Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 July 2014
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic How Humans Recognize Objects: Segmentation, Categorization and Individual Identification View all 17 articles

Perceptions of document relevance

\r\nPeter Bruza*Peter Bruza*Vivien ChangVivien Chang
  • Science and Engineering Faculty, Information Systems School, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, QLD, Australia

This article presents a study of how humans perceive and judge the relevance of documents. Humans are adept at making reasonably robust and quick decisions about what information is relevant to them, despite the ever increasing complexity and volume of their surrounding information environment. The literature on document relevance has identified various dimensions of relevance (e.g., topicality, novelty, etc.), however little is understood about how these dimensions may interact. We performed a crowdsourced study of how human subjects judge two relevance dimensions in relation to document snippets retrieved from an internet search engine. The order of the judgment was controlled. For those judgments exhibiting an order effect, a q–test was performed to determine whether the order effects can be explained by a quantum decision model based on incompatible decision perspectives. Some evidence of incompatibility was found which suggests incompatible decision perspectives is appropriate for explaining interacting dimensions of relevance in such instances.

1. Introduction

This article aims to shed light on how humans judge the relevance of documents. We will, however, take a modern view of what a document is. Nowadays individuals and groups interact with one another in a variety of information environments of ever increasing complexity. They are accessing search engines, sharing messages on Facebook, browsing short messages on their mobile devices from microblog sites like Twitter. In this setting, a document is usually very short, e.g., a Twitter post, or in some cases it is not a document at all, but rather a document surrogate, such as the query-biased summaries (snippets) of documents displayed in rankings produced by search engines.

Document relevance has been carefully studied over more than three decades within the fields of information science usually by identifying or employing known inter-subjective dimensions of relevance (Schamber et al., 1990; Barry, 1994; Mizzaro, 1997; Borlund, 2003). For example, Barry and Schamber (1998) identified the dimensions “presentation quality,” “currency,” “reliability,” “verifiability,” “geographic proximity,” “specificity,” “dynamism” and “accessibility” in a comprehensive study. A recent study examined how users determined which list of search results they preferred over another using five dimensions of relevance: “topicality,” “freshness” (currency), “authority” (credibility), “caption quality,” and “diversity” (Kim et al., 2013). Other dimensions have also been identified with respect to a particular genre document. For example, Chu (2012) identified the dimensions “specificity,” “ease of use” and “breadth” in the context of legal documents.

Whilst it is widely accepted that there are a variety of dimensions at play when it comes to judging relevance, little is known of how these dimensions may interact. The aim of this article is to adopt a decision theoretic perspective and test a novel cognitive decision model in which potential interactions between dimensions are a consequence of incompatible decision perspectives which impose an order effect on relevance judgments. Incompatible perspectives are a recent development in a field called “quantum cognition” (See, for example, Conte et al., 2007; Aerts, 2009; Bruza et al., 2009; Pothos and Busemeyer, 2009; Atmanspacher and Filk, 2010; Khrennikov, 2010; Busemeyer et al., 2011; Conte et al., 2011; Trueblood and Busemeyer, 2011; beim Graben et al., 2012; Busemeyer and Bruza, 2012; Conte, 2012; Dzhafarov and Kujala, 2012; Aerts et al., 2013; Blutner et al., 2013; Haven and Khrennikov, 2013). This field aims to apply the formalism of quantum theory in order to more adequately model cognitive phenemona. For example, decades of research have uncovered a whole spectrum of human judgment that deviates substantially from what would be normatively correct according to logic and probability theory. An example of the latter is the following:

Linda is 31 years old, single, outspoken, and very bright. She majored in philosophy. As a student, she was deeply concerned with issues of discrimination and social justice, and also participated in anti-nuclear demonstrationsÓ. Which is more probable:

(a) Linda is a bank teller, or

(b) Linda is a bank teller and is active in the feminist movement?

In this now famous experiment proposed by Tversky and Kahneman (1983), human subjects consistently rate option (b) as more probable than (a). However, according to probability theory, the probability of a conjunction of events must be less than or equal to the probability of a constituent event. Thereofore, according to the axioms of probabilty theory (b) is less probable than (a). Probability judgment errors of this nature have since become known as the “conjunction fallacy.”

