Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 18 June 2013
Sec. Cognitive Science

Comparing apples and pears in studies on magnitude estimations

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Kassel, Kassel, Germany
  • 2Educational Research and Development, Hogeschool-Universiteit Brussel, Brussel, Belgium
  • 3Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Katholieke University Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

The present article is concerned with studies on magnitude estimations that strived to uncover the underlying mental representation(s) of magnitudes. We point out a number of methodological differences and shortcomings that make it difficult drawing general conclusions. To solve this problem, we propose a taxonomy by which those studies could be classified, taking into account central methodological aspects of magnitude estimation tasks. Finally, we suggest perspectives for future research on magnitude estimations, which might abandon the hunt for the mathematical model that explains estimations best and turn, instead, to investigate the underlying principles of estimations (e.g., strategies) and ways of their improvement.

Introduction

There is an ongoing debate among researchers concerned with magnitude estimations on how the relationship between subjective estimations and objective magnitudes may be described best. This issue is important as poor estimation performance – such as in number line estimation tasks – is associated with limited mathematical abilities in children (e.g., Booth and Siegler, 2008; Geary et al., 2008). Furthermore, the characteristics of the underlying mental representation of magnitudes including its systematic biases are often directly inferred from the estimations (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003).

Initially, two fundamental models have been proposed on how magnitudes might be mentally represented. The logarithmic ruler model (Dehaene et al., 1990; Dehaene, 1997) assumes that magnitudes are represented with constant variability on a mental number line. However, representations of larger numbers are located closer to each other and thus overlap compared to smaller numbers. The accumulator model, in contrast, states that magnitudes are represented equidistantly but with proportionally increasing variability (i.e., scalar variability: Gibbon and Church, 1981; Whalen et al., 1999; Huntley-Fenner, 2001). Studies promoting the logarithmic model usually employed relative magnitude estimation tasks such as identifying the larger of two numbers (e.g., Dehaene et al., 1990), while other studies used absolute magnitude estimations that required the approximate transformation between two magnitudes (e.g., generating 23 key presses without counting: Whalen et al., 1999). Only absolute magnitude estimations will be considered further in this article as only these represent estimations in a narrower sense, that is, “a process of translating between alternative quantitative representations, at least one of which is inexact” (Siegler and Booth, 2005, p. 198).

Based on the initial accounts, Siegler and colleagues (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003) investigated how the estimation pattern of magnitudes develops using the number line task. Participants usually mark the position of given numbers on a number line, ranging for instance from 0 to 100 or from 0 to 1000. It has been demonstrated that in young children and for relatively large number ranges, in particular, the estimation pattern exhibits a logarithmic shape, whereas for small number ranges and in older children and adults, the pattern is linear and quite exact, without scalar variability.

This research was the starting point for further studies aiming to explain typical biases in numerical estimations of children and adults. Alternative models to a logarithmic model with constant variability and a linear model with scalar variability have been proposed, that is, segmented linear models (e.g., Ebersbach et al., 2008; Moeller et al., 2009) or a cyclic power model (e.g., Barth and Paladino, 2011). Moreover, a simple power model, adopted from psychophysical research (e.g., Stevens, 1957), was put forward to describe systematic biases in adults' numerical estimations (e.g., Crollen et al., 2011). A debate has started and is still going on about which model is best suited to explain the relationship between estimations and actual magnitudes (e.g., Barth and Paladino, 2011; Opfer et al., 2011; Ashcraft and Moore, 2012; Bouwmeester and Verkoeijen, 2012).

In the present article, we aim at emphasizing that studies employing absolute magnitude estimations to investigate the characteristics of the mental representation of magnitudes are often hardly to compare as they involve a broad range of methodological approaches that apparently may lead to different outcomes concerning the shape, accuracy, or variability of the estimations. To address this problem, we propose a taxonomy by which many of the conducted (and future) studies may be classified, which might help to evaluate the comparability, reliability, and validity of studies. Finally, implications for future research will be suggested.

