Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 February 2014
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement
This article is part of the Research Topic Multilevel Structural Equation Modeling View all 8 articles

Analyzing indirect effects in cluster randomized trials. The effect of estimation method, number of groups and group sizes on accuracy and power

  • 1Department of Methods and Statistics, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 2Optentia Research Program, Faculty of Humanities, North-West University, South Africa

Cluster randomized trials assess the effect of an intervention that is carried out at the group or cluster level. Ajzen's theory of planned behavior is often used to model the effect of the intervention as an indirect effect mediated in turn by attitude, norms and behavioral intention. Structural equation modeling (SEM) is the technique of choice to estimate indirect effects and their significance. However, this is a large sample technique, and its application in a cluster randomized trial assumes a relatively large number of clusters. In practice, the number of clusters in these studies tends to be relatively small, e.g., much less than fifty. This study uses simulation methods to find the lowest number of clusters needed when multilevel SEM is used to estimate the indirect effect. Maximum likelihood estimation is compared to Bayesian analysis, with the central quality criteria being accuracy of the point estimate and the confidence interval. We also investigate the power of the test for the indirect effect. We conclude that Bayes estimation works well with much smaller cluster level sample sizes such as 20 cases than maximum likelihood estimation; although the bias is larger the coverage is much better. When only 5–10 clusters are available per treatment condition even with Bayesian estimation problems occur.

Introduction

With cluster randomized trials complete groups of individuals, rather than the individuals themselves, are randomized to treatment conditions. Although cluster randomized trials are less efficient than individually randomized trials, they are often preferred in practice for ethical, practical, organizational, or financial reasons, and also to lower the risk of control group contamination (Gail et al., 1996; Moerbeek, 2005). It may be considered unethical to require doctors or therapists to offer a new and promising treatment to some of their patients and to withhold it from others. In educational research it may be more cost-efficient to sample many subjects from a limited number of schools rather than to sample subjects spread over a large number of schools. Control group contamination occurs when information on the intervention leaks to the control and the risk is higher when both treatment and control conditions are available within each group.

Cluster randomized trials are common in the health and behavioral sciences, examples are school-based smoking prevention interventions, body weight reduction trials in general practices, and psychological treatments in groups. Their popularity is demonstrated by four books that are solely devoted to this design (Murray, 1998; Donner and Klar, 2000; Hayes and Moulton, 2009; Eldridge and Kerry, 2012). The main issue with cluster randomized trials is that, as subjects are nested within groups, the outcomes of subjects within the same group cannot be considered to be independent. The correct statistical model that takes such dependencies into account is generally referred to as multilevel model, mixed effects model or random coefficients model (Raudenbush and Bryk, 2002; Hox, 2010; Snijders and Bosker, 2012). This model considers groups of individuals as random effects and the results of the trial can be generalized to the population, provided the number of groups is large enough and the groups can be considered a random sample from this population. Ignoring the multilevel data structure or treating groups as fixed may result in incorrect conclusions with respect to the effect of the intervention (Moerbeek et al., 2003).

