Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 June 2015
Sec. Consciousness Research
This article is part of the Research Topic Interoception, Contemplative Practice, and Health View all 27 articles

Brain mechanisms of short-term habituation and sensitization toward dyspnea

  • 1Department of Systems Neuroscience, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
  • 2Research Group Health Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

Dyspnea is a prevalent and threatening cardinal symptom in many diseases including asthma. Whether patients suffering from dyspnea show habituation or sensitization toward repeated experiences of dyspnea is relevant for both quality of life and treatment success. Understanding the mechanisms, including the underlying brain activation patterns, that determine the dynamics of dyspnea perception seems crucial for the improvement of treatment and rehabilitation. Toward this aim, we investigated the interplay between short-term changes of dyspnea perception and changes of related brain activation. Healthy individuals underwent repeated blocks of resistive load induced dyspnea with parallel acquisition of functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Late vs. early ratings on dyspnea intensity and unpleasantness were correlated with late vs. early brain activation for both, dyspnea anticipation and dyspnea perception. Individual trait and state anxiety were determined using questionnaire data. Our results indicate an involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), midbrain/periaqueductal gray (PAG) and anterior insular cortex in habituation/sensitization toward dyspnea. Changes in the anterior insular cortex were particularly linked to changes in dyspnea unpleasantness. Changes of both dyspnea intensity and unpleasantness were positively correlated with state and trait anxiety. Our findings are in line with the suggested relationship between the anterior insular cortex and dyspnea unpleasantness. They further support the notion that habituation/sensitization toward dyspnea is influenced by anxiety. Our study extends the known role of the midbrain/PAG in anti-nociception to an additional involvement in habituation/sensitization toward dyspnea and suggests an interplay with the OFC.

Introduction

The experience of dyspnea (breathlessness) is the cardinal symptom in prevalent diseases such as asthma and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD). It is also common in other pathologies including cardiovascular and neuromuscular diseases, and panic and anxiety disorders (Parshall et al., 2012; Laviolette et al., 2014). Notably, the perception of dyspnea is not linearly related to objective lung function or sensory input. Instead, dyspnea perception has been shown to be modulated by several psychological factors, including attention, expectation, learning, categorization and comparison processes, emotional predispositions, and current mood (e.g., Janssens et al., 2009; Lansing et al., 2009; Herigstad et al., 2011; Petersen et al., 2011, 2014). Via their influence on dyspnea perception these factors also have a strong impact on coping strategies, disease management and disease progression (Hayen et al., 2013).

Furthermore repeated exposure to dyspnea can result in either increasing (sensitization) or decreasing (habituation) dyspnea perception (Bloch-Salisbury et al., 1996; Carrieri-Kohlman et al., 2001; Wan et al., 2008, 2009; von Leupoldt et al., 2011a; Hayen et al., 2015). Habituation toward dyspnea seems favorable in conditions such as COPD and panic disorder. In both conditions the experience of dyspnea might result in unfavorable avoidance behavior and habituation toward dyspnea might reduce this behavior. In COPD, in particular, avoidance of physical activity is one potential mechanism that accelerates disease progression as reduced physical fitness leads to unfavorable systemic consequences and has a negative effect on dyspnea severity ending in a spiral of decline (e.g., Troosters et al., 2013). In asthma patients, however, habituation to dyspnea might result in delayed treatment and critical under-medication (von Leupoldt et al., 2009, 2011a,b). Here, sensitization toward dyspnea seems more favorable as far as it might improve self-management by supporting the early initiation of actions during the onset of asthma exacerbations and by heightening the compliance with prescriptions.

Both habituation and sensitization toward dyspnea have been shown to interact with psychological factors such as negative affect. When healthy individuals repeatedly underwent hypercapnic rebreathing, more anxious individuals showed less habituation (Li et al., 2006). This reduction in habituation was more pronounced for the affective dimension of dyspnea (=unpleasantness) as compared to the sensory dimension of dyspnea (=intensity) (Wan et al., 2012).

Understanding the interplay of affective traits and -states with habituation vs. sensitization toward dyspnea seems crucial for the improvement of disease management and rehabilitation in those suffering from dyspnea. In this regard, a better knowledge of the underlying neural mechanisms appears important. However, the neural mechanisms involved in habituation vs. sensitization toward dyspnea have rarely been investigated (von Leupoldt et al., 2011a). In a previous study we observed reduced dyspnea unpleasantness ratings in patients with asthma to be related to reduced activations in the insular cortex and increased activations as well as gray matter volume in the anti-nociceptive periaqueductal gray (PAG) when compared with healthy controls (von Leupoldt et al., 2009, 2011b). These neural patterns were partly correlated with disease duration. Although these findings were interpreted as habituation toward dyspnea over time, the study did not directly examine habituation to repeated experiences of dyspnea.

