Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 22 August 2014
Sec. Psychopathology

Biases in facial and vocal emotion recognition in chronic schizophrenia

  • 1EA 4712 ‘Behavior and Basal Ganglia’ Laboratory, Université de Rennes 1, Rennes, France
  • 2Psychiatry Unit, Guillaume Régnier Hospital, Rennes, France
  • 3‘Neuroscience of Emotion and Affective Dynamics’ Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, Switzerland
  • 4Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Switzerland
  • 5Neurology Unit, University Hospital of Rennes, France

There has been extensive research on impaired emotion recognition in schizophrenia in the facial and vocal modalities. The literature points to biases toward non-relevant emotions for emotional faces but few studies have examined biases in emotional recognition across different modalities (facial and vocal). In order to test emotion recognition biases, we exposed 23 patients with stabilized chronic schizophrenia and 23 healthy controls (HCs) to emotional facial and vocal tasks asking them to rate emotional intensity on visual analog scales. We showed that patients with schizophrenia provided higher intensity ratings on the non-target scales (e.g., surprise scale for fear stimuli) than HCs for the both tasks. Furthermore, with the exception of neutral vocal stimuli, they provided the same intensity ratings on the target scales as the HCs. These findings suggest that patients with chronic schizophrenia have emotional biases when judging emotional stimuli in the visual and vocal modalities. These biases may stem from a basic sensorial deficit, a high-order cognitive dysfunction, or both. The respective roles of prefrontal-subcortical circuitry and the basal ganglia are discussed.

Introduction

Chronic schizophrenia is a disabling disease that encompasses both cognitive and emotional disorders (Irani et al., 2011). In recent decades, emotion recognition impairments have received particular attention (Tremeau, 2006), not least because of their impact on social functioning (Kee et al., 2003). A better understanding of emotional impairment in chronic schizophrenia could lead to new therapies (e.g., emotional remediation) and improve social functioning (Penn et al., 2009).

The processing of emotional stimuli in schizophrenia has been investigated in several modalities. Some studies, for instance, have explored the visual modality with faces (see Kohler et al., 2010, for review and meta-analysis) while others have used vocal stimuli, such as emotional prosody (see Hoekert et al., 2007, for review and meta-analysis). These two meta-analyses revealed constant and replicable impairment of the perception of emotional faces and prosody and some clinical features, such as subtype, severity and medication, were found to be related to the impaired perception of emotional faces or prosody.

In addition, few studies have adopted an approach across sensorial modalities, exposing the same patients to both vocal and facial stimuli in two independent tasks. In their review, Edwards et al. (2002) examined seven studies featuring both facial and prosodic emotion recognition tasks. Results revealed a deficit in emotion recognition in both modalities, but highlighted several methodological issues, including clinical and demographic features. In particular, none of these studies examined the results for categorical emotions but used an overall score. Furthermore, the issue of subtype of schizophrenia is important because patients with early schizophrenia showed different performance than patients with chronic schizophrenia. For example, Edwards et al. (2001) identified a specific deficit in the recognition of fear and sadness in both modalities, in patients experiencing their first episode whereas Ramos-Loyo et al. (2012) demonstrated a specific deficit for fear across the two modalities in a group of patients with a paranoid subtype. As for Kucharska-Pietura et al. (2005), when they examined the recognition of emotion across both modalities in patients in either the early or the late stages of schizophrenia, they found that patients in the late stage of schizophrenia performed worse in both modalities and for all emotions than patients in the early stage and healthy controls (HCs). They concluded that emotion recognition impairments increase as the disease progresses. In daily life situations, we use simultaneously visual and vocal cues for recognizing emotion (de Gelder and Vroomen, 2000). Both patients and controls gain from combining modalities (i.e., visual and vocal; Fiszdon and Bell, 2009) but patients with schizophrenia still show poorer recognition abilities compared to controls when performing multimodal recognition tasks (de Gelder et al., 2005; de Jong et al., 2009). In order to attain a further understanding of emotion recognition impairment in schizophrenia, other studies have sought to identify recognition biases. An emotional bias is a systematic deviation in the emotional processing. Several studies have examined biases in emotional recognition in facial modality. In patients with chronic schizophrenia, Kohler et al. (2003) described an emotional bias resulting in the over-attribution of disgust and the under-attribution of happiness when labeling neutral faces. Habel et al. (2010) found that, compared with HCs, patients with schizophrenia over-attributed fear or anger to neutral stimuli. Premkumar et al. (2008), meanwhile, described an over-attribution of fear to angry expressions in a mixed population of outpatients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorders. In another study, men with chronic schizophrenia overattributed anger to neutral faces, whereas neutral faces were mistaken as sad faces by women with chronic schizophrenia (Weiss et al., 2007). Moreover, it has been suggested that patients with paranoid schizophrenia categorize neutral faces as angry, whereas patients with non-paranoid schizophrenia categorize them as sad (Pinkham et al., 2011). Only a few number of studies focused on bias in emotional recognition using vocal modality. Shea et al. (2007) showed that only patients with schizophrenia with auditory hallucinations are more likely to misattribute emotional prosody to neutral stimuli than patients without auditory hallucinations and HCs. Finally, a recent study investigated biases in emotion recognition in patients with stable schizophrenia in two modalities (i.e., visual or vocal) and multimodal settings (i.e., both visual and vocal; Thaler et al., 2013). In this study, patients exhibited a negative over-attribution during the vocal recognition. For a stimulus considered as neutral, Thaler et al. (2013) showed a negative over-attribution for the visual modality and a positive over-attribution for vocal and multimodal task in a group of patients with chronic schizophrenia. However, in this study, biases were examined toward only neutral stimuli and it is unclear whether emotional biases are observed across modalities for a large panel of emotional categories.