The key to explaining the conjunction fallacy using a quantum model is the incompatibility between the perspective that Linda is a bank teller and her being a feminist. Consider Figure 1A. The perspective “Linda is a feminist” is represented as a two dimensional vector space where the basis vector F corresponds to the decision “Linda is a feminist” and F corresponds to “Linda not being a feminist.” A similar two dimensional vector space corresponds to the perspective of Linda being a bank teller B, or not B. Initially, the cognitive state of the subject is represented by the vector Ψ, which is suspended between both sets of basis vectors. This situation represents the subject being undecided about whether Linda is a bank teller or a feminist. Suppose the subject now decides that Linda is a feminist. This decision is modeled by Ψ “collapsing” onto the basis vector labeled F. (The probability of the decision corresponds to the square of the length of the projection of the cognitive state Ψ onto the basis vector F, denoted ‖PFψ‖2). Observe how the subject is now necessarily uncertain about Linda being a bank teller because the basis vector F is suspended between the two basis vectors B and B by the angle θ. The hall mark of incompatibility is the state of indecision from one perspective (e.g., the bank teller perspective) when a decision is taken from another (e.g., the feminist perspective). This indecision means the decision maker can't form the joint probability of Linda being both a feminist and a bank teller, Pr(F, B) (Busemeyer et al., 2011). (This is crucially different to the situation in standard probability theory in which events are compatible, and thus the joint probability is always defined).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Incompatible perspectives in a relevance judgment. (A) Incompatible perspectives in the Linda example. (B) Incompatible perspectives in judging document relevance.

The consequence of incompatibility is an interference term denoted Int. The partial derivation below shows that this term Int appears when the decision of whether Linda is a feminist is made in relation to the incompatible subspace corresponding to the decision perspective of her being a bank teller (represented by projector PB and its dual PB):

p(F)= PFψ2    (1)
       = (PF · I)ψ2    (2)
       =(PF·(PB+PB)ψ2    (3)
       =PFPBψ2+ PFPBψ2+Int    (4)

The intuition behind Equation 4 is that the law of total probability is being modified by the interference term. In probability theory this would be expressed as follows: p(F) = p(F, B) + p(F, B) + Int. When the interference term is zero, the law of total probability holds. This happens when the decision perspectives are compatible.

Incompatible decision perspectives are a recent development in cognitive modeling and their striking characteristic is the use of “quantum” probabilities. By quantum probabilities, we mean that the decision event space is modeled as a vector space rather than a Boolean algebra of sets. A key differentiator is the use of the interference term. When this term is non-zero, violations of the law of total probability occur. The interference term has been used in models of the perception of gestalt images (Conte et al., 2007; Khrennikov, 2010), models of the conjunction and other decision fallacies (Busemeyer et al., 2011; Conte et al., 2011), modeling violations of rational decison theory (Bordley, 1998; Pothos and Busemeyer, 2009; Khrennikov, 2010), modeling belief dynamics (Trueblood and Busemeyer, 2011) and conceptual processing (Gabora and Aerts, 2002; Gabora et al., 2008; Aerts, 2009; Aerts et al., 2013; Blutner et al., 2013). Broader works relate the formal structures used in quantum theory to cognition and other areas (Bruza et al., 2009; Khrennikov, 2010; Busemeyer and Bruza, 2012; Conte, 2012; Haven and Khrennikov, 2013).

Consider Figure 1B which has the same structure as the Linda problem depicted in Figure 1A. This figure comprises two perspectives regarding a decision of document relevance. Assuming that a human subject perceives a document's relevance via different perspectives in relation to their given information need, the “topicality” perspective is represented as a two dimensional vector space where the basis vector T corresponds to the decision “the information is topically related to the information need” and T corresponds to “the information is not topically related to the information need.” A similar two dimensional vector space corresponds to the perspective of the information being understandable U, or not U, to the human subject. Initially, the cognitive state of the human subject is represented by the vector Ψ, which is suspended between both sets of basis vectors. This situation represents the subject being undecided about whether the information being perused is topical or understandable. Suppose the subject now decides that the information is topical. This decision is modeled by Ψ “collapsing” onto the basis vector labeled T. Once again, the probability of the decision corresponds to the square of the length of the projection of the cognitive state Ψ onto the basis vector T, denoted ‖PTψ‖2.

