Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Physiol., 01 December 2016
Sec. Exercise Physiology

Brief Rewarming Blunts Hypothermia-Induced Alterations in Sensation, Motor Drive and Cognition

\r\nMarius Brazaitis*Marius Brazaitis1*Henrikas PaulauskasHenrikas Paulauskas1Albertas SkurvydasAlbertas Skurvydas1Henning Budde,,Henning Budde1,2,3Laura DaniuseviciuteLaura Daniuseviciute4Nerijus EimantasNerijus Eimantas1
  • 1Institute of Sport Science and Innovations, Lithuanian Sports University, Kaunas, Lithuania
  • 2Faculty of Human Sciences, Medical School Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
  • 3Sports Science Department, Reykjavik University, Reykjavik, Iceland
  • 4Faculty of Social Sciences, Arts and Humanities, Kaunas University of Technology, Kaunas, Lithuania

Background: It is well known that cold exposure experienced during occupational or recreational activities may adversely affect motor, cognitive performance, and health. Most research has used prolonged passive external rewarming modalities and focused on the direct effects on the kinetics of physiological and psychological responses in hypothermic subjects. However, the brief whole body rewarming effects on physiological and psychological responses in parallel with functional consequences on cognitive and neurophysiological functions have not been investigated. This study explores these effects in 12 healthy young men.

Methods: Subjects (20 ± 1 years) participated in 4 randomized trials, which were designed to compare the effects of whole-body brief (5-min) rewarming in 37°C water with rewarming for the same duration in 24°C (air) thermoneutral environment in mildly hypothermic subjects. After each rewarming, indicators of neuromuscular function (reflexes, central activation ratio, electromyography of exercising muscle, and contractile properties of calf muscles) and cognitive function (attention, simple motor speed, and information processing speed) were assessed.

Results: Compared to rewarming in thermoneutral environment, after brief rewarming in 37°C water, significantly lower metabolic heat production (MHP) (206 ± 33.4 vs. 121.9 ± 24.3 W·m2, P < 0.01), heart rate (76 ± 16 vs. 60 ± 12 b·min−1, P < 0.01), cold strain (6.4 ± 3.1 vs. 5.3 ± 2.7, P < 0.01), improved thermal comfort and induced cessation of shivering were found. Electrically induced maximum torque amplitudes increased (P100, 102.8 ± 21.3 vs. 109.2 ± 17.5 Nm and PTT100, 83.1 ± 17.1 vs. 92.7 ± 16.0 Nm, P < 0.05), contraction half-relaxation time decreased (599.0 ± 53.8 vs. 589.0 ± 56.3 ms, P < 0.05), and Mmax-wave latency shortened (17.5 ± 2.2 vs. 15.6 ± 2.0 ms, P < 0.05) after 37°C water rewarming. Unlike rewarming in thermoneutral environment, 37°C water rewarming blunted the hypothermia-induced alterations in neural drive transmission (4.3 ± 0.5 vs. 3.4 ± 0.8 mV H-reflex and 4.9 ± 0.2 vs. 4.4 ± 0.4 mV V-wave, P < 0.05), which increased central fatigue during a 2-min maximum load (P < 0.05). Furthermore, only in brief warm water rewarming cerebral alterations were restored to the control level and it was indicated by shortened reaction times (P < 0.05).

Conclusions: Brief rewarming in warm water rather than the same duration rewarming in thermoneutral environment blunted the hypothermia-induced alterations for sensation, motor drive, and cognition, despite the fact that rectal and deep muscle temperature remained lowered.

Introduction

Prolonged exposure to a severe cold environment causes marked whole-body cooling, defined as a decrease in core temperature, which can impair motor and cognitive performance by altering neural drive through central and peripheral failure (Giesbrecht et al., 1995; Rutkove et al., 1997; Cahill et al., 2011; Brazaitis et al., 2014b; Solianik et al., 2014). Even exposure to less severe cold, which does not lower core temperature markedly, may produce cognitive (Palinkas, 2001; Mäkinen et al., 2006; Muller et al., 2012) and physical (Drinkwater and Behm, 2007) decrements, which may adversely affect performance and health (Tanaka et al., 1993; Palinkas, 2001). Brief periodic rewarming breaks throughout the day are recommended for restoring or preventing impairments in physiological and psychological performance for people exposed to the cold in occupational or leisure activities (Ceron et al., 1995; Ozaki et al., 1998; Brown et al., 2012). For example, in a protocol involving 3 sets of alternating 20-min exposure to −25°C in an industrial freezer with 20-min rewarming at 30°C or rest at 10°C, the protocol involving rewarming had a more positive effect than rest at 10°C on body temperature, hand tremor, counting task performance, and thermal and pain sensations. However, the periodic rewarming at 30°C only partially prevented the large decrease in core temperature, and this rewarming protocol could not restore core temperature to the initial level (Ozaki et al., 1998). Most research has used prolonged passive external rewarming lasting from 30 min to several hours using a water bath, air inhalation, or forced air. These studies have focused on the direct effects of rewarming from hypothermia on the kinetics of physiological responses including body temperature (Hoskin et al., 1986; Kumar et al., 2015), cardiovascular parameters (Hayward et al., 1984; Savard et al., 1985), subjective sensation (Kumar et al., 2015), metabolic heat production (MHP), and heat gain (Goheen et al., 1997). The functional consequences of applying brief (5-min) whole-body rewarming on cognitive and neurophysiological functions in mildly hypothermic subjects have not been investigated.

In the laboratory setting, spontaneous endogenous rewarming from hypothermia or cold is performed by placing the subject in a thermoneutral ambient temperature of 21–25°C (with or without external cover), which is insufficient to inhibit cold and/or excite warm receptors and therefore induce a shift in thermogenesis until recovery (Romet, 1988; Giesbrecht and Bristow, 1992; Muller et al., 2012). In our study, we used a water temperature of 37°C (Tw) for whole-body immersion of hypothermic subjects for 5 min to increase skin temperature (Tsk) and to evoke alterations in peripheral thermo-TRP channels without allowing recovery of deep body temperature. Based on previous findings (Craig, 2003; Nakamura and Morrison, 2008; Vay et al., 2012) we reasoned that greater inhibition of cold-induced thermogenesis and cutaneous vasoconstriction in mildly hypothermic subjects are likely to occur during brief (5-min) whole-body rewarming at 37°C Tw rather than during rewarming for the same duration at thermoneutral 24°C (Ta). Through the actions of warm-sensitive receptors during warm water immersion, the lamina I neurons carry temperature signals to the insular cortex, which is involved in discriminative temperature sensation (Craig, 2002, 2003); consequently, a hypothermic subject may feel warm and thermally comfortable despite a reduced rectal temperature (Tre). We expected that a reduction in the physiological cold stress [assessed by cold strain index (CSI), (Moran et al., 1999)] and discomfort induced by Tw rewarming in hypothermic subjects may have a positive effect on cognitive performance because of the reduced amount of distraction (Teichner, 1958).

It has been proposed that ambient temperature and relative humidity can influence central drive through changes in Tsk or through integration of anticipatory and/or sensory feedback mechanisms (Cheung and Sleivert, 2004; Schlader et al., 2011; Levels et al., 2012). The spinal cord is a major site for modulation of motor drive (Racinais et al., 2008). Opposing responses of spinal modulation of neural drive have been observed for hyperthermia (Racinais et al., 2008; Brazaitis et al., 2015) and hypothermia (Brazaitis et al., 2014b; Solianik et al., 2014). Because this modulation relates partly to presynaptic inhibition or activation mediated by temperature-sensitive group III and IV afferents (Bigland-Ritchie et al., 1986; Duchateau et al., 2002; Avela et al., 2006), spinal modulation of neural drive is likely to be altered by brief Tw rewarming in mildly hypothermic subjects. If so, one would expect that increased conduction velocity induced by brief Tw rewarming in hypothermic subjects would require greater energetic resources for a sustained 2-min MVC by providing greater inhibitory feedback to central structures, which may contribute to greater central fatigue (Bigland-Ritchie and Woods, 1984).

Therefore, the main purpose of our study was to determinate whether brief rewarming in warm water is sufficient to negate hypothermia-induced central changes [i.e., central activation ratio (CAR), mean frequency (MnF), and root mean square (RMS) of the electromyography (EMG) signal, cognition, sensation, H-reflex, V-wave, and shift in thermogenesis] and peripheral changes (i.e., contractile properties of calf muscles and the M-wave). We hypothesized that brief (5-min) whole-body rewarming in Tw 37°C would blunt the hypothermia-induced alterations in thermoregulation, sensation, motor drive, and cognition, despite the lowered Tre and deep muscle temperature (Tmu).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twelve healthy male volunteers participated in the study. Each participant read and signed a written informed consent form. Subjects with Raynaud's syndrome, asthma, neurological pathology, or conditions that could be worsened by exposure to cold water were excluded from this study. All participants were in self-reported good health, which was confirmed by medical history. The physical characteristics of the participants are presented in Table 1. Participants were moderately physically active (<3 times per week) and did not participate in any formal physical exercise or sports programme. The additional criteria for inclusion were age 18–30 years, being a non-smoker, and not taking medication or dietary supplements. All procedures were approved by LUHS Kaunas Region Biomedical Research Ethics Committee and were conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Physical characteristics of the subjects.