The present study was designed to test emotional biases in chronic schizophrenia in two modalities (facial and vocal) of emotion recognition. To avoid categorization biases, we asked participants to provide intensity ratings on a set of emotional visual analog scales in two facial and vocal emotion tasks taken from Péron et al. (2010, 2011). This new method allowed intensity rating on target scales (i.e., rating intensity on the emotional scale corresponding to the relevant stimuli) and on several non-target scales (i.e., rating intensity on all the other emotional scales). Consistent with previous studies on emotional biases in the visual modality and vocal modality, we hypothesized that patients with chronic schizophrenia would show more emotional bias (by attribution of greater intensity on non-target scales) in the two sensorial modalities.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty-three (15 men and 8 women) patients with chronic schizophrenia and 23 (12 men and 11 women) HCs participated in this study. All participants were native French speakers.

Patients who tested as clinically stable were recruited from outpatient units at Guillaume Régnier Hospital (Rennes, France). The diagnosis of schizophrenia was established by a clinically trained psychiatrist according to the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Inventory (Sheehan et al., 1998), based on the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994). All the patients were taking antipsychotic medication at the time of testing. Six were also taking antidepressants (selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors).

We also recruited 23 HCs. Inclusion criteria for HCs were no current or past mental illness or any psychotropic treatment.

Exclusion criteria for all participants were neurological and systemic illness, head injury with loss of consciousness longer than 15 min, significantly impaired vision or auditory acuity, and a score below 130 on the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (MDRS; Mattis, 1988).

Patients and HCs were matched on sex, age, education level and handedness (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory; Oldfield, 1971). The clinical and demographic characteristics of the two groups are set out in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Clinical and demographic characteristics of patients with schizophrenia and healthy control participants.

Written informed consent was obtained from each participant and the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (CPP Ouest II- Angers; no. 2012/16).

Psychopathological and Neuropsychological Assessment

The current severity of the patients’ psychiatric symptoms was assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (Kay et al., 1987), which is divided into three subscales (positive symptoms, negative symptoms and general psychopathology), the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (Andreasen, 1982) and the Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia (Addington et al., 1992).

In order to assess the relationship between cognitive dysfunction and emotion processing, participants underwent a neuropsychological assessment by a trained neuropsychologist (Table 2). This assessment took place in single 1-h session prior to the emotional tasks. We used the MDRS to assess overall cognitive functioning, and the Digit Span subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III; Wechsler, 1997) to examine verbal short-term and working memory. Processing speed and attention were evaluated by the Digit Symbol-Coding subtest of the WAIS-III (Wechsler, 1997). A battery of tests was used to assess executive functions: the categorical and literal fluency test (Cardebat et al., 1990), a Stroop Test (Stroop, 1935), the Trail Making Test (TMT; Reitan, 1958), and Nelson’s modified version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (MCST; Nelson, 1982). The integrity of the early stages of face perception was verified using the short version of the Benton Facial Recognition Test (Benton et al., 1983). To ensure that the participants were free of auditory impairment, they underwent the Montreal-Toulouse auditory agnosia battery (PEGA; Agniel et al., 1992). Results of the PEGA and the Benton Facial Recognition Test showed that none of the participants had any auditory impairment or prosopagnosia.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Neuropsychological background of patients with schizophrenia and healthy control participants.

Facial Stimuli

This task featured two different sets of 56 emotional facial expressions produced by eight actors (four male and four female) per set. Six emotions (fear, disgust, anger, sadness, surprise, and happiness) were depicted, alongside neutral faces. For each emotion, there were four male faces and four female ones, making a total of eight stimuli per emotional category. These photographs were taken from Ekman and Friesen’s Pictures of Facial Affect (Ekman and Friesen, 1978) and the Karolinska database (Lundqvist et al., 1998). Mean luminance, apparent contrast and spatial frequency were adjusted according to Delplanque et al. (2007). Each photograph was displayed until the response of participants on all intensity rating scales. Patients and HCs were assessed with two versions of emotional facial recognition task because patients were included in another follow-up study. Eleven patients with schizophrenia and 11 HCs were assessed with Version A (corresponding to the first set of 56 emotional facial expressions). Twelve patients and 12 HCs were assessed with the Version B (corresponding to the second set of stimuli).

Vocal Stimuli

The vocal stimuli were taken from the Montreal Affective Voices database developed and validated by Belin et al. (2008). They consisted of non-verbal affect bursts devoid of semantic content (vowel “ah”). We selected two sets of 35 vocal stimuli pronounced by 10 actors (five men and five women). Seven categories were used in this study (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise, plus a neutral stimulus). The mean duration of the stimuli was 1084.87 ms (range: 240–4310 ms) and the mean energy of the stimuli was 73.40 dB (range: 47.5–85 dB). Participants were told that they could listen again to each stimulus as many as six times, by clicking on a button on upper right the computer interface. All stimuli were played binaurally via stereo headphones. For the reasons described above, patients and HCs were assessed with two versions of emotional vocal recognition task. Eleven patients with schizophrenia and 11 HCs were assessed with Version A (corresponding to the first set of 35 emotional vocal expressions). Twelve patients and 12 HCs were assessed with the Version B (corresponding to the second set of stimuli).

Emotion Recognition Procedure

We administered two emotional tasks: a facial emotion recognition task featuring facial stimuli and a vocal emotion recognition task featuring vocal stimuli. The facial emotion recognition task was always performed before the vocal emotion recognition task. The procedure was the same for both. Participants were seated in a quiet room, in front of a computer. Each condition (faces or voices) was displayed by an Authorware program.

At the beginning of each trial, a progress bar appeared on the computer screen. This was followed by the stimulus and participants were asked to rate its emotional content on scales that were simultaneously displayed on the screen. More specifically, participants were instructed to indicate the extent to which the different emotional categories were expressed on visual analog scales ranging from Not at all (scoring 0) to Very much (scoring 100). Participants therefore rated each stimulus on seven scales (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise, plus neutral). When participants completed all assessments of intensity, a button appeared and the next stimulus could be played by clicking on this button. They were given two examples per task, not used in the main task, in order to familiarize themselves with the procedure. An example of the computer interface for the both emotional tasks was provided in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Computer interface for the facial emotional task.

The entire protocol was completed in 90 min with a pause between clinical assessments and evaluation of emotion recognition.

Statistical Analysis

For the two emotion recognition tasks (facial and vocal), we performed two levels of analysis.