Observe how the subject is now necessarily uncertain about whether the information is understandable because the basis vector T is suspended between the two basis vectors U and U by the angle θ. The intuition behind incompatibility in this case is that the subject may be confident in deciding the information is topically relevant but remain in two minds about whether they understand the information, for example, if the snippet is interspersed with specialized technical vocabulary as in Figure 2. An important consequence of incompatible decision perspectives is an order effect. In the context of the example, this means the probability of judging that the information is relevant differs when first considering “topicality” followed by “understandability” compared to when these decisions are reversed. This is because when decision perspectives are incompatible, projections do not commute, i.e., ‖PUPTψ‖2 ≠ ‖PTPUψ‖2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Example document snippet.

The preceding should not be taken to imply that all relevance judgments are modeled in terms of incompatible decision perspectives. In some cases, the perspectives may be compatible. For example, the subject can make a decision that the document is topically relevant and then also be certain in regard to their decision about the document's understandability. In formal terms, compatible decision perspectives entail that the projectors commute, i.e., ‖PUPTψ‖2 = ‖PTPUψ‖2.

The focus is this artice is to explore whether there is evidence for incompatible decision perspectives. The question then becomes how to determine whether the model presented in Figure 1B explains decisions of document relevance. Wang and Busemeyer (2013) have recently proposed an innovative solution to this question. They proved that if there is an order effect and a so called q−test holds, then a model based on incompatible decision perspectives like those depicted in Figure 1B is a valid cognitive decision model. In terms of our example, the q−test has the following form based on yes(y)/no(n) answers regarding “topicality” and “understandability”:

p(TyUn)+p(TnUy)=p(UyTn)+p(UnTy)    (5)

Let pTU = p(TyUn) + p(TnUy) define the probability of different answers when “topicality” T is asked first, followed by “understandability” U. Conversely, let pUT = p(UyTn) + p(UnTy) be the probability of different answers when the order of questions is first “understandability” followed by “topicality.” The q−test has the following form:

q=pABpBA=0    (6)

The advantage of the q−test is that it is a parameter free test. It has successfully been applied to motivate a quantum model in relation to order effects in political survey data (Wang and Busemeyer, 2013). In this article, we will examine: (1) whether there are order effects in relation to decisions pertaining to specific dimensions of relevance, and (2) whether a quantum model based on incompatible decision perspectives explains these order effects.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects

Relevance judgments were crowdsourced by the internet based Amazon's Mechanical Turk platform. Crowdsourcing is the outsourcing of tasks to an undefined, large group of people. In the case of Amazon's Mechanical Turk, crowdsourcing is a means of gathering data from users via “human intelligence tasks” (HITs) which are typically surveys for subjects, or “turkers” to answer. Turkers are paid a nominal fee, in this case between 12 and 20 cents per relevance judgment. If the data from the turker is deemed of sufficient quality, the owner of the HIT approves the payment. The quality of the data can be determined automatically by the system whereby after a set period of time, say an hour, then the data will be approved whereby the turker will be paid. This process can also be done manually before and after approval; thus increasing the quality of data collected. In this experiment, the data were manually approved.

The advantages of crowdsourcing is that data can be collected quickly, on a fairly large scale and at a reasonable price. The disadvantage is the extra effort needed in order to safeguard the quality of the data. As Mechanical Turk is internet based, there is little control over who the turkers are, where they are, and indeed, whether they are even human. For example, “bots,” i.e., software programs mimicking humans are known to take part and more or less randomly contribute data to an experiment. As a consequence, the quality of crowdsourced data can vary greatly. To combat this, we purposefully inserted questions in the HITS to collect qualitative data—a technique often used in crowdsoured experiments.

Furthermore, as an additional factor to ensure quality data, both “masters” as well as “normal” turkers were used. Masters have “demonstrated excellence” in performing crowdsourced experiments over an extended period with a required HIT Approval Rate of above 95% over at least one thousand HITs. In contrast to the “masters,” nothing much is known of regarding the performance of “normal” turkers. The experiment was timed to primarily source U.S. based turkers, who are thus likely to be proficient in English, however no tests were conducted to verify English proficiency.