Lacking Comparability of the Studies

Studies involving absolute magnitude estimations differ broadly with regard to the tasks, the stimuli, and the methods of analysis. Hence, even additional studies might provide no further clarity on children's and adults' estimation abilities and the nature of their underlying mental representations as long as apples and pears are collected into the same basket. In the following, we will give some examples that are directly related to the taxonomy proposed later.

First, estimations can be conceived as numerical or non-numerical (Siegler and Booth, 2005). Numerical estimations involve a magnitude in a symbolic format (i.e., a number word or a numeral) that has to be transferred approximately into another – symbolic or non-symbolic – magnitude (e.g., telling the number of dots), or vice versa. This type will be referred to as symbolic estimations in the following. Non-numerical – or non-symbolic estimations, in contrast, refer to the approximate transformation between two non-symbolic magnitudes (e.g., reproducing a number of dots by key presses). Crollen et al. (2011) showed that symbolic and non-symbolic estimations of adults differ both qualitatively and quantitatively. Symbolic estimations yielded typical biases – that is, under- or overestimations, respectively – that could be well described by a power function. Non-symbolic estimations (i.e., reproduction task), in contrast, were relatively accurate and were described best by a largely linear function.

The differences between symbolic and non-symbolic estimations might be explained by the assumption of format-dependent representations of magnitudes (e.g., Dehaene, 1992; Cohen Kadosh et al., 2011; Lyons et al., 2012; for a review see Cohen Kadosh et al., 2008), although a format-independent representation has been proposed, too (e.g., McCloskey et al., 1985; Barth et al., 2003; Walsh, 2003). Evidence for format-dependent representations comes from fMRI measures showing that different formats activate distinct brain regions (e.g., Vogel et al., 2013). Furthermore, Roggeman et al. (2007) provided evidence that (at least small) symbolic magnitudes are mentally represented by place codes, that is, as activation of a specific position on the mental number line, corresponding to the target magnitude. Non-symbolic magnitudes, in contrast, are represented by summation codes, that is, as activation of a whole segment of the number line up to the corresponding position of the target magnitude. Place codes reflect a local and thus more precise activation on the number line than summation codes and might thus explain a higher accuracy of symbolic compared to non-symbolic estimations. Furthermore, it has been assumed that different transformation paths exist between distinct representational codes (e.g., bi-directional mapping model: Castronovo and Seron, 2007) that might differently affect children's estimations, in particular, whose number knowledge is not fully developed yet. They might perform poorer in symbolic estimations that require the comprehension or production of number symbols, compared to non-symbolic estimations. Evidence for this assumption stems from children's magnitude comparisons (see Rousselle and Noël, 2007) as well as from differential effects of language characteristics on number line estimations (Helmreich et al., 2011). However, most of the studies so far that strived at examining the mental representation of magnitudes involved only symbolic estimations, which is in particular true for research with children (for an exception see Mejias et al., 2012). It might be worthwhile to directly compare the performance in symbolic and non-symbolic estimations and to relate it to the symbolic number knowledge. Sasanguie et al. (2012) have for instance found that children's performance in both a symbolic and a non-symbolic number line task were highly correlated but that only the symbolic task performance was associated with math performance–even if controlled for non-symbolic task performance (see also Sasanguie et al., 2013).

Furthermore, different types of tasks were used within symbolic estimations, such as position-to-number tasks (or perception tasks), where symbolic numbers have to be assigned to given non-symbolic magnitudes (e.g., Ashcraft and Moore, 2012) and number-to-position tasks (or production tasks), where non-symbolic magnitudes have to be generated that match given symbolic numbers (e.g., Barth and Paladino, 2011). Crollen et al. (2011) have shown that both tasks yield opposing biases (i.e., over-estimations in the production task and underestimations in the perception task) and different error rates in adults. A poorer performance in a production-like task compared to a perception-like task was also reported for children (Mundy and Gilmore, 2009; Mejias et al., 2012).

