Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 August 2015
Sec. Cognition

Symptom severity of depressive symptoms impacts on social cognition performance in current but not remitted major depressive disorder

  • Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide, SA, Australia

The aim of the present study was to investigate the social cognitive functioning of participants with depression when compared with healthy controls, and to assess the impact of symptom severity. One hundred and eight patients with depression (66 remitted and 42 current) and 52 healthy controls were assessed using the Wechsler Advanced Clinical Solutions: Social Perception Subtest, measuring facial affect recognition in isolation and in combination with prosody and body language interpretation. When healthy controls, remitted depression and currently depressed groups were compared, no associations were found on any of the social cognition subscales. Severity of depressive and anxious symptoms predicted performance on all social cognition subscales in currently depressed participants, controlling for age, gender, education and psychotropic medication. Affective depressive symptoms were inversely related to ACS Pairs and Prosody subscales, while somatic symptoms were inversely related to the ACS Affect Recognition and Total scores. There was no association between severity and the WAIS ACS in remitted depression participants. People with MDD exhibiting more severe depressive and anxious symptoms and a cluster of affective symptoms have greater difficulty undertaking complex social cognitive tasks. Given the state like nature to these deficits, these impairments may cause problems with day to day functioning and have implications in targeted therapeutic interventions.

Introduction

Individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD) have reported pervasive impairments in cognitive functioning, including difficulties in attention (Douglas and Porter, 2009; Rock et al., 2013), memory loss (McDermott and Ebmeier, 2009), reduced psychomotor functioning (Sobin and Sackeim, 1997; Lecrubier, 2006) and social cognition (Tse and Bond, 2004).

Social cognition can be defined as the mental operations that underlie social interactions, including perceiving, interpreting, and generating responses to the intentions, dispositions, and behaviors of others (Green et al., 2008). This can include emotion-processing, theory of mind, and social perception and knowledge.

The relationship between social cognition and depression is somewhat ambiguous (Weightman et al., 2014). Numerous studies have found that depressed patients demonstrate significantly poorer social cognition compared with healthy controls (Surguladze et al., 2004; Langenecker et al., 2005; Csukly et al., 2009; Szily and Kéri, 2009; Harkness et al., 2011; Van Wingen et al., 2011). For example, there have been consistent positive findings in the theory of mind literature, particularly in tasks requiring participants to correctly interpret an affective mental state portrayed in images of eyes. The clear majority of studies found that depressed patients were impaired in this skill compared to controls (Lee et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2008; Szily and Kéri, 2009; Harkness et al., 2011; Cao et al., 2013). Additionally, in research investigating emotion perception, depressed individuals are significantly more likely to interpret a neutral stimulus as being sad, and assign more negative interpretations to neutral expressions than healthy controls (Leppänen et al., 2004; Milders et al., 2010; Anderson et al., 2011). Also, depressed patients have been found to need greater intensity of emotion than their non-depressed counterparts when asked to identify happy expressions, and less intensity to identify sad expressions (Gollan et al., 2008, 2010; Milders et al., 2010). However, other studies have found no group difference between depressed patients and controls on a variety of measures of social cognition, including prosodic stimuli (Kan et al., 2004), visual perception (Bazin et al., 2009), and affect recognition (Joormann and Gotlib, 2006; Suslow et al., 2010; Bertoux et al., 2012).

A reason for such disparity in the field could be because the relationship between social cognition and other cognitive process has not been widely or comprehensively studied (Pessoa, 2009; Millan et al., 2012). Studies into disorders such as schizophrenia, ADHD and bipolar disorder have shown that the processes behind social cognition can be partially explained by processing speed (Anselmetti et al., 2009; Dhar et al., 2010; Antila et al., 2011) and executive function (Kerns et al., 2008; Torralva et al., 2011). Given that executive function and information processing have been shown to been detrimentally effected in MDD patients (Fossati et al., 2002; Tsourtos et al., 2002; Baune et al., 2010; Snyder, 2013), it is a reasonable to assume that these would also have an impact social cognition also. While emotional information processing research has been gaining traction in the past decade (Suslow et al., 2004; Gilboa-Schechtman et al., 2005; Karparova et al., 2005), the relationship between executive function and social cognition requires further research (Uekermann et al., 2008; Ladegaard et al., 2014; Thoma et al., 2015).

