Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 10 June 2014
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Dynamic Systems Theory and Embodiment in Psychotherapy Research. A New Look at Process and Outcome View all 12 articles

Rhythm is it: effects of dynamic body feedback on affect and attitudes

  • 1SRH University Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany
  • 2University of Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany

Body feedback is the proprioceptive feedback that denominates the afferent information from position and movement of the body to the central nervous system. It is crucial in experiencing emotions, in forming attitudes and in regulating emotions and behavior. This paper investigates effects of dynamic body feedback on affect and attitudes, focusing on the impact of movement rhythms with smooth vs. sharp reversals as one basic category of movement qualities. It relates those qualities to already explored effects of approach vs. avoidance motor behavior as one basic category of movement shape. Studies 1 and 2 tested the effects of one of two basic movement qualities (smooth vs. sharp rhythms) on affect and cognition. The third study tested those movement qualities in combination with movement shape (approach vs. avoidance motor behavior) and the effects of those combinations on affect and attitudes toward initially valence-free stimuli. Results suggest that movement rhythms influence affect (studies 1 and 2), and attitudes (study 3), and moderate the impact of approach and avoidance motor behavior on attitudes (study 3). Extending static body feedback research with a dynamic account, findings indicate that movement qualities – next to movement shape – play an important role, when movement of the lived body is an independent variable.

Introduction

Movement is central to the human condition (Sheets-Johnstone, 1999). It is dynamic and as pervasive as the air that we breathe. Movement therapies use this basic human capacity in order to restore health, access resources, and diminish suffering (e.g., Koch et al., 2013). Movement also provides us with central cues for indications and can predict therapy outcomes (e.g., Ramsayer and Tschacher, 2011). Dance movement therapy (e.g., Levy, 2005) assumes that next to the shape of a movement (e.g., approach vs. avoidance movement), its’ quality is of central importance (Kestenberg, 1995; Laban, 1960). Next to the what, the how of the movement (e.g., indulgent vs. fighting movement) can have an influence on our affect and attitudes, cognition and interpersonal relations (Kestenberg and Sossin, 1973; Stern, 1985; Rizzolatti, 2013).

With the change from a computer metaphor-based to a more organismic understanding of the human condition (Smith and Semin, 2004) in embodiment research, human movement has moved back into a scientific focus. Research in psychology and neuroscience demonstrated that the observation of the movement of our conspecifics in a goal related task sets of our own motor programs in order to understand the intentions of the others (e.g., Buccino et al., 2001). The bodily reactions of our conspecifics cause empathic bodily reactions in ourselves (Bavelas et al., 1986; Chartrand and Bargh, 1999; Wilson and Knoblich, 2005); and the congruency of motor behavior with a cognitive task influences the effectiveness of our performance (e.g., Wells and Petty, 1980; Förster and Strack, 1996).

Body feedback from postures can cause differential affect (Strack et al., 1988), attitudes (Cacioppo et al., 1993; Neumann and Strack, 2000; Maass and Russo, 2003; Förster, 2004; Schubert, 2004), and cognition (Mussweiler, 2006; Cuddy et al., 2012; for a review of these effects see Niedenthal et al., 2005). Since most of the latter studies have focused on static but not dynamic body feedback, it seems timely to analyze influences of movement on affect, attitudes, and cognition, and to specify clinical implications of these findings.

The expressive function of movement has been a scientific topic ever since Darwin had published “The expression of emotions in men and animals” (Darwin, 1872/1965). The impressive function of movement (Wallbott, 1990) has been a focus in psychology starting with James–Lange theory, and has thrived empirically with the postulation of the facial feedback hypothesis (Buck, 1980; Laird, 1984). Body feedback approaches have subsequently further extended to include postural feedback (e.g., Riskind, 1984; LaFrance, 1985; Rossberg-Gempton and Poole, 1992), and vocal feedback (Hatfield et al., 1995; for a general overview on body feedback research see Hatfield et al., 1994) but most of this research has remained in the static realm of held postures or facial expressions to date.

For clinical applied fields such as body psychotherapy or dance movement therapy working with movement of the lived body as an independent variable all the time, body feedback research needs to move on to investigate effects of movement on affect, cognition, and health-related outcomes. The dynamic character of movement has not yet been fully accounted for by embodiment research in general and body feedback research in particular, which so far focused on effects of static facial expressions and postures on affect and cognitions. Movement, however, is characterized and defined by its dynamic properties: its relation to space, weight (gravity/force), and time (Laban, 1960), and its proprioceptive and kinesthetic properties (Sheets-Johnstone, 1999; Gibbs, 2006). In body feedback studies using held postures, these properties of movements have not been taken into account leading to a lack of knowledge if it comes to effects of movement interventions. Moreover, almost all embodiment research so far has focused exclusively on movement shape (i.e., changes in the form or direction of the movement), and has not considered changes in movement quality (i.e., changes in muscle tension and the dynamic properties related to space, weight, and time).

Round vs. Sharp Reversals

An exception is the study of Aronoff et al. (1992) who considered the impact of movement qualities on perception. Aronoff et al. (1992) investigated emotional implications of round vs. angular movement (study 1), and round and angular facial cues (study 2) reporting that round properties are related to perception of emotional warmth, cordiality and positive roles of actors on stage, whereas angular properties are related to perceptions of threat and negative roles of actors on stage. Angular shapes and sharp transitions had already been demonstrated to cause more attributions of aggressiveness in the classic movie of Heider and Simmel (1944) on the antropomorphization of animated geometric forms. In a similar vein, a study of Bar and Neta (2006) using stimuli from everyday objects (watches, sofas, etc.) found that attitudes toward curved shapes were significantly more positive than attitudes toward sharp-angled shapes. This basic smooth vs. sharp distinction was already found in a classic experiment by gestalt psychologist Köhler (1929). People were asked to assign the names bouba and kiki to one of two shapes (later also exchanged by the words maluma and takete), one looking like a round-curved inkblot and the other like a sharp-edged star. Between 95 and 98% of people asked assigned kiki to the angular shape and bouba to the rounded shape. The effect has been demonstrated in different cultural contexts and also in children as early as two and a half years of age (Maurer et al., 2006). Yet, Aronoff et al. (1992) remain the only researchers to have empirically looked at these properties in movement (study 1). However, they focused on perceptual effects and did not account for body feedback effects (i.e., the effects from peripheral movement on more central processes such as affect or cognition), nor did they explicitly distinguish movement shape from movement quality. Our findings extend Aronoff’s work with a body feedback approach, distinguishing the effects of movement shape from the effects of movement qualities more explicitly than Aronoff et al. (1992).

