Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 23 September 2014
Sec. Emotion Science

The role of oxytocin and alexithymia in the therapeutic process

  • 1Department of Personality Research, Institute of Psychology, Osnabrück University, Osnabrück, Germany
  • 2Behavioral Neuroscience at the Research Triangle Institute International, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
  • 3Department of Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands

A commentary on
Memory reconsolidation, emotional arousal and the process of change in psychotherapy: new insights from brain science

by Lane, R., Ryan, L. R., Nadel, L., and Greenberg, L. (2014). Behav. Brain Sci. 1–80. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14000041

Based on an extant literature review Lane et al. propose that therapeutic success results from changing conflictual, often non-conscious, emotional memories. Specifically, existent internal autobiographical models based on negative emotional memories become activated within the context of a positively colored therapeutic dyad and are reconsolidated as new, more positively colored internal models. Based on empirical evidence, we would like to extend this model by describing how the reconsolidation process may be facilitated on at least three levels of neurobiological functioning. Those three levels relate to an overarching principle in the function of the neuropeptide oxytocin to facilitate the construction and selection of positive internal models of social relationships and support.

First, oxytocin facilitates social bonds and trust as well as counteracts stress by dampening activity of physiological stress-systems and increasing parasympathic activity, and regulates glucocorticoid receptor expression in the hippocampus (Carter, 1998, see also Quirin et al., 2011). As such, oxytocin might facilitate the establishment of a positive therapeutic alliance and a relaxed atmosphere during therapy, which makes it easier for patients to awaken conflictual memories and tolerate concomitantly resurfacing negative emotions.

Second, oxytocin is implicated in the ontogenetic development of the neocortex and thus plays an important role in the construction of internal models (Carter, 2014). For example, it stimulates genetic regulation of the growth of the neocortex and the maintenance of the blood supply to the cortex—conditions that are prerequisites for the formation and sustentation of internal autobiographical models. In humans, oxytocin also facilitates cognitive control that helps individuals to bring behavior in line with internal models (Tops et al., 2013a). This is important since behavior control is a highly valued aspect of therapeutic success, which remained unaddressed by Lane and colleagues.

Third, oxytocin unfolds effects on a higher level of brain functioning. Specifically, while increasing familiarity and trust within a social context, oxytocin promotes the assimilation of novel emotional experiences into internal models (e.g., Tops et al., 2013b; for a review, see Tops et al., 2013a). This process takes place on the basis of semantic elaboration and verbalization of emotions as supported by parts of the left inferior frontal gyrus (Tops et al., 2014). This is compatible with the model by Lane et al., according to which the ability to symbolize and explain traumatic emotional memories in words helps individuals assimilate these memories into their narrative internal models of themselves and the world.

Based on these findings, oxytocinergic interventions may be developed to unfold their potential to facilitate therapeutic change (Harris and Carter, 2013). Such interventions may be especially helpful for so-called alexithymic patients, that is, individuals suffering difficulties in becoming aware of and in verbalizing their emotions (Taylor and Bagby, 2004). In fact, initial evidence indicates that intranasal oxytocin application increases verbal emotional sharing (Lane et al., 2013) and, especially in alexithymic patients, emotion recognition (Luminet et al., 2011). This is of high importance since alexithymia is increased in a broad array of mental disorders such as depression, anxiety, posttraumatic stress disorder, schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder, and predicts poor therapy outcome (Samur et al., 2013). Moreover, in autism spectrum disorder patients, deficiencies that were found to be reduced after intranasal oxytocin application, such as decreased eye-fixation, relate to alexithymia scores but not to autism symptoms, suggesting that it may be alexithymic deficiencies that are reduced by oxytocin (Bird et al., 2011).

Characteristics such as alexithymia may not only relate to larger oxytocin-induced improvements in emotion recognition and perception skills, but also to aversive responses to those improvements because they hinder habitual defenses in those individuals. Results suggest that affiliative effects of oxytocin might be limited to situations in which the subject already feels a sense of social inclusion (De Dreu, 2012) or more generally, when interaction partners are familiar or when there is opportunity to familiarize oneself with interaction partners (Tops et al., 2013a,b, 2014). Oxytocin facilitates the perception of positive and supportive social cues (Theodoridou et al., 2009; Guastella and MacLeod, 2012) allowing for positive familiarization and habituation of social uneasiness (“familiarization-habituation response;” Tops et al., 2013b). Alexithymia, as well as fearful attachment and self-criticism, are associated with negative responses to and avoidance of positive social cues (Dewitte and De Houwer, 2008; Gilbert et al., 2012). In a study of borderline personality disorder patients and healthy controls, fearful attachment mediated decreased trust and cooperation after intranasal oxytocin (Bartz et al., 2011a). Similarly, non-clinical participants higher in self-criticism, and those lower in attachment security, had less positive experiences of compassion-focused imagery after intranasal oxytocin than placebo (Rockliff et al., 2011)1. This suggests that facilitation of the perception of positive and supportive cues by oxytocin may be aversive for individuals who defensively prefer to avoid such cues to prevent painful experiences of loosing love and support (Gilbert et al., 2012). Potential aversive responses to oxytocin treatment means that further research is needed regarding the suitability and optimal strategies of oxytocin treatment to facilitate therapy in specific target groups.