It has also been suggested that the disparity in these findings may stem from the use of a wide variety of social cognition measures (Weightman et al., 2014), different social cognitive paradigms, depression severity or differing clinical presentations (Cusi et al., 2012). While consensus on paradigms or social cognitive measures may be insurmountable, numerous studies have investigated the role of symptom severity. For example, Johnson and Dilorenzo (1998) found that dysphoric female students were less accurate than female non-dysphoric students when identifying positive interpersonal reactions when viewing videotapes of dating scenarios but tended to be more accurate when identifying negative reactions. Donges et al. (2005) demonstrated that depressed patients showed a reduced emotional awareness for other persons. Emotional awareness for others increased as the depression symptoms abated, however they still did not reach the levels of healthy controls. Finally, as depressive symptoms increased, recognition accuracy increased for sad faces, but decreased for other emotions such as surprise (Gollan et al., 2010).

In addition to severity of symptoms, a small body of research has also investigated the impact of the clinical presentation of symptoms on social cognition. Lee et al. (2005) reported that affective depression symptoms, such as anhedonia, had a significant inverse relationship with performance in a theory of mind task. Suicidal behavior has also been found to strongly correlate with the impaired interpretation of social stimuli (Wang et al., 2008; Szanto et al., 2012; Cao et al., 2013), as has excessive rumination (Raes et al., 2006).

Most of the findings described above were obtained in current MDD patients, whereas little is known about social cognitive biases following recovery from a depressive episode. The scar hypothesis proposes that people develop maladaptive characteristics that linger even after a depressive episode has ended, putting them at greater risk for future depressive episodes (Lewinsohn et al., 1981). Those with MDD may develop a certain dysfunctional interpersonal style, as they are at greater risk for being socially rejected than non-depressed people (Hammen, 1991; Marcus and Nardone, 1992; Segrin and Abramson, 1994). Even after recovery, they may continue to exhibit particular beliefs or behaviors in social situations (Abela et al., 2009). Some authors have specifically examined whether currently depressed patients performed differently on social cognitive tasks compared to those with remitted major depressive disorder and healthy controls. Evidence supporting the scar hypothesis in remitted MDD patients has been found particularly in the area of facial emotion recognition. Remitted MDD patients are more likely to identify anger and fear when compared to controls (Bhagwagar et al., 2004; Anderson et al., 2011), while Lemoult et al. (2009) found that patients with remitted MDD required significantly greater emotional intensity to identify happy expressions than controls.

The aims of this study were three-fold: first, to assess performance of participants with a history of depression vs. healthy controls on social cognition measures, to determine whether or not there were deficits in social cognition in the MDD group. Second, to investigate the validity of the scar hypothesis with respect to social cognition; that is, to explore potential differences between healthy controls, remitted MDD and current MDD on measures of facial affect recognition, prosody and elements of theory of mind. The final aim was to assess the extent to which the severity of depressive symptomatology contributes to the effects of MDD on social cognition, in the depressed sample only. In line with the literature that emotion processing is impaired in depression, we also explored the concept that affective symptoms will heighten the association between depression severity and social cognitive performance. In particular, we investigated the relationships between affective and somatic subsets of depression symptoms with social cognitive performance.

Materials and Methods

Study Design

A case-control design was employed to recruit participants with a mood disorder and healthy controls, with the mood disorder group further divided into currently depressed and remitted depressed groups. Current depression was defined as those participants currently depressed as per the MINI 6.0.0, while remitted depression was defined as a participants with a prior (although not current) history of depression based on the MINI 6.0.0.

Approval for the project was gained from the human research ethics committees of the University of Adelaide and Royal Adelaide Hospital. Inclusion criteria for the study were (1) aged 15 or over, and (2) adequate reading and writing skills and the ability to speak the English language. Exclusion criteria included diagnosis of schizophrenia, dementia, a learning disorder, eating disorder, or a Pervasive Developmental Disorder (e.g., autistic spectrum disorder). In total, 160 subjects met the inclusion criteria and were included for analysis.

Participants

MDD participants were recruited from the general community and clinical services within the Eastern Mental Health network, in Adelaide, South Australia. Advertisements were placed throughout the Royal Adelaide Hospital, The University of Adelaide campus, and local community noticeboards to allow further recruitment.

Healthy controls were recruited via word of mouth from the general community, or again through posters advertisements placed throughout the Royal Adelaide Hospital, The University of Adelaide campus, and local community noticeboards. The sample was comprised of 52 healthy controls, 66 participants with remitted MDD, and 42 participants with current MDD.

Measures

Psychiatric Interview

All participants were assessed with the MINI-6.0.0 Neuropsychiatric Diagnostic Interview (Sheehan et al., 1998). The MINI is a short diagnostic tool that takes approximately 30–45 min to administer. It is designed to generate 17 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual DSM-IV or ICD 10 Axis I diagnoses and has good to excellent specificity and sensitivity concordance to both the Structured Clinical Interview for the American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic criteria (SCID), and the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (ICD-10) (Sheehan et al., 1998). The MINI has been widely used in more than 100 studies and translated into more than 30 different languages.