2.2. Materials

The materials comprised queries and information in the form of document snippets.

Five queries were developed for this study, each of which is based around an information need, for example, see Figure 3. The query description comprises the name of a query topic, a short description and an accompanying narrative. The narrative is intended to frame the subject's perception of relevance. There is a possibility that the turker's background may interfere with the narrative around the query. For example, if the turker is a fan of technology, then there is significant likelihood that they will be biased toward specific information or brands of technology. The experimenters viewed that bias is intrinsic to search and therefore did not to try to compensate for it (White, 2013). In addition, the background of the turker may hinder their ability to sufficiently engage with the narrative. However there was evidence via the qualitative feedback questions that turkers were able to role-play in a satisfactory way, particularly the “masters.” For example, “..a little hard to determine what this is talking about and if I were a beginner I would have no clue.” or “…makes [the] document highly relevant, since the focus is for emerging technologies in 2013.” Finally, the narrative structure of the queries was adopted from long running Text Retrieval Conference Series run yearly by the U.S. National Institute of Standards and Technology1. Each query was designed to collect judgments pertaining to two specific dimensions of relevance chosen by the authors. Table 1 details the titles of the queries and the dimensions of relevance which were studied.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. HIT interface.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Queries and relevance dimensions.

The relevance dimensions studied are further detailed in Table 2. “Topicality” has been chosen as a primary dimension to be examined across all queries because this dimension has been consistently identified in previous studies as a primary factor in relevance judgments (e.g., Barry and Schamber, 1998; Borlund, 2003; Chu, 2012). In addition, search engine algorithms are based on queries and finding a match in regards to keywords as a matter or correlating topically related material.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Definition of relevance dimensions.

Secondary dimensions depend on the query. Once the queries had been established, the authors designated likely secondary dimensions. Through pilot studies, the choice for the secondary dimension was refined when other factors began to creep into turker's comments. For example, during initial stages of the pilot, one of the first HITs published was the “Emerging Technology” query involving the dimensions of “topicality” and “understandability.” Very quickly, it was realized that “credibility” was a factor that was constantly brought up by turkers in qualitative feedback. This was possibly also due to the advancement and ubiquity of technology thus rendering “understandability” as not an issue. Other secondary factors were chosen in a similar fashion while some were heavily dependent on the query topic at hand. For example, the topic of global warming is one involving fixed dichotomous positions e.g., people either believe that this is occuring or they don't. Therefore, “believability” seemed likely to be a prominent relevance dimension in this case.

Secondary dimensions that were chosen for study are listed in the column labeled “Dimension 2” of Table 1. “Understandability” was chosen as snippets can sometimes be full of technical jargon, acronyms or specialized terms that can be challenging for the average person to comprehend. The dimension of “Believability” stems from a subject's personal beliefs and biases in relation to the information. A recent study showed that users were subject to their own biases as well as biases inherent in the search engine (White, 2013). “Interest” is the dimension of relevance pertaining to how novel or entertaining the information is. “Sentimentality” is a dimension which pertains to emotional responses to information. Sentiment analysis is a very active area of research in relation to internet-based technologies and applications, for example, data mining techniques to identify positive or negative sentiments or opinions in product reviews.

Corresponding to each query was a query-biased summary of a document, which we will refer to as a document “snippet.” (See Figure 2). Document snippets were used as these are an increasingly prevalent form of information on which decisions of relevance are made in relation to modern information environments. The document snippets used in this study were sourced from the Google search engine.

Snippets were selected based on the likelihood that decisions regarding the two chosen dimensions of relevance were likely to involve some uncertainty. This is because we hypothesize that incompatibility between these dimensions is more likely to occur when such uncertainty was present. Unfortunately, there is no theory to predict which dimensions may be incompatible so a crowsourced pilot study was conducted. This study involved 10 snippets per query with between 8 and 10 master turkers making judgments in each order condition. In order to verify that uncertainty was present a four point rating scale was used to collect decisions. For each query, the snippet for which the q− test was closest to zero was selected as being most likely to be subject to incompatibility. None of the subjects in the pilot took part in the experiment presented here. This could easily be verified as each turker has a unique identifier.