In addition, the target stimuli to be estimated differed. Continuous stimuli, such as in the number line paradigm (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003), and discrete stimuli (e.g., numbers of dots, Crollen et al., 2011) have been used. Boyer et al. (2008) showed that children perform better in proportional judgments of liquids when they were presented as continuous amounts than by discrete units–probably as discrete magnitudes allured them to apply counterproductive counting mechanisms and suppressed a more intuitive approach. Moreover, children were also more accurate in comparing continuous (i.e., lengths of bars) than discrete magnitudes (i.e., numbers of dots; Barth et al., 2009).

Taken together, research so far has shown that the estimation type (i.e., symbolic vs. non-symbolic), task type (i.e., perception, production), and target type (continuous vs. discrete) might differently affect the shape and accuracy of magnitude estimations as well as the direction of the biases in terms of under- and overesti-mations. The next two issues refer to the variability and, again, to the shape of the estimations and, relatedly, to the inferred shape of the underlying mental representation of magnitudes.

Magnitude estimations can be bounded (e.g., number line tasks with lower and upper anchor points: Siegler and Opfer, 2003) or unbounded with no upper anchor cue (e.g., Booth and Siegler, 2006, Exp. 1; Whalen et al., 1999; Cohen and Blanc-Goldhammer, 2011). This issue is relevant in particular for the question of whether or not the estimations exhibit the signature of scalar variability. It seems likely that only unbounded tasks with no upper anchor cue would yield scalar variability as they do not allow for adjusting large estimations to an upper limit (Ebersbach et al., 2008). Furthermore, young children who lack an understanding of large numbers might fail to use the upper numerical anchor and their estimations thus might exhibit scalar variability, too, in cases where the upper anchor is unfamiliar.

Moreover, within the bounded number line tasks, many studies provided a lower and an upper anchor, such as 0 or 1 and 100 (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003; Siegler and Booth, 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2006; Laski and Siegler, 2007; Opfer and Siegler, 2007; Ebersbach et al., 2008; Opfer and Thompson, 2008; Thompson and Opfer, 2008; Ashcraft and Moore, 2012; Ebersbach, in press), while in other studies the location of an additional reference point was explicitly referred to in the pre-test (e.g., the location of 50 on a number line of 0–100; Barth and Paladino, 2011; Bouwmeester and Verkoeijen, 2012; Slusser et al., 2013). The explicit indication of an additional reference point might have affected the shape of the estimations and facilitated the calibration of the estimations around the additional reference point. As a result, estimations might be best described by a cyclic power model with relative accurate estimations near the reference points, while the absence of a third reference point might rather yield a better fit with a logarithmic model.

Methodological Taxonomy

So far, we illustrated methodological differences between studies that might account for the often heterogeneous findings concerning the shape, variability, and accuracy of magnitude estimations. To solve this shortcoming, we propose a taxonomy into which each of the used paradigms might be classified (see Figure 1). This taxonomy accounts for (1) the question of whether symbolic numerals (or number words) are involved in the estimations or not (i.e., symbolic vs. non-symbolic estimations), (2) the type of the estimation tasks (i.e., perception, production, reproduction), (3) the type of the target stimuli (i.e., discrete vs. continuous), (4) the potential range of the estimations (i.e., bounded vs. unbounded), and (5) whether an additional reference point was provided or not. For instance, a classical number line paradigm (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003) involves symbolic/numerical estimations in terms of a production task, in which the target stimuli are continuous, the estimation range is bounded by anchors and no additional reference point is provided.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Taxonomy of paradigms of studies on magnitude estimations.

Suggestions for Future Research

First of all, it could be useful to systematically manipulate the methodological aspects proposed in the taxonomy in magnitude estimation tasks. This might allow determining if previous findings concerning the shape, variability, and accuracy of magnitude estimations and their underlying mental representation, as well as the emergence of systematic estimation skills in the course of development are generalizable or if they apply only to certain paradigms. If the latter was true, the different paradigms might tap different mental representations or, perhaps a certain paradigm might be no reliable and valid instrument to investigate the underlying mental representation (cf. Moeller and Nuerk, 2011).