A Theory on Movement and Meaning

Early attempts to specify dynamic movement qualities have been made by movement analysts1 (e.g., Laban, 1960) and later have been selectively related to psychological properties (e.g., Kestenberg, 1995). One of the most complete and differentiated theory-systems on how movement maps to semantics is the Kestenberg Movement Profile (KMP; Kestenberg and Sossin, 1973, 1979; Kestenberg-Amighi et al., 1999; Koch and Sossin, 2013). With a focus on clinical and developmental applications such as early mother–child interaction, Kestenberg developed nine perspectives on movement (yielding nine diagrams) based on the three dimensions of space, weight (gravity/force) and time, and the three planes of horizontal, vertical and sagittal movement. Following Laban (1960) and Lamb (1965), Kestenberg distinguished two basic movement systems: movement qualities and movement shape. On the basis of psychodynamic theories (Freud, 1965), she related those to the first years of child development as well as to clinical issues and personality traits in the adult. She thereby offered a comprehensive theory-system for a wide range of applications in non-verbal diagnosis and intervention. Since her predictions are directly related to human movement as an observable independent variable her theory is testable and offers a wealth of hypotheses to clinical embodiment research. This article focuses on the movement rhythms and the underlying principles of the Kestenberg system (for a more complete account on the KMP see Kestenberg-Amighi et al., 1999; Koch and Sossin, 2013).

Movement Rhythm

When we hear of rhythms we may think of music rather than of movement. Yet, just like there are external rhythms that can make us move to the beat (Grahn and Brett, 2007), there are internal ones that are related to our own situational needs and affect. They are expressed by the constant subtle alternations in muscle tension and relaxation in the body (Kestenberg and Sossin, 1973, 1979; Kestenberg, 1995). KMP-theory distinguishes 10 prototypical movement rhythms2 (see Figure 1) that correspond to physiological and psychological needs of a person (Kestenberg, 1995; Kestenberg-Amighi et al., 1999). They belong to the broader system of movement qualities and fall in two basic categories: indulgent rhythms and fighting rhythms. Indulgent rhythms have smooth reversals (reversals are the transitions from tension to relaxation and vice versa; examples for smooth rhythms are sucking or swaying) and serve joyful indulgence into new behavior, while fighting rhythms have sharp reversals (e.g., snapping transitions such as in cutting or biting) and serve necessary separation from old behavior and defense against outer or inner demands. These movement rhythms already start to develop in the fetal stage (Loman, 2007). In each developmental phase, one indulgent rhythm that facilitates acquisition, mobilization into new patterns, and libidinal repetition of predominant movements precedes a fighting rhythm that facilitates stabilization, differentiation, and separation from that particular phase (cf. Erikson, 1950). The sucking rhythm is the first rhythm that organizes the body of the child. It has smooth reversals, low intensity, and regular amplitudes spreading from the mouth to all other body parts, particularly when children need to soothe themselves, for example, immediately before falling asleep (Lotan and Yirmiya, 2002). The biting rhythm has sharp reversals and helps the child to cut and separate things, first with the teeth, then with the hands and the entire body. It later serves analytic thinking and separation of categories, and can be observed, for example, when we bite our pencils or finger nails. Thus finding oneself biting on a pen might indicate the need to focus, concentrate and get concepts straight; if one finds oneself curling one’s hair or rocking in a sucking rhythm this can indicate the need to soothe oneself. Likewise, these rhythms are employed to address needs of others (e.g., soothing a baby; consider that all lullabies consist of sucking rhythms) and usually lie at the implicit level of our experience.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Overview of the Kestenberg tension-flow rhythms. The 10 prototypical developmental movement rhythms following KMP-theory (cf. Kestenberg-Amighi et al., 1999). Study 1 used jumping vs. spurting/ramming rhythm, study 2 used swaying vs. snapping/biting and starting–stopping rhythm, and study 3 used sucking and jumping vs. starting–stopping and spurting/ramming rhythm (depending on the intensity and timing participants used). Rhythms may vary in three sets of parameters (attributes): regularity of amplitude (even vs. flexibile), intensity-level (high vs. low intensity; indicated in height of vertical stroke), and timing (abrupt vs. gradual; indicated in steepness of slope), and are related to the three dimensions of space, weight and time respectively while rhythms indicate needs on the individual level, shapes indicate relations to persons or objects.

The method of rhythms notation, done in hand-writing on a blank sheet with a time line, makes use of kinesthetic empathy (Kestenberg, 1995). Observing the target person, notators take the changes in muscle tension and relaxation into their own body (by motor simulation) and from there into their writing arm, fingers and pencil (as bodily extension), finally producing a “tension-flow line” on the sheet. By convention, moving the pencil down thereby indicates an increase of tension in the body of the target, while moving the pencil up indicates a decrease of tension in the body of the target. Once notated, the rhythms are categorized and counted. Rhythm counts usually yield inter-rater reliabilities of Cronbach’s Alphas between 0.74 and 0.91 (Sossin, 1987; Koch, 2006).

Movement Shape

Cacioppo et al. (1993) conducted an experimental series on approach and avoidance motor behavior, demonstrating that the application of pressure toward the body from below a table (held approach movement) produced more positive attitudes toward Chinese ideographs (arbitrary characters) than the application of pressure away from the body from above the table. The experiment was groundbreaking in two respects: it showed that the movement of the basic categories approach and avoidance directly produced pronounced attitudes, and researchers consciously applied the first dynamic variable, i.e., the application of directional force, to demonstrate the meaning of directional movement (toward and away from the body). Neumann and Strack (2000) replicated and extended these findings in Germany.

KMP-theory postulates that changes in shape-flow (i.e., rudimentary directional movement on the basic dimensions of growing/open vs. shrinking/closed) are related to affect and attitudes: approach behavior is expected to be related to positive affect and attitudes, avoidance behavior is expected to be related to negative affect and attitudes (e.g., the child grows toward the smiling mother; the child shrinks away from the angry dog). Shape-flow movements toward and away from the body (shape-flow design) are related to self-object-differentiation as well as to giving and taking. Cacioppo et al. (1993) use an evolutionary account to explain the effect of approach and avoidance motor behavior on attitudes: during ontogenesis – and also phylogenesis – persons have learned to take in good things (e.g., food) and to push away bad things (e.g., angry persons); this life-long learning process causes a conditioned evaluative preparedness of our cognitive-affective system. Similarly, Eberhard-Kaechele (2007) points out that in KMP-theory shape-flow differentiates on a preconscious level between toxic and nourishing stimuli and provides the appropriate response (i.e., growing toward or shrinking away from a stimulus). Thus, KMP-theory predicts an effect of movement qualities on affect and of movement shape on affect as well as on attitude.