Experimental Design

Rationale for the Experiment

The experiment was designed to induce whole-body mild hypothermia in young healthy men to induce alterations in thermoregulation, subjective sensation, motor drive, and cognition (Brazaitis et al., 2014b; Solianik et al., 2015b) and thereafter to compare the effects of whole-body brief (5-min) rewarming in warm water (Tw 37°C) (CL-HT trial) with those induced by rewarming for the same duration in a thermoneutral environment (Ta 24°C) (CL trial) on physiological and psychological variables (Figure 1A). To determine whether whole-body exposure to warm water under normothermia induces any physiological and psychological changes, normothermic subjects were immersed for 5 min in warm water (Tw 37°C) (HT trial), and the same variables were measured. In the control trial (CON), we assessed the same variables under normothermia without causing any changes in body temperatures (Tsk, Tre, Tmu), and these variables were compared with those measured in the CL, CL-HT and HT trials.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental design (A). RM, resting measure; HT, 5-min warming in Tw; TN, thermoneutral environment (Ta); T, testing; T+E, testing with the 2-min MVC exercise. Protocols for the experimental trials (B). Heart rate (HR), body temperatures (rectal, Tre; muscle, Tmu; skin, Tsk); cognition testing (simple, 2-choice and procedural reactions); sarcolemmal (M-wave), spinal (H-wave), and supraspinal (V-wave) excitability; effects of electrical stimulation [EL; i.e., 1, 20, 100 Hz and TT-100 Hz (a 250-ms test train of stimuli at 100 Hz)] on relaxed muscle indicate changes in peripheral capacity; maximum voluntary contraction (MVC) indicates changes in both central and peripheral capacity; central activation ratio was measured by TT-100 Hz superimposed on a 2-min MVC and indicates changes in central capacity.

Familiarization Trial

The subjects were familiarized with the procedures over 3 days. One week before the experiment, participants were introduced to the experimental procedures and cognitive and neuromuscular testing, and informed of the known risks of the study. On each day, the subjects performed the cognitive test battery, and on day 3 they learned to achieve and maintain a maximum effort in plantar flexion for 3–4 s with a 250-ms stimulation train test at 100 Hz (TT-100 Hz) superimposed on an MVC, and their tolerance to electrical stimulation was assessed. Participants attended the laboratory at the same time of day within the time frame of 8:00–13:00 h to avoid circadian fluctuations in body temperature. They were instructed to sleep for a minimum 7–8 h on the night before the experiment; to refrain from alcohol, heavy exercise, and caffeine for at least 24 h; and to refrain from consuming any food for at least 12 h before arrival at the laboratory. To standardize the morning state of hydration, subjects were allowed to drink still water as desired until 60 min before the experiment. The study was performed at a room temperature of 24°C and 60% relative humidity.

CON Trial

On arrival at the laboratory, the subject was asked to rest in a semi-recumbent posture for 10 min dressed in a T-shirt, swim shorts and socks. Resting pulmonary gas exchange was recorded for the next 20 min in the same semi-recumbent posture. Heart rate (HR), Tsk, Tmu, and Tre stabilization were assessed and recorded (Figure 1B). Within ~3 min after these resting measurements, the participant was seated at a table and performed the cognitive testing (see “Cognitive performance”). Upon completion of the cognitive test battery, the subject was positioned in the dynamometer chair, stimulating electrodes were placed over the tibial nerve, EMG electrodes were placed over the soleus (SOL) muscle of the right leg, and reflexes were assessed. After a 2-min rest, during which the electrodes were moved from over the tibial nerve to over the posterior calf muscle, the force-generating capacity of the posterior calf muscle was assessed by applying 1-s trains of electrical stimuli at P1, P20, PTT-100 Hz, and P100 Hz (see “Force-generating capacity of posterior calf muscles”). About 3 s was needed to change the stimulation frequency. After a 2-min rest, a 2-min isometric MVC was then performed. The PTT-100 stimulus was superimposed on the contraction at about 3 s and 120 s. The measurement at 3 s was repeated after the end of the 2-min MVC to measure involuntary force-generating capacity by imposing 1-s trains of electrical stimuli at P1, P20, PTT-100, and P100. In total, cognitive and neuromuscular testing procedures (T+E) took approximately 16 min (Figure 1B).

HT Trial

Pulmonary gas exchange, HR, body temperature, cognitive performance, reflexes, and involuntary force-generating capacity were measured as described in the CON trial. After muscle testing, the subject began a brief (5-min) head-out immersion trial in warm water (Tw 37°C). During immersion, the subject remained in a semi-recumbent posture with his arms folded across the chest and the legs extended almost straight and together. Pulmonary gas exchange was recorded during the 5-min water immersion. Ratings of subjective perception were recorded at the end of the 5-min warming procedure. Within ~1 min after leaving the bath, the volunteer was towel dried and body temperatures were measured. After the end of the warming procedure, cognitive and neuromuscular testing was performed in the same order as before the warm water immersion. Thereafter, 2-min isometric MVC was assessed followed by assessment of involuntary force-generating capacity, as described above for the CON trial.

CL and CL-HT Trials

Baseline measurements were made as described for the CON and HT trials. After muscle testing, the subject began the intermittent water-immersion cooling protocol as described in previous studies (Brazaitis et al., 2014a; Solianik et al., 2015a). Every 20 min during cooling, the subject was asked to step out of the bath and to rest for 10 min in the room environment and to then return to the water bath for the next 20 min of immersion. The water bath temperature was 14°C and the head-out immersion procedure was used. The procedure continued until Tre decreased to 35.5°C or until 170 min total (120 min maximum total immersion time), at which time the immersion ended regardless of Tre. The exposure time until Tre achievement was recorded. During immersion and rest, the subject remained in a semi-recumbent posture with his arms folded across the chest and the legs extended almost straight and together. Ratings of subjective thermal and shivering sensation, and HR and Tre were recorded every 5 min throughout the cooling procedure. Pulmonary gas exchange was recorded only during each 20-min water immersion. Immediately after intermittent cooling, the subject was towel dried, and the body temperatures were measured.

In the CL-HT trial, within the 5 min needed to measure body temperatures and to change the bath water from cold to warm, the subject began brief (5-min) head-out rewarming in warm water (Tw 37°C) as described for the HT trial. In the CL trial, within the 5 min needed to measure body temperature, the subject was asked to rest in a semi-recumbent posture for 5-min rewarming at the thermoneutral Ta 24°C. In total, the time from the moment the subjects exited the cold water bath to the end of rewarming was 10 min and it was of the same duration in both CL and CL-HT trials. During these two brief rewarming procedures, pulmonary gas exchange and HR were recorded. Immediately after the Tw or Ta brief rewarming procedure, body temperatures were measured. Thereafter, cognitive and neuromuscular testing was performed in the same order as before the intermittent cold water immersion and ended with the 2-min isometric MVC followed by measurement of involuntary force-generating capacity, as described above for the CON trial.

Experimental Measurements

Anthropometric Measurements

Body mass and body fat were measured, and body mass index was calculated. Body mass was measured as nude body mass on a body composition analyser (Tanita, TBF-300, Arlington Heights, IL, USA). Body surface area was estimated in m2 as = 128.1 × weight 0.44 × height 0.60 (Tikuisis et al., 2001). Skinfold thickness was calculated as the average thickness for 10 skinfold sites (chin, subscapular, chest, side, suprailium, abdomen, triceps, thigh, knee and calf) (McArdle et al., 1984) using a medical skinfold caliper (Seahan, SH5020, Masan, Korea).

Body Temperature Measurements

Tre was measured at rest and throughout each immersion procedure using a thermocouple (Rectal Probe; Ellab, Hvidovre, Denmark; accuracy ± 0.01°C) inserted to a depth of 12 cm past the anal sphincter. The rectal thermistor sensor was placed by each participant. The temperature measurements were taken before (all four trials) and after cooling (CL and CL-HT trials) or warm water (HT trial) immersion, and after brief rewarming (CL and CL-HT trials). Tsk was measured (Skin/Surface probe, DM852, Ellab; accuracy ± 0.01°C) with thermistors taped at three sites: back, thigh and forearm. The mean Tsk was calculated using the Burton (1935) equation: Tsk = 0.5back + 0.36thigh + 0.14forearm. Tmu was measured with a needle microprobe (MKA; Ellab) inserted at a depth of ~3.5 cm under the skin covering the lateral portion of the gastrocnemius in the right leg. For skin preparation before each intramuscular temperature measurement, the skin was shaved and disinfected before and after insertion of the microprobe using a cotton wool tampon soaked with medicinal alcohol. No local anesthesia was administered before insertion. After the first measurement, the insertion area was marked with a 0.5-cm-diameter circle to ensure that the same insertion point was used in later measurements before and after water immersion and rewarming in different trials.