Second, when aiming at identifying the mathematical model that explains magnitude estimations best, one has to take into account that model fits are affected, amongst others, by the number of trials, the number of parameters of the model, the question of whether a constant is estimated or not, and by the intra-individual variability of the estimations, which is relatively large in young children, in particular. One way to account for the model errors as well as for the number of free parameters would be using the Akaike information criterion (AIC), though it refers only to the relative fit of alternative models.

Third, shortcomings in deciding which model describes the shape of estimations of individual participants best should be prevented. Previous approaches largely differed, ranging from inferential statistics (i.e., comparing adjusted R2 values or the absolute values of the residuals of each model by t-tests: Siegler and Opfer, 2003; Moeller et al., 2009; Berteletti et al., 2012; though it cannot even be assumed that these parameters are Gaussian: May et al., 1989; Edgington and Onghena, 2007) to pure descriptive accounts (i.e., comparing R2 values or likelihoods of each model by visual inspection: Thompson and Opfer, 2008; Barth and Paladino, 2011; Cohen and Blanc-Goldhammer, 2011; Ashcraft and Moore, 2012). A descriptive approach provides maximally a heuristic but not a reliable decision rule (Glover and Dixon, 2004). Even slight differences between concurrent model fits might be overvalued if estimations were, for instance, classified as being linear only because the fit of a linear model is R2 = 0.731 and that of an alternative model is R2 = 0.730. In this regard, a statistically based classification method seems necessary to avoid arbitrary results (see also Moeller and Nuerk, 2011; Bouwmeester and Verkoeijen, 2012). In addition, if individual data will be analyzed in future research, a larger number of trials should be used to get more reliable data – although it also needs to be considered that a multitude of trials might reduce participants' motivation and yield interfering learning effects.

A fourth remark refers to the assumption, made implicitly or explicitly, that linear relationships between estimations and actual magnitudes, as reflected for instance by a better fit of a linear model (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003), are the “idealized developmental endpoint of numerical estimation” (Ashcraft and Moore, 2012, p. 256; see also Hollands et al., 2002). Even if estimations rather obey a linear function, they might significantly and systematically deviate from the actual values, depending on the slope and intercept of the fitted linear function. Thus, even if equidis-tance between neighboring numbers is assumed, the estimations might deviate fundamentally from the actual values (cf. Moeller and Nuerk, 2011). In turn, estimations that are better explained by a power or logarithmic model might correspond on average better to the actual magnitudes than a linear model. However, the use of the best-fitting function might be questioned if the functions make similar predictions with respect to the observable estimation behavior (cf. Wagenaar, 1975; Dehaene, 2001; Thompson and Opfer, 2008; Cantlon et al., 2009). It thus might be useful to consider not only the shape of the estimations but also the accuracy in terms of both absolute and simple deviations from the actual values, as well as the variability of the estimations (see also Holloway and Ansari, 2008; White and Szucs, 2012).

Given the current state of research, future research might rather focus on conditions that lead to biased magnitude estimations and on how these estimations and the underlying “number sense” (Dehaene, 1997) might be improved. First attempts were already provided in the field of developmental research, where number games have proven to support equidistance in the mental representation of numbers (e.g., Wilson et al., 2006; Siegler and Ramani, 2008; Whyte and Bull, 2008). Other approaches might include promoting the familiarity with (e.g., Ebersbach et al., 2008) and the embodiment of numbers (e.g., Fischer et al., 2011) as potential precursors of an appropriate representation of the number system. Furthermore, the nature of estimation processes might be inspected further, such as when and how anchor cues are used or internally created–in particular in the course of development (see Schneider et al., 2008; Ashcraft and Moore, 2012; White and Szucs, 2012). It has been shown that adults use anchors to adjust their numerical estimations (Izard and Dehaene, 2008), but studies on whether children are able to do so and how their use of anchors might be affected (e.g., number knowledge, working memory) are rare (for exceptions see Newman and Berger, 1984; Petitto, 1990). Thus, the question of how and which estimation strategies are applied should be addressed. In addition, as strategies affect estimations (e.g., Ashcraft and Moore, 2012) one might question the fundamental assumption underlying the use of estimation paradigms, namely that estimations are a probate instrument to tap the underlying mental representation at all (Gescheider, 1988; Moeller and Nuerk, 2011). To sum up, we put forward a taxonomy that might contribute to a better comparability of studies on absolute magnitude estimations. We propose that the research focus might switch from trying to identify the model that describes the estimations best toward conditions and strategies that lead to estimation biases and toward procedures that might ward off these biases.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The preparation of this article was supported by a Grant of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG: EB462/1-1) to the first author and a GOA Grant 2012/010 from the Research Fund K.U. Leuven, Belgium.

