Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 June 2015
Sec. Cognition

Effects of psychiatric history on cognitive performance in old-age depression

  • 1Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institutet and Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
  • 2Bologna University, Bologna, Italy
  • 3Stockholm Gerontology Research Center, Stockholm, Sweden

Cognitive deficits in old-age depression vary as a function of multiple factors; one rarely examined factor is long-term psychiatric history. We investigated effects of psychiatric history on cognitive performance in old-age depression and in remitted persons. In the population-based Swedish National Study on Aging and Care in Kungsholmen study, older persons (≥60 years) without dementia were tested with a cognitive battery and matched to the Swedish National Inpatient Register (starting 1969). Participants were grouped according to current depression status and psychiatric history and compared to healthy controls (n = 96). Group differences were observed for processing speed, attention, executive functions, and verbal fluency. Persons with depression and psychiatric inpatient history (n = 20) and late-onset depression (n = 49) performed at the lowest levels, whereas cognitive performance in persons with self-reported recurrent unipolar depression (n = 52) was intermediate. Remitted persons with inpatient history of unipolar depression (n = 38) exhibited no cognitive deficits. Heart disease burden, physical inactivity, and cumulative inpatient days modulated the observed group differences in cognitive performance. Among currently depressed persons, those with inpatient history, and late onset performed at the lowest levels. Importantly, remitted persons showed no cognitive deficits, possibly reflecting the extended time since the last admission (m = 15.6 years). Thus, the present data suggest that cognitive deficits in unipolar depression may be more state- than trait-related. Information on profiles of cognitive performance, psychiatric history, and health behaviors may be useful in tailoring individualized treatment.

Introduction

Depression is not only highly prevalent, but also a highly recurring disorder that causes emotional suffering along with reduced capacity to function (World Health Organization [WHO], 1993; APA, 2014).

Cognitive symptoms in depression are associated with longer episode duration, higher distress, reduced remission rates, and higher relapse rates (Dunkin et al., 2000; McCall and Dunn, 2003; Jaeger et al., 2006; Papakostas, 2014). However, reports on cognitive performance in old-age depression have yielded discrepant results, ranging from deficits in a large number of cognitive domains (processing speed; Butters et al., 2004; Köhler et al., 2010 attention; Ganguli et al., 2009; Thomas et al., 2009, executive functions; Beats et al., 1996; Lockwood et al., 2002, episodic memory; Bäckman and Forsell, 1994; Bäckman et al., 1996; Kramer-Ginsberg et al., 1999, semantic memory; Zakzanis et al., 1998; Herrmann et al., 2007, and spatial ability; Lesser et al., 1996; Elderkin-Thompson et al., 2004), to lack of any such deficits (Baune et al., 2007; Fischer et al., 2008; Krogh et al., 2012).

At least two types of old-age depression can be discerned. Early onset age (≤60 years) of recurrent depression, persisting into late life, and late onset age (>60 years) of the first depressive episode. The latter type has been suggested to be associated with vascular disease, and accompanied by pronounced executive dysfunctions (Alexopoulos, 1997, 2006). Besides age of onset, the neuropsychological profile of depression varies as a function of numerous other factors, including inpatient status, and cumulative illness burden (Lee et al., 2012; Hasselbalch et al., 2013).

Although cognitive performance for some domains may improve during remission of depressive symptoms (Biringer et al., 2005, 2007; Story et al., 2008; Hammar and Årdal, 2012; Lahr et al., 2014) most studies demonstrate incomplete cognitive recovery (Beats et al., 1996; Nebes et al., 2000; Portella et al., 2003; Yuan et al., 2008; Hasselbalch et al., 2011). Heterogeneity of cognitive deficits during acute versus remitted states has raised the question of whether these deficits are state- (present during acute states) or trait- (present both in acute and remitted states) related. However, a limitation of previous studies has been that participants have been followed for relatively short periods of time, which may account for the lack of cognitive recovery.

Investigation of the effects of long-term psychiatric inpatient history on depression-related cognitive deficits is important to deepen the knowledge regarding when cognitive deficits occur in depression. In the present project, we had access to inpatient psychiatric history spanning over 30 years. The chief objective was to study effects of psychiatric history on cognitive performance in old-age depression.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The Swedish National Study on Aging and Care in Kungsholmen (SNAC-K) is a longitudinal population-based study, including randomly selected older (≥60 years) persons. In total, 3353 persons completed baseline assessments consisting of a nurse interview, a medical examination and self-report questionnaires. Out of these, 2848 persons completed neuropsychological testing (see Laukka et al., 2013). SNAC-K has been approved by the ethical committee at the Karolinska Institutet, and the ethical guidelines from the Declaration of Helsinki are followed. Written informed consent was collected from all participants, or, if the person was severely cognitively impaired, from next-of-kin.