In the present study, we repeatedly induced dyspnea in a set of healthy volunteers with parallel acquisition of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data. We hypothesized that habituation/sensitization toward dyspnea unpleasantness as compared to dyspnea intensity would be correlated with activation in brain structures thought to be involved in the processing of dyspnea unpleasantness, in particular the insular cortex and the amygdala (von Leupoldt et al., 2008, 2009; Paulus et al., 2012). The amygdala has also been demonstrated to be involved in sensitization toward repetitive pain exposure (Stankewitz et al., 2013). Additional candidate areas were derived from the anti-nociceptive network involving the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and the midbrain/PAG (e.g., Petrovic et al., 2002; Bingel et al., 2007). A study investigating habituation toward aversive visceral stimulation demonstrated changes in the connectivity between rACC, and amygdala over repeated painful stimulation (Labus et al., 2009). A more recent study using repeated presentations of similar stimuli showed either increases or decreases of brain activation in the insular and cingulate cortex and the amygdala (Lowén et al., 2015).

We furthermore expected to see habituation/sensitization-related brain activation already during the anticipation of dyspnea. Although the anticipation of dyspnea has rarely been studied, one study on the anticipation of hyperventilation (Holtz et al., 2012) suggests an involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) along with insula and ACC. These areas also showed expectancy effects in placebo-studies on the anticipation/perception of pain (e.g., Hsieh et al., 1999) and were, therefore, of particular interest. Finally, we expected an interplay between anxiety-related personality traits and states with the dynamics of dyspnea ratings over late vs. early trials.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We re-analyzed data from 46 healthy individuals from a previous study (to be published elsewhere) with a specific focus on habituation/sensitization toward dyspnea. Normal lung function of participants (mean age 28.5 years, 18 females) was confirmed by standard spirometry (Miller et al., 2005). All participants negated any history of neurological, psychiatric, or respiratory disease. None of the subjects showed any anatomical anomaly of the throat. The average body-mass index was 23.4 (range 19.4–28.7). Trait- and state-anxiety were assessed using the State-Trait-Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T, STAI-S), Version X (Spielberger et al., 1983). Questionnaire data were analyzed after completion of the study. Written informed consent was obtained prior to the study. The study protocol was approved by the local medical ethics committee.

Induction of Dyspnea and Measurement of Respiratory Parameters

Volunteers breathed through a tightly fitted face mask that was connected to a breathing circuit. Dyspnea was induced by the introduction of MRI-compatible resistive loads to the inspiratory end of the breathing circuit. In a pre-test before entering the scanner, subjects were placed in a supine position and presented with loads of increasing magnitude. We explained dyspnea to our participants as a sensation of difficult and uncomfortable breathing. Then, each load was presented for 24 s and dyspnea intensity subsequently rated on a Borg-scale (0 = “not noticeable” to 10 = “maximally imaginable”). Load magnitude was increased until subjects reliably reported a sensation of “severe” dyspnea (Borg Score > 5). The respective load was then used to induce severe dyspnea during scanning (mean/SD = 2.23/1.18 kPa/l/s). For the baseline condition of mild dyspnea the smallest resistive load that was reliably rated higher than unloaded breathing was used (mean/SD = 0.25/0.18 kPa/l/s).

The breathing circuit provided ports for continuous recordings of end-tidal CO2 pressure (PETCO2) and inspiratory mouth pressure (PI). Ports were connected with an MRI compatible pneumotachograph (ZAN 600 unit, ZAN Messgeräte GmbH, Oberhulba, Germany). A Y-valve with open expiratory port prevented re-breathing of CO2 while a 2.6 m tube attached to the inspiratory port allowed for the easy introduction and removal of resistive loads in the scanner environment. PETCO2, PI, tidal volume (VT), breathing frequency (f), minute ventilation (VE), and inspiratory time (TI) were continuously measured with the ZAN unit.

fMRI Data Acquisition

Imaging was performed on a 3-Tesla TRIO-Magnetom Scanner (Siemens, Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany) using a standard 32-channel head-coil. For each data volume we acquired 48 continuous axial-slices in descending order with 2 mm × 2 mm in-plane resolution, 2 mm slice thickness and a 1 mm gap using T2*-weighted echoplanar imaging (TR = 2870 ms, TE = 25 ms, flip angle = 80°, field of view = 208 mm × 208 mm). The first five volumes were discarded to allow for T1-saturation. Following the experiment we also acquired a high-resolution T1-weighted structural brain scan using a standard MP-RAGE sequence (1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm spatial resolution, 240 slices). It took subjects 13–18 min to complete the paradigm, depending on the time subjects required to complete the ratings. Consequently, the number of volumes acquired varied between 275 and 374.