References

Ashcraft, M. H., and Moore, A. M. (2012). Cognitive processes of numerical estimation in children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 111, 246–267. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.08.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barth, H., Baron, A., Spelke, E., and Carey, S. (2009). Children's multiplicative transformations of discrete and continuous quantities. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 441–454. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barth, H., Kanwisher, N., and Spelke, E. (2003). The construction of large number representations in adults. Cognition 86, 201–221. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00178-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barth, H. C., and Paladino, A. M. (2011). The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. Dev. Sci. 14, 125–135. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00962.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berteletti, I., Lucangeli, D., and Zorzi, M. (2012). Representation of numerical and non-numerical order in children. Cognition 124, 304–313. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Booth, J. L., and Siegler, R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. Dev. Psychol. 41, 189–201. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41. 6.189

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Booth, J. L., and Siegler, R. S. (2008). Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. Child Dev. 79, 1016–1031. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01173.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bouwmeester, S., and Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L. (2012). Multiple representations in number line estimation: a developmental shift or cases of representations? Cogn. Instr. 30, 246–260. doi:10.1080/07370008.2012.689384

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boyer, T. W., Levine, S. C., and Huttenlocher, J. (2008). Development of proportional reasoning: where young children go wrong. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1478–1490. doi:10.1037/a0013110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cantlon, J. F., Cordes, S., Libertus, M. E., and Brannon, E. M. (2009). Comment on “Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures”. Science 323, 5910. doi:10.1126/science.1164773

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Castronovo, J., and Seron, X. (2007). Numerical estimation in blind subjects: evidence of the impact of blindness and its following experience. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 33, 1089–1106. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.5.1089

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, D. J., and Blanc-Goldhammer, D. (2011). Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 331–338. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0059-z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen Kadosh, R., Lammertyn, J., and Izard, V. (2008). Are numbers special? An overview of chronometric, neuroimaging, developmental, and comparative studies of magnitude representation. Prog. Neurobiol. 84, 132–147. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.11.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen Kadosh, R., Bahrami, B., Walsh, V., Butterworth, B., Popescu, T., and Price, C. J. (2011). Specialisation in the human brain: the case of numbers. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 5:62. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00062

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Crollen, V., Castronovo, J., and Seron, X. (2011). Under- and over-estimation. Exp. Psychol. 58, 39–49. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000064

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numer-ical abilities. Cognition 44, 1–42. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(92)90049-N

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dehaene, S. (1997). The Number Sense. New York: Oxford University Press.

Dehaene, S. (2001). Subtracting pigeons: logarithmic or linear? Psychol. Sci. 12, 244–246. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00343

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., and Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 16, 626–641. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.16.3.626

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ebersbach, M. (in press). Evidence for a spatial-numerical association in kindergartners using a number line task. J. Cogn. Dev.

Ebersbach, M., Luwel, K., Frick, A., Onghena, P., and Verschaffel, L. (2008). The relationship between the shape of the mental number line and familiarity with numbers in 5- to 9-year-old children: evidence for a segmented linear model. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 99, 1–17. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2007.08.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Edgington, E. S., and Onghena, P. (2007). Randomization Tests, 4th Edn. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman and Hall/CRC.