The aim of a cluster randomized trial, and of randomized controlled trials in general, is to evaluate the effect of an intervention that aims to change behavior, for instance to lower calorie intake of overweight subjects or to encourage smokers to quit smoking. The effect of the intervention on behavior may be indirect, as is stated for example by the Ajzen's theory for planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991; Conner and Armitage, 1998), which is also known as the Theory of Planned Behavior or the Theory of Reasoned Action. This model states that an individual's behavior is influenced by his or her intention, which in its turn is influenced by attitude and social norm. The latter two variables are assumed to not have a direct effect on behavior, so the effects of these variables on behavior are mediated by intention. The theory has been proven to be very effective in psychology and marketing research (Sheppard et al., 1988) but is also useful in experimental research (MacKinnon et al., 2002b). It is assumed that the intervention will affect behavior indirectly via the mediators attitude and intention. Such mediation is called chain mediation or a three path model. The variables attitude, intention and behavior are measured at the first (i.e., subject) level of the multilevel data structure while the intervention is offered at the second (i.e., group) level. The model is then a 2 → 1 → 1 → 1 model in the notation of Krull and MacKinnon (2001). Mediation can be estimated and tested using structural equation models as has been studied for models without multilevel data structures over the past decades (Sobel, 1982; Baron and Kenny, 1986; MacKinnon et al., 2002a, 2007; Shrout and Bolger, 2002; Taylor et al., 2007; MacKinnon, 2008). An extension to mediation in multilevel models has been studied more recently (Raudenbush and Sampson, 1999; Krull and MacKinnon, 2001; Bauer et al., 2006; Dagne et al., 2007; Raykov and Mels, 2007; Zhang et al., 2009; Preacher et al., 2011). A special focus on cluster randomized trials was made by Krull and MacKinnon (2001), Pituch et al. (2005, 2010), and Pituch and Stapleton (2012).

The drawback of structural equation modeling (SEM) is that it is a large sample technique while the number of clusters in a cluster randomized trial is often small, say less than fifty. This may result in estimation problems when maximum likelihood estimation is used, such as convergence problems, inadmissible estimates such as negative variances, biased chi-squared test statistics and standard errors and low statistical power. Furthermore, it is less robust against non-normality, which is a problem in mediation models where the indirect effect is not normally distributed. Bayesian estimation methods do not assume large sample sizes (Gelman et al., 2004), and in mediation analysis have the additional advantages that they directly incorporate the nonnormal mediation effect, and that they are conceptually simpler for multilevel mediation models (Yuan and MacKinnon, 2009).

Simulation studies have shown that using fewer than 50 clusters is problematic while using maximum likelihood estimation (Maas and Hox, 2005; Meuleman and Billiet, 2009; Hox et al., 2010). A recent simulation study suggested that a much lower sample size at the cluster level of approximately 20 is sufficient when Bayesian estimation methods are used (Hox et al., 2012). The model used in this study did not include mediation effects, though.

The aim of our paper is to study the lowest number of clusters that is needed to accurately estimate and test mediation in cluster randomized trials. We focus on convergence problems, accuracy of the point estimate and confidence interval and power of the test on the indirect effect from treatment condition to behavior. We compare maximum likelihood to Bayesian methods and expect the latter to perform better as it does not assume large sample sizes and normality. Furthermore, since in Bayesian estimation the parameter estimates all follow their proper distribution, we expect no inadmissible solutions.

Statistical Model and Estimation Methods

Subjects are nested within clusters and for both the within-and between level a structural equation model is formulated, see Figure 1. Treatment condition is a cluster-level variable and only appears at the between-level. It affects behavior only indirectly through attitude and intention. All other variables are measured at the subject level are assumed to vary both within and between clusters, hence they are included in both the within- and between-level model. Variables that are predicted from one or more others are called endogenous variables and require a disturbance term to capture unexplained variance. For the variable attitude a disturbance term only appears at the between-level. This variable is assumed to correlate with norms at both levels. The numbers in Figure 1 refer to the population values in our simulation, which is discussed in the next section.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The planned behavior model at the between-cluster level (Top panel) and within-cluster level (Bottom panel).

Multilevel Structural Equation Modeling (MSEM) assumes sampling at both individual and group levels. The individual data are collected in a p-variate vector Yij with subscript i for individuals and j for groups. The data Yij are decomposed into a group level (between groups) component YB = Yj and an individual level (within groups) component YW = YijYj. These two components are orthogonal and additive: YT = YB + YW, and the population covariance matrices are also orthogonal and additive: ΣT = ΣB + ΣW. MSEM assumes that the population covariance matrices ΣB and ΣW are described by distinct models for the group level and the individual level structure. Full maximum likelihood estimation for MSEM minimizes the fit function given by

F=i=1Nlog| Σi |+i=1Nlog(xiμi)'Σi1(xiμi) ,    (1)

where the subscript i refers to the observed cases, xi to those variables observed for case i, and μi and Σi contain the population means and covariances of the variables observed for case i (cf. Mehta and Neale, 2005).