CSI

As a rating of cold strain, the CSI was based on Tre and mean Tsk, and was rated using a universal scale of 0 to 10 as follows: 1–2 (no or a little cold strain), 3–4 (low cold strain), 5–6 (moderate cold strain), 7–8 (high cold strain), and 9–10 (very high cold strain). The CSI was calculated as follows (Moran et al., 1999).

CSI = 6.67 (Tret - Tre0) × (35 - Tre0)-1              + 3.33 (Tskt - Tsk0) × (14 - Tsk0)-1

The measurements for CSI calculation were taken before (Tre0, Tsk0) and at the end (Tret, Tskt) of cold water immersion or at the end of brief rewarming; 14°C—water temperature; 35°C—Tre threshold.

Heart Rate Measurement

HR was measured throughout each trial with an HR monitor (S-625X, Polar Electro, Kempele, Finland). HR was recorded before (all four trials) and after cooling (CL and CL-HT trials) or warm water (HT trial) immersion and at the end of the brief rewarming (CL and CL-HT trials).

Spirometry

A mobile spirometry system (Oxycon Mobile, Jaeger/VIASYS Healthcare, Hoechberg, Germany) was used to measure pulmonary gas exchange at rest and during water immersion and rewarming. The system was used to monitor ventilatory parameters such as oxygen uptake (VO2) and CO2 production on a breath-by-breath basis. Calibration of this instrument was performed before recording according to the manufacturer's manual using the automatic volume- and gas-calibration functions. The gas analyser and delay time calibration were automatic as provided by the manufacturer: a calibration gas at 180 kPa (15.2% O2, 5.02% CO2, and 79.62% N2) was introduced to the Oxycon to attain gain, offset, and delay times within 1%. Oxygen consumption was recorded in 5-s increments. The first minute of data collected during each 20-min interval of intermittent cold water immersion and the first minute of data collected during 5-min warm water immersion or rewarming were not used in any calculations, as recommended by Tikuisis et al. (2000) because of reflex hyperventilation caused by cold or warm water immersion. MHP (in watts) was calculated from the respiratory gas exchange measurements of VO2 (in l/min) and the respiratory exchange ratio (RER = VCO2/VO2) according to the equation of Péronnet and Massicotte (1991) as MHP + (281.65 + 80.65 × RER) × VO2.

Measurement of Perception

The method to measure subjective ratings for the whole body has been described elsewhere (Brazaitis et al., 2010, 2014b). Briefly, ratings of thermal sensation ranged from 1 (very cold) to 9 (very hot), with 5 being neutral. Thermal comfort sensation ranged from 1 (comfortable) to 5 (extremely uncomfortable), and the shivering/sweating sensation ranged from 1 (vigorously shivering) to 7 (heavily sweating), with 4 (not at all) being neutral. The rating of perception was reported by the participant every 5 min during immersion and at the end of Ta or Tw rewarming. The mean rating score value per immersion and rewarming was calculated.

Cognitive Performance

To assess cognitive performance, the Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metric (ANAM4, Norman, OK, USA) was administered (Reeves et al., 2010). A programmed cognitive test battery was used to assess changes in simple reaction, 2-choice reaction and procedural reaction times after the different immersion and/or rewarming procedures (see “Research Design”). All tasks were computer controlled, and the information was presented on the screen of a laptop (Samsung R538). All tests were performed in a quiet and semi-darkened laboratory with a laptop screen 40 cm in front of the participant. The participant was introduced to the test battery during the familiarization before the experiments began. The duration of the cognitive test battery was 3.0 ± 0.5 min and included the following tasks, which were randomized between experimental trials.

Simple reaction time test

This task measures simple visuomotor mental flexibility (Mäkinen et al., 2006; Kadota and Gomi, 2010). The test presents a simple stimulus with a symbol of “*” on the screen. The participant is instructed to press a specified response key as quickly as possible each time the stimulus is present. The accuracy for this task was 100% for all participants, and only response time was recorded.

2-choice reaction time test

This task measures processing speed (Tzambazis and Stough, 2000) and alternating attention with a motor speed component (Kubicki et al., 2012). This test presents the user with a 2-choice symbol “*” or “o” on the display. The user is instructed to respond as quickly as possible by pressing the designated button for each stimulus as soon as the stimulus appears. The accuracy on this task was not less than 96% for all participants and did not differ significantly between trials, and only the response time was taken for analyses.

Procedural reaction time test

This test measures information processing speed, visuomotor reaction time, and attention (Churchill et al., 2016). A number (2, 3, 4, or 5) is presented on the display using a large dot matrix. The user is instructed to press 1 designated button for a “low” number (2 or 3) and another designated button for a “high” number (4 or 5). The accuracy on this task was not less than 94% for all participants and did not differ significantly between trials, and only the response time was taken for analyses.

Reflexes Recordings

The participant was positioned in the dynamometer chair, stimulating electrodes were placed over the tibial nerve, EMG electrodes were placed over the SOL of the right leg, and the reflexes were assessed. After careful preparation of the skin (shaving, abrading, and cleaning with alcohol wipes) to obtain low impedance, bipolar Ag–AgCl surface bar electrodes (10-mm diameter, 20-mm center-to-center distance; DataLOG type no. P3X8 USB, Biometrics, Newport, UK) were used for EMG recording. For the SOL, the electrode was placed over the SOL ~13 cm above the calcaneus and below the muscle fibers of the gastrocnemius. The actual electrode position was marked, and the same recording site was used in the different trial measurements. The ground electrode was positioned on the tarsus of the same leg. EMG signals were recorded with an amplifier (gain 1000), with signal measurements using a third-order filter (18 dB/octave) bandwidth of 20–460 Hz. The analog signal was sampled and converted to digital form at a sampling frequency of 5 kHz. The EMG signal was telemetered to a receiver that contained a differential amplifier with an input impedance of 10 MΩ, the input noise level was less than 5 mV, and the common mode rejection ratio was higher than 96 dB. Before recording the EMG, we set the channel sensitivity at 3 V and excitation output at 4600 mV as recommended by the manufacturer. EMG files were stored simultaneously on the biometrics memory card and PC hardware, and dedicated software (Biometrics DataLOG) was used for data processing and analysis.

SOL H-reflexes, V-waves, and M-waves were evoked by 0.5-ms square-wave pulses stimulated by a cathode placed in the popliteal cavity and an anode placed distal to the patella over the posterior tibial nerve with an interelectrode distance of ~4 cm. The resting maximum H-reflex (Hmax), which reflects the efficiency of transmission in Ia afferent motor neuron synapses, and maximum M-wave (Mmax), which reflects sarcolemmal excitability, were obtained by increasing the electrical intensity by 3 V every 10 s over the 30–150 V range. With increasing stimulation intensity, the H-reflex response initially increased progressively and then decreased and disappeared, whereas the M-wave achieved its maximum and remained stable. Thereafter, the participant was instructed to perform three brief MVCs of the plantar flexor muscles for 3–4 s with at least a 1-min rest between contractions. A superimposed stimulus (at Mmax intensity) was evoked to obtain the V-wave (Vsup). The peak-to-peak amplitude of the V-wave reflects the magnitude of the central descending neural drive to spinal motor neurons, although spinal factors such as motor neuron excitability and pre- or post-synaptic inhibition may also be involved (Aagaard et al., 2002). The M-wave amplitude was also used to normalize the amplitude of the reflex waves recorded (i.e., Hmax/Mmax ratio). This was done to ensure that any changes in the evoked Hmax and Vsup amplitudes reflected changes at the muscle fiber membrane or neuromuscular junction. The latencies of the electrically evoked action potentials were calculated from the stimulation artifact at the peak of the wave.

Force-Generating Capacity of Posterior Calf Muscles

The force-generating capacity of the posterior calf muscles was assessed before and immediately after the 2-min MVC. The equipment and procedure for electrically stimulated torque have been described previously (Brazaitis et al., 2012). Briefly, muscle stimulation was applied using flexible surface electrodes (MARP Electronic, Krakow, Poland), covered with a thin layer of electrode gel (ECG–EEG Gel; Medigel, Modi'in, Israel), with one electrode (8 × 12 cm) placed transversely across the width of the proximal portion of the posterior calf just below the popliteal fossa, and the other electrode (8 × 8 cm) covering the distal portion of the muscle just below the muscle fibers of the gastrocnemius. A constant current electrical stimulator (MG 440; Medicor, Budapest, Hungary) was used to deliver 0.5-ms square-wave pulses at 150 V. Peak torques induced by a 1-s electrical stimulation at 1 Hz (P1; representing the properties of muscle excitation–contraction coupling), at 20 Hz (P20; representing the steep section of the force–frequency relationship curve) and at 100 Hz (P100; which is close to maximum force) were measured with a 3-s rest interval between electrical stimulation. The contraction and half-relaxation time (CT+HRT) was measured for the resting TT-100 Hz (250-ms test train of stimuli at 100 Hz) contractions. The CT+HRT was calculated as the time (in ms) taken for the torque to increase to the peak value and then to decrease to half of that value.

