagglutinating activity equal to 16 HAU/mg).

Chocolate-Flavored Beverage

The chocolate-flavored beverage was prepared diluting powdered Nesquik® (Nestlè® Italiana, Milan, Italy) in tap water. Concentration of Nesquik® chocolate powder was 5% (w/v) throughout the study. This concentration was selected on the basis of the results of previous experiments in which it was largely preferred over a wide range of concentrations (see Maccioni and Colombo, 2016). The chocolate-flavored beverage was prepared daily and sipper bottles (see below) were shaken immediately before the start of each session to prevent any powder deposit. The chocolate-flavored beverage provided 0.8 kJ/g.

Previous experiments demonstrated that this chocolate-flavored beverage possesses highly hedonic, reinforcing, and motivational properties in rats (e.g., Maccioni et al., 2008, 2010; Zaru et al., 2013a,b).

Apparatus

Operant sessions were conducted in modular chambers (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT, USA), located in sound-attenuated cubicles, with fans for ventilation and background white noise. The front panel of each chamber was equipped with (i) one retractable response lever, (ii) one green stimulus light mounted above the lever, (iii) one sonalert, and (iv) the retractable spout of a liquid sipper bottle (250-ml capacity) located outside the chamber. A white house light was centered at the top of the back wall of each chamber.

Start of the self-administration session was signaled by illumination of the house light and insertion of the lever. Achievement of the response requirement (RR; see below) resulted in (i) exposure of the sipper bottle spout inside the operant chamber, (ii) illumination of the green stimulus light, and (iii) activation of the sonalert; all these three events lasted 5 s. End of the self-administration session was associated to retraction of the lever and switch off of the house light.

Experimental Procedure

A detailed description of the experimental procedure used in the present study has recently been given elsewhere (see Maccioni and Colombo, 2016).

Training and Maintenance Phases

To facilitate the acquisition of lever-responding behavior, rats were water-deprived in their homecage over the 12 h preceding the first two operant sessions. Self-administration sessions were conducted daily, 5 days per week (Monday to Friday), during the first half of the dark phase of the light/dark cycle. Self-administration sessions lasted 60 min. During the first two sessions, rats were trained to lever-respond on a Fixed Ratio (FR) 1 (FR1) schedule of reinforcement for the chocolate-flavored beverage. FR was progressively increased from FR1 to FR10 over 10 sessions. Subsequently, 20 additional self-administration sessions with FR10 were conducted (maintenance phase), so that number of lever-responses for and intake of the chocolate-flavored beverage stabilized in all rats before the start of the test sessions.

Extinction Responding Phase

Rats were divided into three groups of n = 10, matched for body weight and number of lever-responses for the chocolate-flavored beverage over the last five self-administration sessions of the maintenance phase. At the end of the maintenance phase, rats of all three groups underwent an extinction responding phase, during which daily sessions – lasting 60 min and occurring with no weekend interruptions – were characterized by unavailability of the chocolate-flavored beverage; specifically, sipper-bottle delivery system, green stimulus light, and sonalert were off, and lever-responding was unreinforced. An extinction criterion was set at ≤ 30 lever-responses per session for three consecutive sessions.

Reinstatement session

The day after achievement of the extinction criterion, each rat was exposed to a single reinstatement (test) session, during which a stimulus complex – previously associated to availability of the chocolate-flavored beverage – was presented. This stimulus complex was composed of: (i) click emitted by the introduction, into the operant chamber, of the sipper bottle spout; (ii) turning on of the green stimulus light; (iii) activation of the sonalert; (iv) availability of the chocolate-flavored beverage for 5 s. This stimulus complex was presented for 10 times within 100 s. Immediately after the 10th presentation of the stimulus complex, the lever was introduced inside the chamber, the house light was switched on, and lever-responses were recorded. Lever-responding during the reinstatement session was unreinforced.

Phaseolus vulgaris dry extract was suspended in distilled water with 0.5% methylcellulose and administered intragastrically (infusion volume: 4 ml/kg), at doses of 0, 100, and 500 mg/kg, 120 min before the start of the reinstatement session. Dose-range of P. vulgaris dry extract and pretreatment time were chosen on the basis of results of previous studies from this lab (e.g., Maccioni et al., 2010).

Measured Variables and Data Analysis

In the maintenance phase, measured variables were (i) number of lever-responses for the chocolate-flavored beverage and (ii) amount of self-administered chocolate flavored beverage [expressed in ml/kg and determined by weighing the sipper bottle (0.01-g accuracy) before and after the session]. Data on number of lever-responses over the entire 20-session phase and the last 5-session period were analyzed by separate 1-way ANOVAs with repeated measures.