Depression Diagnosis

Depressive symptoms were assessed with the Comprehensive Psychopathological Rating Scale (CPRS; Åsberg et al., 1978). Information relevant for depression diagnosis was also obtained from self-report questionnaires, thyroid function assessment, and the examining physician’s clinical judgment. A geriatric psychiatrist diagnosed unipolar depression according to ICD-10 criteria (World Health Organization [WHO], 1993). For a detailed description see Pantzar et al. (2014). In case of disagreement between different sources of information, a senior geriatric psychiatrist was consulted to make the final diagnosis. The psychiatrists were blind to pharmacological therapy, as well as to medical and psychiatric history.

The Swedish National Inpatient Register

Swedish physicians are required to report to the National Inpatient Register (IPR). The register became national in 1969 and has been validated for research purposes (Ludvigsson et al., 2011). After matching SNAC-K participants to the IPR with personal identity numbers, information on primary psychiatric diagnoses was obtained from 1969 to baseline examination (2001–2004), thus covering up to 35 years. The ICD-8 and -9 primary psychiatric diagnoses were converted into ICD-10 categories. All persons with F00–F09 diagnoses were excluded. All data in this study, except the primary psychiatric diagnoses of admissions from the IPR, derive from the SNAC-K study.

Dementia Diagnosis

Diagnosis of dementia was made according to DSM-IV criteria (APA, 2000) using a three-step procedure. First a preliminary diagnosis was made by the examining physician. Next, the preliminary diagnosis was compared to a second, independent, diagnosis made by another physician based on computerized data. In case of disagreement, a third physician was consulted to make the final diagnosis. Participants who were non-demented at baseline, but diagnosed with dementia during the follow-up period (maximum 6 years) were considered to be in a prodromal dementia phase at baseline (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Background, health, and clinical characteristics across depression status and psychiatric history.

Study Sample

SNAC-K participants without dementia who completed neuropsychological testing during baseline formed the basis for the study sample. A group of healthy controls (HC, n = 96) was randomly selected and screened for depression, psychiatric inpatient history, stroke, cancer, Parkinson’s disease, diabetes, and medication that may have negative effects on cognitive performance (e.g., psychotropics, opioids, antiepileptics, glucocorticoids, anticholinergics).

Based on psychiatric history and current depression status (SNAC-K diagnosis), four groups were identified. The four groups were (1) late-onset, unipolar depression, no history (Depressed, No History; DNH, n = 49; mild: 57.1%, moderate: 38.8%, severe: 4.1%); (2) recurrent unipolar depression, self-reported history (Depressed, Self-Reported History; DSRH, n = 52; mild: 50.0%, moderate: 46.2%, severe: 3.8%); (3) depressed with a recurrent mix of psychiatric disorders, inpatient history [Depressed, InPatient History; DIPH, n = 20; mild: 45.0%, moderate: 35.0%, severe: 20.0%]; and (4) remitted unipolar depression, inpatient history [Remitted, InPatient History, RIPH, n = 38].

The group with a mixed psychiatric inpatient history had, in total, 114 admissions across five ICD-10 categories (Psychoactive substance use, F10–F19: f = 30; Schizophrenia and delusions, F20–F29: f = 18; Mood/affective disorders, F30–F39: f = 46; Neurotic, stress related, F40–F49: f = 19; and personality disorders, F60–F69: f = 1). Occurrence of multiple psychiatric categories was 45% (two categories: 30.0%; three categories: 15%). Within F30–F39, four persons had admissions for bipolar disorders (15 admissions). The remitted group with an inpatient history had, in total, 67 admissions for unipolar depression only.

Neuropsychological Test Battery

Short-term memory was assessed by Digit Span (WAIS-III; Wechsler, 1981), where outcome scores were number of correct repetitions for forward and backward digit span. Pattern comparison (Salthouse and Babcock, 1991) and digit cancellation (Zazzo, 1974) were used to assess processing speed. Thirty pattern combinations needed to be judged, as fast as possible, to be same or different within 30 s, and the outcome was average number of correct responses across two trials. For digit cancellation, a sheet with 11 rows of random digits was presented, and participants were asked to cross out the target digit (4) as fast as possible. The outcome score was number of correct responses within 30 s. Attention and executive functioning were assessed by the Trail Making Test (TMT; Lezak, 2004). TMT-A (attention) and TMT-B (executive functioning) each consisted of 13 circles with the same distance between them. For TMT-A, participants were required to connect encircled digits in numerical order (1, 2, 3, etc.). In TMT-B, circles included both digits and letters, and the participants’ task was to connect these in alternating order (1-A, 2-B, 3-C, etc.). One careless connection (>2 mm) was allowed, and the first mistake was corrected by the test leader without resulting in a lower score. The outcome for this task was completion times for participants with 12 correct connections. Verbal fluency (Lezak, 2004) required oral generation of as many words as possible beginning with a target letter (F and A; letter fluency), and generation of words belonging to a specific category (occupations and animals; category fluency). Each trial lasted 60 s. Outcomes were the average number of words generated across the two subtests for letter and category fluency, respectively. A list of 16 unrelated nouns (e.g., carrot, ring, fork) was used to assess episodic memory (free recall and recognition; Laukka et al., 2013). The outcome score for free recall was number of correctly recalled nouns within 2 min. Immediately following free recall, a recognition task was administered. Here, 32 nouns were presented (16 targets, 16 distracters), and participants were asked to judge whether or not a word had been presented earlier (yes–no). The outcome score for recognition was number of hits minus number of false alarms. Semantic memory was assessed with tests of general knowledge and vocabulary. Ten general knowledge questions (e.g., When did Aristotle live?) with two response alternatives was administered (Dahl et al., 2009), where the outcome score was number of correct answers. The vocabulary task (SRB:1; Dureman, 1960; Nilsson et al., 1997) consisted of 30 words, each presented along with five alternatives. Participants’ task was to choose the correct alternative. The outcome score was number of correctly selected synonyms within 7 min. A 10-item, simplified version (Vandenberg and Kuse, 1971; Rehnman and Herlitz, 2006) of the Shepherd–Metzler mental rotation test was used to assess spatial ability. A target figure plus three rotated figures were presented, and participants’ task was to decide which of the three rotated figures matched the target figure within the 45 s time limit. The outcome score was number of correctly selected figures.