Exercise: 2-min MVC

The isometric torque of the ankle plantar flexion muscles was measured using an isokinetic dynamometer (System 4, Biodex Medical Systems, Shirley, NY) calibrated according to the manufacturer's service manual with a correction for gravity performed using the Biodex Advantage program (version 4.X). Participants were seated in the dynamometer chair with the trunk inclined at 45° with respect to the vertical and with hip, knee, and ankle joint angulations of 90°, 100° (full knee extension = 180°), and 90°, respectively. To assess 2-min sustained MVC endurance, the participant was instructed to achieve and maintain a maximum effort of ankle plantar flexion for 120 s. The trace was inspected visually to ensure that there were no artifact spikes at the start of the signal curve. The participant's arms were crossed on the chest with the hands grasping the trunk-supporting belt during all tests on the dynamometer. To help ensure a maximum effort, standard vocal encouragement was provided during the voluntary ankle plantar flexion trial by the same experienced investigator. During the 2-min MVC, the TT-100 Hz stimulus was superimposed on the contraction at 3 s (MVC-3s) and 120 s (MVC-120s) to assess the CAR of the plantar flexors (Brazaitis et al., 2015; Solianik et al., 2015a). The CAR was calculated using the following equation: CAR = MVC/(MVC + PTT100), where a CAR of 1 indicates complete activation, and a CAR <1 indicates central activation failure or inhibition. The percentage changes in CAR and fatigue index (FI; percentage decline in MVC torque) were calculated for MVC-3s and MVC-120s.

Muscles Activity-Generating Capacity

The skin preparation, surface bar electrode, software, hardware, and measurement settings were the same as those described above for reflex recording. The EMG signals of the SOL were recorded by amplifiers (gain 1000) with signal measurements using a third-order filter bandwidth of 20–460 Hz. The analog signal was sampled and converted to the digital form at a sampling frequency of 5 kHz. EMG analysis was performed by calculating the RMS (as a measure of the EMG amplitude values) and MnF (as a measure of the EMG signal frequency content) for a 1000-ms epoch coinciding with a 1-s force interval just before each TT-100 Hz was superimposed on an MVC.

Statistical Analysis

The data were tested for normal distribution using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, and all data were found to be normally distributed. Descriptive data are presented as mean ± standard deviation (SD). Two-way ANOVA for repeated measures was used to determine the effects of experimental trials of two levels (CON vs. HT; CON vs. CL; CL vs. CL-HT; CON vs. CL-HT) and time of two levels [before (or MVC-3s) vs. after (or MVC-120s) 2-min MVC] on MVC, CAR, electrically induced muscle properties (P1, P20, P100, PTT-100, and CT+HRT), and RMS and MnF of the SOL EMG signal. Two-way ANOVA for repeated measures was also performed to study the effects of rewarming (two levels: CL vs. CL-HT; three levels: before cooling vs. after cooling vs. after rewarming) on body temperatures (Tre, Tsk, Tmu), MHP and HR; and on the CSI (two levels: CL vs. CL-HT; two levels: after cooling vs. after rewarming). One-way ANOVA for repeated measures was used to determine the effects of the CON, HT, CL, and CL-HT experimental trials of four levels on resting body temperatures (Tre, Tsk, Tmu), MHP and HR; and the effects of experimental trials of two levels as a between-trial factor (CON vs. HT; CON vs. CL; CL vs. CL-HT; CON vs. CL-HT) on the FI of MVC torque and changes in CAR, electrically induced muscle properties, and RMS and MnF of the RMS of SOL; and on the spinal and supraspinal reflex excitability and cognition. If significant effects were found, Sidak's post hoc adjustment was used for multiple comparisons across a set of conditions within each repeated-measure ANOVA. Statistical significance was defined as P < 0.05. Statistical power (SP, as a percentage) was calculated, and the partial eta squared (ηp2) was estimated as a measure of the experimental trial effect size. The SP for a significant effect was >80%. The non-parametric Wilcoxon signed-rank test for two related samples was used to compare changes in subjective ratings of perceptions (thermal, comfort, and shivering sensations) after rewarming (vs. post-cooling) between CL and CL-HT trials. Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics software (v. 22; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY).

Results

For values measured before the experimental trials, there were no significant differences in body temperatures, HR, MHP, MVC, CAR, RMS, and MnF of the EMG signal, electrically induced muscle properties, cognition, and reflexes between conditions (CON vs. HT vs. CL vs. CL-HT) (P > 0.05; ηp2 < 0.4, SP < 50%). Body temperatures, HR, MHP, CSI, and subjective sensations were not different between trials (CL vs. CL-HT) before and after whole-body cooling (P > 0.05; ηp2 < 0.2, SP < 40%).

Effects of CL-HT on Body Temperatures

In the CL trial, the time to cool the body from Tre 36.8 ± 0.3°C before cooling to 35.6 ± 0.7°C was 126.5 ± 48.4 min. In the CL-HT trial, the time to cool the body from Tre 36.9 ± 0.2°C before cooling to 35.5 ± 0.7°C was 123.1 ± 50.1 min (time effect; P < 0.001, ηp2 > 0.9, SP > 99%; CL vs. CL-HT trial effect; P > 0.05; ηp2 < 0.1, SP < 15%). Tre did not change 10 min after cooling in the thermoneutral environment in the CL trial (Ta 24°C) or after brief whole-body rewarming in warm water in the CL-HT trial (Tw 37°C) compared with the values measured immediately after cooling (time effect; P > 0.05, ηp2 < 0.3, SP < 45%; CL vs. CL-HT trial effect; P > 0.05, ηp2 < 0.2, SP < 30%) (Table 2). Tmu decreased after cooling and further still after rewarming (time effect; P < 0.01, ηp2 > 0.8, SP > 99%). There were no differences between CL and CL-HT experimental trials (CL vs. CL-HT trial effect; P > 0.05, ηp2 < 0.35, SP < 40%). Tsk decreased to a similar extent after whole-body cooling in the CL and CL-HT trials. Whole-body rewarming in Ta increased Tsk by about 6°C from the value immediately after cooling, whereas rewarming in Tw increased Tsk by about 16°C (time effect; P < 0.001, ηp2 > 0.9, SP > 99%; CL vs. CL-HT trial effect; P < 0.001, ηp2 > 0.9, SP > 99%), which restored Tsk to the normal pre-cooling value. Tmu and Tre did not change but Tsk increased by about 2°C during the HT trial.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Body temperature, heart rate (HR), metabolic heat production (MHP), cold strain index (CSI), and thermal, thermal comfort, and shivering sensations in the HT, CL, and CL-HT trials.

Effects of CL-HT on HR and MHP

HR and MHP increased in the HT, CL and CL-HT trials (time effect; P < 0.01, ηp2 > 0.85, SP > 95%) (Table 2). Rewarming in Ta did not changed HR and MHP values compared with immediately after cooling, whereas rewarming in Tw reduced HR to the value measured before cooling and lessened MHP by about 45% compared with immediately after cooling (CL vs. CL-HT trial effect; P < 0.01, ηp2 > 0.8, SP > 90%).

Effects of CL-HT on CSI and Subjective Ratings of Sensation

Whole-body cooling caused high cold strain in both the CL and CL-HT trials (Table 2). The CSI decreased more after Tw rewarming than after Ta rewarming (CL vs. CL-HT trial effect; P < 0.01, ηp2 > 0.8, SP > 90%). The subjects felt warmer and experienced greater thermal comfort, and reported that their shivering stopped during Tw rewarming. By contrast, no changes in these aspects of sensation were reported by the subjects during Ta rewarming.

Effects of CL-HT on Spinal and Supraspinal Reflex Excitability

Whole-body cooling (CL trial) increased the amplitude of Hmax and Vsup, Hmax/Mmax ratio, Vsup/Mmax ratio, and the latency times of Hmax, Vsup, and Mmax responses (time effect; P < 0.01, ηp2 > 0.8, SP > 95%) (Table 4). All of these reflex parameters decreased significantly after Tw rewarming (CL vs. CL-HT trial effect; P < 0.05, ηp2 > 0.8, SP > 90%), although none reached the CON values. Spinal and supraspinal reflex excitability did not change during the HT trial.

Effects of CL-HT on Skeletal Muscle Torque-Generating Capacity

The 2-min MVC significantly decreased the electrically induced muscle torque (P1, P20, P100, and PTT-100) and significantly increased the CT+HRT of PTT100 in all four experimental trials (time effect; P < 0.01, ηp2 > 0.8, SP > 95%) (Table 3). Intermittent whole-body cooling decreased P20, P100, and PTT100 torque and increased CT+HRT of PTT100 measured before exercise compared with the normothermic condition (P < 0.01, ηp2 > 0.8, SP > 90%). Brief rewarming in Tw increased P100 and PTT100 torque and decreased CT+HRT of PTT100 measured before exercise compared with the values in the CL trial (P < 0.05, ηp2 > 0.7, SP > 80%). However, these changes remained significantly different from those measured in the CON trial. The changes in P20 torque and CT+HRT of PTT100 from before to after the 2-min MVC were significantly smaller, and the change in P100 torque was significantly larger in the CL trial than in the CON trial. Brief rewarming in Tw decreased only the size of the change in P100 torque compared with the CL trial (P < 0.05, ηp2 > 0.7, SP > 80%). The percentage change in P1 and PTT100 did not differ significantly between trials. The electrically induced muscle properties were not affected by the HT trial.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Electrically induced muscle properties before and after the 2-min MVC in the control trial (CON) and experimental trials after 5-min body heating (HT), prolonged body cooling (CL), and prolonged body cooling followed by 5-min of rewarming (CL-HT).