Maximum likelihood estimation assumes large samples, and relies on numerical methods to integrate out random effects. In comparison, Bayesian methods are reliable in small samples, and deal better with complex models. The Bayesian approach is fundamentally different from classical statistics (Barnett, 2008). In classical statistics, the population parameter has one specific value that is unknown. In Bayesian statistics, we express the uncertainty about the population value of a model parameter by specifying a probability distribution of possible values. This probability distribution is called the prior distribution, because it is specified independently from the data. After we have collected our data, this prior distribution is combined with the Likelihood of the data to produce a posterior distribution, which describes our uncertainty about the population values after observing our data. Typically, the variance of the posterior distribution is smaller than the variance of the prior distribution, which means that observing the data has reduced our uncertainty about the possible population values.

Obtaining the posterior distribution is done by simulation, using so-called Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) methods. The general idea of MCMC is that instead of attempting to analytically solve for the point estimates, as with ML estimation, an iterative simulation procedure is used to estimate the parameters. It is beyond the scope of the current paper to provide a full introduction to Bayesian modeling, we refer the non-informed reader to, among many others, Kruschke (2011), van de Schoot et al. (2013) and for a more technical introduction see Gelman et al. (2004) or Lynch (2007). For Bayesian multilevel modeling we refer to Hamaker and Klugkist (2011) and Fahrmeir et al. (2013).

The mode of the marginal posterior distribution is an attractive point estimate of the unknown parameter, because it is the most likely value, and therefore the Bayesian equivalent of the maximum likelihood estimator. Since the mode is more difficult to determine than the mean, the mean of the posterior distribution is also often used. In skewed posterior distributions, the median is an attractive choice. In Bayesian estimation, the standard deviation of the posterior distribution is comparable to the standard error in classical statistics. However, the confidence interval generally is based on the ½α and 100-½α percentiles around the point estimate. In the Bayesian terminology, this is referred to as the 100-α% credible interval. We will return to the interpretation of frequentist and Bayesian intervals in the discussion. Given the non-normality of the mediation effect, we have chosen to use the median of the posterior distribution for the point estimate, and the percentile-based 95% credible interval.

Bayesian methods have some advantages over classical methods. As mentioned before, in contrast to the asymptotic maximum likelihood method, they are valid in small samples. Given the correct probability distribution, the estimates are always proper, which solves the problem of negative variance estimates. Finally, since the random draws are taken from the correct distribution, there is no assumption of normality when variances are estimated. In this study, we examine if Bayesian estimation will help in drawing correct inferences in multilevel SEM if the number of groups (clusters) is relatively small. The simulation studies cited in the introduction typically find that at smaller group level sample sizes the Maximum Likelihood based parameter estimates themselves are unbiased, but that the corresponding standard errors are underestimated, which leads to poor control of the alpha level and undercoverage for the confidence intervals. We expect that the credible intervals in our Bayesian estimation will perform better at lower group level sample sizes.

Design of the Simulation Study

Data are simulated on basis of the model in Figure 1. The number of clusters per treatment condition was 5, 10, 25, or 50. The cluster size was either 5 or 10. The eight combinations of the sample sizes at the cluster and subject level are henceforth called populations and for each of them 5000 data sets were generated using the program Mplus 7.0 (Muthén and Muthén, 2012). Treatment condition was a dichotomous variable whereas attitude, norms, intention and behavior were all multivariate normally distributed. The effect of treatment on attitude was set at 0.5. All mediation paths at the within-level were set to the medium effect size 0.5, which implies that the mediation effect is 0.125. The error variance of the endogenous variable attitude was fixed to 2 at the within-level and 1 at the between-level. The error variances of intention and behavior were fixed to 1 and 0.5 at the within- and between-level. Hence, the intra-class correlation coefficients for all three endogenous variables were 0.33. The population values used in the simulation are presented in the path model in Figure A1.