Statistical Analyses

ANOVAs and χ2 tests were used to assess group differences in background characteristics. Cognitive performance differences were assessed with one-way ANCOVAs (five groups), using age and gender as covariates. Regression analyses were used to examine contributions from clinical and health indicators to group differences in cognitive performance.

Results

Background Characteristics

Background and health characteristics across the five groups are reported in Table 1. Significant group differences were found for age: DNH was older than all other groups, ps < 0.001; DSRH was older than the remitted group, p < 0.01; Montgomery and Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS; Montgomery and Asberg, 1979): HC and the remitted group scored lower than all depressed groups, ps < 0.05; DIPH had a higher score compared to the other depressed group, ps < 0.01; Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975); DNH was outperformed by all groups, ps < 0.01, except DIPH, and DIPH was further outperformed by HC and the remitted group, p < 0.01; prodromal dementia: DNH had a higher proportion of preclinical cases compared to all groups, ps < 0.05; heart disease burden: DNH had a higher burden compared to all groups, p < 0.05, except DIPH; physical inactivity: HC was less physically inactive relative to all depressed groups, ps < 0.001, but not relative to the remitted group; DIPH was more physically inactive than all other groups, ps < 0.05; pharmacological therapy: DNH was less medicated compared to the other groups, ps < 0.01; DSRH was less medicated compared to DIPH.

Clinical characteristics for groups with a psychiatric inpatient history are also reported in Table 1. Differences were observed for all variables (ps < 0.05), except age at fist admission, (i.e., both groups had an on-set before age 60). The depressed group with an inpatient history was older at last admission, had a longer psychiatric period, had fewer years since last admission, and had more admissions and inpatient days compared to the remitted group.

Group Differences in Cognitive Performance

Reliable main effects of group were observed for pattern recognition (F4,224 = 3.05, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.052), digit cancellation (F4,225 = 3.95, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.066), TMT-A (F4,224 = 2.53, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.043), TMT-B (F4,188 = 4.04, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.079), letter fluency (F4,243 = 4.76, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.073), and category fluency (F4,242 = 3.64, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.057). See Table 2 for raw scores.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Raw scores of cognitive performance across depression status and psychiatric history.

Follow-up analyses revealed that HC outperformed DIPH and DNH for all significant outcomes (ps < 0.05), except for TMT-A (DIPH), and also outperformed DSRH for pattern comparison (ps < 0.05). DIPH was further outperformed by DSRH and the remitted group on digit cancellation, letter and category fluency, and on TMT-B by the remitted group (ps < 0.05). DNH was outperformed by DSRH on digit cancellation and letter fluency, and by the remitted group on letter fluency (p < 0.05).

A trend level group effect was observed for episodic free recall (p = 0.074), whereas no deficits were observed for semantic memory or spatial ability.

Calculations of effect sizes (Cohen’s d; Cohen, 1992) revealed medium to large performance advantages of HC and depressed with self-reported history, and medium advantages for the remitted group, see Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Effect sizes (Cohen’s d) for significant cognitive group differences.

Effects of Background Characteristics on Group Differences in Cognitive Performance

As complementary analyses, a series of regression analyses were run to identify contributions of different characteristics to group differences in cognitive performance. These analyses focused on cognitive tasks where reliable group differences were observed in conjunction with reliable group differences in background, health, or clinical characteristics (see above).

Heart disease burden (coronary heart disease, heart failure, and atrial fibrillation) had significant effects for all cognitive outcomes on the performance differences between HC and DNH (ps < 0.01), except for TMT-A, so that group differences were attenuated when this variable was included. Change in beta coefficients, calculated as 100∗(beta1–beta2)/beta1, ranged from 23.3% (category fluency) to 32.9% (pattern comparison). For TMT-A, prodromal dementia modulated the performance difference between HC and DNH (p < 0.01) with a change in beta coefficient of 38.6%. Heart disease burden continued to modulate performance differences between DSRH and DNH on digit cancellation (p < 0.01) and letter fluency (ps < 0.05) with change in beta coefficients of 21.9, and 12.3%, respectively.