2.3. Procedure

The experiment (i.e., the HIT) consisted of five elements which were presented in sequence. Each element was based around a query, and a subject was required to process all five elements.

Each element comprises the query description followed by a document snippet, two judgments and finally the input of qualitative data. Figure 3 depicts one such element. In each judgment a subject is asked to rate a dimension of relevance on a four point scale. It was assumed that a subject can make judgments on dimensions within a given query topic independently of other query topics.

A single factor design was employed where the order of the judgments was manipulated. For example, in one condition a given dimension, e.g., “topicality” is rated first (the “non-comparative” context for the decision on topicality), followed by a rating of a the “understability” dimension. In the second condition, the order of the ratings is reversed e.g., the rating on “topicality” is second after the “understandability” dimension is rated (the “comparative” context for the decision on topicality). As each turker has a unique identifier, those turkers who attempted both conditions were removed from the data.

Subsequent to the judgments, subjects were asked to comment on factors that influenced their judgments. This aspect served for both quality control as well as a source of qualitative data to better undertsand the factors involved when turkers make judgments. By doing so, we discarded the data from any turker where the answers were blank, superfluous, e.g., “this is very good and gainful,” or didn't make sense, e.g., “The sway there marketed with different topics.” In the event that qualitative data were borderline acceptable such as “don't know,” or “not sure” (both of which could be supplied by a bot), the time taken to complete the HIT was also taken into consideration: If the time spent was less than 50 s for the HIT, the data were also discarded as we deemed a minimum of 10 s per query as being required to meaningfully read the query topic, rate two dimensions and supply qualitative feedback.

Finally, the Mechanical Turk interface does not afford the ability to time a turker per query, so the time taken to make judgments in relation to a given query could not be collected for analysis.

3. Results

A total of fifty “normal” turkers submitted data for the condition where the “topicality” dimension was presented first (non-comparative context for topicality), of which eighteen were discarded. Conversely, thirty-six “normal” turkers submitted data for the comparative context of “topicality,” of which four were discarded. This left n = 32 subjects in each condition. Despite repeated attempts to recruit “master” turkers, we failed to secure numbers sufficient for reliable statistical analysis. Therefore, their rating data are not reported but some qualitative responses were retained for illustrative purposes.

The results are presented in yes/no contingency tables in order for the q−test to be applied. This was achieved by mapping the four point graded relevance judgments to yes/no decisions in the following way: A grade of 3 or 4 was translated to a “yes,” whereas a grade of 1 or 2 was translated to a “no.” For example, consider Figure 3. Using the proposed mapping, a topical judgment of “4 = Very topically related” and “3 = Topically related” translate into a decision of “yes.” After the yes/no mapping, contingency tables can be constructed for each decision and these are presented in Figure 4 for the “normal” turkers. Some of the queries have data with less than 32 subjects as for these queries a turker rated one dimension, without rating the other. In such cases, the data for that query were omitted.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Yes/no contingency tables from “normal” turkers. The left hand side represents the condition where topicalilty is decided first (Non-comparative context for a decision on topicality). The right hand side represents the condition where topicality is decided second (Comparative context for a decision on topicality).

In order to apply the q−test, the presence of an order effect must first be established. An order effect is determined by comparing the agreement rates obtained in a non-comparative vs. a comparative context. An order effect occurs when the proportion of subjects who decided “yes” differs significantly in the comparative vs. non-comparative contexts. A two-tailed χ−square test of equality of proportions between populations was carried out (α = 0.05) and those queries exhibiting an order effect are bolded in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary table of q-test values.

Based on the contingency tables presented in Figure 4, the q− test values for the “normal” turkers were computed using equation (6) and presented in Table 3.