Hypotheses

Based on the approaches of Kestenberg and Sossin (1979) and Cacioppo et al. (1993), and on the grounds of theories of embodied cognition (Barsalou, 1999; Niedenthal et al., 2005; Niedenthal, 2007), we predicted how movement will influence affect and attitudes. The study extends existing findings in body feedback research using dynamic movement instead of statically held postures. Following KMP-theory, movement quality was included through the manipulation of movement rhythms with smooth vs. sharp reversals assuming that smooth rhythms would cause more indulgent affect in movers.

We employed two one-factorial (smooth vs. sharp rhythms) and one two-factorial (movement rhythm × movement shape) designs. Studies 1 and 2 tested the hypotheses that indulgent vs. fighting movement rhythms (smooth vs. sharp rhythms) would cause congruent3 answers on cognitive and affect measures (one-factorial between-group design). Study 3 tested the hypotheses that approach vs. avoidance motor behavior and smooth vs. sharp movement rhythms would cause congruent answers on an affect and attitude measure. The relative magnitude of the main effects and the interaction was explored (2 × 2 between-group design).

Study 1: Dynamic Body Feedback from Movement Rhythms on Affect and Cognition

Study 1 focused on systematic effects of dynamic body feedback from rhythms on affect and cognition. In addition to the expectation that movement rhythms with smooth reversals would cause more positive affect, we expected two motor congruency effects on the cognitive level. In a categorization task (online embodiment; i.e., embodiment effects directly caused in the situation), participants in the indulgent groups were expected to categorize “smooth” words (e.g., sway) faster than “sharp” words (e.g., bite), and participants in the fighting groups were expected to categorize “sharp” words faster than “smooth” words. In the memory task of study 1 (offline embodiment; i.e., embodiment effects from memory), participants were expected to remember more congruent words, respectively.

Method

Sample

Sixty participants (30 women, 30 men; mean age = 23.83; SD = 8.54) were tested in a one-factorial between group designs. Thirty used jumping rhythm (indulgent; smooth reversals), and thirty used spurting/ramming rhythm (fighting; sharp reversals). Participants had been either recruited in the psychology department at the local university or in the local central pedestrian zone (about 50% from each location). Most participants were students. In all studies, we matched men and women to the otherwise randomized groups. Gender was controlled in all studies but did not account for any differences related to the main hypotheses. Participants signed an informed consent form before the experiments started. A debriefing was provided at the end of the experiments. In all studies, participants received either course credit or sweets for their participation.

Cover Story

Participants were told in the beginning that this experiment aimed to measure the influence of different levels of physical arousal on a number of tasks. In all three studies, their pulse was taken before and after the movement, and served as a control variable, but did not have any significant influence. Their attention was thus turned away from the movement qualities.

Movement Manipulation

We chose jumping rhythm vs. spurting/ramming rhythm as examples of indulgent vs. fighting rhythms because due to their high intensity and magnitude they were particularly easy to observe and embody, and particularly clear and easy to distinguish from one another (Figure 1). Participants in the indulgent condition were told to bounce on both feet, almost as if rope skipping, but without leaving the floor; those in the fighting condition were told to kick an imaginary ball with the left and right leg in alternation. Both movements were performed in high intensity and abrupt, differing merely in smoothness vs. sharpness of reversals (bouncing/jumping: smooth; kicking: sharp). Movements were performed for ~2 min, while participants categorized verbs into smooth (“rund”) and sharp (“eckig”) by mouse-clicks.

Instruments and Scales

Reaction time measure

Reaction times were measured for the semantic categorization task. Participants had to categorize 22 pretested verbs into the two categories of “smooth” or “sharp.” Verbs were taken directly from the rhythms terminology of the KMP translated to German, for example, “swaying” (wiegen) or “sucking” (saugen) for smooth, and “biting” (beissen) or “knocking” (klopfen) for sharp. We conducted a pretest with 20 participants and finally only used words correctly categorized by at least 14 persons. The presentation was programmed in Experimental Runtime System (ERTS, Beringer, BeriSoft, Frankfurt, Germany), verbs were presented in random order. Reaction time was measured computing the duration from display of the verbs to the mouse click by the participant.

Recall

The recall of the formerly categorized words was to be given in free format. We calculated with both number of “smooth” and “sharp” words recalled.

Affect measure

Participants were asked “How do you feel? Please take some time to sense the effects of the movement just performed.” We employed a self-constructed movement based-affect scale (MBAS; Koch and Müller, 2007; Figure 2; non-italicized items) consisting of seven bipolar adjective items on a 7-point scale from a longer pretested version of the affect scale (KMP-questionnaire; Koch and Müller, 2007) containing the interpretative semantic terms from KMP-theory from the KMP-book by Kestenberg-Amighi et al. (1999) on the level of movement rhythms (indulgent vs. fighting; Koch and Müller, 2007). Sample items were tense vs. relaxed, nervous vs. letting go, etc., Cronbach’s Alpha was 0.70. Factor analysis revealed the expected one-factor solution with 63% of the variance explained. We used the sum score for computations. After reversion of polarization, higher values indicated more negative affect.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Movement-based affect scale (MBAS; Koch and Müller, 2007). The movement-based affect scale (13 items; originally “Brief KMP affect scale,” Koch and Müller, 2007) consists of eight items related to movement qualities and five items related to movement shapes and is based on the tension-flow and shape-flow concepts of Kestenberg (1995), Kestenberg and Sossin (1973, 1979); items related to movement shape are set in italics; those were added for study 3, when movement shape was introduced. All items have been derived from the KMP textbook of Kestenberg-Amighi et al. (1999) via the KMP-questionnaire (Koch and Müller, 2007) yielding good internal consistencies of scales of the German version (with Cronbach’s Alphas between 0.70 and 0.95). The affect scale in our studies showed Cronbach’s Alphas of 0.70 and 0.82 (without shape items in studies 1 and 2) and of 0.89 including shape items (in study 3).