Physical inactivity had significant effects on all outcomes on the performance differences between HC and DIPH (ps < 0.05), with change in beta coefficients ranging from 31.3% (TMT-B) to 76.7% (pattern comparison). Physical inactivity also modulated the performance difference between the remitted and DIPH for letter fluency (p < 0.05), with a change in beta coefficient of 39.3%.

Number of cumulative inpatient days had significant effects on performance differences between the remitted group and DIPH for digit cancellation (p < 0.05) and TMT-B (p < 0.001). Change in beta coefficients were 52.4 and 84.9%, respectively. Age at last admission modulated the performance difference between the remitted group and DIPH for digit cancellation (p < 0.05), with a 33.9% change in beta coefficient.

Medication status and years since last inpatient admission did not contribute to the observed group differences in cognitive performance.

Discussion

The main findings of this study were that; (a) among currently depressed persons, those with a psychiatric inpatient history, and late onset performed at the lowest levels, (b) remitted unipolar depression exhibited no cognitive deficits.

Cognitive Performance in Current Depression

In line with previous research, currently depressed persons exhibited deficits in processing speed, attention, executive functions, and verbal fluency (Beats et al., 1996; Lockwood et al., 2002; Butters et al., 2004; Ganguli et al., 2009; Thomas et al., 2009; Köhler et al., 2010).

Depression is linked to neurobiological alterations (e.g., at functional, structural, and neurochemical levels), especially in the prefrontal cortex, and the temporal lobes (for reviews see Drevets, 2000; Lorenzetti et al., 2009). In addition, the prefrontal cortex has been shown to be especially sensitive to corticosteroids (i.e., stress; Lupien et al., 1999; Young et al., 1999). This is consistent with the observed depression-related deficits in processing speed, attention, executive functions, and verbal fluency, all of which draw on the prefrontal cortex.

Depressed persons with recurrent mixed psychiatric inpatient history consistently performed at a low level, indicating that psychiatric inpatient history is related to poorer cognitive performance in old-age depression. Recurrent depression has been associated with exacerbated cognitive deficits, a possible reason being that depressive symptoms worsens with each new episode (Takami et al., 2007; Robinson and Sahakian, 2008).

Neurobiologically, this may be reflected in prolonged stress-reactivity on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, causing damaging levels of corticosteroids, and cortisol (see Pariante and Lightman, 2008). In conjunction with the presence of psychiatric co-morbidity, which has been shown to be a predictor of cognitive deficits in depression (Baune et al., 2009), and the vulnerabilities of an aging brain (Bäckman et al., 2000; Raz et al., 2005), it is therefore not unexpected that DIPH would perform at a low cognitive level. However, note that psychiatric inpatient history per se, without a current depressive episode (i.e., the remitted group), did not affect cognitive performance. Thus, cognitive deficits were only observed in combination with current depression.

Although lower cognitive performance in DIPH was an expected finding, performance of persons with late-onset current depression (DNH) was at a similar low level. Whereas cumulative psychiatric illness may explain the poor cognitive performance of DIPH, heart disease burden was shown to significantly contribute to performance differences for DNH for all significant cognitive outcomes. An exception was TMT-A, where prodromal dementia modulated the performance difference. Co-morbidity of cardiovascular disease and depression is highly frequent, and also well known to have a bidirectional association (Barth et al., 2004; Halaris, 2013). Further, heart disease is also known to be associated with cognitive deficits (Pressler et al., 2010; Dardiotis et al., 2012). Taken together, high frequencies of vascular disease (Alexopoulos, 1997), and prodromal dementia, which is well known to cause broad cognitive deficits (Bäckman et al., 2005), may have resulted in a comparable negative impact on cognition as recurrent psychiatric episodes requiring inpatient care.

Cognitive Performance in Remitted Unipolar Depression

The remitted group performed at the same cognitive level as HC. The lack of cognitive deficits in remitted recurrent unipolar depression is an interesting finding, as past research suggests otherwise (Beats et al., 1996; Nebes et al., 2000; Portella et al., 2003; Yuan et al., 2008; Hasselbalch et al., 2011, 2013; Bora et al., 2013). A recent review on cognitive functioning in remitted depression (Douglas and Porter, 2009) suggests that deficits in attention and executive functions may be trait markers of depression, whereas deficits in other cognitive domains may be state-related. However, heterogeneity of definitions and durations of remission make previous results difficult to interpret. For example, remission definitions varied from MADRS scores of 50% improvement, to a score less than 10, 9, or 8. Durations typically ranged from 1 to 6 months, or were not specified (Beats et al., 1996; Beblo et al., 1999; O’Brien et al., 2004; Lee et al., 2007). Studies on cognitive performance and long-term remission in depression are scarce, but one study with a remission duration of 6 years reported a remaining deficit in global cognitive functioning (Hasselbalch et al., 2013). On the other hand, a study with a follow-up time of 10 years reported normalization of effortful information processing (Hammar and Årdal, 2012).