Experimental Protocol

Before starting the experiment in the scanner, subjects learned the association of visual cues and experimental conditions (see below) using standardized computer based instructions and practiced navigation through the Borg-scales on dyspnea intensity and unpleasantness. The subjects then entered the scanner with the face mask tightly fitted. A mirror attached to the head coil allowed the subjects to see the cues and scales that were projected into the bore. Before image acquisition started, subjects were allowed to familiarize with the scanner environment and the MRI-compatible button-box response-system. A test-run ensured the full visibility of all cues and scales and the tight fitting of the mask.

During the experiment, 10 blocks of mildly loaded breathing (“baseline”) alternated with 10 blocks of severely loaded breathing (“dyspnea”) using the individually pre-selected loads. Each block was visually cued for 6 s by a thin cross (red indicating baseline, green indicating dyspnea). After 6 s the thin cross changed into a solid cross and the load was introduced for 24 s. Each block of loaded breathing was followed by ratings on two Borg-scales, presented in random order: one for the unpleasantness of dyspnea and one for the intensity of dyspnea as perceived during the preceding block.

All experimental events were presented and logged using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Albany, CA, USA). The ZAN-system, collecting the respiratory data, received triggers for the beginning of each experimental event.

Data Analysis

Ratings for dyspnea intensity and dyspnea unpleasantness were averaged across the five early and across the five late blocks of dyspnea, respectively. The development of the ratings over time was expressed as delta (Δ) by subtracting the average across early ratings from the average across late ratings. Thus, a positive Δ indicated increasing ratings over time, interpreted as “sensitization” while a negative Δ indicated decreased ratings, interpreted as “habituation.” Δ intensity and Δ unpleasantness were tested for correlations with questionnaire data. These statistical analyzes were calculated using SPSS 20.0 software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Preprocessing and statistical analysis of fMRI data were carried out using SPM8 software1. Data were unwarped and realigned to the first image using six affine spatial transformation parameters, then normalized to the SPM standard template and finally smoothed using a 12 mm × 12 mm × 12 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian filter. Data were further filtered with a temporal highpass cut-off of 128 s. Statistical analysis on the first level was carried out within the framework of a general linear model using separate regressors for cue baseline, baseline, cue dyspnea, dyspnea, and ratings. The mean BOLD signal intensity of each volume and PETCO2 time logged to the beginning of each scan were included as covariates-of-no-interest. On the first level, we contrasted severe late dyspnea (blocks 6–10) with severe early dyspnea (blocks 1–5) and late cue dyspnea conditions with the early cue dyspnea conditions using the respective mild conditions as baseline. The second level analysis correlated the beta-values of the two contrast images obtained on the first level [(Δ cue dyspnea vs. cue baseline) and (Δ dyspnea vs. baseline)] with Δ intensity and Δ unpleasantness using separate models. All models included individual Δ breathing frequency and Δ inspiratory mouth pressure (averaged across cue dyspnea vs. cue baseline and dyspnea vs. baseline conditions, respectively) as covariates-of-no-interest as these breathing parameters showed slight, but statistically significant changes over time (see Results).

Correlations throughout the brain were accepted as significant if exceeding a family-wise-error (FWE) corrected threshold of p < 0.05. Given our a priori hypotheses for habituation/sensitization-related correlations during dyspnea anticipation und perception we conducted further region-of interest (ROI) analyses. Masks for the insula, the amygdala, the ACC, OFC, and dmPFC were generated from the automated anatomical labeling (AAL) template described by Tzourio-Mazoyer et al. (2002). A midbrain ROI centered on PAG was defined using a 10 mm sphere around the average coordinates for PAG activation reported by Linnman et al. (2012). Activation within these ROIs was considered significant, if exceeding as threshold of p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons within each ROI.

An additional ROI covering the visual cortex was included as control area. Changes in brain activation within this area were not expected to show any significant correlation with changes in dyspnea intensity and unpleasantness ratings over time. For this ROI we employed a more liberal threshold of p < 0.001 uncorrected.