Procedure

Informed consent was obtained for participation and the fact that the session was video-taped. Then participants received the following instruction: “In this study we investigate the effects of bodily exhaustion on performance in a number of areas. For each of these areas you will complete a short task.” Heart rate and blood pressure were then obtained as a base-rate. Participants received instructions on how to move in the according condition (jumping vs. spurting ramming rhythm) with a short description and demonstration by the experimenter. On correct repetition, the person was asked to rehearse the movement for 15 more seconds before moving on to the first task. Subsequently, they received instructions for the categorization task. They had to hold a wireless mouse in both hands and categorize verbs that appeared via beamer on a white 2 m × 2 m screen in a distance of about 2 m. Words had to be categorized as fast as possible into the categories smooth vs. sharp by pressing the right or left mouse button using the right or left thumb respectively. After two exercise trials the categorization task started. Participants had to continue to perform the movement during the entire categorization task (~2 min). Immediately after the task, heart rate and blood pressure were taken again. Then the experimenter asked the participants to do the movement for another 15 s focusing on “how the movement feels” (without being distracted by the categorization task). Thereafter, participants had to characterize their impression using the affect scale (Figure 2). After that, participants had to recall as many words as possible from the categorization task writing them on a blank sheet. Finally, participants completed a demographic data sheet and were then debriefed about the aims of the study. In the end, they either received course credit or selected a small present from a selection of sweets. The study took ~30 min altogether.

Data Analysis

We computed an analysis of variance (ANOVA) with movement rhythm (jumping vs. spurting/ramming) as independent variable, and reaction times, recall, and affect (measured with the MBAS) as dependent variables, using SPSS (2002, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and an 0.05 alpha-level.

Results and Discussion

Results indicate that indulgent vs. fighting rhythms led neither to a faster classification of congruent words, nor to a more frequent recall of congruent words (cognitive measures). They did, however, cause congruent affect in participants F(60,1) = 4.34, p = 0.042, η2 = 0.07. Descriptive statistics are provided in Table 1. The use of indulgent vs. fighting rhythms affected the affective level in the hypothesized direction: participants who performed indulgent movement felt more relaxed, joyful, indulgent, peaceful, playful, etc. Whereas participants who performed fighting movement felt more tense, intruding, fighting, aggressive, retaining, etc. The “missing effect” on the cognitive measures may indicate that movement qualities – at least on the rhythm level, i.e., the earliest and most implicit level – do not affect cognition, but it could also mean that our cognitive measures were not sensitive enough to the experimental manipulation or on a different cognitive level. We speculated that – consistent with KMP-theory – an evaluative measure may have been more adequate than a reaction time measure.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Descriptives of studies 1 and 2.

In sum, the manipulation of prototypical movement rhythms as basic dimensions of movement qualities showed the hypothesized effects on the affective level only. In order to investigate whether the changes in affect were in fact due to differential effects of movement qualities (indulgent vs. fighting), or just to this particular combination of rhythms (jumping vs. spurting/ramming), to any laterality effects (alternating vs. parallel leg movement), or even just to the very specific movements used in study 1 (pretending to bounce similar to rope-jumping vs. pretending to kick a ball), we conducted study 2. One of the cognitive measures was replaced by an evaluative measure in order to further investigate which dependent variables are generally affected by movement rhythms.

Study 2: Consistency of Dynamic Body Feedback from Movement Rhythms

In order to investigate the generalizability of the effects of study 1 to other combinations of rhythms, we conducted a second study similar to the first study using swaying rhythm (indulgent/smooth) vs. biting rhythm (fighting/sharp). Variables, design, cover story, instruments, procedure and hypotheses were parallel to those of study 1, except for the replacement of the categorization and recall task by a face evaluation and recognition task.

Method

Sample

Sixty-seven participants, mostly psychology students from the local university, were tested. Sixty-two (22 men, 40 women; mean age = 22.75; SD = 3.97) were included into the final analyses. The others did not perform the movement correctly or consistently enough, as determined by a blind expert rater. Students received course credits for participation.

Movement Manipulation and Hypotheses

Participants sat on a table and swung their legs alternatingly (using swaying rhythm), or had to pull up their feet in parallel and push them down again (flexion and extension of foot ankle using biting rhythm, sometimes starting–stopping rhythm; this variation, however, was not important as long as it was a fighting rhythm) while they performed the evaluation task on a laptop. During the performance of the movement they had to push and hold down either a right or a left key to indicate the degree of sympathy of 60 neutral-expression stimulus faces (from -100 very unsympathetic to +100 very sympathetic). In addition to the motor congruency effect on affect, that is, that the smooth rhythm would again cause more positive affect, we expected two cognitive motor congruency effects: (a) higher sympathy ratings in the smooth rhythms group and (b) an increased recognition of the known faces where movement had been congruent to the valence of the initial evaluation. In the recognition task, they received 40 neural-expression facial stimuli: 10 known and 30 unknown. The 10 known were the ones they formerly had rated most extreme: their five most sympathetic and their five most unsympathetic. We computed an ANOVA using a 0.05 alpha-level with movement rhythm (swaying vs. biting/starting–stopping) as independent variable, and evaluations of faces, recognition of faces, and affect (MBAS) as the dependent variables. Cronbach’s Alpha for the MBAS was 0.82.

Results and Discussion

In line with our assumptions, findings were almost identical to study 1. We found no effects of indulgent vs. fighting rhythms on the cognitive-evaluative measure (neither online nor offline), but an effect on the affective measure F(1,62) = 7.77; p = 0.007; η2 = 0.12. Again, the use of indulgent vs. fighting rhythms particularly affected the affective level in the hypothesized direction: when participants performed indulgent movement they felt more relaxed, joyful, etc.; when they performed fighting movement they felt more tense, aggressive, etc. Study 2 thus replicated the results of the first study. In sum, studies 1 and 2 demonstrate the initial validity of indulgent (smooth) vs. fighting (sharp) movement qualities as a meaningful basic dimension of movement and their link to the affect system as hypothesized by KMP-theory (Kestenberg, 1995). Cognition, also in the more evaluative operationalization, remained unaffected (see Table 1; Figure 3). Since the gist of these findings corresponded to the predictions of KMP-theory, and at the same time added the essential movement qualities to body feedback research, we felt encouraged to carry our studies further. On the basis of our results, we speculated that while fighting vs. indulgent movement may not have caused a main effect on our first evaluative measure selected, it may still affect other evaluative measures, maybe in conjunction with other embodiment effects on attitudes.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Affect ratings (sum values) after indulgent vs. fighting movement in study 1 and 2. Results of studies 1 and 2: fighting rhythms caused higher negative affect (tense, aggressive, nervous, etc.), whereas indulgent rhythms caused higher positive affect (relaxed, joyful, playful, etc.); higher values indicate more negative affect.

Study 3: Does Movement Quality Moderate Approach and Avoidance Motor Effects?