Effects of CL-HT on MVC and CAR

The MVC torque (Figure 2A) and CAR (Figure 2C) decreased over the exercise time (2-min MVC) in all four experimental trials (time effect; P < 0.001, ηp2 > 0.9, SP > 99%). The FI of MVC did not differ significantly between the trials (Figure 2B). The percentage change in CAR was smaller in the CL trial than in the CON trial (P < 0.01, ηp2 > 0.8, SP > 95%) (Figure 2D). Rewarming in Tw (CL-HT) caused a greater increase in CAR over the exercise time compared with the change in the CL trial (P < 0.05, ηp2 > 0.75, SP > 90%). The change in CAR did not differ between the CON and CL-HT trials, or between the CON and HT trials.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Changes in maximum voluntary contraction (MVC) torque (A), fatigue index of MVC torque (B), central activation ratio (CAR) (C), and percentage change in CAR (D) during the 2-min MVC in the control trial (CON) and trials with 5-min body heating (HT), prolonged body cooling (CL), and prolonged body cooling with 5-min of rewarming (CL-HT). *P < 0.05 compared with MVC-3s; P < 0.05, compared with CON; #P < 0.05, compared with CL. Values are mean ± SD.

Effects of CL-HT on Skeletal Muscle EMG

The RMS (Figure 3A) and MnF (Figure 3C) of the surface EMG signal of SOL decreased over the exercise time (2-min MVC) in all four experimental trials (time effect; P < 0.001, ηp2 > 0.9, SP > 99%). Body cooling (CL) significantly increased the SOL RMS and decreased the SOL MnF measured at MVC-3s and MVC-120s in the 2-min MVC (CL vs. CON trial effect; P < 0.001, ηp2 > 0.9, SP > 99%). In contrast to CL, brief rewarming in Tw decreased the SOL RMS and increased the SOL MnF (P < 0.05, ηp2 > 0.8, SP > 90%); however, neither of these EMG parameters reached the CON values. The change in the SOL RMS was greater in CL than in CON (P < 0.01, ηp2 > 0.85, SP > 99%) (Figure 3B), whereas the change in the SOL MnF did not differ between the CL and CON trials (Figure 3D). Interestingly, brief rewarming in Tw decreased the change in the SOL RMS and increased the change in the SOL MnF over the exercise time compared with the change in the CL trial (P < 0.01, ηp2 > 0.8, SP > 95%). Moreover, brief rewarming in Tw caused a similar change in SOL RMS during the exercise compared with the CON trial. The HT trial caused no changes in the surface EMG parameters measured in SOL.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Changes in the root mean square (RMS) values (A), percentage changes in RMS (B), mean frequency (MnF) values (C), and percentage changes in MnF (D) of the surface EMG signal of soleus muscle during the 2-min MVC in the control trial (CON) and trials with 5-min brief body heating (HT), prolonged body cooling (CL), and prolonged body cooling followed by 5-min rewarming (CL-HT). *P < 0.05 compared with MVC-3s; P < 0.05, compared with CON; #P < 0.05, compared with CL. Values are mean ± SD.

Effects of CL-HT on Cognitive Performance

Simple (Figure 4A), 2-choice (Figure 4B) and procedural (Figure 4C) reaction times increased significantly (i.e., slower reactions) in the CL trial compared with the CON trial (P < 0.05, ηp2 > 0.75, SP > 90%). Brief rewarming in Tw decreased the reaction time (faster reaction) (CL vs. CL-HT trial effect; P < 0.05, ηp2 > 0.7, SP > 80%), and the results for all three tests did not differ significantly from those in the CON trial. The HT trial caused no changes in reaction time. The accuracy (errors) for simple reaction task was 100%, for 2-choise reaction task > 96%, and for procedural reaction task > 94% for all participants, respectively, and did not differ significantly between the four trials.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Simple (A), 2-choice (B), and procedural (C) reaction times in the control trial (CON) and experimental trials after 5-min body heating (HT), prolonged body cooling (CL), and prolonged body cooling followed by 5-min rewarming (CL-HT). P < 0.05, compared with CON; #P < 0.05, compared with CL. Values are mean ± SD.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the effects of brief rewarming on thermoregulation, sensation, motor drive, and cognition in mildly hypothermic subjects. The unique finding of our study is that, in mildly hypothermic subjects, brief (5-min) whole-body rewarming at 37°C Tw rather than rewarming for the same duration at 24°C Ta induced a thermosensory excitation shift by inhibiting body heat production (decreased MHP, HR, and cessation of shivering). In parallel, despite the reduced deep Tmu and Tre, the subjects reported sensations of warmth and thermal comfort, which were accompanied by reduced CSI. Brief Tw rewarming increased the electrically induced maximum torque amplitude of TTP100 and P100, and decreased CT+HRT, and these changes were accompanied by shortened Mmax-wave latency. Brief rewarming in warm water blunted the hypothermia-induced alterations in the transmission of the neural drive at the spinal and neuromuscular junction levels. Thus, during prolonged contraction (2-min MVC), the additional failures at the supraspinal level seemed to impair motor drive to a greater extent in the CL-HT than in the CL trial. Cerebral alterations caused by hypothermia were confirmed by the increased reaction times in simple and more complex tasks, which were restored to the CON levels by brief rewarming in Tw.

Brief Tw Rewarming Inhibits Heat Production, Induces Sensations of Warmth and Thermal Comfort in Hypothermic Subjects

It has been shown that the skin plays a large modulatory role in thermoregulation and thermal perception in the cold, despite the fact that core (i.e., tympanic) temperature is low (Benzinger, 1963; Cabanac et al., 1987). In agreement with these observations, we found that brief whole-body Tw rewarming rather than Ta rewarming of the same duration in mildly hypothermic male subjects induced a thermosensory excitation shift. This thermoregulatory shift is indicated in our study by the significant decrease in HR and MHP, and cessation of shivering reported by the subjects. This shift seems to have been affected solely by the increase in Tsk at ~16°C (before the cooling of Tsk ~33°C was reached) during the brief Tw rewarming phase (Table 2). However, these changes do not appear to have been affected by the increase of ~6°C during the brief Ta rewarming phase despite the similar decline and no further change in core temperature observed in both rewarming phase trials (CL-HT and CL). The brief rewarming in Ta of ~24°C increased the mean Tsk from 17.4°C to 23.1°C, and this change probably corresponded to an innocuous cool thermal threshold, which may have been too low to activate the predominant TRPM8 innocuous cool channel to shift and/or excite potential TRPV3 innocuous warm channels (Craig, 2002; Vay et al., 2012). The findings of our study suggest that, the 5-min Tw (peripheral) rewarming was sufficient to inhibit shivering and to reduce HR and MHP activities despite the reduced Tre.

We observed that whole-body immersion in cold water at 14°C increased the CSI, subjective thermal sensation, and thermal discomfort sensation evaluated throughout the cooling period. TRPA1 and/or TRPM8 channels are activated by 14°C cold water, and this information is conveyed to the insular cortex via thin myelinated Aδ fibers to temperature-sensing lamina-1 neurons with a relay in the posterolateral thalamus (specifically, the posterior part of the ventromedial nucleus) or with two relays (in the parabrachial nucleus and the basal part of the ventromedial nucleus of the thalamus) (Craig, 2002, 2003). These two branches of the spino-thalamo-cortical pathway are involved in discriminative temperature sensation but do not play a significant role in the triggering of involuntary thermoregulatory responses (Craig, 2002). This means that psychological (thermal perception for making decisions) and physiological (thermoregulatory to defend body temperature) responses to the same temperature input probably act independently. Our study results are consistent with these observations in that we found that the thermal discomfort sensation during cooling increased by only 0.5 point (i.e., ranged from “comfortable” to “a little uncomfortable”) and thermal sensation ranged from “slightly cool” to “cool.” These slight changes suggest that prolonged immersion in cold water caused only a low to moderate psychological stress in these subjects, whereas the physiological CSI increased to “high cold strain.” In such conditions, brief rewarming in 37°C Tw rather than rewarming in 24°C Ta may excite TRPV3 channels and temperature signal sensing. This should induce a shift from cold-activated myelinated Aδ sensing fibers to warm-activated unmyelinated C sensing fibers and then to activate lamina-1 neurons and finally the insular cortex; consequently, the subjects felt warm and thermal comfort (Table 2).

Brief Tw Rewarming Modulates Reflexes in Hypothermic Subjects

The CL trial increased the excitability (amplitude and latency) of spinal (Hmax-reflex) and supraspinal (Vsup-wave) reflexes. Among the measures of sarcolemmal excitability (Mmax-wave), cooling significantly increased only latency time. As reported by Drinkwater and Behm (2007), a lower muscle temperature may be needed to affect Mmax-wave amplitude than the Tmu found in this study (~22°C vs. ~30°C, respectively). These data agree with those of Brazaitis et al. (2014b) and Solianik et al. (2014), who showed that the H−, V−, and M-waves, and Hmax/Mmax and Vsup/Mmax were facilitated after cold exposure.