Categorical judgments

First, we compared the percentages of correct responses for the two tasks. For each trial, we compared the rating of intensity for each scale. A response was deemed to be correct when a participant rated the target scale higher than all the non-target scales. If the intensity were higher for the target scale (e.g., the anger scale for an angry face), we scored “1”; if not we scored “0.” We summed the score for each task and for each emotional category and then, we calculated the percentage of correct responses. We performed a repeated measures ANOVA with group (two levels: patients and HCs) as the between-subjects variable and task (two levels: visual and. vocal) and emotion (seven levels: anger, fear, sadness, disgust surprise, happiness, and neutral) as the within-subjects variable.

Intensity ratings

Then, we compared the ratings given by the two groups for each task (facial and vocal) on the scales for each type of emotion and for each individual scale, distinguishing between (1) the target scales, that is, the mean ratings on the scales (e.g., Anger scale) corresponding to the relevant stimuli (e.g., anger stimulus), and (2) the non-target scales, that is, the mean ratings on the scales that did not correspond to the stimuli (e.g., Fear scale for anger stimulus). This second analyses enabled us to take into account target ratings on incorrect responses (e.g., when recognizing an “anger” stimuli, rating 80% on the “anger” scale and “90%” on the surprise scale). In order to pinpoint impaired emotion biases in schizophrenia, we performed contrasts for the non-target ratings for each condition. To this end, we ran repeated-measures ANOVA with group (patients and HCs; two levels) as a between-participants factor, and emotion (seven levels) and scale (seven levels) as within-participants factors. We compared the HC and patient groups on each emotional category and each rating scale.

Others

Sociodemographic, psychiatric and neuropsychological data were compared using at-test for two independent groups for two independent groups (patients and HCs).

Versions A and B of the emotional recognition tasks were compared using a t-test for two independent groups (version A versus Version B) in the HC group.

Correlations between (1) clinical assessments and daily neuroleptic dose and vocal and facial emotion recognition (2) executive functions performances and the results of the emotion recognition tasks were assessed using Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient for the patients group.

Statistical analyses were performed using Statistica 8.0. The significance threshold was set at p = 0.05.

Results

Neuropsychological Assessments

The patients with schizophrenia scored significantly lower than HCs on the MDRS (t = 2.58; p = 0.01), categorical fluency (t = 2.8; p = 0.007), TMT Part A (t = -3.54; p < 0.001), TMT Part B (t = -3.05; p = 0.004), TMT B–A (t = -2.21; p = 0.03), MCST perseverative errors (t = -2.64; p = 0.01), and the Digit Symbol-Coding subtest (t = 4.06; p < 0.001), but not on any of the other cognitive tests (see Table 2).

Emotion Recognition Tasks

Analysis of categorical judgments (Table 3 and Figure 2)

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Number of correct responses, expressed as a percentage of total responses (standard errors, SE) for categorical judgments in the facial and vocal emotion recognition tasks of patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Accuracy in percentage of correct responses for facial and vocal emotion recognition task. Error bars indicate standard errors of mean.

Analysis revealed an Emotion × Task interaction [F(6,264) = 27.97, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.39]. Contrasts revealed that participants performed better at “anger” [F(1,44) = 34.08, p < 0.001] and “surprise” [F(1,44) = 154.89, p < 0.001] stimuli during the facial recognition task. Conversely, participants performed better at “sadness” [F(1,44) = 19.95, p < 0.001] and “fear” [F(1,44) = 5.31, p = 0.03] in vocal recognition task.

Analysis revealed a main effects of task [F(1,44) = 17.30, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.28] and a main effect of emotion [F(6,264) = 25.38, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.37]. A main effect of group was observed [F(1,44) = 12.58, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.22]. Patients were less accurate than HC’s.

There was no Group × Emotion interaction [F(1,44) = 1.75, p = 0.13, partial η2 = 0.04], no Group × Task interaction [F(1,44) = 1.64, p = 0.21, partial η2 = 0.04] and no Group × Task × Emotion [F(6,264) = 1.93, p = 0.10, partial η2 = 0.04].

Intensity ratings

Facial recognition emotion task (Figure 3). The significant differences between the two groups on intensity ratings of target emotions on the non-target scales are shown in Figure 3. We found a Group × Emotion × Scale interaction, F(36,1584) = 4.21, p < 0.0001, partial eta-squared (η2) = 0.09. Analysis revealed main effects of emotion, F(6,264) = 23.97, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.35, group, F(1,44) = 6.50, p = 0.01, partial η2 = 0.13, and scale, F(6,264) = 18.26, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.29, and an Emotion × Scale interaction, F(36,1584) = 273.55, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.86. There was no Group × Emotion interaction, F(6,264) = 1.59, p = 0.15, partial η2 = 0.03, and no Group × Scale interaction, F(6,264) = 0.85, p = 0.53, partial η2 = 0.02. Results of the extended statistical analyses for each stimulus and each scale are reported next. Anger stimulus. Ratings on the Anger Scale showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC [F(1,44) = 1.77, p = 0.19]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Neutral scale [F(1,44) = 5.41, p = 0.02], the Happiness Scale [F(1,44) = 7.63, p = 0.008], the Fear scale [F(1,44) = 6.41, p = 0.01] and the Disgust scale [F(1,44) = 8.34, p = 0.006] significantly higher than HC did. Disgust stimulus. Rating on the Disgust Scale showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC, [F(1,44) = 1.90, p = 0.18]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Neutral scale [F(1,44) = 5.33, p = 0.03], the Fear scale [F(1,44) = 6.11, p = 0.02], the Sadness scale [F(1,44) = 9.19, p = 0.004] and the Anger scale [F(1,44) = 13.71, p < 0.001] significantly higher than HC did. Fear stimulus. Rating on the “Fear” scale showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC, [F(1,44) = 0.65, p = 0.42]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Neutral scale [F(1,44) = 4.25, p = 0.045], Anger scale [F(1,44) = 8.99, p = 0.004], Happiness scale [F(1,44) = 5.07, p = 0.03] and the Sadness scale [F(1,38) = 8.92, p = 0.005] significantly higher than HC did. Happiness stimulus. Rating on “Happiness” scale, contrasts showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC [F(1,44) = 0.25, p = 0.61]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Neutral scale [F(1,44) = 11.99, p = 0.001] significantly higher than HC did. Neutral stimulus. Rating on “Neutral” scale, contrasts showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC [F(1,44) = 3.47, p = 0.07]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Happiness scale [F(1,44) = 7.36, p = 0.009], Fear scale [F(1,44) = 5.87, p = 0.02], Disgust scale [F(1,44) = 5.35, p = 0.03] and the Surprise Scale [F (1,44) = 5.24, p = 0.03] significantly higher than HC did. Sadness stimulus. Rating on the “Sadness” scale showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC, [F(1,44) = 0.2, p = 0.90]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Neutral scale [F(1,44) = 3.35, p = 0.03] significantly higher than HC did. Surprise stimulus. Rating on “Surprise” scale showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC [F(1,44) = 0.44, p = 0.51]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Neutral scale [F(1,38) = 5.24, p = 0.03], Happiness scale [F(1,44) = 6.13, p = 0.02] and the Fear Scale [F(1,44) = 5.55, p = 0.02] significantly higher than HC did.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Emotional biases on target and non-target scales for each category of emotional stimuli in the facial emotion recognition task. Error bars indicate standard errors of mean.