In the extinction responding phase, the measured variable was the number of sessions needed to achieve the extinction criterion. These data were analyzed by 1-way ANOVA and Log Rank test.

In the reinstatement session, the measured variable was the number of lever-responses. Data on the effect of P. vulgaris dry extract on this variable were analyzed by a 2-way [phase (extinction/reinstatement); treatment (dose of P. vulgaris dry extract)] ANOVA with repeated measures on the factor “phase,” followed by the Newman–Keuls test for post hoc comparisons.

Results

Maintenance Phase

Over the 20 daily self-administration sessions of the maintenance phase (occurring in between training and extinction responding phases), lever-responding for the chocolate-flavored beverage under the FR10 schedule of reinforcement was extremely high, averaging approximately 2200 responses/session (Figure 1, top). These data confirm that this chocolate-flavored beverage had strong reinforcing properties in rats (Maccioni and Colombo, 2016). Over the entire 20-session phase, mean lever-responding for the chocolate-flavored beverage displayed some inter-session variability [F(19,551) = 5.28, P < 0.0001] (Figure 1, top), that vanished completely over the last five sessions [i.e., the time period used for rat allocation to the three different experimental groups (see above); F(4,116) = 1.75, P > 0.05] (Figure 1, top). Amount of self-administered chocolate-flavored beverage averaged approximately 75 and 70 ml/kg/session over the entire 20-session phase and the last 5-session period, respectively.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. (Top) The self-administration behavior of a chocolate-flavored beverage in Wistar rats. Rats were trained to lever-respond for the chocolate-flavored beverage [5% (w/v) Nesquik® chocolate powder in water] under the Fixed Ratio (FR) 10 schedule of reinforcement in daily 60 min self-administration sessions. During these 20 daily self-administration sessions (maintenance phase), self-administration behavior stabilized in all rats. Each point of this panel is the mean ± SEM of n = 30 rats. (Center) The extinction responding behavior. The extinction responding phase occurred after the self-administration maintenance phase. During the sessions of the extinction responding phase, lever-responding was unreinforced (the chocolate-flavored beverage was unavailable). Based on the number of lever-responses for the chocolate-flavored beverage over the last five sessions of the maintenance phase, rats were divided into the three groups (n = 10) that – after completion of the extinction responding phase – received the three different doses of Phaseolus vulgaris. Each point of this panel is the mean ± SEM of sample sizes varying between 1 and 10, depending on the session in which each single rat achieved the extinction criterion (i.e., ≤30 lever-responses per session for three consecutive sessions). (Bottom) The effect of treatment with different doses of a dry extract of P. vulgaris (Beanblock) on reinstatement of seeking for the chocolate-flavored beverage. This test session occurred once each single rat had achieved the extinction criterion. In the reinstatement session (lasting 60 min and occurring the day after the extinction responding phase), unreinforced lever-responding was reinstated by the repeated presentation of a complex of gustatory, olfactory, auditory, and visual stimuli previously associated to availability of the chocolate-flavored beverage. P. vulgaris dry extract was administered intragastrically, at doses of 0, 100, and 500 mg/kg, 120 min before the start of the session. Each bar of this panel is the mean ± SEM of n = 10 rats. P < 0.05, ∗∗P < 0.01, and ∗∗∗P < 0.0005 in comparison to the same rat group in the last session of the extinction responding phase (Newman–Keuls test); #P < 0.005 in comparison to the vehicle-treated rat group in the reinstatement session (Newman–Keuls test).

Extinction Responding Phase

Log Rank test indicated that the profile of lever-responding over the extinction responding phase did not differ among the three rat groups subsequently treated with 0, 100, and 500 mg/kg P. vulgaris dry extract (χ2 = 2.064, P > 0.05; Figure 1, center). Additionally, the three rat groups did not differ in number of sessions of the extinction responding phase needed to achieve the extinction criterion [7.2 ± 0.6, 9.4 ± 1.2, and 8.1 ± 0.7 (mean ± SEM) in rats subsequently treated with 0, 100, and 500 mg/kg P. vulgaris dry extract, respectively, F(2,27) = 1.64, P > 0.05].