4. Discussion

For the query topics where there is an order effect, the quantum model based on incompatible decision perspectives predicts q = 0 (Wang and Busemeyer, 2013). Table 3 accords with this prediction for the queries “Treatment for Arthritis” and “Causes of Global Warming.” However, there are two other queries exhibiting an order effect but for which q ≠ 0. In these cases, the prediction of the quantum model may not actualize due to the quite small sample sizes in both conditions, or that the quantum model is not a valid explanation for these queries. More experimentation with larger sample sizes is needed to resolve this distinction.

Four out of five queries displayed an order effect (α = 0.05). The presence of an order effect means that the subjects' decision cannot be validly modeled by a joint probability distribution spanning binary variables corresponding to the underlying dimensions of relevance. For example, consider Figures 4A,B. In the non-comparative context for a decision on topicality, the marginal probability that the document is topical is summed across understandability:

p(T=y)=p(T=y,U=y)+p(T=y,U=n)    (7)
              =0.4063+0.2813     (8)
              =0.6876    (9)

Note that this probability is significantly different (α = 0.05) when understandability provides the comparative context for deciding topicality: p(T = y) = 0.1936 + 0.3871 = 0.5807. It is this difference which identifies an order effect but as the marginal probability is not constant, it is not possible to construct a single joint probability distribution p(T, U) to model the relevance decisions. As a consequence, a common modeling approach is ruled out. This approach assumes p(T, U) exists whereby the decision in the non-comparative context around topicality is modeled by the marginal probability p(T) and the decision in the comparative context is modeled by conditioning the distribution based on how understanding was first decided, i.e., p(T|U = y) or p(T|U = n).

In summary, order effects were detected between dimensions of relevance for the majority of queries and some evidence that a quantum model based on incompatible decision perspectives is a valid explanation. However, this evidence is not yet strong. Experiments with larger sample sizes and a larger collection of queries and snippets are required to determine the prevalence of incompatible perspectives in perceptions of document relevance. It should be mentioned, however, that this study differentiates itself from many previous studies in that a much larger number of subjects were involved. For example, nine subjects provided relevance judgments in Chu (2012).

According to Cooper (1971) the concept of relevance comprises both “logical relevance” and “utility.” Logical relevance is defined as “whether or not a piece of information is on a subject which has some topical bearing on the information need” and utility has to do with “the ultimate usefulness of the piece of information.” It seems that perceptions of utility or usefulness of a particular snippet involves cognitive processing of a variety of factors including those dimensions examined in this study. It became apparent from the qualitative feedback that relevance is a multifaceted, dynamic decision process. For example, in the “Global Warming” query, “reputation,” “credibility” and “scientific” were used to describe factors that the turkers themselves ranked highly compared to “believability” which was the chosen secondary dimension. This could suggest that the dimensions of “credibility” and “believability” mentioned as being distinct in previous studies are in fact hardly distinguishable during some relevance decisions. Not only were there more than a few factors at play, but the dimensions of “topicality” and “understandability” were featured in qualitative feedback across all queries. Furthermore, comments mentioning multiple (i.e., greater than two) factors were reasonably common. For example, one turker elegantly wrote “whether it (the search result) is on topic, credible, and goes into sufficient detail.” Interestingly, many of these comments noted “topicality” in ways that suggested that even though a snippet was topically related, this did not necessarily translate to the snippet being deemed relevant. This was a shift from the pilot study where turkers would state very clearly in their comments that topicality was nearly always the first factor they considered and if a snippet was topically related, then they would judge it to be relevant. The shift may have been due to the final design in which turkers processed five different queries which exposed them to a broader spectrum of relevance dimensions than was the case in the pilot study. Such qualitative feedback calls the experimental design into question, namely, is it methodologically sound to focus the subjects' attention on two dimensions when more are at play? In addition, were these extra dimensions coming into play because the subject was learning about relevance as they proceeded through the queries? The experimental design did not control for such a learning effect as it was assumed that each query topic could be judged independently of the others. An alternate design would allow subjects to select the two dimensions they deem most prominent and then rate them, or only allow subjects to rate a singe query topic.