Vocal recognition emotion task (Figure 4). Figure 4 shows the significant differences between patients and HCs on intensity ratings of target emotions on the non-target scales. We found a Group × Emotion × Scale interaction, F(36,1584) = 3.68, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.08. Analyses revealed main effects of emotion, F(6,264) = 4.96, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.10, and scale, F(6,264) = 11.53, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.21, and Emotion × Scale, F(36,1584) = 198.95, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.82, Group × Emotion, F(6,264) = 3.28, p = 0.003, partial η2 = 0.07, and Group × Scale, F(6,264) = 2.25, p = 0.04, partial η2 = 0.05 interactions. There was no effect of group, F(1,44) = 3.02, p < 0.09, partial η2 = 0.06. Results of the extended statistical analyses for each stimulus and each scale are reported next. Anger stimulus. Rating on “Anger” scale showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC [F(1,44) = 1.77, p = 0.19]. When the stimulus was “Anger,” contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Neutral [F(1,44) = 5.37, p = 0.03], Fear scale [F(1,44) = 6.48, p = 0.01] and the Sadness scale [F(1,44) = 4.59, p = 0.04] significantly higher than HC did. Disgust stimulus. Rating on “Disgust” scale, contrasts showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC, [F(1,44) = 3.34, p = 0.07]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Happiness [F(1,44) = 3.67, p = 0.06], Anger scale [F(1,44) = 5.01, p = 0.03] and the Surprise scale [F(1,44) = 4.52, p = 0.04] significantly higher than HC did. Fear stimulus. Rating on “Fear” scale, contrasts showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC, [F(1,44) = 2.56, p = 0.12]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Neutral scale [F(1,44) = 6.63, p = 0.01], Happiness scale [F(1,44) = 5.43, p = 0.02] and the Sadness scale [F(1,44) = 5.48, p = 0.02] significantly higher than HC did. Happiness stimulus. Rating on “Happiness” scale showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC [F(1,38) = 2.39, p = 0.13]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the neutral scale [F(1,38) = 4.55, p = 0.04] and the Surprise scale [F(1,44) = 5.03, p = 0.03] significantly higher than HC did. Neutral stimulus. Rating on “Neutral” scale showed significant difference between SCHZ and HC [F(1,44) = 4.54, p = 0.04]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Happiness scale [F(1,38) = 6.38, p = 0.02] significantly higher than HC did. Sadness stimulus. Rating on “Sadness” scale showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC, [F(1,44) = 1.59, p = 0.21]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Neutral scale [F(1,44) = 7.43, p = 0.009] and the Happiness Scale [F(1,44) = 4.31, p = 0.04] significantly higher than HC did. Surprise stimulus. Rating on “Surprise” scale showed no significant difference between SCHZ and HC [F(1,44) = 2.22, p = 0.14]. Contrasts showed that SCHZ rated the Happiness scale [F(1,44) = 9.69, p = 0.003] significantly higher than HC did.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Emotional biases on target and non-target scales for each category of emotional stimuli in the vocal emotion recognition task. Error bars indicate standard errors of mean.

Version a Versus Version B Comparison

No significant difference was found between the accuracy (percentage of correct responses) in the HC group for version A and B for facial recognition task (t =0.50; p = 0.62) and vocal recognition task (t =-0.23; p = 0.82).

Correlations Between Emotion Recognition Results and Clinical Variables in the Patients With Schizophrenia

We found a significant correlation between the PANSS scores and the overall scores on the facial (r = -0.42; p = 0.04) and vocal (r = -0.43; p = 0.04) emotion recognition tasks. More specifically, scores on the PANSS positive subscale were correlated with overall scores on the facial (r = -0.54; p = 0.008) and vocal (r = -0.60; p = 0.002) recognition tasks, and scores on the PANSS general psychopathology subscale were correlated with overall scores on the facial (r = -0.51; p = 0.01) and vocal (r = -0.55; p = 0.006) recognition tasks.

Spearman’s correlation coefficients showed a significant correlation between digit span forward and the overall score on the vocal emotion recognition task (r = 0.58; p = 0.004), and between perseverative errors on the MCST and the overall score on the facial emotion recognition task (r = 0.43; p = 0.04). None of the other cognitive tests were shown to be related to the overall scores.

Discussion

In this study, we sought to pinpoint the presence of emotional biases in both facial and vocal emotion recognition in chronic schizophrenia, controlling for confounding factors. In both the visual and vocal modalities, our main findings pointed to emotional biases in the patients’ ratings of emotional intensity of all the target emotions.