Fischer, U., Moeller, K., Bientzle, M., Cress, U., and Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 177–183. doi:10.3758/s13423-010-0031-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., and Byrd-Craven, J. (2008). Development of number line representations in children with mathematical learning disability. Dev. Neuropsychol. 33, 277–299. doi:10.1080/87565640801982361

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gescheider, G. A. (1988). Psychophysical scaling. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 39, 169–200. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.39.020188.001125

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gibbon, J., and Church, R. M. (1981). Time left: linear versus logarithmic subjective time. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 7, 87–107.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Glover, S., and Dixon, P. (2004). Likelihood ratios: a simple and flexible statistic for empirical psychologists. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 11, 791–806. doi:10.3758/BF03196706

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Helmreich, I., Zuber, J., Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., Nuerk, H.-C., and Moeller, K. (2011). Language effects on children's mental number line: how cross-cultural differences in number word systems affect spatial mappings of numbers in a non-verbal task. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 42, 598–613. doi:10.1177/0022022111406026

CrossRef Full Text

Hollands, J. G., Tanaka, T., and Dyre, B. P. (2002). Understanding bias in pro-portion production. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 3, 563–574. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.28.3.563

CrossRef Full Text

Holloway, I. D., and Ansari, D. (2008). Domain-specific and domain-general changes in children's development of number comparison. Dev. Sci. 11, 644–649. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00712.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Huntley-Fenner, G. (2001). Children's understanding of number is similar to adults' and rats': numerical estimation by 5–7-year-olds. Cognition 78, B27–B40. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00122-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Izard, V., and Dehaene, S. (2008). Calibrating the mental number line. Cognition 106, 1221–1247. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laski, E. V., and Siegler, R. S. (2007). Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. Child Dev. 76, 1723–1743. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01087.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lyons, I. M., Ansari, D., and Beilock, S. L. (2012). Symbolic estrangement: evidence against a strong association between numerical symbols and the quantities they represent. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 635–641. doi:10.1037/a0027248

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

May, R. B., Masson, M. E. J., and Hunter, M. A. (1989). Randomization tests: viable alternatives to normal curve tests. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 21, 482–483. doi:10.3758/BF03202823

CrossRef Full Text

McCloskey, M., Caramazza, A., and Basili, A. (1985). Cognitive mechanisms in number processing and calculation: evidence from dyscal-culia. Brain Cogn. 4, 171–196. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(85)90069-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mejias, S., Mussolin, C., Rousselle, L., Grégoire, J., and Noël, M.-P. (2012). Numerical and non-numerical estimation in children with and without mathematical learning disabilities. Child Neuropsychol. 18, 550–575. doi:10.1080/09297049.2011.625355

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moeller, K., and Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). Psychophysics of numerical representation: why seemingly logarithmic representations may rather be multi-linear. J. Psychol. 219, 64–70. doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000048

CrossRef Full Text

Moeller, K., Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., and Nuerk, H.-C. (2009). Children's early mental number line: logarithmic or decomposed linear? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 503–515. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mundy, E., and Gilmore, C. K. (2009). Children's mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of number. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 490–502. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Newman, R. S., and Berger, C. F. (1984). Children's numerical estimation: flexibility in the use of counting. J. Educ. Psychol. 78, 55–64. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.76.1.55