We report a complete lack of cognitive deficits in remitted depression in old-age. Conceivably, a key factor here is the extended time since the last admission (m = 15.6 years). Furthermore, this remitted group had a MADRS score of 2.6, and thus few residual depressive symptoms. These results, along with those of prior research, suggest that cognitive deficits in unipolar depression may be more state- than trait-related, and take considerably longer time to remit in comparison to depressive symptoms.

Physical Inactivity, Cumulative Inpatient Days, Age at Last Admission

In analyzing factors contributing to cognitive group differences, we found that physical inactivity (all cognitive outcomes), cumulative inpatient days (processing speed, executive functions), and older age at last admission (processing speed), modulated performance differences between DIPH and the HC as well as with the remitted group. Thus, these factors may partly explain the poor cognitive performance observed for DIPH. As cumulative illness burden is associated with relapse (APA, 2014), and physical inactivity is associated with old-age depression (Wassink-Vossen et al., 2014), these factors may have contributed to relapse for DIPH, and also to the exacerbated cognitive deficits observed in this group (Takami et al., 2007; Robinson and Sahakian, 2008).

Limitations

Matching older SNAC-K participants to the IPR provided an opportunity to target all primary psychiatric inpatient diagnoses dating 35 years back in time. However, given the time span, with changes from ICD-8 to -9- and -10, and differences in clinical settings – the overall positive predictive value (PPV) of diagnoses in the IPR is estimated to be 85–95%, and 3.1% of psychiatric primary diagnoses are estimated to be missing (Ludvigsson et al., 2011). Also, treatment information (e.g., pharmacological, ECT) received during admissions was lacking. Because findings from this cross-sectional study suggest that cognitive deficits in unipolar depression may be more state than trait related, longitudinal research on long-term remission, and cognitive performance in aging is warranted.

Implications

Relapse rates in psychiatric disorders remain high (APA, 2014). Cognitive symptoms are associated with higher relapse rates (Dunkin et al., 2000; Jaeger et al., 2006) and reduced quality of life (Shimuzu et al., 2013). As shown in this study, they may also take long time to remit. Therefore, combined profiles of psychiatric history, cognitive performance and health behaviors may provide important clues to individualized treatment. This is especially so as 30–60% of depressed persons may be classified with treatment-resistant depression (Fava, 2003). For example, a depressed older person may benefit from aerobic exercise not only in terms of symptom relief (Greer and Trivedi, 2009), but also with regard to executive functioning (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003).

Conclusion

Currently depressed persons exhibited deficits in processing speed, attention, executive functions, and verbal fluency, and those with a psychiatric inpatient history and late-onset depression performed at the lowest levels. Another important finding is that cognitive residuals in unipolar depression may largely be a function of time spent in remission; as we observed no cognitive deficits in the remitted group.

Funding

The funding sources did not play a role in this study. The Swedish National Study on Aging and Care, SNAC (www.snac.org) is financially supported by the Ministry of Health and Social Affairs, Sweden, the participating County Councils and Municipalities, and the Swedish Research Council. In addition, specific grants were obtained from Karolinska Institutet (AP, EL), “Gamla Tjänarinnor” foundation (AP), Osterman’s foundation (EL), the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research (LB), the Swedish Research Council (EL, LB), Swedish Brain Power (LB) an Alexander von Humboldt Research Award (LB), and a donation from the af Jochnick Foundation (LB).

Author Contributions

AP, conception of the study, execution of statistical analyses, manuscript first draft. EL, conception of the study, review and critique of the manuscript, supervised all stages of the study. AA, made all depression diagnoses, review and critique of the manuscript. LB, wrote the protocol for the cognitive data collection in SNAC-K, conception of the study, review and critique of the manuscript, supervised all stages of the study.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all staff involved in the data collection and management in the SNAC-K study; Professor Diana De Ronchi, Bologna University, for her expert advice on depression diagnostics; Kristina Johnell for her expert advice on register-based studies and Ingemar Kåreholt and Giola Santoni for their statistical guidance.