Reinstatement Session

ANOVA indicated a significant effect of presentation of the stimulus complex previously associated to the chocolate-flavored beverage [F(1,54) = 54.81, P < 0.0001] and of treatment with P. vulgaris dry extract [F(2,54) = 4.35, P < 0.05], as well as a significant interaction between the two factors [F(2,54) = 3.24, P < 0.05], on number of lever-responses. Number of lever-responses during the last session of the extinction responding phase was virtually identical in the three rat groups subsequently treated with 0, 100, and 500 mg/kg P. vulgaris dry extract (Figure 1, bottom).

Under vehicle condition (0 mg/kg P. vulgaris dry extract), presentation of the stimulus complex robustly reinstated lever-responding: the number of lever-responses averaged indeed 90.8 ± 12.8 and was approximately five times higher than that recorded in the same rat group during the last session of the extinction responding phase (P < 0.0005, Newman–Keuls test; Figure 1, bottom). Administration of P. vulgaris dry extract resulted in a substantial reduction in lever-responding: although still significantly elevated in comparison to data collected in the last session of the extinction responding phase, lever-responding was indeed reduced by 40 and 45% in the rat groups treated with 100 and 500 mg/kg P. vulgaris dry extract, respectively, in comparison to vehicle-treated rat group (P < 0.005; Newman–Keuls test; Figure 1, bottom).

Discussion

Presentation of different gustatory, olfactory, auditory, and visual stimuli, previously associated to the availability of a chocolate-flavored beverage, powerfully reinstated seeking behavior (i.e., unreinforced lever-responding) for the chocolate-flavored beverage in rats in which lever-responding for the chocolate-flavored beverage had been initially established and then extinguished. The high number of lever-responding recorded in the vehicle-treated rat group during the reinstatement session (i) is consistent with values observed in previous studies conducted using the same experimental procedure (Zaru et al., 2013a; Maccioni and Colombo, 2016), (ii) reflects the motivation of rats to seek for the chocolate-flavored beverage after a prolonged period of abstinence, and (iii) is conceived to model several aspects of loss of control over palatable foods and episodes of relapse into disordered eating of palatable foods in humans (see Martín-García et al., 2011; Calu et al., 2014; Maccioni and Colombo, 2016).

Acute treatment with P. vulgaris dry extract resulted in a remarkable reduction in number of lever-responses during the reinstatement session, suggesting that this treatment effectively attenuated the development of reinstatement of seeking behavior for the chocolate-flavored beverage. Even the lowest dose tested (100 mg/kg) was highly effective, producing a reduction in lever-responding (40% in comparison to vehicle condition) that did not differ largely from reduction produced by the highest dose tested (500 mg/kg); the comparable magnitude of the reducing effect of the two tested doses is suggestive of a saturable mechanism underlying the action of the P. vulgaris dry extract. These data contribute a further element to the behavioral characterization of the anorectic profile of this P. vulgaris dry extract: besides reducing regular food intake and body weight, intake and reinforcing properties of highly palatable nourishments (Carai et al., 2009; Fantini et al., 2009; Maccioni et al., 2010; Loi et al., 2013), this extract also reduces relapse-like behavior for a highly palatable, hedonic food.

Reduction in lever-responding during the reinstatement session was likely not due to any unspecific, sedative effect or possible malaise induced by P. vulgaris dry extract. A previous study (Fantini et al., 2009) demonstrated indeed that doses up to 500 mg/kg of this P. vulgaris dry extract failed to affect, even minimally, spontaneous locomotor activity in Wistar rats exposed to an open-field arena (a parameter highly sensitive to alterations in the state of well-being of rats).

Extracts from P. vulgaris are thought to exert their anorectic effects via two different mechanisms (both mediated by the active ingredients, lectins): (1) inhibition of the pancreatic enzyme, α-amylase, resulting in (a) suppression of starch metabolism and, in turn, decrease in glycemia and (b) delay of gastric emptying and, in turn, promotion of feelings of satiety (Jain et al., 1989, 1991; Tormo et al., 2004, 2006; for review, see Astell et al., 2013); (2) phytohemoagglutinin-stimulated release from the gut epithelial cells of the gastroenteric hormones, cholecystokinin (CCK), and glucagone-like peptide, known to contribute to the central regulation of appetite, satiety, food consumption, and hedonic eating (Carai et al., 2009; for review, see Astell et al., 2013). Since lever-responding was completely unreinforced in the reinstatement session, and no chocolate-flavored beverage was made available and ingested, the possibility that reduction in lever-responding induced by P. vulgaris dry extract during the reinstatement session was secondary to any inhibition of carbohydrate metabolism can reasonably be ruled out. Conversely, it is most likely that suppression of reinstatement of seeking behavior for the chocolate-flavored beverage was mediated by some central mechanism(s) regulating the rewarding and hedonic properties of food. Taking into account its role in the reinstatement of seeking behavior for cocaine (Lu et al., 2002) and morphine (Wen et al., 2013), the CCK system is a likely mediator of this action. Alternatively to lectins, a currently undisclosed ingredient of the P. vulgaris dry extract might be responsible for its central effects. Future studies will challenge this hypothesis.