The program Mplus was also used for model parameter estimation. Default convergence criteria were used for maximum likelihood estimation but not for Bayesian estimation. For the Bayesian estimation we used the Mplus 7.0 default priors which are a very flat normal distribution (N(0,1010)) for the path coefficients and a flat inverse gamma distribution (IG(−1,0)) for the variances. By default, Mplus 7.0 uses two independent MCMC chains, and uses the Gelman-Rubin potential scale reduction (PSR; Gelman and Rubin, 1992) to assess convergence. Convergence is reached when the PSR criterion is less than 0.05 from 1. In our simulation, we have run four independent MCMC chains, forced a chain length of at least 5000, and set the PSR criterion to 0.01 for convergence to be reached. For the point estimate we used the median of the posterior, and for the 95% credible interval we used the percentile method.

The performance of the two estimation methods was based on four criteria: convergence, parameter bias, coverage of the 95% confidence interval (maximum likelihood estimation) or 95% credible interval (Bayes estimation) and power. Our focus is on the mediation effect from treatment condition to mediation.

Results

Table 1 displays the mean, (standard deviation), relative bias, coverage of the 95% CI, and percentage significant results over 5000 replications per cell for the mediation effect from treatment condition to mediation.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Simulation study results for the eight populations using Maximum Likelihood and Bayes.

Contrary to expectations, the Bayesian estimation ran into biased estimates for all eight populations' mediation effect whereas the Maximum Likelihood estimation was able to provide only slightly biased estimates for the mediation effect so long as there were about 50 clusters with 5–10 observations each. However, as a consequence of the biased standard errors in ML estimation, the 95% coverage and significance intervals for the Bayesian estimation outperformed those of the Maximum Likelihood estimation. Bayesian estimation rendered overall higher coverage rates and higher significance rates for the first four populations—until the number of clusters drops below 25.

Table 2 displays information about the convergence of the analyses for the Maximum Likelihood estimation. The pattern in Table 2 suggests that as soon as the number of clusters drops below 25, Maximum Likelihood runs into considerable convergence problems. The types of convergence warnings are displayed below Table 2; their frequencies (if any) are displayed in the final column of the table.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Convergence in the simulation study with ML estimation.

Discussion

The simulation results indicate that Bayesian estimation works better at smaller second level sample sizes than Maximum Likelihood estimation. Actually, Bayesian estimation results in a larger bias than Maximum Likelihood estimation at all simulated sample sizes. However, the standard errors in the ML estimation were inaccurate at the lower sample sizes, and as a result the Bayesian 95% CI shows a much better coverage than the ML 95% CI. This is not only the result of having more accurate standard errors, but also due to the fact that the mediation effect is a multiplication of effects that are assumed to be normal, and therefore the mediation effect does not follow a normal distribution. The Bayesian posterior 95% credible interval is established by the percentile method, which follows the asymmetric distribution of the mediation effect much better than the symmetric intervals established by the Maximum Likelihood method (Yuan and MacKinnon, 2009). The ML estimation method shows a better power, but at the expense of having standard errors that are estimated with a downward bias, resulting in poor type I error control due to an operating alpha level which is higher than the nominal 5% level.

At the smaller sample sizes ML estimation exhibits convergence problems, as expected. Bayesian estimation can also encounter estimation problems, but of a different kind. In MCMC estimation, convergence means convergence of the chain to the correct distribution. In our simulation we must rely on the automatic convergence criteria available in Mplus. Textbooks on Bayesian statistics caution users to always use diagnostic tools such as plots of the iteration history (trace plots, cf. Gelman et al., 2004; Lynch, 2007), and we completely agree with these recommendations; we consider such inspections mandatory. Especially with small sample sizes, we recommend inspection of autocorrelations and setting much stricter criteria for convergence. In addition, with smaller sample sizes, the use of informative priors could be helpful. The disadvantage is of course, that in small samples such prior information can easily dominate the information in the data. Here, we have taken the position that this is undesirable, and prefer to work with uninformative priors. If informative priors are to be used, we recommend using several of these priors to determine to what extend the choice of a specific prior determines the results.