In summary, oxytocin might act as a facilitator or even mediator for the therapeutic effects described by the authors of the target article: Since oxytocin facilitates both the establishment of positive, trustful relationships and the assimilation of emotional information into internal models, it may constitute a central neurobiological promoter of the reconsolidation of adverse or even traumatic memories within the context of psychotherapy. However, personality traits, disorder severity, context, and potentially other factors may moderate effects of oxytocin on psychosocial functioning (Bartz et al., 2011b) and have to be taken into account when thinking about how to implement oxytocinergic interventions. Moreover, individuals who normally avoid or react aversively to positive and supportive social cues need special attention in the context of therapy and oxytocin augmentation.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

1. ^In this double-blind randomized within-subject study all subjects took part in an identical placebo and oxytocin treatment session. Interestingly, if the samples were split according to high and low self-criticism score, effects were only present for high self-critics who showed significantly more resistance to compassion-focused imagery when given oxytocin first. This suggests that intranasal oxytocin may have amplified perception of positive and supportive social cues in the novel social context of the first session, reflecting amplification of oxytocin's natural function in familiarization-habituation responses (Tops et al., 2013b). In turn, perception of supportive positive social cues increases resistance in high self-critical individuals.

References

Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., and Bolger, N., and Ochsner, K. N. (2011b). Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 301–309. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bartz, J., Simeon, D., Hamilton, H., Kim, S., Crystal, S., Braun, A., et al. (2011a). Oxytocin can hinder trust and cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 556–563. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq085

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bird, G., Press, C., and Richardson, D. C. (2011). The role of alexithymia in reduced eye-fixation in autism spectrum conditions. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 1556–1564. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1183-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 779–818. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00055-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carter, C. S. (2014). Oxytocin pathways and the evolution of human behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 17–39. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

De Dreu, C. K. (2012). Oxytocin modulates cooperation within and competition between groups: an integrative review and research agenda. Horm. Behav. 61, 419–428. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dewitte, M., and De Houwer, J. (2008). Adult attachment and attention to positive and negative emotional face expressions. J. Res. Pers. 42, 498–505. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.07.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Gibbons, L., Chotai, S., Duarte, J., and Matos, M. (2012). Fears of compassion and happiness in relation to alexithymia, mindfulness, and self-criticism. Psychol. Psychother. 85, 374–390. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.2011.02046.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Guastella, A. J., and MacLeod, C. (2012). A critical review of the influence of oxytocin nasal spray on social cognition in humans: evidence and future directions. Horm. Behav. 61, 410–418. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harris, J. C., and Carter, C. S. (2013). Therapeutic interventions with oxytocin: current status and concerns. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 52, 998–1000. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.08.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lane, A., Luminet, O., Rimé, B., Gross, J. J., de Timary, P., and Mikolajczak, M. (2013). Oxytocin increases willingness to socially share one's emotions. Int. J. Psychol. 48, 676–681. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2012.677540

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Luminet, O., Grynberg, D., Ruzette, N., and Mikolajczak, M. (2011). Personality-dependent effects of oxytocin: greater social benefits for high alexithymia scorers. Biol. Psychol. 87, 401–406. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.05.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Quirin, M., Kuhl, J., and Düsing, R. (2011). Oxytocin buffers cortisol responses to stress in individuals with impaired emotion regulation abilities. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 898–904. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.12.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rockliff, H., Karl, A., McEwan, K., Gilbert, J., Matos, M., and Gilbert, P. (2011). Effects of intranasal oxytocin on ‘compassion focused imagery.’ Emotion 11, 1388–1396. doi: 10.1037/a0023861

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Samur, D., Tops, M., Schlinkert, C., Quirin, M., Cuijpers, P., and Koole, S. L. (2013). Four decades of research on alexithymia: moving toward clinical applications. Front. Psychol. 4:861. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00861

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Taylor, G. J., and Bagby, R. M. (2004). New trends in alexithymia research. Psychother. Psychosom. 73, 68–77. doi: 10.1159/000075537

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Theodoridou, A., Rowe, A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., and Rogers, P. J. (2009). Oxytocin and social perception: oxytocin increases perceived facial trustworthiness and attractiveness. Horm. Behav. 56, 128–132. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.03.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tops, M., Buisman-Pijlman, F. T. A., and Carter, C. S. (2013a). “Oxytocin and attachment facilitate a shift from seeking novelty to recognizing and preferring familiarity: the key to increasing resilience?” in The Resilience Handbook: Approaches to Stress and Trauma, eds M. Kent, M. C. Davis, and J. W. Reich (London, UK: Routledge Publishers), 115–130.

Tops, M., Huffmeijer, R., Linting, M., Grewen, K. M., Light, K. C., Koole, S. L., et al. (2013b). The role of oxytocin in familiarization-habituation responses to social novelty. Front. Psychol. 4:761. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00761

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tops, M., Koole, S. L., IJzerman, H., and Buisman-Pijlman, F. T. A. (2014). Why social attachment and oxytocin protect against addiction and stress: insights from the dynamics between ventral and dorsal corticostriatal systems. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 119, 39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.07.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: oxytocin, memory reconsolidation, therapeutic alliance, stress, emotions

Citation: Quirin M, Carter CS, Bode RC, Düsing R, Radtke EL and Tops M (2014) The role of oxytocin and alexithymia in the therapeutic process. Front. Psychol. 5:1074. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01074

Received: 11 August 2014; Accepted: 06 September 2014;
Published online: 23 September 2014.

Edited by:

Alexander J. Shackman, University of Maryland, USA

Reviewed by:

Matthias Gamer, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany

Copyright © 2014 Quirin, Carter, Bode, Düsing, Radtke and Tops. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: mquirin@uni-osnabrueck.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.