Results

Ratings

In 30.5% of all subjects we observed decreasing ratings over time indicating habituation for both dyspnea intensity and dyspnea unpleasantness. In 54.5% we observed increasing ratings with regard to dyspnea intensity, while 63% showed increasing dyspnea unpleasantness ratings indicating sensitization. In the remainder of subjects (seven and three subjects, respectively) Δ intensity and Δ unpleasantness equaled zero (Table 1). The Δ for dyspnea intensity ranged from -1.3 to +3 while Δ unpleasantness ranged from -1.7 to +3 (Figure 1, see Supplementary Figure S1 for absolute ratings).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Portion of subjects (%) showing sensitization, habituation, and no changes for Δ intensity and Δ unpleasantness.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Early (mean of block 1–5) and late (mean of block 6–10) ratings on dyspnea intensity (A) and unpleasantness (B). The figure shows the change from early ratings (mean set to zero) to late ratings. Red lines thus indicate increases of ratings over time (“sensitization”) while green lines indicate decreases (“habituation”).

Personality Traits

Both, state and trait anxiety as measured using the STAI-T and STAI-S questionnaires, showed a significant positive correlation with Δ unpleasantness (both r = 0.46, p = 0.001) and Δ intensity (r = 0.36 and 0.32, respectively, p = 0.007 and 0.017, respectively). As higher scores indicate higher anxiety levels, a positive correlation indicates that individuals with higher anxiety were more likely to show sensitization while subjects with low anxiety were more likely to show habituation (Figure 2). When early and late ratings instead of difference scores were correlated with anxiety scores, only late dyspnea intensity and unpleasantness ratings showed a significant relation with anxiety (Supplementary Figure S2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Trait anxiety as measured by the State-Trait-Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T) correlates significantly with both Δ intensity (r = 0.36, p = 0.007) and Δ unpleasantness (r = 0.46, p = 0.001) of dyspnea ratings (mean late-mean early).

Respiratory Parameters

The changes of respiratory parameters between dyspnea and baseline blocks were comparable for late vs. early blocks with the exception of ΔPETCO2, Δf, and ΔPI (Table 2). The relation between PETCO2 during dyspnea as compared to baseline showed a slight but significant increase over time, while PI decreased slightly over time during dyspnea as compared to baseline. The difference of f between cue dyspnea and cue baseline decreased from early to late blocks. While fluctuations in PETCO2 were accounted for on the first level analysis, fluctuations of PI and f were included into the second level analysis as covariates-of-no-interest (see Materials and Methods).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Mean (SD) Δ of breathing parameters between early and late experimental blocks (Δ cue dyspnea vs. cue baseline and Δ dyspnea vs. baseline averaged across subjects).

fMRI

There were no significant differences of either dyspnea anticipation or dyspnea perception between the first (blocks 1–5) and second (blocks 6–10) half of the experiment. The whole-brain analysis based on a FWE-corrected p < 0.05 showed no significant correlations of Δ intensity or Δ unpleasantness during dyspnea anticipation (Δ cue dyspnea vs. cue baseline) or dyspnea perception (Δ dyspnea vs. baseline). The visual cortex as a control area showed no significant correlations with either Δ intensity or Δ unpleasantness during either dyspnea anticipation or dyspnea perception at a liberal threshold of p uncorr < 0.001.

For dyspnea anticipation, the ROI-based analysis showed a significant negative correlation of Δ intensity with the Δ of brain activation (Δ cue dyspnea vs. cue baseline) within the right OFC, extending into the ACC (Figure 3A, Supplementary Figure S3A). This indicated that sensitization toward dyspnea intensity was associated with decreased OFC activation, while habituation was associated with increasing OFC activation. For Δ unpleasantness a significant negative correlation was found within the midbrain/PAG (Figure 3B, Supplementary Figure S3B) while positive correlations were found for the anterior insular cortex bilaterally (Figure 3C, Supplementary Figures S3C–E). Sensitization toward dyspnea unpleasantness was, thus, associated with decreasing activation in the midbrain/PAG and increasing activation in the anterior insula. Conversely, habituation was associated with increasing midbrain/PAG activation and decreasing anterior insula activation.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Localization of correlations between Δ intensity (A) and Δ unpleasantness (B,C) ratings and changes in fMRI activation over time for dyspnea anticipation [(cue dyspnea late-cue baseline late)-(cue dyspnea early-cue baseline early)]. (A) and (B) show negative correlations, while (C) shows positive correlations. Correlations displayed at p uncorr < 0.005 are superimposed on a representative single subjects T1-weighted MR image. Color-bars indicate T-values. OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; PAG, periaqueductal gray; R, right.