Conclusion

The results of the present study indicate that acute treatment with a standardized extract of beans from the functional food, P. vulgaris, prevented reinstatement of seeking behavior for a highly hedonic chocolate-flavored beverage in rats. These data extend to a relapse-like behavior the capability of this P. vulgaris dry extract to ameliorate several behaviors motivated by food (including palatable food) in laboratory rodents.

Author Contributions

IL, GC, GG, and PMa conceived the study and designed the experiment. GC and PMa set up the experimental procedure. MC managed the literature search and summaries of previous related work. AR and PMo prepared and analyzed the plant extract. IL, VP, and PMa conducted the experiment. IL, MC, and PMa analyzed the data. GC, GG, and PMa drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to and approved the final draft of the manuscript.

Funding

The present study was funded by Italian National Research Council [Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche (CNR)] and Indena S.p.A., Milan, Italy.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

The authors are grateful to Mrs. Carla Acciaro for animal care and Ms. Anne Farmer for language editing of the manuscript.

References

Astell, K. J., Mathai, M. L., and Su, X. Q. (2013). A review on botanical species and chemical compounds with appetite suppressing properties for body weight control. Plant Foods Hum. Nutr. 68, 213–221. doi: 10.1007/s11130-013-0361-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bardocz, S., Grant, G., and Pusztai, A. (1996). The effect of phytoaemagglutinin at different dietary concentrations on the growth, body composition and plasma insulin of the rat. Br. J. Nutr. 76, 613–626. doi: 10.1079/BJN19960067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boivin, M., Zinsmeister, A. R., Go, V. L., and DiMagno, E. P. (1987). Effect of a purified amylase inhibitor on carbohydrate metabolism after a mixed meal in healthy humans. Mayo Clin. Proc. 62, 249–255. doi: 10.1016/S0025-6196(12)61900-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bossert, J. M., Marchant, N. J., Calu, D. J., and Shaham, Y. (2013). The reinstatement model of drug relapse: recent neurobiological findings, emerging research topics, and translational research. Psychopharmacology 229, 453–476. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3120-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calu, D. J., Chen, Y. W., Kawa, A. B., Nair, S. G., and Shaham, Y. (2014). The use of the reinstatement model to study relapse to palatable food seeking during dieting. Neuropharmacology 76, 395–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carai, M. A. M., Colombo, G., Maccioni, P., and Gessa, G. L. (2006). Efficacy of rimonabant and other cannabinoid CB1 receptor antagonists in reducing food intake and body weight: preclinical and clinical data. CNS Drug Rev. 12, 91–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1527-3458.2006.00091.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carai, M. A. M., Fantini, N., Loi, B., Colombo, G., Gessa, G. L., Riva, A., et al. (2011). Multiple cycles of repeated treatments with a Phaseolus vulgaris dry extract reduce food intake and body weight in obese rats. Br. J. Nutr. 106, 762–768. doi: 10.1017/S0007114511000778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carai, M. A. M., Fantini, N., Loi, B., Colombo, G., Riva, A., and Morazzoni, P. (2009). Potential efficacy of preparations deriving from Phaseolus vulgaris in the control of appetite, energy intake, and carbohydrate metabolism. Diabetes Metab. Syndr. Obes. Targ. Ther. 2, 145–153. doi: 10.2147/DMSOTT.S4236