Depressive Symptom Severity Measure

Participants were also administered the Structured Interview Guide for the Hamilton Depression and Anxiety Scales (SIGH-AD; Williams, 1988) to assess symptom severity. The SIGH-AD is a 31-item questionnaire that combines a 17-item depression scale (HAM-D) with a 14-item anxiety scale (HAM-A). Each item relates to a particular group of symptoms in the week prior to interview.

Social Cognitive Assessment

Social cognition was assessed using the novel Wechsler Advanced Clinical Solutions: Social Perception Subtest (WAIS-ACS) (Pearson, 2009). This is an integrated test including facial affect, prosody, body language and mental state interpretation. It has the advantage of a large normative sample of 800 subjects matched to the US census (Holdnack et al., 2011; Kandalaft et al., 2012). Subtest raw scores of the ACS use a scaled score metric, with a mean of 10 and a standard deviation of 3. The WAIS-ACS comprises of:

• Affect recognition (ACS Affect): This section involved showing the participant multiple pictures of male and female faces expressing one of seven different emotions—happiness, sadness, anger, fear, disgust, surprise, or neutral—and instructing them to select the correct emotion expressed from the above list. The maximum raw score for this subscale is 24.

• Prosody-face matching (ACS Prosody): This subset involved playing the participant a recorded voice and instructing them to match the emotional tone of the speaker with one of six faces displaying various expressions (the seven emotions from Affect Naming with sarcasm and confusion added). The maximum raw score for this subscale is 24.

• Prosody-pair matching (ACS Pairs): For this task, the participant again listened to a recorded statement and selected the most appropriate picture for the tone of the speaker from four different pictures of two actors interacting. After selecting a picture, the participant was asked to describe the emotion expressed by the speaker, and to determine whether the tone of voice used altered the meaning of the comment (such as with sarcasm). If so, they were required to state the real meaning behind the speaker's comment. The maximum raw score for this subscale is 42.

• Social Perception Total Score (ACS Total): The Social Perception Total score is derived by adding all of the correct matches of pictures of one to two people from the affect naming and prosody tasks, with a raw maximum score of 48.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using Stata, version 12 (Stata Statistical Software: Release 12. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP). Categorical variables were analyzed using chi-squared tests and continuous variables were analyzed using One-Way ANOVA. Adjustments for multiple comparisons were not performed, as these corrections have been criticized for masking potentially important findings in clinical research (Perneger, 1998). Due to the exploratory nature of this study, the authors decided that the risk of making a type I error was less important than the risk of Type II error.

ANOVAs were conducted to identify differences between the healthy controls, the remitted depression and current depression groups for the Affect Naming subscale, Prosody subscale, Pairs subscale and Social Perception Total Score. Age, gender, years of education and use of psychotropic medication were controlled for. Multivariate linear regression models were utilized to assess the association between the social cognition measures (Affect Naming, Prosody, Pairs and Social Perception Total Score) and symptom severity as assessed by the SIGH-AD. Age, gender, years of education and use of psychotropic medication were controlled for in the linear regression analyses. In the first step of the multiple regression analysis, the covariates were entered into the model. In the second step, the covariates plus the main effects of depression, anxiety or total symptom severity were entered into the model.

Two depressive symptom clusters were also composed from the SIGH-AD (Steer et al., 1987; Lee et al., 2005) by summing scores of individual items to generate continuous scores. The first scale, affective depression, was comprised of summed scores of depressed mood, guilt, suicide, work and activities, and retardation. The second scale, somatic depression, comprised of early, middle, and late insomnia, anxiety-somatic, somatic-gastrointestinal, somatic-general, genital symptoms, hypochondriasis, and weight loss. The continuous variables representing the two clusters were then both entered into a multivariate linear regression analysis to assess their relationship with the social cognition scales. Again, age, gender, years of education and use of psychotropic medication were controlled for.

Results

Demographics

Table 1 displays demographic and clinical data for the sample. The study groups did not differ in gender or years of education. The healthy controls were significantly younger (t = −2.66, df = 157, p = 0.009), and significantly less likely have a family history of MDD (χ = 18.8, df = 1, p < 0.001) or have a current prescription for psychotropic medication (χ = 34.4, df = 1, p < 0.001). Significant differences in symptom severity were found between MDD and healthy control participants for the SIGH-AD Depression (t = −9.1, df = 156, p < 0.001), SIGH-AD Anxiety (t = −9.0, df = 156, p < 0.001) and SIGH-AD total score (t = −9.4, df = 156, p < 0.001). One-Way ANOVAs revealed there was a significant difference between healthy controls, remitted depression and currently depressed groups on the SIGH-AD depression [F(2, 154) = 101.01, p < 0.001] and anxiety [F(2, 154) = 79.55, p < 0.001] subscales. As expected, post-hoc Scheffe's tests showed that patients with current depression had significantly higher SIGH-AD depression scores compared with the healthy controls (p < 0.001) and the remitted depression group (p < 0.001), significantly higher SIGH-AD anxiety scores (Healthy controls: p < 0.001; Remitted: p < 0.001), and significantly higher SIGH-AD total scores (Healthy controls: p < 0.001; Remitted: p < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics.