References

Alexopoulos, G. (1997). Vascular depression hypothesis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 54, 915–922. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830220033006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexopoulos, G. (2006). The vascular depression hypothesis: 10 years later. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 1304–1305. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

APA. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-IV-TR, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

APA. (2014). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM 5, 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

Åsberg, M., Montgomery, S., Perris, C., Schalling, D., and Sedvall, G. (1978). A comprehensive psychopathological rating scale. Acta Psyciatr. Scand. Suppl. 271, 5–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1978.tb02357.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bäckman, L., and Forsell, Y. (1994). Episodic memory functioning in a community-based sample of old adults with major depression: utilization of cognitive support. Abnorm. Psychol. 103, 361–370. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.103.2.361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bäckman, L., Ginovart, N., Dixon, R. A., Wahlin, T. B., Wahlin,Å., Halldin, C., et al. (2000). Age-related cognitive deficits mediated by changes in the striatal dopamine system. Am. Psychiatr. 157, 635–637. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.4.635

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bäckman, L., Hill, R., and Forsell, Y. (1996). The influence of depressive symptomatology on episodic memory functioning among clinically non-depressed older adults. Abnorm. Psychol. 105, 97–105. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.1.97

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bäckman, L., Jones, S., Berger, A. K., Laukka, E. J., and Small, B. J. (2005). Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 19, 520–531. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barth, J., Schumacher, M., and Herrmann-Lingen, C. (2004). Depression as a risk factor for mortality in patients with coronary heart disease: a meta-analysis. Psychosom. Med. 66, 802–813. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000146332.53619.b2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baune, B., McAfoos, J., Leach, G., Quirk, F., and Mitchell, D. (2009). Impact of psychiatric and medical comorbidity on cognitive function in depression. Psychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 63, 392–400. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01971.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baune, B., Suslow, T., Arolt, V., and Berger, K. (2007). The relationship between psychological dimensions of depressive symptoms and cognitive functioning in the elderly – The MEMO-Study. Psychiatr. Res. 41, 247–254. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beats, B. C., Sahakian, B. J., and Levy, R. (1996). Cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in the elderly depressed. Psychol. Med. 26, 591–603. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700035662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beblo, T., Baumann, B., Bogerts, B., Wallesch, C., and Hermann, M. (1999). Neuropsychological correlates of major depression: a short-term follow-up. Cogn. Neurosci. 4, 333–341. doi: 10.1080/135468099395864

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biringer, E., Lundervold, A., Stordal, K., Mykletun, A., Egeland, J., Bottlender, R., et al. (2005). Executive function improvement upon remission of recurrent unipolar depression. Eur. Arch. Psychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 255, 373–380. doi: 10.1007/s00406-005-0577-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biringer, E., Mykletun, A., Sundet, K., Kroken, R., Stordal, K. I., and Lund, A. (2007). A longitudinal analysis of neurocognitive function in unipolar depression. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 29, 879–891. doi: 10.1080/13803390601147686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bora, E., Harrisson, B. J., Yücel, M., and Pantelis, C. (2013). Cognitive impairment in euthymic major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 43, 2017–2026. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butters, M. A., Whyte, E. M., Nebes, R. D., Begley, A. E., Dew, M. A., Mulsant, B. H., et al. (2004). The nature and determinants of neuropsychological functioning in late-life depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 61, 587–595. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.6.587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colcombe, S., and Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 14, 125–130. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahl, M., Allwood, C. M., and Hagberg, B. (2009). The realism in older people’s confidence judgments of answers to general knowledge questions. Psychol. Aging 24, 234–238. doi: 10.1037/a0014048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dardiotis, E., Giamouzis, G., Mastrogiannis, D., Vogiatzi, C., Skoularigis, J., Triposkiadis, F., et al. (2012). Cognitive impairment in heart failure. Cardiol. Res. Pract. 2012, 1–9. doi: 10.1155/2012/595821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, K. M., and Porter, R. J. (2009). Longitudinal assessment of neuropsychological function in major depression. Aust. N. Z. Psychiatry 43, 1105–1117. doi: 10.3109/00048670903279887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunkin, J., Leuchter, A., Cook, I., Kasl-Godley, J., Abrams, M., and Rosenberg-Thompson, S. (2000). Executive dysfunction predicts non-response to fluoxetine in major depression. Aff. Dis. 60, 13–23. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00157-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drevets, W. C. (2000). Functional anatomical abnormalities in limbic and prefrontal cortical structures in major depression. Prog. Brain Res. 126, 413–431. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(00)26027-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dureman, I. (1960). SRB:1. Stockholm: Psykologiförlaget.