In body feedback research, there is a well-known effect of arm flexion and extension causing more positive vs. more negative attitudes toward initially valence-free stimuli (Cacioppo et al., 1993). With a series of six experimental studies, Cacioppo et al. (1993) were among the first researchers to take the entire body plus a held directional force into account as an independent measure influencing attitude formation. They showed that non-facial and rudimentary dynamic motor manipulations can influence participants’ attitudes toward initially valence-free stimuli (Chinese ideographs). Participants either performed an approach movement (i.e., arm flexion: they pressed their palms against the underside of a table, thereby mobilizing force upward and toward the body) or an avoidance movement (i.e., arm extension: they pressed their palms against the surface of a table, thereby mobilizing force downward and away from the body). While performing the movement, participants watched a series of 24 initially valence-free Chinese ideographs. When they later evaluated the ideographs, participants in the approach condition rated the ideographs significantly more positively than participants in the avoidance condition. These findings have been supplemented by empirical studies identifying moderators such as laterality (Cretenet and Dru, 2004; Dru and Cretenet, 2005), hemispheric processing asymmetries, and personality traits (Maxwell and Davidson, 2007), as well as the valence of the stimuli, and the relation to the effects’ situated meaning (Centerbar and Clore, 2006). All of these studies have begun to take movement into account by inducing a basic movement direction, and by this means bringing rudiments of goal direction into the equation. Cacioppo et al. (1993) interpreted their findings as a direct effect of motor behavior on attitude. KMP-theory suggests that, because of their strong relation to affect, rhythms with smooth vs. sharp reversals should influence rudimentary attitudes in a similar fashion as approach vs. avoidance motor behavior; they may thus be components of, or contributors to, attitudes.

In this study, we aimed to replicate and extend the results from Cacioppo et al. (1993) that arm flexion and extension (as a manipulation of movement shape) has a differential effect on attitudes toward valence-free stimuli. Since we were interested in the effects of movement proper, rather than mere expense of held force, participants were instructed to move their arms rhythmically either toward the body or away from the body (palm direction oriented accordingly). We further wanted to find out whether movement qualities and movement shape are related to the evaluative system in a similar way and with similar effect sizes. On the basis of Cacioppo et al. (1993) and Kestenberg-Amighi et al. (1999), we hypothesized a main effect for movement shape (approach vs. avoidance) and a main effect for movement quality (smooth vs. sharp rhythms): smooth rhythms, just like approach movements, were assumed to cause more positive attitudes.

Method

Sample and Design

Forty participants (21 women, 19 men; mean age 22.90, SD = 7.37) were tested in a 2 × 2 design: independent variables were movement rhythms (smooth vs. sharp rhythms) and movement shape (approach vs. avoidance movement). Dependent variables were the offline-evaluation of the Chinese ideographs from the original experiment by Cacioppo et al. (1993; attitude measure), and an affect scale, including the seven original items related to movement qualities, and five new items related to movement shape (Figure 2). Participants, mostly students, had been recruited in the local pedestrian zone and in the psychology department and received either course credits or sweets as a reward.

Movement Manipulation

In all four conditions participants were sitting, using both lower arms bilaterally (in parallel), which were moved in four (successive) steps rhythmically toward or away from the torso. Ten participants did an approach movement toward the body (palms also facing toward their body) combined with a smooth rhythm (round reversals, circular movement), 10 participants did an avoidance movement away from the body (palms facing away from their bodies) combined with the smooth rhythm. Ten participants performed an approach movement toward the body combined with a sharp rhythm (sharp reversals, angular movement), and 10 participants did an avoidance movement away from the body combined with a sharp rhythm.

Materials and Scales

Chinese ideographs

We employed the Chinese ideographs from the original study Cacioppo et al. (1993). This material had been tested in many – also international – contexts. We used 12 out of the 24 ideographs (the ones that had not been mirror imaged) and displayed them in a power point presentation at a rate of one ideograph every 10 s on a 2 m × 2 m screen at about 2 m distance from the observer. The entire duration of the presentation was 2 min. Departing from the original experiment, we did not use an initial evaluation during the first presentation of the ideographs.

Attitude scale

Participants had to rate the ideographs on a scale from 1 very negative to 6 very positive.

Affect scale

The affect scale used in the rhythms studies before was extended by five items related to change in movement shape (see Figure 2; Koch and Müller, 2007). The new items again were taken from the semantic interpretations of KMP-theory on the meaning of changes in movement shapes. Sample items were: open vs. closed (offen vs. verschlossen), comfortable vs. uncomfortable (fühle mich wohl vs. fühle mich unwohl), inclined toward vs. disinclined (zugeneigt vs. abgeneigt; see Figure 2; italicized items). Cronbach’s Alpha was 0.89.

Procedure

Participants met one of two experimenters (a man and a woman) and signed an informed consent sheet. Then they received the following information: “This is an experiment on the effects of arousal level on different dependent variables you are in the low arousal condition.” Subsequently, their pulse was taken (base rate) followed by a short training of the movement by one of the experimenters. Thereafter, they were told that they would now see a series of Chinese ideographs that they should merely watch and let them sink in. In a second circulation, they watched the ideographs while performing one of the four movements described above. Afterward, their pulse was taken again. Then they were asked to do the movement for a few more times, before they received the affect scale where they indicated their affect after the movement on the bipolar adjective scales. In a third circulation, they saw the ideographs again and had to rate them on a 6-point scale from very negative to very positive. On the final sheet, they provided their demographic data, received their reward, and were debriefed by one of the experimenters.

Data Reduction and Statistical Analyses

The sum scores of the affect items, and the means of the evaluations of the ideographs served as the basis for calculations. A MANOVA was computed with rhythm (smooth vs. sharp movement; sucking vs. biting) and shape (approach vs. avoidance movement; toward or away from one’s own body) as independent variables, and attitudes (evaluation of ideographs) and affect (MBAS) as dependent variables.

Results and Discussion

Results indicated that the movement condition had a systematic influence on attitudes and affect but not always in accordance with our expectations. While movement shape (approach vs. avoidance) had only a marginal influence on attitudes F(1,40) = 3.94; p = 0.055; η2 = 0.09, it did have a significant influence on the affect measure F(1,40) = 5.56; p = 0.024; η2 = 0.13: after the approach movement, participants felt significantly more relaxed, peaceful, etc.; after the avoidance movement, they felt significantly more tense, aggressive, etc. (no matter whether they had used indulgent or fighting rhythms). Movement rhythms unexpectedly had no influence on the affect measure (as we had seen in the two studies before), but significantly influenced the attitude measure: after using smooth rhythms, participants judged the initially valence-free ideographs more positively, than after using sharp rhythms F(1,40) = 4.63; p = 0.038; η2 = 0.11. The interaction of movement rhythms and shapes was significant for the attitude measure F(1,40) = 5.89; p = 0.020; η2 = 0.14 (see Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Attitude ratings (M) in study 3: interaction of movement shape and movement quality (N = 40) for bilateral movement. Ratings on a 6-point scale from 1 very negative to 6 very positive; *p < 0.05.