Current vs. Remitted MDD Group Differences in Social Cognition

There were no differences between the healthy controls, the remitted group or the currently depressed group on any of the ACS subscales or the total score (Table 2). The means and standard deviations for all groups are within the normal range, and inspection of the frequency distributions did not demonstrate any evidence of a ceiling effect. The overall depressed sample did not differ significantly on any of the social cognition measures when compared with the healthy controls. Further analyses, comparing healthy controls with remitted MDD and current MDD, also revealed no significant differences on the social cognition measures.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means and standard deviations of the standardized ACS SP subscales, among participants with Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) and healthy controls.

Relationship between Symptom Severity and Social Cognition

Both the ACS and SIGH-AD total scores were assessed for restriction of variance separately in the current and remitted groups. Figures 1, 2 display the scatterplots for the ACS total score and the SIGH-AD. Neither scatterplot shows evidence of restriction of variance.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scatterplot of SIGH-AD and total ACS scores for the remitted depression group.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Scatterplot of SIGH-AD and total ACS scores for the acute depression group.

Multivariate linear regression analyses were performed in the overall depressed sample to assess the impact of symptom severity on social cognition (Table 3). In the overall depressed sample, performance on the ACS Prosody was significantly inversely associated with SIGH-AD Anxiety and Total scores; and the ACS Pairs test was significantly inversely associated with SIGH-AD Anxiety, SIGH-AD Depression and Total scores. The association between the ACS total score, SIGH-AD Anxiety and SIGH-AD Total approached significance.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Multivariate linear regression models for assessing the relationship between depression severity and social cognition performance in acute and remitted major depressive disorder (MDD).

Further diagnostic group stratified analyses showed that higher scores on the SIGH-AD scale (total scores and anxiety and depression subscales) in the currently depressed group were all significantly associated with poorer social cognitive scores on all ACS subscales and the ACS total score. In contrast, no significant effects of the SIGH-AD scores on social cognition were found in the remitted depression subgroup.

Effects of Affective and Somatic Symptom Clusters on Social Cognition

Finally, analysis of the affective and somatic symptom clusters revealed significant relationships with the ACS subscales and total score in the currently depressed group. The affective symptom subscale had a significant inverse relationship with both the ACS Pairs (standardized β = −0.44, SE = 0.17, p = 0.014) and Prosody (standardized β = −0.34, SE = 0.15, p = 0.033), while the somatic subscale had a significant inverse relationship with ACS affect recognition (standardized β = -0.58, SE = 0.6, p = 0.001) and Total score (standardized β = −0.50, SE = 0.17, p = 0.007).

Discussion

The present study investigated social cognitive functioning in a sample of healthy controls and participants with current or remitted MDD. Impairments on several social cognitive domains were not observed when depressed individuals regardless of the currency of symptoms were compared to control subjects. However, our results suggest that certain characteristics of a currently depressed and anxious state are responsible for the social performance deficits observed in some mood disorder populations, with severity being one of these factors.

However, multivariate regression analyses demonstrated significant associations between symptom severity and social cognition in the MDD group that were currently depressed. Additionally, significant relationships were found between depressive symptom clusters and the ACS subscales and total score in the group they were currently depressed.

The literature is divided over the influence of diagnostic categories in social cognition. While a number of studies have found that depressed patients demonstrate significantly poorer performance on social cognition measures when compared with healthy controls (Leppänen et al., 2004; Surguladze et al., 2004; Langenecker et al., 2005; Csukly et al., 2009), there have been other studies that found no differences (Bediou et al., 2005; Matthews et al., 2008; Gollan et al., 2010; Suslow et al., 2010). Depressed participants in our study did not differ from healthy controls in either recognition of facial or auditory affect. Our results suggest that if sufficient information is available, depressed participants did not perform significantly worse from controls in their interpretation of facial and prosody affect. However, level of depressive symptoms relate to poorer ACS scale performance only in current depression. As has been found in this study, both clusters or intensity of illness symptomatology may provide important clinical correlates that are more likely to be specifically associated with deficits in social functioning, irrespective of diagnosis.