5. Conclusion

This article put forward an experimental framework for examining whether dimensions of relevance interact via an order effect. The data collected from a crowdsourced study suggests that in some decisions regarding dimensions of relevance, this interaction can be explained in terms of a quantum model based on incompatible decision perspectives. Assuming that such interactions are fairly prevalent, what are the consequences? Currently in information processing systems, such as search engines, there is a general lack of effective user models. Should the user be making decisions of relevenance based on incompatible decision perspectives, then a model of the user based on standard probability would not be appropriate. The field of quantum cognition has shown that incompatibility implies that the law of total probability does not hold. Current computational systems are founded on standard probability theory. For example, consider the corpus-based computational model proposed by Lin and He (2009). This model takes the dimensions of both “topicality” and “sentiment” into account.

At the heart of the model is the following factorization: p(w, z, s) = p(w| z, s)p(z, s), where w is a random variable over a vocabulary of terms extracted from the corpus, z is a random variable over a set of latent topics, and s is a random variable over a set of sentiment labels (e.g., a binary variable describing a positive or negative sentiment). Note at its foundation, the model relies on the joint probability p(z, s), which describes the joint probability over topics and sentiments. In other words, the model assumes that “topicality” and “sentiment” are compatible. Should incompatibility manifest in the user's cognition, such a joint probability is undefined. This opens the door for dissonance between the relevance decisions made by the system as opposed to those made by the user. In short, the presence of incompatible decision perspectives suggests users can better be modeled by a “non-classical” probability theory like that proposed by the field of quantum cognition.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

The authors would like to thank Jerome Busemeyer for his advice on the application of the q−test as well as the helpful comments of the reviewers.

Footnotes

References

Aerts, D. (2009). Quantum structure in cognition. J. Math. Psychol. 53, 314–348. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2009.04.005

CrossRef Full Text

Aerts, D., Gabora, L., and Sozzo, S. (2013). Concepts and their dynamics: a quantum-theoretic modeling of human thought. Top. Cogn. Sci. 5, 737–772. doi: 10.1111/tops.12042

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Atmanspacher, H., and Filk, T. (2010). A proposed test of temporal nonlocality in bistable perception. J. Math. Psychol. 54, 314–321. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2009.12.001

CrossRef Full Text

Barry, C. (1994). User-defined relevance criteria: an exploratory study. J. Am. Soc. Inform. Sci. 45, 145–159.

Barry, C., and Schamber, L. (1998). Users' criteria for relevance evaluation: a cross-situational comparison. Inform. Process. Manag. 34, 219–236.

beim Graben, P., Filk, T., and Atmanspacher, H. (2012). Epistemic entanglement due to non-generating partitions of classical dynamical systems. Int. J. Theor. Phys. 52, 723–734. doi: 10.1007/s10773-012-1381-6

CrossRef Full Text

Blutner, R., Pothos, E., and Bruza, P. (2013). A quantum probability perspective on borderline vagueness. Top. Cogn. Sci. 5, 711–736. doi: 10.1111/tops.12041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bordley, R. (1998). Violations of compound probability principles: toward a generalized Heisenberg uncertainty principle. Operat. Res. 46, 923–926. doi: 10.1287/opre.46.6.923

CrossRef Full Text

Borlund, P. (2003). The concept of relevance in IR. J. Am. Soc. Inform. Sci. Tech. 54, 913–925. doi: 10.1002/asi.10286

CrossRef Full Text

Bruza, P., Busemeyer, J., and Gabora, L. (2009). Introduction to the special issue on quantum cognition. J. Math. Psychol. 53, 303–305. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2009.06.002

CrossRef Full Text

Busemeyer, J., and Bruza, P. (2012). Quantum Cognition and Decision. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511997716

CrossRef Full Text

Busemeyer, J., Pothos, E., Franco, R., and Trueblood, J. (2011). A quantum theoretical explanation for probability judgment errors. Psychol. Rev. 118, 193–218. doi: 10.1037/a0022542

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chu, H. (2012). Factors affecting relevance judgment: a report from TREC legal track. J. Document. 67, 264–287. doi: 10.1108/00220411111109467

CrossRef Full Text

Conte, E. (2012). Advances in Application of Quantum Mechanics in Neuroscience and Psychology: A Clifford Algebraic Approach. New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers.