Elderkin-Thompson, V., Kumar, A., Mintz, J., Boone, K., Bahng, E., and Lavretsky, H. (2004). Executive dysfunction and visuospatial ability among depressed elders in a community setting. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 19, 597–611. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2003.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fava, M. (2003). Diagnosis and definition of treatment-resistant depression. Biol. Psychiatry 53, 649–659. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00231-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, C., Scweizer, T., Atkins, J., Bozanovic, R., Norris, M., Hermann, N., et al. (2008). Neurocognitive profiles in older adults with and without major depression. Int. Geriatr. Psychiatr. 3, 851–856. doi: 10.1002/gps.1994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M., Folstein, S., and McHugh, P. (1975). Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganguli, M., Snitz, B., Vander Bilt, J., and Chang, C.-C. (2009). How much do depressive symptoms affect cognition at the population level? Int. Geriatr. Psychiatr. 24, 1277–1284. doi: 10.1002/gps.2257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greer, T. L., and Trivedi, M. H. (2009). Exercise in the treatment of depression. Curr. Psychiatr. Rep. 11, 466–472. doi: 10.1007/s11920-009-0071-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halaris, A. (2013). Co-morbidity between cardiovascular pathology and depression: role of inflammation. Mod. Trends Paharmacopsychiatri. 28, 144–161. doi: 10.1159/000343981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammar, Å., and Årdal, G. (2012). Effortful information processing in patients with major depression – A 10-year follow-up study. Psychiatr. Res. 198, 420–423. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasselbalch, B. J., Knorr, U., Hasselbalch, S. G., Gade, A., and Kessing, L. V. (2013). The cumulative load of depressive illness is associated with cognitive function in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder. Eur. Psychiatr. 28, 349–355. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2012.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasselbalch, B. J., Knorr, U., and Vedel Kessing, L. (2011). Cognitive impairment in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder: a systematic review. Aff. Dis. 134, 20–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, L., Goodwin, G., and Ebmeier, K. (2007). The cognitive neuropsychology of depression in the elderly. Psychol. Med. 37, 1693–1702. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeger, J., Berns, S., Uzelac, S., and Davis-Conway, S. (2006). Neurocognitive deficits and disability in major depressive disorder. Psychiatr. Res. 145, 39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer-Ginsberg, E., Greenwald, B. S., Krishnan, K. R., Christiansen, B., Ashtari, M., Patel, M., et al. (1999). Neuropsychological functioning and MRI signal hyperintensities in geriatric depression. Am. Psychiatry 156, 438–444.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Krogh, J., Videbech, P., Renvillard, S. G., Garde, A. H., Jörgensen, M. B., and Nordentoft, M. (2012). Cognition and HPA axis reactivity in mildly to moderately depressed outpatients. A case-control study. Nord. J. Psychiatry 66, 414–421. doi: 10.3109/08039488.2012.665081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Köhler, S., Thomas, A., Barnett, N., and O’Brien, J. (2010). The pattern and course of cognitive impairment in late-life depression. Psychol. Med. 40, 591–602. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lahr, D., Beblo, T., and Hartje, W. (2014). Cognitive performance and subjective complaints before and after remission of major depression. Cogn. Neuropsychiatr. 12, 25–45. doi: 10.1080/13546800600714791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laukka, E. J., Lövdén, M., Herlitz, A., Karlsson, S., Ferencz, B., Pantzar, A., et al. (2013). Genetic effects on old-age cognitive functioning: a population-based study. Psychol. Aging 1, 262–274. doi: 10.1037/a0030829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. S., Potter, G. G., Wagner, H. R., Welsh-Bohmer, K. A., and Steffens, D. C. (2007). Persistent mild cognitive impairment in geriatric depression. Int. Psychogeriatr. 19, 125–135. doi: 10.1017/S1041610206003607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, R., Hermens, D., Porter, M., and Redoblado-Hodge, M. (2012). A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode major depressive disorder. Aff. Dis. 140, 113–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesser, I. M., Boone, K. M., Mehringer, C. M., Wohl, M. A., Berman, N., and Miller, B. L. (1996). Cognition and white-matter hyperintensitites in older depressed patients. Am. Psychiatry 153, 1280–1287. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.10.1280

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment, 4th Edn. New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lockwood, K. A., Alexopoulos, G. S., and van Gorp, W. G. (2002). Executive dysfunction in geriatric depression. Am. Psychiatry 159, 1119–1126. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzetti, V., Allen, N. B., Fornito, A., and Yücel, M. (2009). Structural brain abnormalities in major depressive disorder: a selective review of recent MRI studies. Aff. Dis. 117, 1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludvigsson, J. F., Andersson, E., Ekbom, A., Feychting, M., Kim, J.-L., Reuterwall, C., et al. (2011). External review and validation of the Swedish national inpatient register. BMC Public Health 11:450. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupien, S., Gillin, C., and Hauger, R. (1999). Working memory is more sensitive than declerative memory to the acute effects of corticosteroids: a dose-response in humans. Behav. Neurosci. 113, 420–430. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.113.3.420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCall, W. V., and Dunn, A. G. (2003). Cognitive deficits are associated with functional impairment in severely depressed patients. Psychiatr. Res. 121, 179–184. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2003.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montgomery, S. A., and Asberg, M. (1979). A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br. J. Psychiatry 134, 382–389. doi: 10.1192/bjp.134.4.382

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nebes, R. D., Pollock, B. G., Houck, P. R., Butters, M. A., Mulsant, B. H., Zmuda, M. D., et al. (2000). Persistence of cognitive impairment in geriatric patients following antidepressant treatment: a randomized double-blind clinical trial with nortriptyline and paroxetine. Psychiatr. Res. 37, 99–108. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3956(02)00085-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson, L.-G., Bäckman, L., Erngrund, K., Nyberg, L., Adolfsson, R., Bucht, G., et al. (1997). The Betula prospective cohort study: memory, health, and aging. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 4, 1–32. doi: 10.1080/13825589708256633