Furthermore, the finding that there were no relationships between symptom severity and the ACS in the remitted group alone yet such relationships existed in the currently depressed group, suggest that patients with MDD display recovery in social cognition after symptomatic remission, contrary to the scar hypothesis. Previous research has found inconsistent evidence for a scarring effect —Lemoult et al. (2009) reported that patients with remitted major depressive disorder made fewer errors on facial affect recognition than controls, but needed significantly greater intensity to identify certain emotions. Other studies have found that patients in remission were more likely to identify fear (Bhagwagar et al., 2004) or anger (Anderson et al., 2011). It is possible that our study may not have assessed the social cognition variables most likely to show scarring effects, or perhaps the effect is more subtle. It could be that social cognition in remitted patients is mediated by other factors such as general day to day functioning or neurocognition in general, or is a mediator itself, as has been hypothesized in the psychosis literature (Ventura et al., 2009; Lin et al., 2013).

It has also been suggested that the phenotype of depression could be important, with affective symptoms identified as having a greater effect on performance than neurovegetative features (Lee et al., 2005). Our research adds to the body of research that has demonstrated a relationship between specific depression symptoms and deficits in social cognitive performance. Given the relationship between the affective symptom subscale and the more complicated prosody subscales of the ACS, this could provide support for the hypothesis put forth by Lee et al. (2005). In participants suffering from current depression, affective symptoms including anhedonia and hopelessness could mean that they are unlikely to allocate cognitive resources to attending to and processing social information. We also found that currently depressed participants who scored higher on the somatic symptoms performed worse on the affect recognition task. Recent studies have demonstrated that somatoform patients recognized a significantly lower proportion of emotional expressions than did the healthy controls (Pedrosa Gil et al., 2009; Pollatos et al., 2011). Pedrosa Gil and colleagues suggested that impaired facial emotion recognition might be a general feature of somatoform disorders, Pollatos et al. (2011) reported that while healthy controls reacted to an emotion recognition task with an increase in parasympathetic nervous system activity, this “normal” reactivity pattern was affected in somatoform disorder patients. It is possible that this may not be restricted to somatoform disorder patients, and that currently depressed patients who experience a high level of somatic symptoms could react in a similar way.

There were a number of limitations with the study. One of the main limitations of the study is that motivation, IQ, overall cognitive function or, at the very least, subdomains of cognitive function, were not assessed and therefore could not be controlled for. This is of particular importance given that, in depressive patients, brain dysfunctions have been reported in regions that are assumed to be critical for emotional processing and cognitive (particularly executive) functions (Cusi et al., 2012). However, a recent study by Ladegaard et al. (2014) found that controlling for basic cognitive function did not change significant differences between healthy controls and a MDD group on the social cognitive tasks. The authors concluded that social cognition may be an independent domain for which non-social cognition represents a necessary but not sufficient condition. Another limitation of this study was that the depression group was heterogeneous in composition, incorporating patients with a single historical major depressive episode as well as those with repeated episodes of depression. Also, while the ACS-SP is a useful and unique tool, it is an American test, making it difficult to assess utility into an Australian context, particularly for prosody. While the recorded voices were of American accents, there were no colloquialisms unfamiliar to Australians. Finally, the ACS-SP test did not have standardized scores for performance on particular valences of emotion. This is widely performed in the literature and would have indicated if certain emotions showed greater deficits, even if not reflected in the overall score. It is therefore difficult to comment on the negative interpretative bias from these results.

Implications for Future Research

The key question arising from this investigation is whether impairments with social cognition causes problems with functioning or quality of life over and above that which is already associated with depression. Finally, further studies need to elucidate whether social cognition is impaired by both emotion-processing deficits as well as by cognitive deficits within the same study. These questions are beyond the scope of the present study, but are important in order to establish the clinical significance of the findings.

Conclusions

The results suggest that in a currently depressed state, as symptom severity increases, people with MDD have more difficulties in processing complex social visual and auditory cues. It also appears that specific symptom clusters are likely to be associated with aspects of impaired social cognition. Further study is needed to determine the relationship between social cognition, overall cognition, depression and functioning.

Author Contributions

TA collected data, analyzed data and wrote a first draft. MW collected data, managed data and performed first analyses. BB conceived the study, the study design and revised and edited manuscript drafts. All authors approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Leonard Angelstorf, Margarete Fuhr, Carola Hering, Lisa Renger, Jennifer Chadbourne, and Mareike Bergman for their roles in data collection.