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Celleno, L., Tolaini, M. V., D’Amore, A., Perricone, N. V., and Preuss, H. G. (2007). A dietary supplement containing standardized Phaseolus vulgaris extract influences body composition of overweight men and women. Int. J. Med. Sci. 4, 45–52. doi: 10.7150/ijms.4.45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fantini, N., Cabras, C., Lobina, C., Colombo, G., Gessa, G. L., Riva, A., et al. (2009). Reducing effect of a Phaseolus vulgaris dry extract on food intake, body weight, and glycemia in rats. J. Agric. Food Chem. 57, 9316–9323. doi: 10.1021/jf900711z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, G., Dorward, P. M., Buchan, W. C., Armour, J. C., and Pusztai, A. (1995). Consumption of diets containing raw soya beans (Glycine max), kidney beans (Phaseolus vulgaris), cowpeas (Vigna unguiculata) or lupin seeds (Lupinus angustifolius) by rats for up to 700 days: effects on body composition and organ weights. Br. J. Nutr. 73, 17–29. doi: 10.1079/BJN19950005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jain, N. K., Boivin, M., Zinsmeister, A. R., Brown, M. L., Malagelada, J. R., and DiMagno, E. P. (1989). Effect of ileal perfusion of carbohydrates and amylase inhibitor on gastrointestinal hormones and emptying. Gastroenterology 96, 377–387.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jain, N. K., Boivin, M., Zinsmeister, A. R., Brown, M. L., Malagelada, J. R., and DiMagno, E. P. (1991). The ileum and carbohydrate-mediated feedback regulation of postprandial pancreatico-biliary secretion in normal humans. Pancreas 6, 495–505. doi: 10.1097/00006676-199109000-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Layer, P., Carlson, G. L., and DiMagno, E. P. (1985). Partially purified white bean amylase inhibitor reduces starch digestion in vitro and inactivates intraduodenal amylase in humans. Gastroenterology 88, 1895–1902.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Leite, C. E., Mocelin, C. A., Petersen, G. O., Leal, M. B., and Thiesen, F. V. (2009). Rimonabant: an antagonist drug of the endocannabinoid system for the treatment of obesity. Pharmacol. Rep. 61, 217–224. doi: 10.1016/S1734-1140(09)70025-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loi, B., Fantini, N., Colombo, G., Gessa, G. L., Riva, A., Bombardelli, E., et al. (2013). Reducing effect of an extract of Phaseolus vulgaris on food intake in mice – Focus on highly palatable foods. Fitoterapia 85, 14–19. doi: 10.1016/j.fitote.2012.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, L., Zhang, B., Liu, Z., and Zhang, Z. (2002). Reactivation of cocaine conditioned place preference induced by stress is reversed by cholecystochinin-B receptors antagonist in rats. Brain Res. 954, 132–140. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)03359-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maccioni, P., and Colombo, G. (2016). “Operant self-administration of chocolate in rats: an addiction-like behavior,” in In Vivo Neuropharmacology and Neurophysiology, ed. A. Philippou (New York, NY: Springer).

Maccioni, P., Colombo, G., Riva, A., Morazzoni, P., Bombardelli, E., Gessa, G. L., et al. (2010). Reducing effect of a Phaseolus vulgaris dry extract on operant self-administration of a chocolate-flavoured beverage in rats. Br. J. Nutr. 104, 624–628. doi: 10.1017/S0007114510001017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maccioni, P., Pes, D., Carai, M. A. M., Gessa, G. L., and Colombo, G. (2008). Suppression by the cannabinoid CB1 receptor antagonist, rimonabant, of the reinforcing and motivational properties of a chocolate-flavoured beverage in rats. Behav. Pharmacol. 19, 197–209. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3282fe8888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markou, A., Weiss, F., Gold, L. H., Caine, S. B., Schulteis, G., and Koob, G. F. (1993). Animal models of drug craving. Psychopharmacology 112, 163–182. doi: 10.1007/BF02244907

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martín-García, E., Burokas, A., Kostrzewa, E., Gieryk, A., Korostynski, M., Ziolkowska, B., et al. (2011). New operant model of reinstatement of food-seeking behavior in mice. Psychopharmacology 215, 49–70. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2110-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obiro, W. C., Zhang, T., and Jiang, B. (2008). The nutraceutical role of the Phaseolus vulgaris alpha-amylase inhibitor. Br. J. Nutr. 100, 1–12. doi: 10.1017/S0007114508879135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onakpoya, I., Aldaas, S., Terry, R., and Ernst, E. (2011). The efficacy of Phaseolus vulgaris as a weight-loss supplement: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised clinical trials. Br. J. Nutr. 106, 196–202. doi: 10.1017/S0007114511001516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pusztai, A., Bardocz, S., and Ewen, S. W. B. (2008). Uses of plant lectins in bioscience and biomedicine. Front. Biosci. 13:1130–1140. doi: 10.2741/2750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pusztai, A., Grant, G., Buchan, W. C., Bardocz, S., de Carvalho, A. F., and Ewen, S. W. (1998). Lipid accumulation in obese Zucker rats is reduced by inclusion of raw kidney bean (Phaseolus vulgaris) in the diet. Br. J. Nutr. 79, 213–221. doi: 10.1079/BJN19980033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pusztai, A., Grant, G., Duguid, T., Brown, D. S., Peumans, W. J., Van Damme, E. J., et al. (1995). Inhibition of starch digestion by α-amylase inhibitor reduces the efficiency of utilization of dietary proteins and lipids and retards the growth of rats. J. Nutr. 125, 1554–1562.