The influence of rhythms on attitudes was a new finding. The influence of movement shape on affect is predicted by KMP-theory just the way it occurred in the experiment. An interesting finding is the interaction of movement rhythms and movement shape. It suggests that rhythm could be a moderator for shape in its effects on attitudes and potentially also on affect (Table 2). The effects were of comparable magnitude for movement quality and movement shape. In general, however, we only had a minimal sample size in study 3, leaving the power very low. Given the small effect sizes, results need replication.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Descriptives of study 3 (N = 40).

General Discussion

In this study, we investigated effects of dynamic body feedback, that is, effects from movement proper, on affect, attitudes and cognition. Based on KMP-theory, we introduced indulgent (smooth) and fighting (sharp) movement qualities as two basic principles from movement analysis (Kestenberg-Amighi et al., 1999). Movement qualities in general and movement rhythms with smooth vs. sharp reversals in particular were found to be important factors influencing affect and moderating effects of movement shape. Apart from the fact which movement is enacted (shape; here: approach vs. avoidance motor behavior), it seems equally important how the movement is enacted (quality). In sum, movement rhythms influenced the affect of participants and their attitudes toward initially valence-free stimuli, and moderated the influence of movement shape on attitude formation. Methodologically, the affect and attitude scale may be seen as measuring two aspects of a basic evaluation variable: affect as operationalized here can be seen as the self-related component of a dependent evaluative measure whereas attitude can be seen as the object-related component.

Studies 1 and 2 established smooth and sharp rhythms as basic dimensions of movement with differential effects on affect but not on cognition. It did not matter what particular pair of rhythms we selected or whether there was parallel or alternating limb action, smooth rhythms generally caused more positive (relaxed, peaceful, etc.) affect than sharp rhythms. Other operationalizations of cognitive variables may bear more potential to detect causal relationships between rhythms and cognition than the ones employed here. Following KMP-theory, fighting qualities could for example help people differentiate better by putting them in a more analytic mode and indulging qualities could lead people to blend categories more by putting them in a more integrative and intuitive mode. Overall, our findings are conform with KMP-theory, since Kestenberg (1995) assumes that movement rhythms are foremost associated with needs and affect.

Study 3 showed that body rhythms also influenced attitudes. However, in study 3, affect was not influenced by movement rhythms but by movement shape. Moreover, movement qualities and movement shape interacted significantly in their effects on attitudes. The magnitude of the influence of both independent variables seemed to be comparable in this first joined test. In the study of Cacioppo et al. (1993), participants provided an initial evaluation of the ideographs while watching them for the first time – a condition that the researchers established as necessary for the occurrence of the effect. Since the effects in our study occurred without the initial evaluation of the ideographs, it may be possible that dynamic movement manipulations have stronger effects than statically held postures. Given this is correct, it may be due to the greater naturalness of dynamic movement as part of our everyday experience: the missing spatio-temporal and kinesthetic features in held postures could be exactly the ones that are decisive for the occurrence of the effect. However, because of the merely marginal significance of movement shape on attitudes (p = 0.055), and the small sample size, a replication of the same study with a larger sample is needed in order to analyze the complex influences of movement qualities and movement shape on attitudes and affect. As a next step, it may be useful to separate rhythms and shape manipulations within one design to find out more about potential hierarchies among the effects.

Beyond Static Body Feedback

On a theoretical level, our results underline the importance of effects from movement shape (e.g., Neumann and Strack, 2000; Raab and Green, 2005; Friedman and Elliot, 2007) and complement the picture by adding movement qualities to the tradition of embodiment research. Movement qualities clearly modify the meaning of movements adding a second semantic dimension (cf. Suitner et al., 2012). Our studies are among the first to demonstrate an influence of dynamic movement quality in a body feedback context and the first using a differentiated theory background on how movement maps to semantics to derive its’ predictions. The theory employed in this research and our empirical findings are compatible with other recent theoretical approaches in psychology and the neurosciences (e.g., Damasio, 1994; Barsalou, 1999; Gallese and Lakoff, 2005). All of these approaches assume action and action simulation in sensory-motor areas of the brain at the basis of affect, thinking and reasoning, while the mere duplication of information in abstract symbols, as postulated by amodal theories, is assumed to be implausible and uneconomic.

Clinical Applications

Approach and avoidance motor behavior in interaction with movement qualities can take on different meaning in clinical contexts and psychopathology. To a borderline patient approach movements with sharp rhythm may cause more positive affect when he/she is in a state where self-harm is a goal or a means of relief. The obsessive-compulsive patient may benefit from smooth avoidance movements in order to overcome compulsive approach actions. In general, movement qualities employed in therapy can be assumed to cause changes in affect and attitudes. This assumption and its long term implications need to be explored in future clinical studies.

Approach and avoidance movements have self-related and interpersonal affective implications. Self-related implications have been described above, interactional implications have been described for example by Kafka (1950). Kafka dealt with the basic affects (Uraffekte) and assumed four of them, two approach- and two avoidance-related ones:

profusion: “along with me to you” (love, affection);

ingestion: “along with you to me” (desire, greed);

recession: “away with me from you” (fear, disgust);

ejection: “away with you from me” (anger, hatred).

These tendencies are picked up in the works of Shai and Belsky (2011), and Fuchs and Koch (2014; this issue) on embodied affectivity, both emphasizing the huge overlap between affect and motor action in general (see also Cipolletta, 2013), and the influence of intersubjective factors in particular. Such intersubjective factors are presently investigated in our research on movement rhythm and their communicative functions in handshakes and embraces (Koch, unpublished).

In psychopathology, patients often get stuck in one self-related or interpersonal way of being. One clinical goal would be to have chronically stuck patients expand their movement repertoire in order to extend their action and affective options, coping mechanisms, self-efficacy, and sense of agency. In dance movement therapy, changes in movement are assumed to produce global and specific changes in affect, attitudes and cognition, as differentially predicted by the KMP (Kestenberg, 1995) or Laban Movement Analysis (Laban, 1960); many of these assumptions still need empirical testing.