References

Abela, J., Auerbach, R., Sarin, S., and Lakdawalla, Z. (2009). Core beliefs and history of major depressive episodes in currently non-depressed university students. Cogn. Ther. Res. 33, 50–58. doi: 10.1007/s10608-008-9185-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, I. M., Shippen, C., Juhasz, G., Chase, D., Thomas, E., Downey, D., et al. (2011). State-dependent alteration in face emotion recognition in depression. Br. J. Psychiatry 198, 302–308. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.078139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anselmetti, S., Bechi, M., Bosia, M., Quarticelli, C., Ermoli, E., Smeraldi, E., et al. (2009). ‘Theory’of mind impairment in patients affected by schizophrenia and in their parents. Schizophr. Res. 115, 278–285. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antila, M., Kieseppä, T., Partonen, T., Lönnqvist, J., and Tuulio-Henriksson, A. (2011). The effect of processing speed on cognitive functioning in patients with familial bipolar I disorder and their unaffected relatives. Psychopathology 44, 40–45. doi: 10.1159/000317577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baune, B. T., Miller, R., McAfoose, J., Johnson, M., Quirk, F., and Mitchell, D. (2010). The role of cognitive impairment in general functioning in major depression. Psychiatry Res. 176, 183–189. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bazin, N., Brunet-Gouet, E., Bourdet, C., Kayser, N., Falissard, B., Hardy-Baylé, M. C., et al. (2009). Quantitative assessment of attribution of intentions to others in schizophrenia using an ecological video-based task: a comparison with manic and depressed patients. Psychiatry Res. 167, 28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bediou, B., Krolak-Salmon, P., Saoud, M., Henaff, M. A., Burt, M., Dalery, J., et al. (2005). Facial expression and sex recognition in schizophrenia and depression. Can. J. Psychiatry 50, 525.

PubMed Abstract

Bertoux, M., Delavest, M., De Souza, L. C., Funkiewiez, A., Lépine, J. P., Fossati, P., et al. (2012). Social cognition and emotional assessment differentiates frontotemporal dementia from depression. J. Neurol Neurosurg. Psychiatry 83, 411–416. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2011-301849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhagwagar, Z., Cowen, P. J., Goodwin, G. M., and Harmer, C. J. (2004). Normalization of enhanced fear recognition by acute SSRI treatment in subjects with a previous history of depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 166–168. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.1.166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, Y., Zhao, Q. D., Hu, L. J., Sun, Z. Q., Sun, S. P., Yun, W. W., et al. (2013). Theory of mind deficits in patients with esophageal cancer combined with depression. World J. Gastroenterol. 19, 2969–2973. doi: 10.3748/wjg.v19.i19.2969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csukly, G., Czobor, P., Szily, E., Takács, B., and Simon, L. (2009). Facial expression recognition in depressed subjects: the impact of intensity level and arousal dimension. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 197, 98–103. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181923f82

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cusi, A. M., Nazarov, A., Holshausen, K., Macqueen, G. M., and McKinnon, M. C. (2012). Systematic review of the neural basis of social cognition in patients with mood disorders. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 37, 154–169. doi: 10.1503/jpn.100179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhar, M., Been, P. H., Minderaa, R. B., and Althaus, M. (2010). Information processing differences and similarities in adults with dyslexia and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during a continuous performance test: a study of cortical potentials. Neuropsychologia 48, 3045–3056. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donges, U.-S., Kersting, A., Dannlowski, U., Lalee-Mentzel, J., Arolt, V., and Suslow, T. (2005). Reduced awareness of others' emotions in unipolar depressed patients. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 193, 331–337. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000161683.02482.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, K. M., and Porter, R. J. (2009). Longitudinal assessment of neuropsychological function in major depression. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 43, 1105–1117. doi: 10.3109/00048670903279887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fossati, P., Coyette, F., Ergis, A.-M., and Allilaire, J.-F. (2002). Influence of age and executive functioning on verbal memory of inpatients with depression. J. Affect. Disord. 68, 261–271. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00362-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Presburger, G., Marom, S., and Hermesh, H. (2005). The effects of social anxiety and depression on the evaluation of facial crowds. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 467–474. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollan, J. K., McCloskey, M., Hoxha, D., and Coccaro, E. F. (2010). How do depressed and healthy adults interpret nuanced facial expressions? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 119, 804–810. doi: 10.1037/a0020234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollan, J. K., Pane, H. T., McCloskey, M. S., and Coccaro, E. F. (2008). Identifying differences in biased affective information processing in major depression. Psychiatry Res. 159, 18–24. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, M. F., Penn, D. L., Bentall, R., Carpenter, W. T., Gaebel, W., Gur, R. C., et al. (2008). Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 1211–1220. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammen, C. (1991). Generation of stress in the course of unipolar depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 100, 555–561. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harkness, K. L., Washburn, D., Theriault, J. E., Lee, L., and Sabbagh, M. A. (2011). Maternal history of depression is associated with enhanced theory of mind in depressed and nondepressed adult women. Psychiatry Res. 189, 91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holdnack, J., Goldstein, G., and Drozdick, L. (2011). Social perception and WAIS-IV performance in adolescents and adults diagnosed with asperger's syndrome and autism. Assessment 18, 192–200. doi: 10.1177/1073191110394771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, T. J., and Dilorenzo, T. M. (1998). Social information processing biases in depressed and nondepressed college students. J. Soc. Behav. Pers. 13, 517–530.