Using an original emotion recognition procedure that had already been validated (Péron et al., 2010, 2011), we compared a group of 23 patients with chronic schizophrenia and 23 HCs on the recognition of emotion in both visual and vocal modalities. We used two complementary methods to analyze performances on the two emotional tasks. In what was effectively a forced-choice procedure, participants produced the categorical judgments that are classically used in the literature. Thus, in our first analysis, we compared the percentages of correct responses provided by the two groups in order to assess these categorical judgments. Our second analysis allowed us to describe the emotion recognition biases in greater depth, by looking at the intensity ratings provided for each emotional category. Results for categorical judgments (i.e., number of correct responses expressed as a percentage of total responses) for the two emotional tasks revealed that patients with schizophrenia performed more poorly than HCs. However, we failed to demonstrate an effect of modality on emotional recognition impairments for patients with schizophrenia. Very few studies have compared the effect of modality (vocal vs. visual) on emotion recognition, and their designs are too heterogeneous to draw clear conclusions.

At a second level of analysis, the patients with schizophrenia were found to be less discriminative than HCs in their intensity ratings for both modalities (facial and vocal). Although they provided the same intensity ratings on the target scales (i.e., categorical judgments), they responded differently on the non-target scales thus exhibiting biases in their responses. These results are consistent with previous studies reporting emotional biases (Kohler et al., 2003) in chronic schizophrenia.

Delusions and hallucinations in schizophrenia may contribute to the introduction of biases. Patients with greater positive (including delusions and hallucinations) and general symptoms (including anxiety, attentional deficit, and depression) achieve lower overall recognition scores in the two emotional paradigms (vocal and facial). Kee et al. (1998) reported a relationship between positive symptoms and facial and vocal emotion identification, consistent with greater impairment of emotion recognition in paranoid patients (Pinkham et al., 2011).

Moreover, patients with poorer digit span forward scores had poorer overall scores on the facial emotion recognition task, while a higher number of perseverations on the MCST was related to a lower overall score on the vocal recognition task. Bozikas et al. (2004) reported an association between neuropsychological deficits (especially in executive functions) and facial and vocal emotion identification. Our results point to a cognitive difficulty in inhibiting non relevant information (introducing biases) that could explain the deficit in facial and vocal emotion recognition in schizophrenia.

The biases in emotion processing observed here can be explained in three ways. The first explanation refers to the sensory deficit in schizophrenia. Several studies have reported a basic sensory disturbance in the processing of emotion in the facial (Turetsky et al., 2007; Wynn et al., 2008; Norton et al., 2009; Laprevote et al., 2010; McBain et al., 2010; Bedwell et al., 2013; Jahshan et al., 2013) and vocal (Leitman et al., 2005, 2007, 2010, 2011a; Gold et al., 2012; Kantrowitz et al., 2013) modalities. This is unlikely to be the case in our study since patients with basic sensory disturbances, as assessed with the Benton test and the PEGA, were excluded from our sample. However, one cannot rule out that biases in our sample were the due to a deficit in multisensory integration.

The second explanation concerns the lack of cognitive control in schizophrenia (Chambon et al., 2008; Lesh et al., 2011; Kring and Elis, 2013). Cognitive control is defined as the ability to mobilize cognitive resources to process relevant information and inhibit irrelevant information in daily life situations. Anticevic and Corlett (2012) discussed interference between relevant and irrelevant emotional stimuli resulting in over-responsiveness to neutral stimuli. Similarly, in our study, we found that patients with schizophrenia provided extremely high intensity ratings on the neutral scale when they listened to neutral stimuli. In a recent study featuring an emotion identification task with gradual exposure to stimuli, Lee et al. (2011) found that patients with schizophrenia explored happy and fearful faces differently. In an fMRI study, Fakra et al. (2008) examined the brain activity of patients with schizophrenia while they performed emotional face matching and identification tasks. Comparisons between patients and HCs revealed similar patterns of brain activity for the identification task, whereas the matching task was characterized by a lack of amygdala activity in the patient group, with patients undertaking more cognitive exploration than the HCs. According to Fakra et al. (2008), focusing on specific facial features requires more cognitive resources than overall exploration, and could explain the lack of amygdala activity. Using implicit and explicit tasks, Roux et al. (2010) demonstrated impairment for the explicit recognition of emotional prosody, whereas the implicit processing of emotional prosody (emotional version of the Stroop Test) was preserved. This study highlighted the impact of cognitive control on the processing of emotional prosody. This is likely to be the case here, as we found a relationship between overall performances and an executive function deficit in our sample of patients with chronic schizophrenia. Others studies yielded some explanations as to the mechanism behind the emotional deficit in schizophrenia, and highlighted the impact of misusing cognitive resources in emotion processing (Bach et al., 2009; Christensen et al., 2013).

Third and last, the mutual influence of sensory disorder and cognitive control impairment in schizophrenia may explain the emotional disorders described in the literature. In a recent study using event-related potentials, Pinheiro et al. (2013) reported impairment in both the early sensory and late cognitive stages of emotional prosody processing in patients with chronic schizophrenia.

Schizophrenia is related to brain dysfunction, especially in the prefrontal striatonigral circuit. Some studies have shown that a dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex is related to the identification of emotional context in schizophrenia (Gur et al., 2007; Fakra et al., 2008; Leitman et al., 2011b). Using an emotional go/no go task, Vercammen et al. (2012) compared the fMRI brain activity of schizophrenia patients with that of healthy participants. Inhibiting responses to negative emotional stimuli elicited increased activity in a brain network including the prefrontal, cingulate and parietal cortices in healthy participants, but not in patients with schizophrenia. During the inhibition of responses to positive emotional stimuli, patients with schizophrenia exhibited greater activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex than HCs. Other studies have reported a deficit in the balance of prefrontal cortex–amygdala activity in an easy emotion processing task (no cognitive load; Anticevic and Corlett, 2012) and an abnormal modulation in prefrontal-subcortical connectivity during a working memory task (Anticevic et al., 2012). Some studies have highlighted the role of the primary sensory cortex in the impairment of emotion recognition. Rolls et al. (2008) described a dynamic neural network in the frontal cortex with a stable state involved in several types of cognitive and emotion processing. This neural network can be affected by neural noise (i.e., stochastic neural firing), leading to maladaptive behavior that includes executive and emotional disorders. Moreover, the emotional noise observed in our study can be related to an imbalance between the prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia. Péron et al. (2013), based on Graybiel (1997) work, developed a model wherein the basal ganglia are involved in the generation of patterns of brain activation related to habits (or engrams), of over-learned cognitive, motor, and emotional sequences in the context of emotional processing. These habits or engrams can be uploaded by the basal ganglia in order to adapt more quickly and more accurately. Graybiel (1997) postulated that the basal ganglia system plays a role in the generation of positive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia.