For dyspnea perception (Δ dyspnea vs. baseline), we found a significant negative correlation within the right OFC for both Δ intensity and Δ unpleasantness, which extended into the ACC (Figures 4A,B, Supplementary Figures S3F,G). Thus, as during dyspnea anticipation, increasing OFC activation was associated with habituation, while decreasing OFC activation was associated with sensitization toward dyspnea, for both, intensity and unpleasantness. The Δ of brain activation (Δ dyspnea vs. baseline) within the right anterior insular cortex showed a significant positive correlation with Δ unpleasantness (Figure 4C, Supplementary Figure S3H), indicating that increasing activation of the anterior insular cortex during dyspnea perception was associated with sensitization and decreasing activation with habituation. Coordinates, Z-, r-, and p-values are summarized in Table 3.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Localization of correlations between Δ intensity (A) and Δ unpleasantness (B,C) ratings and changes in fMRI activation over time for dyspnea perception [(dyspnea late-baseline late)-(dyspnea early-baseline early)]. (A) and (B) show negative correlations, while (C) shows positive correlations. Correlations displayed at p uncorr < 0.005 are superimposed on a representative single subjects T1-weighted MR image. Color-bars indicate T-values. OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; R, right.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. MNI-space coordinates, Z-, r-, and small-volume corrected p-values for peak voxels within areas showing a significant correlation with Δ intensity and Δ unpleasantness (partial correlations controlled for changes in breathing parameters PI and f).

To investigate whether brain activation changes were rather related to general anxiety than changes in dyspnea intensity or unpleasantness perception, we included individual STAI-T scores as additional covariate-of -no-interest in a post hoc analysis, correlating extracted beta-values from the brain areas reported in Table 3 with Δ intensity and Δ unpleasantness, respectively, (Supplementary Table S1). Correlations within all areas maintained significant in this analysis. When STAI-T scores were directly correlated with the extracted beta-values, mainly cue-related beta-values showed significant correlations with general anxiety. These correlations were attenuated or disappeared when changes in breathing parameters and dyspnea ratings were added as control variables (Supplementary Table S2).

Discussion

In this study we investigated the development of perceived dyspnea intensity and unpleasantness over repeated blocks of dyspnea together with parallel changes of brain activation. We observed significant correlations of late vs. early dyspnea ratings (Δ intensity and Δ unpleasantness) with late vs. early brain activity during dyspnea anticipation and dyspnea perception. These correlations were restricted to a subset of our ROIs, namely the OFC, midbrain/PAG, and the anterior insular cortex. A control area covering the visual cortex failed to show any significant rating-related changes indicating the specificity of our findings. While brain activity changes in the OFC and the midbrain/PAG were negatively correlated with Δ intensity and/or Δ unpleasantness, respectively, the anterior insular cortex showed positive correlations. Correlations were furthermore specific to the dimension of perceived dyspnea: Only Δ unpleasantness ratings showed significant positive correlations with the anterior insular cortex, and a negative correlation with the midbrain/PAG. While negative correlations of brain activation changes in the midbrain/PAG with Δ unpleasantness were limited to the anticipation period, the significant correlations of the OFC and the anterior insula were observed for both, dyspnea anticipation and dyspnea perception.

Both, Δ intensity and Δ unpleasantness, were significantly correlated with state and trait anxiety levels. This is in line with the notion that high anxious as compared to low anxious individuals are less likely to show short-term habituation toward dyspnea, which has previously been shown for hypercapnic rebreathing (Li et al., 2006). In addition, data from Wan et al. (2008, 2012) suggest that the effect of anxiety on short-term habituation is more pronounced for dyspnea unpleasantness as compared to dyspnea intensity. This is partly supported by our data, as correlations for state and trait anxiety with Δ unpleasantness were more pronounced (r = 0.46) compared to correlations with Δ intensity (0.32 and 0.36, respectively). Moreover, while the portion of individuals showing habituation toward dyspnea was identical for the two dimensions of dyspnea perception, the portion of subjects showing sensitization was higher for dyspnea unpleasantness (63%) as compared to dyspnea intensity (54.5%).

The close relationship of Δ intensity and Δ unpleasantness with anxiety raises the question, whether any correlation of changes in brain activity might be better explained by individual anxiety scores rather than rating dynamics. However, post hoc partial correlation analyses yielded no support for this notion.

The functional neuroimaging data presented here mirror the higher impact of trial repetition on dyspnea unpleasantness as compared to dyspnea intensity. While significant correlations of Δ intensity with changes in brain activation were limited to the OFC, Δ unpleasantness was significantly correlated with changes in the midbrain/PAG and the bilateral anterior insular cortex as well. This observation of short-term habituation/sensitization in healthy individuals parallels results from a previous study in patients with asthma, which linked long-term habituation toward dyspnea unpleasantness with similar brain areas (von Leupoldt et al., 2009). More specifically, that study observed reduced insula activation and increased PAG activation to resistive load induced dyspnea in asthma patients compared to healthy controls, which were paralleled by reduced dyspnea unpleasantness ratings in the patient group and correlated with asthma duration (von Leupoldt et al., 2009). Interestingly, longer asthma duration and reduced dyspnea unpleasantness in the same patients were also correlated with structural brain changes in terms of increased gray matter volume in the PAG, which was interpreted as another potential mechanism of long-term habituation to dyspnea (von Leupoldt et al., 2011b).