CrossRef Full Text

Opfer, J. E., and Siegler, R. S. (2007). Representational change and children's numerical estimation. Cogn. Psychol. 55, 169–195. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.09.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Opfer, J. E., Siegler, R. S., and Young, C. J. (2011). The powers of noise-fitting: reply to Barth and Paladino. Dev. Sci. 14, 1194–1204. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01070.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Opfer, J. E., and Thompson, C. A. (2008). The trouble with transfer: insights from microgenetic changes in the representation of numerical magnitude. Child Dev. 79, 790–806. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01158.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Petitto, A. (1990). Development of numberline and measurement concepts. Cogn. Instr. 7, 55–78. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0701_3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roggeman, C., Verguts, T., and Fias, W. (2007). Priming reveals differential coding of symbolic and non-symbolic quantities. Cognition 105, 380–394. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rousselle, L., and Noël, M. P. (2007). Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: a comparison of symbolic vs. non-symbolic number magnitude processing. Cognition 102, 361–395. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sasanguie, D., de Smedt, B., Defever, E., and Reynvoet, B. (2012). Association between basic numerical abilities and mathematics achievement. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30, 344–357. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02048.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sasanguie, D., Göbel, S. M., Moll, K., Smets, K., and Reynvoet, B. (2013). Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: what underlies mathematics achievement? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 114, 418–431. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schneider, M., Heine, A., Thaler, V., Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L., et al. (2008). A validation of eye movements as a measure of elementary school children's developing number sense. Cogn. Dev. 23, 409–422. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.07.002

CrossRef Full Text

Siegler, R. S., and Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Dev. 75, 428–444. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00684.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Siegler, R. S., and Booth, J. L. (2005). “Development of numerical estimation: a review,” in Handbook of Mathematical Cognition, ed. J. I. D. Campbell (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press), 197–212.

Siegler, R. S., and Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychol. Sci. 14, 237–243. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.02438

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Siegler, R. S., and Ramani, G. B. (2008). Playing linear numerical board games promotes low-income children's numerical development. Dev. Sci. 11, 655–661. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00714.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Slusser, E. B., Santiago, R. T., and Barth, H. C. (2013). Developmental change in numerical estimation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 193–208. doi:10.1037/a0028560

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychol. Rev. 64, 153–181. doi:10.1037/h0046162

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thompson, C. A., and Opfer, J. E. (2008). Costs and benefits of representational change: effect of context on age and sex differences in magnitude estimation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 101, 20–51. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vogel, S. E., Grabner, R. H., Schneider, M., Siegler, R. S., and Ansari, D. (2013). Overlapping and distinct brain regions involved in estimating the spatial position of numerical and non-numerical magnitudes: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 51, 979–989. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.02.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wagenaar, W. A. (1975). Stevens vs Fechner: a plea for dismissal of the case. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 39, 225–235. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(75)90037-2

CrossRef Full Text

Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 7, 483–488. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Whalen, J., Gallistel, C. R., and Gel-man, R. (1999). Non-verbal counting in humans: the psychophysics of number representation. Psychol. Sci. 10, 130–137. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00120

CrossRef Full Text

White, S. L. J., and Szucs, D. (2012). Representational change and strategy use in children's number line estimation during the first years of primary school. Behav. Brain Funct. 8, 1. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-8-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Whyte, J. C., and Bull, R. (2008). Number games, magnitude representation, and basic number skills in preschoolers. Dev. Psychol. 44, 588–596. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.588

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, A. J., Revkin, S. K., Cohen, D., Cohen, L., and Dehaene, S. (2006). An open trial assessment of “The Number Race”, an adaptive computer game for remediation of dyscalculia. Behav. Brain Funct. 2, 20. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-2-20

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: magnitude estimations, mental representation, number line, symbolic and non-symbolic magnitudes, estimation biases

Citation: Ebersbach M, Luwel K and Verschaffel L (2013) Comparing apples and pears in studies on magnitude estimations. Front. Psychol. 4:332. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00332

Received: 03 April 2013; Accepted: 22 May 2013;
Published online: 18 June 2013.

Edited by:

Andrea Bender, University of Freiburg, Germany

Reviewed by:

Maria Olkkonen, Rutgers University, USA
Korbinian Moeller, Knowledge Media Research Center, Germany
Samar Zebian, Lebanese American University, Lebanon

Copyright © 2013 Ebersbach, Luwel and Verschaffel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Mirjam Ebersbach, Department of Developmental Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Kassel, Hollaendische Str. 36-38, D-34121 Kassel, Germany e-mail: mirjam.ebersbach@uni-kassel.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.