This study had several limitations that need to be addressed. First, only 23 patients were assessed. However, we took care to recruit patients with the same clinical characteristics, in order to construct a homogenous group. Second, as described by Salgado-Pineda et al. (2005), dopamine influences emotion recognition. Our results may thus have been skewed by the patients’ medication (antipsychotics and, for some of them, antidepressants). Furthermore, we did not control for the physical properties (pitch, intensity and timbre) of the vocal stimuli we used, even though it is these variations in physical properties that create the profiles of emotional prosodies (Banse and Scherer, 1996; Grandjean et al., 2006). Another limit might be that the emotional task order was not counterbalanced and the presentation of stimuli within a task was not randomized raising the issue of the practice effect of our tasks. However, all participants were familiarized with the procedure before the two emotional tasks. Practice effect had a major impact on reaction time which was not the main variable in the present study (Poulton, 1982). Moreover, others studies using similar tasks did not report practice effect (e.g., Bach et al., 2009; Fiszdon and Bell, 2009).

Further research is needed to investigate the involvement of cognitive control in the management of top-down and bottom-up processing, using both implicit and explicit emotion processing tasks.

In summary, this study showed that chronic schizophrenia induces emotional biases for all emotions in two sensory modalities (visual and vocal), and appears to cause interference in emotion recognition. There are now at least two mechanisms that need to be, considered if we are to explain impairments in emotion recognition: a deficit in sensory functions and a lack of cognitive control. These results could help to enhance current cognitive and emotional remediation in schizophrenia.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Elizabeth Wiles-Portier for revising the English style as well as the reviewers for their precious comments on the earlier versions of this manuscript. We thank Dr. Florian Naudet (CIC INSERM 0203; Rennes) for his help in the statistical analysis of this study. The study was carried out at the Psychiatry Unit (Prof. Bruno Millet) of Guillaume Régnier Hospital in Rennes, France. This work was supported by a grant from the Fondation de la Recherche and the Fondation Pierre Deniker. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

Addington, D., Addington, J., Maticka-Tyndale, E., and Joyce, J. (1992). Reliability and validity of a depression rating scale for schizophrenics. Schizophr. Res. 6, 201–208. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(92)90003-N

CrossRef Full Text

Agniel, A., Joanette, P., Doyon, B., and Duchein, C. (1992). Protocole d’Evaluation des Gnosies Visuelles. Isbergues: OrthoEditions.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). DSM-IV: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Andreasen, N. C. (1982). Negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Definition and reliability. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 39, 784–788. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290070020005

CrossRef Full Text

Anticevic, A., and Corlett, P. R. (2012). Cognition-emotion dysinteraction in schizophrenia. Front. Psychol. 3:392. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00392

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Anticevic, A., Repovs, G., Krystal, J. H., and Barch, D. M. (2012). A broken filter: prefrontal functional connectivity abnormalities in schizophrenia during working memory interference. Schizophr. Res. 141, 8–14. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.07.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bach, D. R., Buxtorf, K., Grandjean, D., and Strik, W. K. (2009). The influence of emotion clarity on emotional prosody identification in paranoid schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 39, 927–938. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004704

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Banse, R., and Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 614–636. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.614

CrossRef Full Text

Bedwell, J. S., Chan, C. C., Cohen, O., Karbi, Y., Shamir, E., and Rassovsky, Y. (2013). The magnocellular visual pathway and facial emotion misattribution errors in schizophrenia. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 44C, 88–93. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.01.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Belin, P., Fillion-Bilodeau, S., and Gosselin, F. (2008). The Montreal Affective Voices: a validated set of nonverbal affect bursts for research on auditory affective processing. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 531–539. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.2.531

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. S., Varney, N., and Spreen, O. (1983). Contribution to Neuropsychological Assessment: A Clinical Manual. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Bozikas, V. P., Kosmidis, M. H., Kioperlidou, K., and Karavatos, A. (2004). Relationship between psychopathology and cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. Compr. Psychiatry 45, 392–400. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2004.03.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cardebat, D., Doyon, B., Puel, M., Goulet, P., and Joanette, P. (1990). Evocation lexicale formelle et sémantique chez des sujets normaux : performances et dynamiques de production en fonction du sexe, de l’âge et du niveau d’étude. Acta Neurol. Belg. 90, 207–217.

Chambon, V., Franck, N., Koechlin, E., Fakra, E., Ciuperca, G., Azorin, J. M.,et al. (2008). The architecture of cognitive control in schizophrenia. Brain 131, 962–970. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Christensen, B. K., Spencer, J. M., King, J. P., Sekuler, A. B., and Bennett, P. J. (2013). Noise as a mechanism of anomalous face processing among persons with Schizophrenia. Front. Psychol. 4:401. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00401

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

de Gelder, B., and Vroomen, J. (2000). The perception of emotions by ear and by eye. Cogn. Emot. 14, 289–311. doi: 10.1080/026999300378824

CrossRef Full Text

de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., de Jong, S. J., Masthoff, E. D., Trompenaars, F. J., and Hodiamont, P. (2005). Multisensory integration of emotional faces and voices in schizophrenics. Schizophr. Res. 72, 195–203. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.02.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

de Jong, J., Hodiamont, P., Van Den Stock, J., and de Gelder, B. (2009). Audiovisual emotion recognition in schizophrenia: reduced integration of facial and vocal affect. Schizophr. Res. 107, 286–293. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.10.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Delplanque, S., N’Diaye, K., Scherer, K., and Grandjean, D. (2007). Spatial frequencies or emotional effects? A systematic measure of spatial frequencies for IAPS pictures by a discrete wavelet analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 165, 144–150. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.05.030

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Edwards, J., Jackson, H. J., and Pattison, P. E. (2002). Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: a methodological review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 22, 789–832. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00130-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Edwards, J., Pattison, P. E., Jackson, H. J., and Wales, R. J. (2001). Facial affect and affective prosody recognition in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 48, 235–253. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00099-2

CrossRef Full Text

Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. (1978). Facial Action Coding System: A Technique for the Measurement of Facial Movement. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press.