In our study, we examined whether brief rewarming in the CL-HT trial would suppress excitability of reflexes in SOL compared with the CL trial. We found that, immediately after cooling, the Tmu at ~3.5 cm depth reached 31.6° and 30.7°C in the CL and CL-HT trials, respectively. Brief rewarming in both trials caused further decreases of about 1.5° and 0.5°C in Tmu, respectively. Although we did not measure Tmu at different muscle depths, the increased Tsk to the value before cooling in the CL-HT trial indicating a withdrawal of vasoconstriction in the skin (Romanovsky, 2014) and greater heat exchange between the body and warm bath water (whose heat transfer is 25 times greater than air Toner and McArdle, 1996). This may have increased temperature in the superficial skin layer and possibly in nearby muscles without increasing Tre and deep Tmu. As a result, the amplitude and latency of Hmax-reflex and Vsup-wave were lower in the CL-HT than in the CL trial. It has been suggested that this spinal and/or supraspinal modulation is related to presynaptic inhibition, in which the decline in transmission from Ia afferent stimulation in α-motoneuron excitation may result from presynaptic inhibition mediated by temperature-sensitive group III and IV afferents (Bigland-Ritchie et al., 1986; Duchateau et al., 2002; Avela et al., 2006).

Brief Tw Rewarming Improves Electrophysiological Function of Exercising Muscle in Hypothermic Subjects

In parallel with the changes in excitability of spinal and/or supraspinal reflexes, whole-body cooling in the CL trial decreased the MnF and increased the amplitude RMS of MVC in the surface EMG in SOL. These changes may reflect slowed opening and closing of the Na+ channels in the nerve and muscle membranes (Hodgkin and Katz, 1949). Specifically, the slower depolarization, which translates into a reduced axonal conduction velocity, and repolarization produce a longer channel-open time and, therefore, a longer duration response. The movement of additional Na+ ions into the cell during the prolonged opening leads to greater depolarization and response amplitude (Rutkove, 2001). We also observed a suppression effect of brief rewarming in the CL-HT compared with the CL trial that was sufficient to increase the MnF and decrease the RMS. This supports the idea of opposing mechanisms of excitation of the motor neuron pool caused by heating (Rutkove, 2001), which in our study seemed to have been modified by changes in peripheral rather than central temperature input.

Brief Tw Rewarming Improves Contractile Properties of Calf Muscles in Hypothermic Subjects

In the CL trial, much of the decreases in electrically induced maximum torque amplitude of P20, TTP100, and P100, and of the increase in CT+HRT are consistent with peripheral muscle effects, including slowing of sarcoplasmic reticulum (SR) ATPase activity and ATP utilization (Ferretti, 1992), slowed Ca2+ release and uptake rate from the SR (Kössler et al., 1986), delayed cross-bridge formation, and detachment (Vanggaard, 1975; Faulkner et al., 1990), and decreased actomyosin sensitivity to Ca2+ (Sweitzer and Moss, 1990). It is postulated that, during and after cold exposure, Mmax-wave latency time increases because of the reduced ATP hydrolysis rate, which causes slowing of sarcolemmal action pumps through reduced activity of Na/K-ATPase (Drinkwater and Behm, 2007).

The novel contribution of the present study was the use of brief Tw rewarming after whole-body cooling (CL-HT vs. CL), during which TTP100 and P100 torque increased and CT+HRT decreased. These changes were accompanied by shortened Mmax-wave latency (Table 4), which generally corresponds to the increased muscle fiber conduction velocity, probably because of a temperature-mediated effect on voltage-gated Na+ channels (Rutkove, 2001), alongside the elevated ATP turnover (Gray et al., 2006). Specifically, the opening and closing of these channels accelerate, which allows less Na+ to enter cells and leads to a more rapid onset of depolarization (Rutkove et al., 1997). In the CL trial, P100 torque decreased more than P20 torque. This might be explained by the rise of [K+]o and [Na+]i during the high-frequency stimulation and may indicate that the Na+/K+ pump capacity is limited by the decrease in Tmu, particularly in the T-tubules whose pump density is lower than that in the sarcolemma (Fitts, 1994). In the CL trial, potential depletion of ATP immediately after the 2-min MVC (Edwards et al., 1972) may have decreased P100 torque even more than that for P20 torque (high-frequency fatigue). Interestingly, the P20/P100 ratio was partly restored by brief Tw rewarming in the CL-HT trial (Table 3).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Spinal and supraspinal reflex excitability in the control trial (CON) and experimental trials after 5-min body heating (HT), prolonged body cooling (CL), and prolonged body cooling followed by 5-min of rewarming (CL-HT).

Brief Tw Rewarming Increases Central Fatigue in Hypothermic Subjects

We found that the maximum voluntary activation (CAR) of exercising muscles (MVC-3s) increased after whole-body cooling in the CL trial and was restored to the CON level by the brief Tw rewarming in the CL-HT trial (Figure 2C). These changes in CAR may be linked to modification of the transmission of motor drive in any of the stages from cortical activity to sarcolemma depolarization. This modification may have maintained MVC torque, which was similar in all four trials (Figure 2A), despite the fact that the electrically induced contractile properties of the muscle were impaired by whole-body cooling and were partly restored by brief Tw rewarming (Table 3). The sustained 2-min MVC combined with the superimposed electrical stimulation (TT-100 Hz) allowed us to provoke and distinguish central from peripheral fatigue of the ankle plantar flexors (Bernecke et al., 2015). Consistent with previous reports (Cahill et al., 2011; Solianik et al., 2015a), our data suggest that hypothermia offset central failure during sustained MVC (Figure 2C). Moreover, the more rapid muscle energetics (indicated by shortened M-wave latency, faster CT-HRT, increased MnF, and decreased RMS) and accelerated metabolite accumulation with brief Tw rewarming (vs. CL) in fatiguing muscle may have contributed to greater central fatigue by providing inhibitory feedback to central structures (Bigland-Ritchie and Woods, 1984), similar to that related to impaired excitability of spinal and/or supraspinal reflexes. It was previously suggested that, during sustained maximum contraction, the magnitude of voluntary activation of exercising muscle is inversely proportional to whole-body temperature within a moderate range from normal (Todd et al., 2005; Cahill et al., 2011). Our data add a specified functional perspective to this observation by showing that central voluntary activation of exercising muscle can be modified by brief Tw rewarming after whole-body hypothermia without affecting Tre and deep Tmu.

Brief Tw Rewarming Restores Attention in Hypothermic Subjects

Our data suggest that these subjects experienced significant alterations in the synaptic transmission of neural influx at both the peripheral and spinal levels, and raise questions about similar alterations at the supraspinal level. We assessed cognitive function as an indicator of changes at the cortical level. Consistent with previous research (Mäkinen et al., 2006; Muller et al., 2012), we found that acute whole-body cold stress (CL vs. CON) caused slower reaction times in simple and more complex tasks (Figure 4). These findings are consistent with the distraction hypothesis, which suggests that discomfort (increased shivering, MHP, CSI, HR, subjective thermal sensations, and thermal discomfort) caused by cold stress causes a shift of attention from the primary task and leads to impaired performance (Teichner, 1958). A significant slowing in nerve conduction velocity (Table 4) could also explain the slower reaction in the CL trial. Interestingly, in the study by Muller et al. (2012) cognitive dysfunction persisted into the recovery period (60-min rest in 25°C air) after removal from the cold (120-min rest in 10°C air). By contrast, in our study, brief (5-min) whole-body Tw rewarming in the CL-HT trial blunted the hypothermia-induced alterations in simple, 2-choice, and procedural reaction times and allowed these parameters to return to the CON values (Figure 4). The restored reaction times in the CL-HT trial are also consistent with improved nerve conduction velocity (Table 4).

Limitations

The findings of this study suggest that brief Tw rewarming in hypothermic subjects improved cognition, neural drive, and contractile properties of calf muscles, and reduced physiological and psychological cold stress. In our study Tre was measured as a marker of body core temperature. The temperature of this location is known to be relatively delayed during thermal transients. Thus, rectal temperature may not truly be indicative of the core temperature input received by the brain and/or other body locations. Deep body temperatures were not restored by brief Tw rewarming to normal, and we can only speculate on the later post-rewarming effects on thermoregulation, motor drive, cognition, and sensation. Moreover, the thermal conditions in the CL trials are not steady state as the testing environment is different from the prior intervention environment. To rewarm the mildly hypothermic subjects, we used brief (5-min) whole-body immersion in a warm water (37°C) bath, which donated a large amount of heat. However, whole-body warm water immersion is contra-indicated because it increases the risk of cardiovascular collapse in hypothermic people (Zafren et al., 2014). Notably, in this study, brief Tw rewarming did not alter the larger Tre after-drop in mildly hypothermic subjects compared with those subjected to spontaneous rewarming in Ta. Therefore, the results of any study involving mildly hypothermic subjects should be interpreted cautiously with respect to the situation of severe hypothermia (Hoskin et al., 1986). The anatomical differences between men and women (Stephenson and Kolka, 1993; Janssen et al., 2000; van Marken Lichtenbelt et al., 2009), and weakened thermoregulatory system response to extreme temperature conditions in older people (Kenney and Munce, 2003) suggest that the results of the present study may not be directly applicable to women, children, or older people. Water immersion is known to increase venous return and hence stroke volume (Schmid et al., 2007). However, we failed to find any other study which has investigated how these changes in pressure due to water immersion affect cognition, motor drive and perception. These responses in our study were out of scope and thus not evaluated.