In the section on the model and estimation method we introduced the frequentist 95% confidence interval and the Bayesian 95% credible interval, tacitly implying that these are more or less the same (they both abbreviate to 95% CI). The software used (Mplus 7; Muthén and Muthén, 2012) encourages this view since to employ Bayesian estimation we simply have to specify a different estimation method. We do think that users should realize that by choosing Bayesian estimation, from a principled standpoint, they have chosen to employ a different kind of statistics. As a result, the 95% credibility interval now may correctly be interpreted as the interval that contains the population parameter with 95% probability. In our power section in Table 1, we refer to significance of results, based on frequentist and Bayesian. p-values. In the Bayesian case, this is not the identical to the frequentist p-value, it is the so-called posterior predictive p-value. This is roughly interpreted as a standard p-value, but it is actually a different entity. Bayesian modelers in general prefer that decisions about parameters are based on credibility intervals and not p-values, and that decisions about models are based on comparative evidence, such as information criteria or Bayes factors. A discussion of these issues is beyond the scope of this paper (for a very thorough discussion see Barnett, 2008), but we believe that applied researchers should be aware that doing a Bayesian analysis is not just choosing a different estimation method.

One of the reviewers raised the issue that, since the mediation effect is a product term of three coefficients, the priors that are specified to be uninformative on the coefficients themselves, may become informative on the indirect mediation effect. This concern is quite right. The default prior for regression coefficients in Mplus 7 is a normal distribution with a mean of zero and a very large variance. The distribution of products of random variables is generally hard to assess (see Glen et al., 2004, for a discussion and a computational approach). When the normal distributions have a mean of zero, the product of three normally distributed variables is a complicated function that is symmetric with a peak at zero (see the explication on the Mathematica website at http://mathworld.wolfram.com/NormalProductDistribution.html). It appears that, even if the standard deviation of the prior normal distributions are very large, there is still a real possibility that their convolution is informative, and will tend to move the posterior toward zero. The fact that the Bayesian estimates reported in Table 1 have a downward bias in all simulated conditions suggests that this is in our simulation setup indeed the case. Using uniform priors, although their convolution also does not lead to a uniform prior for the mediation effect, may ameliorate the situation by producing a less peaked prior for the mediation effect. This is clearly an area that needs further research.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Hum. Dec. Process. 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

CrossRef Full Text

Barnett, V. (2008). Comparative Statistical Inference. Chicester: Wiley.

Baron, R. M., and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bauer, D. J., Preacher, K. J., and Gil, K. M. (2006). Conceptualizing and testing random indirect effects and moderated mediation in multilevel models: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods 11, 142–163. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.142

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Conner, M., and Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the theory of planned behavior: a review and avenues for further research. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 28, 1429–464. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01685.x

CrossRef Full Text

Dagne, G. A., Hendricks Brown, C., and Howe, G. W. (2007). Hierarchical modeling of sequential behavioral data: examining complex association patterns in mediation models. Psychol. Methods 12, 298–316. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.3.298

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Donner, A., and Klar, N. (2000). Design and Analysis of Cluster Randomization Trials in Health Research. London: Edward Arnold.

Eldridge, M., and Kerry, S. (2012). A Practical Guide to Cluster Randomized Trials in Health Services Research. Chichester: Wiley. doi: 10.1002/9781119966241

CrossRef Full Text

Fahrmeir, L., Kneib, T., and Lang, S. (2013). “Bayesian multilevel models,” in The Sage Handbook of Multilevel Modeling, eds M. A. Scott, J. S. Simonoff, and B. D. Marx (Los Angeles, CA: Sage), 53–72. doi: 10.4135/9781446247600.n4

CrossRef Full Text

Gail, M., Mark, S., Carroll, R., and Green, S. (1996). On design considerations and randomization-based inference for community intervention trials. Stat. Med. 15, 1069–1092. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0258(19960615)15:11<1069::AID-SIM220>3.0.CO;2-Q

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gelman, A., Carlin, J. B., Stern, H. S., and Rubin, D. B. (2004). Bayesian Data Analysis, 2nd Edn. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman and Hall/CRC.