Fakra, E., Salgado-Pineda, P., Delaveau, P., Hariri, A. R., and Blin, O. (2008). Neural bases of different cognitive strategies for facial affect processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 100, 191–205. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fiszdon, J. M., and Bell, M. D. (2009). Effects of presentation modality and valence on affect recognition performance in schizophrenia and healthy controls. Psychiatry Res. 170, 114–118. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.11.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gold, R., Butler, P., Revheim, N., Leitman, D. I., Hansen, J. A., Gur, R. C.,et al. (2012). Auditory emotion recognition impairments in schizophrenia: relationship to acoustic features and cognition. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 424–432. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11081230

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grandjean, D., Banziger, T., and Scherer, K. R. (2006). Intonation as an interface between language and affect. Prog. Brain Res. 156, 235–247. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56012-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Graybiel, A. M. (1997). The basal ganglia and cognitive pattern generators. Schizophr. Bull. 23, 459–469. doi: 10.1093/schbul/23.3.459

CrossRef Full Text

Gur, R. E., Loughead, J., Kohler, C. G., Elliott, M. A., Lesko, K., Ruparel, K.,et al. (2007). Limbic activation associated with misidentification of fearful faces and flat affect in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64, 1356–1366. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.12.1356

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Habel, U., Chechko, N., Pauly, K., Koch, K., Backes, V., Seiferth, N.,et al. (2010). Neural correlates of emotion recognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 122, 113–123. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.06.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hoekert, M., Kahn, R. S., Pijnenborg, M., and Aleman, A. (2007). Impaired recognition and expression of emotional prosody in schizophrenia: review and meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 96, 135–145. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.07.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Irani, F., Kalkstein, S., Moberg, E. A., and Moberg, P. J. (2011). Neuropsychological performance in older patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Schizophr. Bull. 37, 1318–1326. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq057

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jahshan, C., Wynn, J. K., and Green, M. F. (2013). Relationship between auditory processing and affective prosody in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 143, 348–353. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.11.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kantrowitz, J. T., Leitman, D. I., Lehrfeld, J. M., Laukka, P., Juslin, P. N., Butler, P. D.,et al. (2013). Reduction in tonal discriminations predicts receptive emotion processing deficits in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Schizophr. Bull. 39, 86–93. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr060

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., and Opler, L. A. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 13, 261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

CrossRef Full Text

Kee, K. S., Green, M. F., Mintz, J., and Brekke, J. S. (2003). Is emotion processing a predictor of functional outcome in schizophrenia? Schizophr. Bull. 29, 487–497. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007021

CrossRef Full Text

Kee, K. S., Kern, R. S., Marshall, B. D. Jr., and Green, M. F. (1998). Risperidone versus haloperidol for perception of emotion in treatment-resistant schizophrenia: preliminary findings. Schizophr. Res. 31, 159–165. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00026-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kohler, C. G., Turner, T. H., Bilker, W. B., Brensinger, C. M., Siegel, S. J., Kanes, S. J.,et al. (2003). Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: intensity effects and error pattern. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 1768–1774. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1768

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kohler, C. G., Walker, J. B., Martin, E. A., Healey, K. M., and Moberg, P. J. (2010). Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophr. Bull. 36, 1009–1019. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn192

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kring, A. M., and Elis, O. (2013). Emotion deficits in people with schizophrenia. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 9, 409–433. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185538

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kucharska-Pietura, K., David, A. S., Masiak, M., and Phillips, M. L. (2005). Perception of facial and vocal affect by people with schizophrenia in early and late stages of illness. Br. J. Psychiatry 187, 523–528. doi: 10.1192/bjp.187.6.523

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laprevote, V., Oliva, A., Delerue, C., Thomas, P., and Boucart, M. (2010). Patients with schizophrenia are biased toward low spatial frequency to decode facial expression at a glance. Neuropsychologia 48, 4164–4168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lee, J., Gosselin, F., Wynn, J. K., and Green, M. F. (2011). How do schizophrenia patients use visual information to decode facial emotion? Schizophr. Bull. 37, 1001–1008. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leitman, D. I., Foxe, J. J., Butler, P. D., Saperstein, A., Revheim, N., and Javitt, D. C. (2005). Sensory contributions to impaired prosodic processing in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 56–61. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leitman, D. I., Hoptman, M. J., Foxe, J. J., Saccente, E., Wylie, G. R., Nierenberg, J.,et al. (2007). The neural substrates of impaired prosodic detection in schizophrenia and its sensorial antecedents. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 474–482. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.3.474

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leitman, D. I., Laukka, P., Juslin, P. N., Saccente, E., Butler, P., and Javitt, D. C. (2010). Getting the cue: sensory contributions to auditory emotion recognition impairments in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 36, 545–556. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn115

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leitman, D. I., Wolf, D. H., Laukka, P., Ragland, J. D., Valdez, J. N., Turetsky, B. I.,et al. (2011a). Not pitch perfect: sensory contributions to affective communication impairment in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 611–618. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leitman, D. I., Wolf, D. H., Loughead, J., Valdez, J. N., Kohler, C. G., Brensinger, C.,et al. (2011b). Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the effects of task demand context on facial affect appraisal in schizophrenia. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 66–73. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lesh, T. A., Niendam, T. A., Minzenberg, M. J., and Carter, C. S. (2011). Cognitive control deficits in schizophrenia: mechanisms and meaning. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 316–338. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.156

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., and Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces – KDEF. Stockholm: Karolinsra Institutet.

Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia Rating Scale. Odessa, FL: Ressources Inc.

McBain, R., Norton, D., and Chen, Y. (2010). Differential roles of low and high spatial frequency content in abnormal facial emotion perception in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 122, 151–155. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.03.034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nelson, H. (1982). The National Adult Reading Test. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.