The PAG, in particular, is thought to be a key area of the so called anti-nociceptive network as repeatedly shown by studies on pain modulation (e.g., Fairhurst et al., 2007; for review see Tracey and Mantyh, 2007). Interestingly, midbrain/PAG activation during pain anticipation has been demonstrated to have a significant effect on subsequent pain perception (Brodersen et al., 2012). This is in line with our results, showing a significant correlation of Δ unpleasantness with midbrain/PAG activation changes for dyspnea anticipation only. For the OFC, there was a significant effect on pain perception for both, brain activation during pain anticipation and during painful simulation (Brodersen et al., 2012). This is also in line with the data presented here, as a significant correlation of changes in dyspnea ratings with changes in brain activation was found during anticipation and perception of dyspnea. Increasing activation in both areas, midbrain/PAG and OFC, were related to decreasing ratings of dyspnea unpleasantness indicating that increased activation in these areas support habituation. Further studies are required to investigate whether habituation is effectuated, e.g., by top–down (OFC) and/or bottom–up (midbrain/PAG) inhibition of other brain areas relevant for dyspnea perception (von Leupoldt and Dahme, 2005; Davenport and Vovk, 2009; Evans, 2010). However, one candidate target area for inhibition is the insular cortex, as this area showed the reversed correlation pattern: increasing activation in the insular cortex was related to increasing ratings of dyspnea unpleasantness. A similar pattern of insular and midbrain/PAG activation was described for the anticipation of pain by Ploner et al. (2010).

Changes of anterior insular activation over time were significantly linked to changes in dyspnea unpleasantness only. Therefore, the present findings support the notion, that activation of the anterior insular cortex is particularly relevant for the perception of the affective dimension of aversive events in general (Seeley et al., 2007) and dyspnea unpleasantness in particular (von Leupoldt et al., 2008; Paulus et al., 2012).

The positive correlations of changes in dyspnea ratings with brain activation changes during the preceding anticipation periods suggest an effect of expectancy and/or learning. As our results are limited to short-term habituation/sensitization in healthy individuals, future studies on long-term habituation and sensitization in patients with chronic dyspnea are required to investigate whether this association between anticipatory brain activations and subsequent dyspnea experience provides a potential target to correct for unfavorable learning of either avoidance behavior (maladaptive sensitization) or under-medication (maladaptive habituation).

In this study we present a novel approach for the investigation of habituation and sensitization toward dyspnea by using the correlation between late vs. early ratings on perceived dyspnea and late vs. early brain activation patterns. This approach allowed the inclusion of all participants irrespective of individual rating patterns as the variance across subjects allowed to differentiate between habituation and sensitization processes. However, correlation studies are limited in their ability to derive causal or directional conclusions. The investigation of connectivity patterns between the brain areas identified as critical in this study and by others might be a useful next step to clarify the way in which relevant areas interact and modulate each other. The connection between state and trait anxiety and habituation/sensitization toward dyspnea suggested by others (Li et al., 2006; Wan et al., 2008, 2012) and confirmed by this study provides another promising target for future studies on treatment and rehabilitation optimization.

Author Contributions

MCS: Substantial contributions to the design of the work, data acquisition, data analysis, drafting the manuscript and revising it critically for important intellectual content, final approval of the version to be published, agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

RE: Substantial contributions to the design of the work, data acquisition, revising the manuscript critically for important intellectual content, final approval of the version to be published, agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

MG: Substantial contributions to the design of the work, interpretation of data for the work, data analysis, revising the manuscript critically for important intellectual content, final approval of the version to be published, agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

CB: Substantial contributions to the design of the work, interpretation of data for the work, revising the manuscript critically for important intellectual content, final approval of the version to be published, agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

AvL: Substantial contributions to the conception and design of the work, interpretation of data for the work, revising the manuscript critically for important intellectual content, final approval of the version to be published, agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank all our participants and Timo Krämer, Kathrin Wendt, and Katrin Müller for technical assistance. This study was supported by grants from the German research foundation (DFG LE 1843/9-2, 10-1, and 10-3) to AvL.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00748/abstract