Gelman, A., and Rubin, D. B. (1992). Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Stat. Sci. 7, 457–511. doi: 10.1214/ss/1177011136

CrossRef Full Text

Glen, A. G., Leemis, L. M., and Drew, J. H. (2004). Computing the distribution of the product of two continuous random variables. Comput. Stat. Data Anal. 44, 451–464. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9473(02)00234-7

CrossRef Full Text

Hamaker, E. L., and Klugkist, I. (2011). “Bayesian estimation of multilevel models,” in Handbook of Advanced Multilevel Analysis, eds J. J. Hox and J. K. Roberts (New York, NY: Routledge), 137–162.

Hayes, R. J., and Moulton, L. H. (2009). Cluster Randomised Trials. Boca Rotan, FL: CRC Press. doi: 10.1201/9781584888178

CrossRef Full Text

Hox, J. J. (2010). Multilevel Analysis. Techniques and Applications, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Routledge.

Hox, J. J., Maas, C. J. M., and Brinkhuis, M. J. S. (2010). The effect of estimation method and sample size in multilevel structural equation modelling. Stat. Neerl. 64, 157–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9574.2009.00445.x

CrossRef Full Text

Hox, J., van de Schoot, R., and Matthijsse, S. (2012). How few countries will do? Comparative survey analysis from a Bayesian perspective. Surv. Res. Methods 6, 87–93.

Kruschke, J. K. (2011). Doing Bayesian Data Analysis. Burlingon, MA: Academic Press.

Krull, J. L., and MacKinnon, D. P. (2001). Multilevel modelling of individual and group level mediated effects. Multivariate Behav. Res. 36, 249–277. doi: 10.1207/S15327906MBR3602_06

CrossRef Full Text

Lynch, S. (2007). Introduction to Applied Bayesian Statistics and Estimation for Social Scientists. New York, NY: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-71265-9

CrossRef Full Text

Maas, C. J. M., and Hox, J. J. (2005). Sufficient sample sizes for multilevel modelling. Methodology 1, 86–92. doi: 10.1027/1614-2241.1.3.85

CrossRef Full Text

MacKinnon, D. P. (2008). Statistical Mediation Analysis. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., and Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 593–614. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., and Sheets, V. (2002a). A comparison to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychol. Methods 7, 83–104. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.83

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MacKinnon, D. P., Taborga M. P., and Morgan–Lopez, A. A. (2002b). Mediation designs for tobacco prevention research. Drug Alcohol Depend. 68, 569–583. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(02)00216-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mehta, P. D., and Neale, M. C. (2005), People are variables too: multilevel structural equations modeling. Psychol. Methods 10, 259–284. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.10.3.259

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meuleman, B., and Billiet, J. (2009). A Monte Carlo sample size study: how many countries are needed for accurate multilevel SEM? Surv. Res. Methods 3, 45–58.

Moerbeek, M. (2005). Randomization of clusters versus randomization of persons within clusters: which is preferable? Am. Stat. 59, 72–78. doi: 10.1198/000313005X20727

CrossRef Full Text

Moerbeek, M., Van Breukelen, G. J. P., and Berger, M. P. F. (2003). A comparison between traditional methods and multilevel regression for the analysis of multi-center intervention studies. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 56, 341–350. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(03)00007-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Murray, D. M. (1998). Design and Analysis of Group-Randomized Trials. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus User's Guide, 7th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén.