Norton, D., Mcbain, R., Holt, D. J., Ongur, D., and Chen, Y. (2009). Association of impaired facial affect recognition with basic facial and visual processing deficits in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 1094–1098. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.01.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

CrossRef Full Text

Penn, D. L., Keefe, R. S., Davis, S. M., Meyer, P. S., Perkins, D. O., Losardo, D.,et al. (2009). The effects of antipsychotic medications on emotion perception in patients with chronic schizophrenia in the CATIE trial. Schizophr. Res. 115, 17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.08.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Péron, J., El Tamer, S., Grandjean, D., Leray, E., Travers, D., Drapier, D.,et al. (2011). Major depressive disorder skews the recognition of emotional prosody. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35, 987–996. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.01.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Péron, J., Fruhholz, S., Verin, M., and Grandjean, D. (2013). Subthalamic nucleus: a key structure for emotional component synchronization in humans. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 358–373. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Péron, J., Grandjean, D., Le Jeune, F., Sauleau, P., Haegelen, C., Drapier, D.,et al. (2010). Recognition of emotional prosody is altered after subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 48, 1053–1062. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.12.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pinheiro, A. P., Del Re, E., Mezin, J., Nestor, P. G., Rauber, A., Mccarley, R. W.,et al. (2013). Sensory-based and higher-order operations contribute to abnormal emotional prosody processing in schizophrenia: an electrophysiological investigation. Psychol. Med. 43, 603–618. doi: 10.1017/S003329171200133X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pinkham, A. E., Brensinger, C., Kohler, C., Gur, R. E., and Gur, R. C. (2011). Actively paranoid patients with schizophrenia over attribute anger to neutral faces. Schizophr. Res. 125, 174–178. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Poulton, E. (1982). Influential companions: effects of one strategy on another in the within-subjects designs of cognitive psychology. Psychol. Bull. 91, 673–690. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.91.3.673

CrossRef Full Text

Premkumar, P., Cooke, M. A., Fannon, D., Peters, E., Michel, T. M., Aasen, I.,et al. (2008). Misattribution bias of threat-related facial expressions is related to a longer duration of illness and poor executive function in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Eur. Psychiatry 23, 14–19. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.10.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ramos-Loyo, J., Mora-Reynoso, L., Sanchez-Loyo, L. M., and Medina-Hernandez, V. (2012). Sex differences in facial, prosodic, and social context emotional recognition in early-onset schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. Treat. 2012, 584725. doi: 10.1155/2012/584725

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reitan, R. (1958). Validity of the trail making test as an indication of organic brain domage. Percept. Mot. Skills 8, 271–276. doi: 10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271

CrossRef Full Text

Rolls, E. T., Loh, M., Deco, G., and Winterer, G. (2008). Computational models of schizophrenia and dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 696–709. doi: 10.1038/nrn2462

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roux, P., Christophe, A., and Passerieux, C. (2010). The emotional paradox: dissociation between explicit and implicit processing of emotional prosody in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia 48, 3642–3649. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Salgado-Pineda, P., Delaveau, P., Blin, O., and Nieoullon, A. (2005). Dopaminergic contribution to the regulation of emotional perception. Clin. Neuropharmacol. 28, 228–237. doi: 10.1097/01.wnf.0000185824.57690.f0

CrossRef Full Text

Shea, T. L., Sergejew, A. A., Burnham, D., Jones, C., Rossell, S. L., Copolov, D. L.,et al. (2007). Emotional prosodic processing in auditory hallucinations. Schizophr. Res. 90, 214–220. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.09.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E.,et al. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59(Suppl. 20), 22–33; quiz 34–57.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Stroop, J. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

CrossRef Full Text

Thaler, N. S., Strauss, G. P., Sutton, G. P., Vertinski, M., Ringdahl, E. N., Snyder, J. S.,et al. (2013). Emotion perception abnormalities across sensory modalities in bipolar disorder with psychotic features and schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 147, 287–292. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.04.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tremeau, F. (2006). A review of emotion deficits in schizophrenia. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 8, 59–70.

Turetsky, B. I., Kohler, C. G., Indersmitten, T., Bhati, M. T., Charbonnier, D., and Gur, R. C. (2007). Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: when and why does it go awry? Schizophr. Res. 94, 253–263. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.05.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vercammen, A., Morris, R., Green, M. J., Lenroot, R., Kulkarni, J., Carr, V. J.,et al. (2012). Reduced neural activity of the prefrontal cognitive control circuitry during response inhibition to negative words in people with schizophrenia. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 37, 379–388. doi: 10.1503/jpn.110088

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

Weiss, E. M., Kohler, C. G., Brensinger, C. M., Bilker, W. B., Loughead, J., Delazer, M.,et al. (2007). Gender differences in facial emotion recognition in persons with chronic schizophrenia. Eur. Psychiatry 22, 116–122. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2006.05.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wynn, J. K., Lee, J., Horan, W. P., and Green, M. F. (2008). Using event related potentials to explore stages of facial affect recognition deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 679–687. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn047

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: emotional biases, facial emotion recognition, prosody, chronic schizophrenia, vocal emotion recognition

Citation: Dondaine T, Robert G, Péron J, Grandjean D, Vérin M, Drapier D and Millet B (2014) Biases in facial and vocal emotion recognition in chronic schizophrenia. Front. Psychol. 5:900. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00900

Received: 26 January 2014; Accepted: 29 July 2014;
Published online: 22 August 2014.

Edited by:

Beatrice De Gelder, Louvain University, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Jan Van Den Stock, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Belgium
Rebecca Watson, University of Glasgow, UK

Copyright © 2014 Dondaine, Robert, Péron, Grandjean, Vérin, Drapier and Millet. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Thibaut Dondaine, EA 4712 ‘Behavior and Basal Ganglia’ Laboratory, Université de Rennes 1, 2 Avenue du Professeur Léon Bernard, 35043 Rennes Cedex, France e-mail: thibaut.dondaine@gmail.com

Thibaut Dondaine and Gabriel Robert have contributed equally to this work.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.