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, J. T., Lloyd, A., McKeith, I., Gholkar, A., and Ferrier, N. (2004). A longitudinal study of hippocampal volume, cortisol levels, and cognition in older depressed subjects. Am. Psychiatry 161, 2081–2090. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.11.2081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pantzar, A., Laukka, E. J., Atti, A.-R., Fastbom, J., Fratiglioni, L., and Bäckman, L. (2014). Cognitive deficits in unipolar old age depression: a population-based study. Psychol. Med. 44, 937–947. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001736

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papakostas, G. I. (2014). Cognitive symptoms in patients with major depressive disorder and their implications for clinical practice. Clin. Psychiatry 75, 8–14. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13r08710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pariante, C. M., and Lightman, S. L. (2008). The HPA axis in major depression: classical theories and new developments. Trends Neurosci. 31, 464–468. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portella, M. J., Marcos, T., Rami, L., Navarro, V., Gasto, C., and Salamero, M. (2003). Residual cognitive impairment in late-life depression after a 12-month period follow-up. Int. Geriatr. Psychiatr. 18, 571–576. doi: 10.1002/gps.895

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pressler, S., Subramanian, U., Kareken, D., Perkins, S., Gradus-Pizlo, I., Sauve, M. J., et al. (2010). Cognitive deficits in chronic heart failure. Nurse Res. 59, 127–139. doi: 10.1097/NNR.0b013e3181d1a747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson, A., et al. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1676–1689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rehnman, J., and Herlitz, A. (2006). Higher face recognition ability in girls: magnified by own-sex and own-ethnicity bias. Memory 14, 289–296. doi: 10.1080/09658210500233581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, O. J., and Sahakian, B. J. (2008). Recurrence in major depressive disorder: a neurocognitive perspective. Psychol. Med. 38, 315–318. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A., and Babcock, R. L. (1991). Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Dev. Psychol. 27, 763–776. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.5.763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimuzu, Y., Kitagawa, N., Mitsui, N., Fujii, Y., Toyomaki, A., Hashimoto, N., et al. (2013). Neurocognitive impairments and quality of life in unemployed patients with remitted major depressive disorder. Psychiatr. Res. 210, 913–918. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.08.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Story, T. J., Potter, G. G., Attix, D. K., Welsh-Bohmer, K. A., and Steffens, D. C. (2008). Neurocognitive correlates of response to treatment in late-life depression. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 16, 752–759. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31817e739a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takami, H., Okamoto, Y., Yamashita, H., Okada, G., and Yamawaki, S. (2007). Attenuated anterior cingulate activation during a verbal fluency task in elderly patients with a history of multiple-episode depression. Am. Geriatr. Psychiatry 15, 594–603. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0b013e31802ea919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, A., Gallagher, P., Robinson, L., Porter, R., Young, A., Ferrier, I., et al. (2009). A comparison of neurocognitive impairment in younger and older adults with major depression. Psychol. Med. 39, 725–733. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandenberg, S. G., and Kuse, A. R. (1971). Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Percept. Mot. Skills 47, 599–604. doi: 10.2466/pms.1978.47.2.599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wassink-Vossen, S., Collard, R. M., Oude Voshaar, R. C., Comjis, H. C., de Vocht, H. M., and Naarding, P. (2014). Physical (in)activity and depression in older people. Aff. Dis. 161, 65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. A. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

World Health Organization [WHO]. (1993). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Young, A., Sahakian, B., Robbins, T., and Cowen, P. (1999). The effects of chronic administration of hydrocortisone on cognitive function in normal male volunteers. Psychopharmacology 145, 260–266. doi: 10.1007/s002130051057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, Y., Zhang, Z., Bai, F., Yu, H., Shi, Y., Qian, Y., et al. (2008). Abnormal neural activity in the patients with remitted geriatric depression: a resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Aff. Dis. 111, 145–152. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zakzanis, K., Leach, L., and Kaplan, E. (1998). On the nature and pattern of neurocognitive function in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychiatr. Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 11, 111–119.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Zazzo, R. (1974). Test Des Deux Barrages. Actualités Pédagogiques et Psychologiques, Vol. 7. Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Nestlé.

Keywords: depression, remission, psychiatric history, old-age, cognition

Citation: Pantzar A, Atti AR, Bäckman L and Laukka EJ (2015) Effects of psychiatric history on cognitive performance in old-age depression. Front. Psychol. 6:865. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00865

Received: 23 March 2015; Accepted: 12 June 2015;
Published: 29 June 2015.

Edited by:

Kathrin Finke, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Germany

Reviewed by:

Kristina Hennig-Fast, University of Vienna, Austria
Nicola Grossheinrich, RWTH Aachen University Hospital, Germany

Copyright © 2015 Pantzar, Atti, Bäckman and Laukka. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alexandra Pantzar, Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institutet and Stockholm University, Gävlegatan 16, 113 30 Stockholm, Sweden, alexandra.pantzar@ki.se

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.