Google Scholar

Rondanelli, M., Giacosa, A., Orsini, F., Opizzi, A., and Villani, S. (2011). Appetite control and glycaemia reduction in overweight subjects treated with a combination of two highly standardized extracts from Phaseolus vulgaris and Cynara scolymus. Phytother. Res. 25, 1275–1282. doi: 10.1002/ptr.3425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherma, M., Fattore, L., Castelli, M. P., Fratta, W., and Fadda, F. (2014). The role of the endocannabinoid system in eating disorders: neurochemical and behavioural preclinical evidence. Curr. Pharm. Des. 20, 2089–2099. doi: 10.2174/13816128113199990429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spadafranca, A., Rinelli, S., Riva, A., Morazzoni, P., Magni, P., Bertoli, S., et al. (2013). Phaseolus vulgaris extract affects glycometabolic and appetite control in healthy human subjects. Br. J. Nutr. 109, 1789–1795. doi: 10.1017/S0007114512003741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tormo, M. A., Gil-Exojo, I., Romero de Tejada, A., and Campillo, J. E. (2004). Hypoglycaemic and anorexigenic activities of an α-amylase inhibitor from white kidney beans (Phaseolus vulgaris) in Wistar rats. Br. J. Nutr. 92, 785–790. doi: 10.1079/BJN20041260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tormo, M. A., Gil-Exojo, I., Romero de Tejada, A., and Campillo, J. E. (2006). White bean amylase inhibitor administered orally reduces glycaemia in type 2 diabetic rats. Br. J. Nutr. 96, 539–544.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Udani, J., Hardy, M., and Madsen, D. C. (2004). Blocking carbohydrate absorption and weight loss: a clinical trial using Phase 2 brand proprietary fractionated white bean extract. Altern. Med. Rev. 9, 63–69.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Udani, J., and Singh, B. B. (2007). Blocking carbohydrate absorption and weight loss: a clinical trial using a proprietary fractionated white bean extract. Altern. Ther. Health Med. 13, 32–37.

Google Scholar

Wen, D., Zang, G., Sun, D., Yang, S., Yu, F., Li, S., et al. (2013). Effects of CCK-8 on the reinstatement of morphine-induced CPP and expression of behavioural sensitization in rats. Neuroscience 238, 230–241. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.02.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaru, A., Maccioni, P., Colombo, G., and Gessa, G. L. (2013a). The dopamine β-hydroxylase inhibitor, nepicastat, suppresses chocolate self-administration and reinstatement of chocolate seeking in rats. Br. J. Nutr. 110, 1524–1533. doi: 10.1017/S0007114513000743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaru, A., Maccioni, P., Riva, A., Morazzoni, P., Bombardelli, E., Gessa, G. L., et al. (2013b). Reducing effect of a combination of Phaseolus vulgaris and Cynara scolymus extracts on operant self-administration of a chocolate-flavoured beverage in rats. Phytother. Res. 27, 944–947. doi: 10.1002/ptr.4814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Phaseolus vulgaris dry extract (Beanblock®), chocolate-flavored beverage, reinstatement of seeking behavior, relapse-like behavior, rats

Citation: Lorrai I, Piga V, Carai MAM, Riva A, Morazzoni P, Gessa GL, Colombo G and Maccioni P (2016) A Phaseolus vulgaris Extract Reduces Cue-Induced Reinstatement of Chocolate Seeking in Rats. Front. Pharmacol. 7:109. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2016.00109

Received: 16 February 2016; Accepted: 11 April 2016;
Published: 26 April 2016.

Edited by:

Adolfo Andrade-Cetto, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Keliang Xie, General Hospital of Tianjin Medical University, China
Fang-Rong Chang, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan

Copyright © 2016 Lorrai, Piga, Carai, Riva, Morazzoni, Gessa, Colombo and Maccioni. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giancarlo Colombo, colomb@unica.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.