Conclusion

This research investigated the influence of movement on affect, attitudes and cognition, extending previous, more statically focused work on the effects of motor behavior. It extends Aronoff et al.’s (1992) and other researchers’ findings on the basic smooth/sharp distinction with a body feedback approach. The general aim of the studies was to investigate the meaning of movement qualities, here in particular whether movement rhythms with smooth vs. sharp reversals are basic dimensions of movement with differential implications for affect, attitudes and cognition (as also evidenced from other lines of empirical studies such as Köhler, 1929; Aronoff et al., 1992; Bar and Neta, 2006). Merleau-Ponty (1965) assumed that each experience of a quality is in reality an experience of a certain way of movement. Is it possible that we have greatly overlooked the meaning of movement in clinical psychology? Could this be due to the fact that the dynamics of body movement cannot easily be investigated with classic experimental methods but in fact would be more appropriately modeled within a dynamic systems theory framework and methods? Our findings trace effects that start as movement rhythms in the body: in the alternation of muscle tension and relaxation and its smooth vs. sharp reversals dependent on our organisms’ need to indulge or to separate. The results from our findings indicate that dynamic movement and movement qualities are an important research topic with potentially far reaching implications for clinical and health-related questions, but also for social cognition, interaction, communication, thinking, learning, memory, research methods, and in any case – as demonstrated here – for affect and attitudes as core themes of social embodiment research.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Professor Dr. Sabine C. Koch, Department of Therapeutic Sciences, SRH University Heidelberg. I would like to thank Nancy Brechtel, Stefanie Glawe, Caroline Berude, Gloria Fischer, Marc Hörster and particularly Stephanie M. Müller for data collection, John Cacioppo for sharing the Chinese ideographs from the original studies, and Nancy Brechtel, Susan Loman, Janet Kestenberg Amighi, Michael Kimmel, Stephanie M. Müller, Mark Sossin, and Jörg Zumbach for helpful comments on the manuscript. Correspondence concerning this paper should be addressed to Professor Dr. Sabine C. Koch, SRH University Heidelberg, Maria-Probst-Straße 3, 69123 Heidelberg, Germany. email: sabine.koch@hochschule-heidelberg.de

Footnotes

  1. ^Most movement analysis systems have been developed in dance-related contexts. Just like note writing in music, there was a need for notational systems of movement in dance; movement analysis developed alongside movement notation (Laban, 1960; ).
  2. ^The 10 rhythms corresponding to 10 developmental movement stages are: sucking, snapping/biting, twisting, straining-release, running–drifting, starting–stopping, swaying, surging/birthing, jumping and spurting/ramming (Kestenberg and Sossin, 1973).
  3. ^The word congruent is always used in the sense that the content of the answers matches the assumed semantics of the movement.

References

Aronoff, J., Woike, B. A., and Hyman, L. M. (1992). Which are the stimuli in facial displays of anger and happiness? Configural bases of emotion recognition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 62, 1050–1066. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.62.6.1050

CrossRef Full Text

Bar, M., and Neta, M. (2006). Humans prefer curved visual objects. Psychol. Sci. 17, 645–648. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01759.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 577–609.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., and Mullett, J. (1986). “I show you how I feel”: motor mimicry as a communicative act. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50, 322–329. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.322

CrossRef Full Text

Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., et al. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 400–404.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Buck, R. (1980). Nonverbal behavior and the theory of emotion: the facial feedback hypothe-sis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 38, 811–824. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.38.5.811

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., and Berntson, G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes II: arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 5–17. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Centerbar, D. B., and Clore, G. L. (2006). Do approach-avoidance actions create attitudes? Psychol. Sci. 17, 22–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01660.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chartrand, T. L., and Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 7, 893–910. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cipolletta, S. (2013). Construing in action: experiencing embodiment. J. Constr. Psychol. 26, 293–305. doi: 10.1080/10720537.2013.812770

CrossRef Full Text

Cretenet, J., and Dru, V. (2004). The influence of unilateral and bilateral arm flexion versus extension on judgments: an exploratory case of motor congruence. Emotion 4, 282–294. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.3.282

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cuddy, A. J. C., Wilmuth, C. A., and Carney, D. A. (2012). Preparatory Power Posing Affects Performance and Outcomes in Social Evaluations. Harvard Business School Working Paper, No. 13-027, September 2012, Boston, MA.

Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Putnam.

Darwin, C. (1872/1965). The Expression of Emotions in Men and Animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Dru, V., and Cretenet, J. (2005). Influence of bilateral motor actions on judgements: additional evidence for the motor congruence hypothesis. Laterality 10, 389–398. doi: 10.1080/13576500500143314

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eberhard-Kaechele, M. (2007). “The regulation of interpersonal relationships by means of shape flow: a psychoeducational intervention for traumatised individuals,” in Movement Analysis - Bewegungsanalyse. The Legacy of Laban, Bartenieff, Lamb and Kestenberg, eds S. C. Koch and S. Bender (Berlin: Logos), 203–212.

Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and Society. New York: Norton.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Förster, J. (2004). How body feedback influences consumer’s evaluation of products. J. Consum. Psychol. 14, 415–425. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1404_10

CrossRef Full Text

Förster, J., and Strack, F. (1996). Influence of overt head movements on memory for valenced words: a case of conceptual-motor compatibility. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 421–430. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.71.3.421

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Freud, A. (1965). Normality and Pathology in Childhood: Assessments of Development, Vol. 6. New York: International University Press.

Friedman, R., and Elliot, A. J. (2007). The effect of arm crossing on persistence and performance. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 449–461. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.444

CrossRef Full Text

Fuchs, T., and Koch, S. C. (2014). Embodied affectivity: on moving and being moved. Front. Psychol. 5:508. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00508

CrossRef Full Text

Gallese, V., and Lakoff, G. (2005). The brain’s concepts: the role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 22, 455–479. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000310

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gibbs, R. W. (2006). Embodiment and Cognitive Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Grahn, J. A., and Brett, M. (2007). Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 893–906. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.893

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., and Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional Contagion. Paris: Cambridge University Press.

Hatfield, E., Hsee, C. K., Costello, J., Weisman, M. S., and Denney, C. (1995). The impact of vocal feedback on emotional experience and expression. J. Soc. Behav. Pers. 10, 293–312.

Heider, F., and Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am. J. Psychol. 57, 243–259. doi: 10.2307/1416950

CrossRef Full Text

Kafka, G. (1950). Über Uraffekte. Acta Psychol. 7, 257–278. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(50)90018-7

CrossRef Full Text

Kestenberg-Amighi, J., Loman, S., Lewis, P., and Sossin, K. M. (1999). The Meaning of Movement: Developmental and Clinical Perspectives of the Kestenberg Movement Profile. New York: Routledge.

Kestenberg, J. S. (1995). Sexuality, Body Movement and Rhythms of Development. Northvale: Jason Aronson. (Originally published in 1975 under the title Parents and Children).

Kestenberg, J. S., and Sossin, K. M. (1973). The Role of Movement Patterns in Development, Vol. 1. New York: Dance Notation Bureau Press.