Conclusion

We examined whether brief rewarming in warm water is sufficient to modify hypothermia-induced central and peripheral changes. Our primary finding was that brief (5-min) whole-body rewarming in Tw (37°C) rather than rewarming in Ta (24°C) for the same duration blunted hypothermia-induced alterations in sensation, motor drive, and cognition despite similarly lower Tre and deep Tmu in whole-body rewarming in Tw (37°C) and rewarming in Ta (24°C). We have offered one possible explanation for our findings, but further research is needed to draw firm conclusions, especially regarding the brief post-rewarming effects on neurophysiological and cognitive functional kinetics until full body temperature recovery using other external heating methods that are more suitable for field situations.

Author Contributions

MB designed the studies; HP and NE performed the experiments; HB and LD provided assistance; MB, AS, HP, LD, and NE analyzed and interpreted data; MB and HP wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the subjects who took part in the study.

References

Aagaard, P., Simonsen, E. B., Andersen, J. L., Magnusson, P., and Dyhre-Poulsen, P. (2002). Neural adaptation to resistance training: changes in evoked v-wave and h-reflex responses. J. Appl. Physiol. 92, 2309–2318. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01185.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avela, J., Finni, J., and Komi, P. V. (2006). Excitability of the soleus reflex arc during intensive stretch–shortening cycle exercise in two power-trained athlete groups. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 97, 486–493. doi: 10.1007/s00421-006-0209-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benzinger, T. H. (1963). Peripheral cold- and central warm-reception, main origins of human thermal discomfort. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 49, 832–839. doi: 10.1073/pnas.49.6.832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernecke, V., Pukenas, K., Imbrasiene, D., Mickeviciene, D., Baranauskiene, N., Eimantas, N., et al. (2015). Test-retest cross-reliability of tests to assess neuromuscular function as a multidimensional concept. J. Strength Cond. Res. 29, 1972–1984. doi: 10.1519/jsc.0000000000000841

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigland-Ritchie, B. R., and Woods, J. J. (1984). Changes in muscle contractile properties and neural control during human muscular fatigue. Muscle Nerve 7, 691–699. doi: 10.1002/mus.880070902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigland-Ritchie, B. R., Dawson, N. J., Johansson, R. S., and Lippold, O. C. (1986). Reflex origin for the slowing of motoneurone firing rates in fatigue of human voluntary contractions. J. Physiol. 379, 451–459. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp016263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazaitis, M., Eimantas, N., Daniuseviciute, L., Baranauskiene, N., Skrodeniene, E., and Skurvydas, A. (2014a). Time course of physiological and psychological responses in humans during a 20-day severe-cold-acclimation programme. PLoS ONE 9:e94698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazaitis, M., Eimantas, N., Daniuseviciute, L., Mickeviciene, D., Steponaviciute, R., and Skurvydas, A. (2014b). Two strategies for response to 14°C cold-water immersion: is there a difference in the response of motor, cognitive, immune and stress markers? PLoS ONE 9:e109020. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazaitis, M., Skurvydas, A., Pukenas, K., Daniuseviciute, L., Mickeviciene, D., and Solianik, R. (2012). The effect of temperature on amount and structure of motor variability during 2-minute maximum voluntary contraction. Muscle Nerve 46, 799–809. doi: 10.1002/mus.23397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazaitis, M., Eimantas, N., Daniuseviciute, L., Vitkauskiene, A., Paulauskas, H., and Skurvydas, A. (2015). Two strategies for the acute response to cold exposure but one strategy for the response to heat stress. Int. J. Hyperthermia 31, 325–335. doi: 10.3109/02656736.2015.1004135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazaitis, M., Kamandulis, S., Skurvydas, A., and Daniuseviciute, L. (2010). The effect of two kinds of t-shirts on physiological and psychological thermal responses during exercise and recovery. Appl. Ergon. 42, 46–51. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2010.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, D. J. A., Brugger, H., Boyd, J., and Paal, P. (2012). Accidental hypothermia. N. Engl. J. Med. 367, 1930–1938. doi: 10.1056/nejmra1114208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burton, A. C. (1935). Human calorimetry: the average temperature of the tissues of the body. J. Nutr. 9, 261–280.

Google Scholar

Cabanac, M., Germain, M., and Brinnel, H. (1987). Tympanic temperatures during hemiface cooling. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 56, 534–539. doi: 10.1007/bf00635366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahill, F., Kalmar, J. M., Pretorius, T., Gardiner, P. F., and Giesbrecht, G. G. (2011). Whole-body hypothermia has central and peripheral influences on elbow flexor performance. Exp. Physiol. 96, 528–538. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2010.054973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceron, R. J., Radwin, R. G., and Henderson, C. J. (1995). Hand skin temperature variations for work in moderately cold environments and the effectiveness of periodic rewarming. Am. Ind. Hyg. Assoc. J. 56, 558–567. doi: 10.1080/15428119591016782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, S. S., and Sleivert, G. G. (2004). Lowering of skin temperature decreases isokinetic maximal force production independent of core temperature. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 91, 723–728. doi: 10.1007/s00421-004-1062-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Churchill, S., Miller, R. S., Deru, K., Wilson, S. H., and Weaver, L. K. (2016). Simple and procedural reaction time for mild traumatic brain injury in a hyperbaric oxygen clinical trial. Mil. Med. 181, 40–44. doi: 10.7205/MILMED-D-15-00148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2003). Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 500–505. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00090-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drinkwater, E. J., and Behm, D. G. (2007). Effects of 22°C muscle temperature on voluntary and evoked muscle properties during and after high-intensity exercise. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 32, 1043–1051. doi: 10.1139/h07-069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duchateau, J., Balestra, C., Carpentier, A., and Hainaut, K. (2002). Reflex regulation during sustained and intermittent submaximal contractions in humans. J. Physiol. 541, 959–967. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.016790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, R. H., Hill, D. K., and Jones, D. A. (1972). Effect of fatigue on the time course of relaxation from isometric contractions of skeletal muscle in man. J. Physiol. 227, 26P-27P.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Faulkner, J. A., Zerba, E., and Brooks, S. V. (1990). Muscle temperature of mammals: cooling impairs most functional properties. Am. J. Physiol. 259, R259–R265.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ferretti, G. (1992). Cold and muscle performance. Int. J. Sports Med. 13, S185–S187. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-1024634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitts, R. H. (1994). Cellular mechanisms of muscle fatigue. Physiol. Rev. 74, 49–94.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Giesbrecht, G. G., and Bristow, G. K. (1992). A second postcooling afterdrop: more evidence for a convective mechanism. J. Appl. Physiol. 73, 1253–1258.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Giesbrecht, G. G., Wu, M. P., White, M. D., Johnston, C. E., and Bristow, G. K. (1995). Isolated effects of peripheral arm and central body cooling on arm performance. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 66, 968–975.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Goheen, M. S. L., Ducharme, M. B., Kenny, G. P., Johnston, C. E., Frim, J., Bristow, G. K., et al. (1997). Efficacy of forced-air and inhalation rewarming by using a human model for severe hypothermia. J. Appl. Physiol. 83, 1635–1640.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gray, S. R., De Vito, G., Nimmo, M. A., Farina, D., and Ferguson, R. A. (2006). Skeletal muscle atp turnover and muscle fiber conduction velocity are elevated at higher muscle temperatures during maximal power output development in humans. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 290, R376–R382. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00291.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, J. S., Eckerson, J. D., and Kemna, D. (1984). Thermal and cardiovascular changes during three methods of resuscitation from mild hypothermia. Resuscitation 11, 21–33.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hodgkin, A. L., and Katz, B. (1949). The effect of temperature on the electrical activity of the giant axon of the squid. J. Physiol. 109:240. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1949.sp004388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoskin, R. W., Melinyshyn, M. J., Romet, T. T., and Goode, R. C. (1986). Bath rewarming from immersion hypothermia. J. Appl. Physiol. 61, 1518–1522.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Janssen, I., Heymsfield, S. B., Wang, Z. M., and Ross, R. (2000). Skeletal muscle mass and distribution in 468 men and women aged 18–88 yr. J. Appl. Physiol. 89, 81–88.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kadota, K., and Gomi, H. (2010). Implicit visuomotor processing for quick online reactions is robust against aging. J. Neurosci. 30, 205–209. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2599-09.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenney, W. L., and Munce, T. A. (2003). Invited review: aging and human temperature regulation. J. Appl. Physiol. 95, 2598–2603. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00202.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kössler, F., Lange, F., and Küchler, G. (1986). Isometric twitch and tetanic contraction of frog skeletal muscles at temperatures between 0 to 30 degrees C. Biomed. Biochim. Acta 46, 809–813.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kubicki, A., Bonnetblanc, F., Petrement, G., Ballay, Y., and Mourey, F. (2012). Delayed postural control during self-generated perturbations in the frail older adults. Clin. Interv. Aging 7, 65–75. doi: 10.2147/cia.s28352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, P., McDonald, G. K., Chitkara, R., Steinman, A. M., Gardiner, P. F., and Giesbrecht, G. G. (2015). Comparison of distal limb warming with fluidotherapy and warm water immersion for mild hypothermia rewarming. Wilderness Environ. Med. 26, 406–411. doi: 10.1016/j.wem.2015.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levels, K., de Koning, J. J., Foster, C., and Daanen, H. A. M. (2012). The effect of skin temperature on performance during a 7.5-km cycling time trial. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 112, 3387–3395. doi: 10.1007/s00421-012-2316-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mäkinen, T. M., Palinkas, L. A., Reeves, D. L., Pääkkönen, T., Rintamäki, H., Leppäluoto, J., et al. (2006). Effect of repeated exposures to cold on cognitive performance in humans. Physiol. Behav. 87, 166–176. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McArdle, W. D., Magel, J. R., Gergley, T. J., Spina, R. J., and Toner, M. M. (1984). Thermal adjustment to cold-water exposure in resting men and women. J. Appl. Physiol. 56, 1565–1571.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Moran, D. S., Castellani, J. W., O'Brien, C., Young, A. J., and Pandolf, K. B. (1999). Evaluating physiological strain during cold exposure using a new cold strain index. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 277, R556–R564.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Muller, M. D., Gunstad, J., Alosco, M. L., Miller, L. A., Updegraff, J., Spitznagel, M. B., et al. (2012). Acute cold exposure and cognitive function: evidence for sustained impairment. Ergonomics 55, 792–798. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2012.665497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, K., and Morrison, S. F. (2008). Preoptic mechanism for cold-defensive responses to skin cooling. J. Physiol. 586, 2611–2620. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.152686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozaki, H., Enomoto-Koshimizu, H., Tochihara, Y., and Nakamura, K. (1998). Thermal responses from repeated exposures to severe cold with intermittent warmer temperatures. Appl. Hum. Sci. 17, 195–205. doi: 10.2114/jpa.17.195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palinkas, L. A. (2001). Mental and cognitive performance in the cold. Int. J. Circumpolar Health 60, 430–439.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Péronnet, F., and Massicotte, D. (1991). Table of nonprotein respiratory quotient: an update. Can. J. Sport Sci. 16, 23–29.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Racinais, S., Gaoua, N., and Grantham, J. (2008). Hyperthermia impairs short-term memory and peripheral motor drive transmission. J. Physiol. 586, 4751–4762. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.157420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeves, D., Kane, R., Winter, K., Raynsford, K., and Pancella, T. (2010). Anam4™ Sports Medicine Battery. Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metrics (version 4). Norman, OK: Center for the Study of Human Operator Performance.