Pituch, K. A., Murphy, D. L. and Tate, R. L. (2010). Three-level models for indirect effects in school- and class-randomized experiments in education. J. Exp. Educ. 78, 60–95. doi: 10.1080/00220970903224685

CrossRef Full Text

Pituch, K. A., and Stapleton, L. M. (2012). Distinguishing between cross- and cluster-level mediation processes in the cluster randomized trial. Sociol. Methods Res. 41, 630–670. doi: 10.1177/0049124112460380

CrossRef Full Text

Pituch, K. A., Whittaker, T. A., and Stapleton, L. A. (2005). A comparison of methods to test for mediation in multisite experiments. Multivariate Behav. Res. 40, 1–23. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr4001_1

CrossRef Full Text

Preacher, K. J., Zhang, Z., and Zyphur, M. J. (2011). Alternative methods for assessing mediation in multilevel data: the advantages of multilevel SEM. Struct. Equ. Model. 18, 161–182. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2011.557329

CrossRef Full Text

Raudenbush, S. W., and Bryk, A. (2002). Hierarchical Linear Models. Applications and Data Analysis Methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Raudenbush, S. W., and Sampson, R. (1999). Assessing direct and indirect effects in multilevel designs with latent variables. Sociol. Methods Res. 28, 123–153. doi: 10.1177/0049124199028002001

CrossRef Full Text

Raykov, T., and Mels, G. (2007). Lower level mediation effect analysis in two- level multilevel structural equation modeling approach. Struct. Equ. Model. 14, 636–648. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575511

CrossRef Full Text

van de Schoot, R., Kaplan, D., Denissen, J., Asendorpf, J. B., Neyer, F. J., and van Aken, M. A. G. (2013). A gentle introduction to bayesian analysis: applications to research in child development. Child Dev. doi: org/10.1111/cdev.12169. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J., and Warshaw, P. R. (1988). The theory of reasoned action: a meta- analysis of past research with recommendations for modifications and future research. J. Consum. Res. 15, 325–343. doi: 10.1086/209170

CrossRef Full Text

Shrout, P. E., and Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods 7, 422–445. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Snijders, T. A. B., and Bosker, R. J. (2012). Multilevel Analysis: An Introduction to Basic and Advanced Multilevel Modeling. London: Sage.

Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural Equation models. Sociol. Methodol. 13, 290–312. doi: 10.2307/270723

CrossRef Full Text

Taylor, A. B., MacKinnon, D. P., and Tein, J.-Y. (2007). Test of the three-path mediated effect. Organ. Res. Methods 11, 241–269. doi: 10.1177/1094428107300344

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yuan, Y., and MacKinnon, D. P. (2009). Bayesian mediation analysis. Psychol. Methods 14, 301–322. doi: 10.1037/a0016972

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhang, Z., Zyphur, M. J., and Preacher, K. J. (2009). Testing multilevel mediation using hierarchical linear models. Problems and solutions. Organ. Res. Methods 12, 695–719. doi: 10.1177/1094428108327450

CrossRef Full Text

Appendix

FIGURE A1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure A1. The planned behavior model at the between-cluster level (Top panel) and within-cluster level (Bottom panel) with population parameter values.

Keywords: multilevel sem, sample size, cluster randomized trial, Bayesian estimation, mediation

Citation: Hox JJ, Moerbeek M, Kluytmans A and van de Schoot R (2014) Analyzing indirect effects in cluster randomized trials. The effect of estimation method, number of groups and group sizes on accuracy and power. Front. Psychol. 5:78. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00078

Received: 25 October 2013; Paper pending published: 19 December 2013;
Accepted: 20 January 2014; Published online: 04 February 2014.

Edited by:

Michael Eid, Freie University Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Shevaun D. Neupert, North Carolina State University, USA
Mike W. L. Cheung, National University of Singapore, Singapore

Copyright © 2014 Hox, Moerbeek, Kluytmans and van de Schoot. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Joop J. Hox, Utrecht University, PO Box 80140, Padualaan 14, 3584 CH, NL-3508 TC Utrecht, Netherlands e-mail: j.hox@uu.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.