Joormann, J., and Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Is this happiness I see? Biases in the identification of emotional facial expressions in depression and social phobia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 115, 705. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kan, Y., Mimura, M., Kamijima, K., and Kawamura, M. (2004). Recognition of emotion from moving facial and prosodic stimuli in depressed patients. J. Neurol Neurosurg. Psychiatry 75, 1667–1671. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.036079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kandalaft, M. R., Didehbani, N., Cullum, C. M., Krawczyk, D. C., Allen, T. T., Tamminga, C. A., et al. (2012). The wechsler ACS social perception subtest:: a preliminary comparison with other measures of social cognition. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 30, 455–465. doi: 10.1177/0734282912436411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karparova, S. P., Kersting, A., and Suslow, T. (2005). Disengagement of attention from facial emotion in unipolar depression. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 59, 723–729. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2005.01443.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerns, J. G., Nuechterlein, K. H., Braver, T. S., and Barch, D. M. (2008). Executive functioning component mechanisms and schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladegaard, N., Larsen, E. R., Videbech, P., and Lysaker, P. H. (2014). Higher-order social cognition in first-episode major depression. Psychiatry Res. 216, 37–43. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langenecker, S. A., Bieliauskas, L. A., Rapport, L. J., Zubieta, J.-K., Wilde, E. A., and Berent, S. (2005). Face emotion perception and executive functioning deficits in depression. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 27, 320–333. doi: 10.1080/13803390490490515720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lecrubier, Y. (2006). Physical components of depression and psychomotor retardation. J. Clin. Psychiatry 67(Suppl. 6), 23–26.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lee, L., Harkness, K. L., Sabbagh, M. A., and Jacobson, J. A. (2005). Mental state decoding abilities in clinical depression. J. Affect. Disord. 86, 247–258. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemoult, J., Joormann, J., Sherdell, L., Wright, Y., and Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Identification of emotional facial expressions following recovery from depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 118, 828. doi: 10.1037/a0016944

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leppänen, J. M., Milders, M., Bell, J. S., Terriere, E., and Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Depression biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces. Psychiatry Res. 128, 123–133. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewinsohn, P. M., Steinmetz, J. L., Larson, D. W., and Franklin, J. (1981). Depression-related cognitions: antecedent or consequence? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 90, 213–219. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.90.3.213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C. H., Huang, C. L., Chang, Y. C., Chen, P. W., Lin, C. Y., Tsai, G. E., et al. (2013). Clinical symptoms, mainly negative symptoms, mediate the influence of neurocognition and social cognition on functional outcome of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 146, 231–237. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcus, D. K., and Nardone, M. E. (1992). Depression and interpersonal rejection. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 12, 433–449. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(92)90126-S