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm

References

Bingel, U., Schoell, E., Herken, W., Büchel, C., and May, A. (2007). Habituation to painful stimulation involves the antinociceptive system. Pain 131, 21–30. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloch-Salisbury, E., Shea, S. A., Brown, R., Evans, K., and Banzett, R. B. (1996). Air hunger induced by acute increase in PCO2 adapts to chronic elevation of PCO2 in ventilated humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 81, 949–956.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Brodersen, K. H., Wiech, K., Lomakina, E. I., Lin, C.-S., Buhmann, J. M., Bingel, U., et al. (2012). Decoding the perception of pain from fMRI using multivariate pattern analysis. Neuroimage 63, 1162–1170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrieri-Kohlman, V., Gormley, J. M., Eiser, S., Demir-Deviren, S., Nguyen, H., Paul, S. M., et al. (2001). Dyspnea and the affective response during exercise training in obstructive pulmonary disease. Nurs. Res. 50, 136–146. doi: 10.1097/00006199-200105000-00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davenport, P. W., and Vovk, A. (2009). Cortical and subcortical central neural pathways in respiratory sensations. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 167, 72–86. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2008.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, K. C. (2010). Cortico-limbic circuitry and the airways: insights from functional neuroimaging of respiratory afferents and efferents. Biol. Psychol. 84, 13–25. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairhurst, M., Wiech, K., Dunckley, P., and Tracey, I. (2007). Anticipatory brainstem activity predicts neural processing of pain in humans. Pain 128, 101–110. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayen, A., Herigstad, M., and Pattinson, K. T. S. (2013). Understanding dyspnea as a complex individual experience. Maturitas 76, 45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2013.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayen, A., Herigstad, M., Wiech, K., and Pattinson, K. T. S. (2015). Subjective evaluation of experimental dyspnoea – Effects of isocapnia and repeated exposure. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 208, 21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2014.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herigstad, M., Hayen, A., Wiech, K., and Pattinson, K. T. S. (2011). Dyspnoea and the brain. Respir. Med. 105, 809–817. doi: 10.1016/j.rmed.2010.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holtz, K., Pané-Farré, C. A., Wendt, J., Lotze, M., and Hamm, A. O. (2012). Brain activation during anticipation of interoceptive threat. Neuroimage 61, 857–865. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsieh, J. C., Stone-Elander, S., and Ingvar, M. (1999). Anticipatory coping of pain expressed in the human anterior cingulate cortex: a positron emission tomography study. Neurosci. Lett. 262, 61–64. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00060-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janssens, T., Verleden, G., De Peuter, S., Van Diest, I., and Van den Bergh, O. (2009). Inaccurate perception of asthma symptoms: a cognitive-affective framework and implications for asthma treatment. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 317–327. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Labus, J. S., Naliboff, B. D., Berman, S. M., Suyenobu, B., Vianna, E. P., Tillisch, K., et al. (2009). Brain networks underlying perceptual habituation to repeated aversive visceralstimuli in patients with iritable bowel syndrome. Neuroimage 47, 952–960. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lansing, R. W., Gracely, R. H., and Banzett, R. B. (2009). The multiple dimensions of dyspnea: review and hypotheses. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 167, 53–60. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2008.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laviolette, L., Laveneziana, P., and ERS Research Seminar Faculty. (2014). Dyspnoea: a multidimensional and multidisciplinary approach. Eur. Respir. J. 43, 1750–1762. doi: 10.1183/09031936.00092613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W., Daems, E., Van de Woestijne, K. P., Van Diest, I., Gallego, J., De Peuter, S., et al. (2006). Air hunger and ventilation in response to hypercapnia: effects of repetition and anxiety. Physiol. Behav. 88, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linnman, C., Moulton, E. A., Barmettler, G., Becerra, L., and Borsook, D. (2012). Neuroimaging of the periaqueductal gray: state of the field. Neuroimage 60, 505–522. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowén, M. B. O., Mayer, E., Tillisch, K., Labus, J., Naliboff, B., Lundberg, P., et al. (2015). Deficient habituation to repeated rectal distensions in irritable bowel syndrome patients with visceral hypersensitivity. Neurogastroenterol. Motil. 27, 646–655. doi: 10.1111/nmo.12537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, M. R., Hankinson, J., Brusasco, V., Burgos, F., Casaburi, R., Coates, A., et al. (2005). Standardisation of spirometry. Eur. Respir. J. 26, 319–338. doi: 10.1183/09031936.05.00034805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parshall, M. B., Schwartzstein, R. M., Adams, L., Banzett, R. B., Manning, H. L., Bourbeau, J., et al. (2012). An official American Thoracic Society statement: update on the mechanisms, assessment, and management of dyspnea. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 185, 435–452. doi: 10.1164/rccm.201111-2042ST