Kestenberg, J. S., and Sossin, K. M. (1979). The Role of Movement Patterns in Development, Vol. 2. New York: Dance Notation Bureau Press.

Koch, S. C. (2006). “Gender at work: differences in use of rhythms, efforts, and preefforts,” in Advances in Dance/Movement Therapy. Theoretical Perspectives and Empirical Findings, eds S. C. Koch and I. Bräuninger (Berlin: Logos).

Koch, S. C., Caldwell, C., and Fuchs, T. (2013). On body memory and embodied therapies. Body Mov. Dance Psychother. 8, 82–94. doi: 10.1080/17432979.2013.775968

CrossRef Full Text

Koch, S. C., and Müller, S. M. (2007). “Assessments with the KMP questionnaire and the brief KMP-based affect scale,” in Movement Analysis. The Legacy of Laban, Bartenieff, Lamb and Kestenberg, eds S. C. Koch and S. Bender (Berlin: Logos), 195–202.

Koch, S. C., and Sossin, K. M. (2013). “Kestenberg movement analysis,” in Handbook of Language, Communication and Gesture, eds C. Mueller, E. Fricke, A. Cienki, and D. McNeill (Berlin: DeGruyter).

Köhler, W. (1929). Gestalt Psychology. New York: Liveright.

Laban, R. v. (1960). The Mastery of Movement. London: MacDonald & Evans.

LaFrance, M. (1985). Postural mirroring and intergroup relations. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 11, 207–217. doi: 10.1177/0146167285112008

CrossRef Full Text

Laird, J. D. (1984). The real role of facial response in the experience of emotion: a response to Tourangeau and Ellsworth, and others. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 47, 909–917. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.4.909

CrossRef Full Text

Lamb, W. (1965). Posture and Gesture. London: Gerald Duckworth.

Levy, F. (2005). Dance Movement Therapy: A Healing Art. Reston, VA: National Dance Association.

Loman, S. (2007). “The KMP and pregnancy: developing early empathy through notating fetal movement,” in Movement analysis – Bewegungsanalyse: The legacy of Laban, Bartenieff, Lamb and Kestenberg, eds S. C. Koch and S. Bender (Berlin: Logos).

Lotan, N., and Yirmiya, N. (2002). Body movement, presence of parents and the process of falling asleep in toddlers. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 26, 81–88. doi: 10.1080/01650250042000627

CrossRef Full Text

Maass, A., and Russo, A. (2003). Directional bias in the mental representation of spatial events: nature or culture? Psychol. Sci. 14, 296–301. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.14421

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maurer, D., Pathman, T., and Mondloch, C. J. (2006). The shape of boubas: sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. Dev. Sci. 9, 316–322. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00495.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maxwell, J. S., and Davidson, R. J. (2007). Emotion as motion: asymmetries in approach and avoidant actions. Psychol. Sci. 18, 1113–1119. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02033.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Merleau-Ponty, M. (1965). Die Phänomenologie der Wahrnehmung. Berlin: DeGruyter.

Mussweiler, T. (2006). Doing is for thinking! Stereotype activation by stereotypic movements. Psychol. Sci. 17, 17–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01659.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neumann, R., and Strack, F. (2000). Approach and avoidance: the influence of proprioceptive and exteroceptive cues on encoding of affective information. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 39–48. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.1.39

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science 316, 1002–1005. doi: 10.1126/science.1136930

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., and Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 9, 194–211. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0903_1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raab, M., and Green, N. (2005). Motion as input: a functional explanation of movement effects on cognitive processes. Percept. Mot. Skills 100, 333–348. doi: 10.2466/pms.100.2.333-348

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ramsayer, F., and Tschacher, W. (2011). Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: coordinated body-movement reflects relationship quality and outcome. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 79, 284–295. doi: 10.1037/a0023419

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Riskind, J. H. (1984). They stoop to conquer: guiding and self-regulatory functions of physical posture after success and failure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 47, 479–493. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.3.479

CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G. (2013). The mirror mechanism: a mechanism for understanding the actions of others. Paper Presentation at the TESIS-Workshop, September 12th, 2013, Parma.

Rossberg-Gempton, I., and Poole, G. D. (1992). The relationship between body movement and affect: from historical and current perspectives. Arts Psychother. 19, 39–46. doi: 10.1016/0197-4556(92)90062-S

CrossRef Full Text

Schubert, T. W. (2004). The power in your hand: gender differences in bodily feedback from making a fist. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 757–769. doi: 10.1177/0146167204263780

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shai, D., and Belsky, J. (2011). When words just won’t do: introducing Parental Embodied Mentalizing. Child Dev. Pers. 5, 173–180. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00181.x

CrossRef Full Text

Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1999). The Primacy of Movement. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/aicr.14

CrossRef Full Text

Sossin, K. M. (1987). Reliability of the Kestenberg Movement Profile. Movement Studies 2, 23–28.

Smith, E. R., and Semin, G. R. (2004). “Socially situated cognition. Cognition in its social context,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 36, ed. M. P. Zanna (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 53–117.

Stern, D. (1985). The Interpersonal World of the Infant. New York: Basic Books.

Strack, F., Martin, L. L., and Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 768–777. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.768

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Suitner, C., Koch, S. C., Bachmeier, K., and Maass, A. (2012). “Dynamic embodiment and its functional role: a body feedback perspective,” in Body Memory, Metaphor and Movement, eds S. C. Koch, T. Fuchs, M. Summa, and C. Müller (Philadelphia: John Benjamins).

Wallbott, H. G. (1990). Mimik im Kontext. Die Bedeutung verschiedener Informationskomponen-ten für das Erkennen von Emotionen. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Wells, G. L., and Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of head movement on persuasion: compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1, 219–230. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp0103_2

CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, M., and Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychol. Bull. 131, 460–473. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.460

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: embodiment, body feedback, approach and avoidance motor behavior, attitudes, movement qualities, movement rhythms, movement analysis, dance movement therapy

Citation: Koch SC (2014) Rhythm is it: effects of dynamic body feedback on affect and attitudes. Front. Psychol. 5:537. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00537

Received: 09 March 2014; Accepted: 14 May 2014;
Published online: 10 June 2014.

Edited by:

Sergio Salvatore, University of Salento, Italy

Reviewed by:

Sabrina Cipolletta, University of Padua, Italy
Alessandro Gennaro, University of Salento, Italy

Copyright © 2014 Koch. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sabine C. Koch, SRH University Heidelberg, Maria-Probst-Straße 3, 69123 Heidelberg, Germany e-mail: sabine.koch@hochschule-heidelberg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.