Conte, E., Khrennikov, A., Todarello, O., De Robertis, R., Federici, A., and Zbilut, J. (2011). On the possibility that we think in a quantum mechanical manner: an experimental verification of existing quantum interference effects in cognitive anomaly of conjunction fallacy. Chaos Complex. Lett. 4, 123–136.

Conte, E., Todarello, O., Federici, A., Vitiello, F., Lopane, M., Khrennikov, A., et al. (2007). Some remarks on an experiment suggesting quantum-like behaviour of cognitive entities and formulation of an abstract quantum mechanical formalism to describe cognitive entity and dynamics. Chaos Solit. Fract. 33, 1076–1088. doi: 10.1016/j.chaos.2005.09.061

CrossRef Full Text

Cooper, W. (1971). A definition of relevance for information retrieval. Infor. Storgae Retrieval 7, 19–37. doi: 10.1016/0020-0271(71)90024-6

CrossRef Full Text

Dzhafarov, R., and Kujala, J. (2012). Selectivity in probabilistic causality: where psychology runs into quantum physics. J. Math. Psychol. 56, 54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2011.12.003

CrossRef Full Text

Gabora, L., and Aerts, D. (2002). Contextualizing concepts using a mathematical generalization of the quantum formalism. J. Exp. Theor. Artif. Intell. 14, 327–358. doi: 10.1080/09528130210162253

CrossRef Full Text

Gabora, L., Rosch, E., and Aerts, D. (2008). Toward an ecological theory of concepts. Ecol. Psychol. 20, 84–116. doi: 10.1080/10407410701766676

CrossRef Full Text

Haven, E., and Khrennikov, A. (2013). Quantum Social Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139003261

CrossRef Full Text

Khrennikov, A. (2010). Ubiquitous Quantum Structure: From Psychology to Finance. Heidelberg; Dordrecht; London; New York, NY: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-05101-2

CrossRef Full Text

Kim, J., Kazai, G., and Zitouni, I. (2013). “Relevance dimensions in preference-based IR evaluation,” in Proceedings of the 36th Annual ACM Conference of Research and Development in Information Retrieval (SIGIR' 13) (New York, NY: ACM Press), 913–916.

Lin, C., and He, Y. (2009). “Joint sentiment/topic model for sentiment analysis,” in Proceedings of the 18th ACM Conference on Information and Knowledge Management (New York, NY: ACM Press), 375–384.

Mizzaro, S. (1997). Relevance: the whole history. J. Am. Soc. Inform. Sci. 48, 810–832.

Pothos, E., and Busemeyer, J. (2009). A quantum probability explanation for violations of “rational” decision theory. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 2171–2178. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0121

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schamber, L., Eisenberg, M., and Nilan, M. (1990). A re-examination of relevance: toward a dynamic, situational definition. Inform. Process. Manag. 26, 755–775. doi: 10.1016/0306-4573(90)90050-C

CrossRef Full Text

Trueblood, J., and Busemeyer, J. (2011). A quantum probability explanation for order effects on inference. Cogn. Sci. 35, 1518–1552. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01197.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunctive fallacy in probability judgment. Psychol. Rev. 90, 293–315. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.90.4.293

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wang, J., and Busemeyer, J. (2013). A quantum question order model supported by empirical tests of an a priori and precise prediction. Top. Cogn. Sci. 5, 689–710. doi: 10.1111/tops.12040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

White, R. (2013). “Beliefs and biases in web search,” in Proceedings of the 36th Annual ACM Conference of Research and Development in Information Retrieval (SIGIR' 13) (New York, NY), 3–12.

Keywords: document relevance, quantum cognition, information retrieval, cognitive modeling, user modeling

Citation: Bruza P and Chang V (2014) Perceptions of document relevance. Front. Psychol. 5:612. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00612

Received: 22 December 2013; Accepted: 30 May 2014;
Published online: 02 July 2014.

Edited by:

Chris Fields, New Mexico State University, USA (retired)

Reviewed by:

Emmanuel Pothos, City University London, UK
Falk Scholer, RMIT University, Australia

Copyright © 2014 Bruza and Chang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Peter Bruza, Science and Engineering Faculty, Information Systems School, Queensland University of Technology, GPO Box 2434, Brisbane, QLD 4001, Australia e-mail: p.bruza@qut.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.