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, M. P., Flagan, T., Simmons, A. N., Gillis, K., Kotturi, S., Thom, N., et al. (2012). Subjecting elite athletes to inspiratory breathing load reveals behavioral and neural signatures of optimal performers in extreme environments. PLoS ONE 7:e29394. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S., Schroijen, M., Mölders, C., Zenker, S., and Van den Bergh, O. (2014). Categorical interoception: perceptual organization of sensations from inside. Psychol. Sci. 25, 1059–1066. doi: 10.1177/0956797613519110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S., van den Berg, R. A., Janssens, T., and Van den Bergh, O. (2011). Illness and symptom perception: a theoretical approach towards an integrative measurement model. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 428–439. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrovic, P., Kalso, E., Petersson, K. M., and Ingvar, M. (2002). Placebo and opioid analgesia– imaging a shared neuronal network. Science 295, 1737–1740. doi: 10.1126/science.1067176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ploner, M., Lee, M. C., Wiech, K., Bingel, U., and Tracey, I. (2010). Prestimulus functional connectivity determines pain perception in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 1071, 355–360. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0906186106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 279, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., and Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Stankewitz, A., Valet, M., Schulz, E., Wöller, A., Sprenger, T., Vogel, D., et al. (2013). Pain sensitisers exhibit grey matter changes after repetitive pain exposure: a longitudinal voxel-based morphometry study. Pain 154, 1732–1737. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tracey, I., and Mantyh, P. W. (2007). The cerebral signature for pain perception and its modulation. Neuron 55, 377–391. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troosters, T., van der Molen, T., Polkey, M., Rabinovich, R. A., Vogiatzis, I., Weisman, I., et al. (2013). Improving physical activity in COPD: towards a new paradigm. Respir. Res. 14, 115. doi: 10.1186/1465-9921-14-115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Leupoldt, A., and Dahme, B. (2005). Cortical substrates for the perception of dyspnea. Chest 128, 345–354. doi: 10.1378/chest.128.1.345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Leupoldt, A., Sommer, T., Kegat, S., Baumann, H. J., Klose, H., Dahme, B., et al. (2008). The unpleasantness of perceived dyspnea is processed in the anterior insula and amygdala. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 177, 1026–1032. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200712-1821OC

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Leupoldt, A., Sommer, T., Kegat, S., Eippert, F., Baumann, H. J., Klose, H., et al. (2009). Down-regulation of insular cortex responses to dyspnea and pain in asthma. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 180, 232–238. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200902-0300OC

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Leupoldt, A., Vovk, A., Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J., and Davenport, P. W. (2011a). Habituation in neural processing and subjective perception of respiratory sensations. Psychophysiology 48, 808–812. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01141.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Leupoldt, A., Brassen, S., Baumann, H. J., Klose, H., and Büchel, C. (2011b). Structural brain changes related to disease duration in patients with asthma. PLoS ONE 6:e23739. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wan, L., Stans, L., Bogaerts, K., Decramer, M., and Van den Bergh, O. (2012). Sensitization in medically unexplained dyspnea: differential effects on intensity and unpleasantness. Chest 141, 989–995. doi: 10.1378/chest.11-1423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wan, L., Van Diest, I., De Peuter, S., Bogaerts, K., Oyen, N., Hombroux, N., et al. (2008). Repeated experiences of air hunger and ventilatory behavior in response to hypercapnia in the standardized rebreathing test: effects of anxiety. Biol. Psychol. 77, 223–232. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wan, L., Van Diest, I., De Peuter, S., Bogaerts, K., and Van den Bergh, O. (2009). Repeated breathlessness experiences induced by hypercapnia: differential effects on intensity and unpleasantness. Chest 135, 455–461. doi: 10.1378/chest.08-1226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dyspnea, breathlessness, habituation, sensitization, fMRI, unpleasantness, insular cortex

Citation: Stoeckel MC, Esser RW, Gamer M, Büchel C and von Leupoldt A (2015) Brain mechanisms of short-term habituation and sensitization toward dyspnea. Front. Psychol. 6:748. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00748

Received: 27 February 2015; Accepted: 19 May 2015;
Published online: 02 June 2015.

Edited by:

Olga Pollatos, University of Ulm, Germany

Reviewed by:

Roy Salomon, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Switzerland
Takamitsu Watanabe, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience – University College London, UK

Copyright © 2015 Stoeckel, Esser, Gamer, Büchel and von Leupoldt. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andreas von Leupoldt, Research Group Health Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, 3000 Leuven, Belgium, andreas.vonleupoldt@ppw.kuleuven.be

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.