Google Scholar

Romanovsky, A. A. (2014). Skin temperature: its role in thermoregulation. Acta Physiol. (Oxf.) 210, 498–507. doi: 10.1111/apha.12231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romet, T. T. (1988). Mechanism of afterdrop after cold water immersion. J. Appl. Physiol. 65, 1535–1538.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Rutkove, S. B. (2001). Effects of temperature on neuromuscular electrophysiology. Muscle Nerve 24, 867–882. doi: 10.1002/mus.1084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutkove, S. B., Kothari, M. J., and Shefner, J. M. (1997). Nerve, muscle, and neuromuscular junction electrophysiology at high temperature. Muscle Nerve 20, 431–436. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4598(199704)20:4<431::AID-MUS5>3.0.CO;2-B

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savard, G. K., Cooper, K. E., Veale, W. L., and Malkinson, T. J. (1985). Peripheral blood flow during rewarming from mild hypothermia in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 58, 4–13.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schlader, Z. J., Simmons, S. E., Stannard, S. R., and Mündel, T. (2011). The independent roles of temperature and thermal perception in the control of human thermoregulatory behavior. Physiol. Behav. 103, 217–224. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid, J. P., Noveanu, M., Morger, C., Gaillet, R., Capoferri, M., Anderegg, M., et al. (2007). Influence of water immersion, water gymnastics and swimming on cardiac output in patients with heart failure. Heart 93, 722–727. doi: 10.1136/hrt.2006.094870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solianik, R., Skurvydas, A., Mickevičienė, D., and Brazaitis, M. (2014). Intermittent whole-body cold immersion induces similar thermal stress but different motor and cognitive responses between males and females. Cryobiology 69, 323–332. doi: 10.1016/j.cryobiol.2014.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solianik, R., Skurvydas, A., Pukėnas, K., and Brazaitis, M. (2015a). Comparison of the effects of whole-body cooling during fatiguing exercise in males and females. Cryobiology 71, 112–118. doi: 10.1016/j.cryobiol.2015.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solianik, R., Skurvydas, A., Urboniene, D., Eimantas, N., Daniuseviciute, L., and Brazaitis, M. (2015b). Similar cold stress induces sex-specific neuroendocrine and working memory responses. Cryo Letters 36, 120–127.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Stephenson, L. A., and Kolka, M. A. (1993). Thermoregulation in women. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 21, 231–262. doi: 10.1249/00003677-199301000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweitzer, N., and Moss, R. (1990). The effect of altered temperature on ca 2+ sensitive force in skinned single cardiac myocytes—evidence for force dependence of thin filament activation. J. Gen. Physiol. 96, 1221–1245. doi: 10.1085/jgp.96.6.1221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, M., Nakamura, K., Shimai, S., and Takahashi, H. (1993). Work at cold storage. J. Therm. Biol. 18, 647–650. doi: 10.1016/0306-4565(93)90107-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teichner, W. H. (1958). Reaction time in the cold. J. Appl. Psychol. 42, 54–59. doi: 10.1037/h0049145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tikuisis, P., Jacobs, I., Moroz, D., Vallerand, A. L., and Martineau, L. (2000). Comparison of thermoregulatory responses between men and women immersed in cold water. J. Appl. Physiol. 89, 1403–1411.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tikuisis, P., Meunier, P., and Jubenville, C. (2001). Human body surface area: measurement and prediction using three dimensional body scans. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 85, 264–271. doi: 10.1007/s004210100484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, G., Butler, J. E., Taylor, J. L., and Gandevia, S. C. (2005). Hyperthermia: a failure of the motor cortex and the muscle. J. Physiol. 563, 621–631. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.077115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toner, M. M., and McArdle, W. D. (1996). “Human thermoregulatory responses to acute cold stress with special reference to water immersion,” in Handbook of Physiology: Environmental physiology, eds M. J. Fregley and C. M. Blatteis (Bethesda, MD: American physiological society), 379–418.

Google Scholar

Tzambazis, K., and Stough, C. (2000). Alcohol impairs speed of information processing and simple and choice reaction time and differentially impairs higher-order cognitive abilities. Alcohol Alcohol. 35, 197–201. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/35.2.197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanggaard, L. (1975). Physiological reactions to wet-cold. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 46, 33–36.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

van Marken Lichtenbelt, W. D., Vanhommerig, J. W., Smulders, N. M., Drossaerts, J. M., Kemerink, G. J., Bouvy, N. D., et al. (2009). Cold-activated brown adipose tissue in healthy men. N. Engl. J. Med. 360, 1500–1508. doi: 10.1056/nejmoa0808718

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vay, L., Gu, C., and McNaughton, P. A. (2012). The thermo-trp ion channel family: properties and therapeutic implications. Br. J. Pharmacol. 165, 787–801. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2011.01601.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zafren, K., Giesbrecht, G. G., Danzl, D. F., Brugger, H., Sagalyn, E. B., Walpoth, B., et al. (2014). Wilderness medical society practice guidelines for the out-of-hospital evaluation and treatment of accidental hypothermia: 2014 update. Wilderness Environ. Med. 25, S66–S85. doi: 10.1016/j.wem.2014.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: rewarming, cold stress, hypothermia, thermoregulation, motor and cognitive performance

Citation: Brazaitis M, Paulauskas H, Skurvydas A, Budde H, Daniuseviciute L and Eimantas N (2016) Brief Rewarming Blunts Hypothermia-Induced Alterations in Sensation, Motor Drive and Cognition. Front. Physiol. 7:592. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2016.00592

Received: 23 August 2016; Accepted: 15 November 2016;
Published: 01 December 2016.

Edited by:

Gary W. Mack, Brigham Young University, USA

Reviewed by:

David Andrew Low, Liverpool John Moores University, UK
Zachary Schlader, University at Buffalo, USA
Daniel Gagnon, Université de Montréal, Canada
Stephen Cheung, Brock University, Canada

Copyright © 2016 Brazaitis, Paulauskas, Skurvydas, Budde, Daniuseviciute and Eimantas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marius Brazaitis, marius.brazaitis@lsu.lt

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.