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, S. C., Strigo, I. A., Simmons, A. N., Yang, T. T., and Paulus, M. P. (2008). Decreased functional coupling of the amygdala and supragenual cingulate is related to increased depression in unmedicated individuals with current major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 111, 13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDermott, L. M., and Ebmeier, K. P. (2009). A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. J. Affect. Disord. 119, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milders, M., Bell, S., Platt, J., Serrano, R., and Runcie, O. (2010). Stable expression recognition abnormalities in unipolar depression. Psychiatry Res. 179, 38–42. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millan, M. J., Agid, Y., Brüne, M., Bullmore, E. T., Carter, C. S., Clayton, N. S., et al. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 11, 141–168. doi: 10.1038/nrd3628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearson. (2009). Advanced Clinical Solutions for WAIS-IV and WMS-IV. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Pedrosa Gil, F., Ridout, N., Kessler, H., Neuffer, M., Schoechlin, C., Traue, H. C., et al. (2009). Facial emotion recognition and alexithymia in adults with somatoform disorders. Depress. Anxiety 26, E26–E33. doi: 10.1002/da.20456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perneger, T. V. (1998). What's wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. BMJ 316, 1236–1238. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7139.1236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 160–166. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Herbert, B. M., Wankner, S., Dietel, A., Wachsmuth, C., Henningsen, P., et al. (2011). Autonomic imbalance is associated with reduced facial recognition in somatoform disorders. J. Psychosom. Res. 71, 232–239. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raes, F., Hermans, D., and Williams, J. M. (2006). Negative bias in the perception of others' facial emotional expressions in major depression: the role of depressive rumination. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 194, 796–799. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000240187.80270.bb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., and Blackwell, A. D. (2013). Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 44, 2029–2040. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Segrin, C., and Abramson, L. Y. (1994). Negative reactions to depressive behaviors - a communication theories analysis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 103, 655–668. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.103.4.655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59, 22–33.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Snyder, H. R. (2013). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: a meta-analysis and review. Psychol. Bull. 139, 81. doi: 10.1037/a0028727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobin, C., and Sackeim, H. A. (1997). Psychomotor symptoms of depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 4–17. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.1.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steer, R. A., Beck, A. T., Riskind, J. H., and Brown, G. (1987). Relationships between the Beck depression inventory and the Hamilton rating scale for depression in depressed outpatients. J. Psychopathol. Behav. 9, 327–339. doi: 10.1007/BF00964561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Surguladze, S. A., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brébion, G., Travis, M. J., and Phillips, M. L. (2004). Recognition accuracy and response bias to happy and sad facial expressions in patients with major depression. Neuropsychology 18, 212–218. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.2.212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suslow, T., Dannlowski, U., Lalee-Mentzel, J., Donges, U.-S., Arolt, V., and Kersting, A. (2004). Spatial processing of facial emotion in patients with unipolar depression: a longitudinal study. J. Affect. Disord. 83, 59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2004.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suslow, T., Konrad, C., Kugel, H., Rumstadt, D., Zwitserlood, P., Schöning, S., et al. (2010). Automatic mood-congruent amygdala responses to masked facial expressions in major depression. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 155–160. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szanto, K., Dombrovski, A. Y., Sahakian, B. J., Mulsant, B. H., Houck, P. R., Reynolds, C. F. III., et al. (2012). Social emotion recognition, social functioning, and attempted suicide in late-life depression. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 20, 257–265. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31820eea0c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szily, E., and Kéri, S. (2009). Anomalous subjective experience and psychosis risk in young depressed patients. Psychopathology 42, 229–235. doi: 10.1159/000218520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thoma, P., Schmidt, T., Juckel, G., Norra, C., and Suchan, B. (2015). Nice or effective? social problem solving strategies in patients with major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.05.015. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torralva, T., Gleichgerrcht, E., Torrente, F., Roca, M., Strejilevich, S. A., Cetkovich, M., et al. (2011). Neuropsychological functioning in adult bipolar disorder and ADHD patients: a comparative study. Psychiatry Res. 186, 261–266. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tse, W. S., and Bond, A. J. (2004). The impact of depression on social skills. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 192, 260–268. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000120884.60002.2b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsourtos, G., Thompson, J. C., and Stough, C. (2002). Evidence of an early information processing speed deficit in unipolar major depression. Psychol. Med. 32, 259–265. doi: 10.1017/S0033291701005001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uekermann, J., Channon, S., Lehmkämper, C., Abdel-Hamid, M., Vollmoeller, W., and Daum, I. (2008). Executive function, mentalizing and humor in major depression. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 14, 55–62. doi: 10.1017/s1355617708080016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Wingen, G. A., Van Eijndhoven, P., Tendolkar, I., Buitelaar, J., Verkes, R. J., and Fernández, G. (2011). Neural basis of emotion recognition deficits in first-episode major depression. Psychol. Med. 41, 1397–1405. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710002084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ventura, J., Hellemann, G. S., Thames, A. D., Koellner, V., and Nuechterlein, K. H. (2009). Symptoms as mediators of the relationship between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 113, 189–199. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.03.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y. G., Wang, Y. Q., Chen, S. L., Zhu, C. Y., and Wang, K. (2008). Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: a componential view. Psychiatry Res. 161, 153–161. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weightman, M. J., Air, T. M., and Baune, B. T. (2014). A review of the role of social cognition in major depressive disorder. Front. Psychiatry 5:179. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. B. (1988). A structured interview guide for the hamilton depression rating scale. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 45, 742–747. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800320058007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: major depressive disorder, social cognition, facial affect, prosody, depressive symptoms

Citation: Air T, Weightman MJ and Baune BT (2015) Symptom severity of depressive symptoms impacts on social cognition performance in current but not remitted major depressive disorder. Front. Psychol. 6:1118. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01118

Received: 26 February 2015; Accepted: 20 July 2015;
Published: 04 August 2015.

Edited by:

Kathrin Finke, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Germany

Reviewed by:

Kristina Hennig-Fast, University of Vienna, Austria
Nicola Grossheinrich, University Hospital of the RWTH Aachen, Germany

Copyright © 2015 Air, Weightman and Baune. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bernhard T. Baune, Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, The University of Adelaide, Level 4 Eleanor Harrald Building, Adelaide, SA 5005, Australia, bernhard.baune@adelaide.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.