Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 17 September 2015
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Interoception and the uneasiness of the mind: affect as perceptual style

  • 1Institute for Health and Behaviour, University of Luxembourg, Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg
  • 2Research Group on Health Psychology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

Autonomous system models of interoception describe perception of bodily sensations as an active process in which the brain generates and tests hypotheses about the body on the basis of proximal information. This view of perception as inference allows a new perspective on the role of affect in perception. Affect and interoception are closely linked, but processes underlying this link are poorly understood. We suggest that a predictive coding perspective allows acknowledging affect as integral part of information processing. We outline how affect may intrinsically modify processes of interoception by acting as threshold mechanism in stimulus grouping and information compression. We outline how well-established methods, for example, from categorization research may allow quantifying this influence of affect on perception in empirical tests of predictive coding models. We discuss how this may enrich the study of the relationship between affect and interoception and may have important clinical relevance.

Introduction

How accurate are people when they describe their internal states? This is a question of high clinical relevance, especially in a medical context where report of bodily symptoms has important diagnostic and therapeutic implications. Symptoms such as pain or dyspnea cannot be measured by objective means, but only by self-report (Parshall et al., 2012). Unfortunately, we are apparently poor judges of what is going on inside of our body (Wiebe et al., 1994; Janssens et al., 2009; Petersen et al., 2011). Studies estimate that depending on disease domain up to 81.6% of patients report at least one symptom that cannot be related to physiological dysfunction (Hiller et al., 2006). This is frustrating for both, patients and medical doctors, since distress communicated by patients is usually profound and genuine (Brown, 2004; Edwards et al., 2010). On the other hand, a high number of individuals with severe pathology does not report symptoms and does not seek medical help. This can have serious consequences, for example (among other), in the case of stroke (e.g., Lacy et al., 2001), pain (e.g., Yong et al., 2001; Gagliese, 2009), or respiratory disease (e.g., Petersen et al., 2014a).

Anxiety and negative affect are usually treated as prime suspects in research on bias in the perception of bodily sensations. As state and trait, they seem to play a prominent role in the perception of the world within and outside the body (Watson and Pennebaker, 1989; Van den Bergh et al., 2004; Cisler and Koster, 2010). A meta-analysis on attentional bias toward threatening visual stimuli in anxiety reports an aggregated effect size of d = 0.45 (Bar-Haim et al., 2007). Effects of anxiety on the perception of heartbeat are even more robust and a review reports a weighted mean effect size of d = 0.62 for anxiety sensitivity, indicating that individuals high in fear of bodily sensations are more sensitive to perceive their heartbeat (Domschke et al., 2010). At the same time, negative affect and anxiety are positively related to an interoceptive bias and a tendency for catastrophizing interpretations (Watson and Pennebaker, 1989; Van den Bergh et al., 2004; Leeuw et al., 2007).

Despite robust empirical evidence that anxiety has an effect on perception and interoception, recent reviews agree that there is a problematic lack of theory-driven studies on why and how anxiety affects attention, information processing, and self-report, in other words on how anxiety becomes part of the perceptual process (Bar-Haim et al., 2007; Cisler and Koster, 2010). In the following, we discuss (1) how to define accurate perception, (2) how recently developed theories of perception as hypotheses testing may allow to integrate affect as part of perceptual algorithms, and (3) how these models can be tested with validated research paradigms.

The perception of bodily sensations is a particularly interesting field to conceptualize affect as part of the perceptual process. Bodily sensations interact closely with emotions (Dunn et al., 2010). Furthermore, they are often complex and ambiguous, involving multiple afferent sources (Parshall et al., 2012). They can also be ambiguous because of a low signal to noise ratios (e.g., heartbeat) or because of low perceived predictability (Browning et al., 2015; Seidel et al., 2015). We suggest below that affect is part of the perceptual processes by guiding disambiguation. Thus, ambiguity of bodily sensations make them an example par excellence to outline our ideas. We suggest, however, that the model presented here is not restricted to the perception of the body, but can be applied also to other forms of perception.

The Quest for Truth in the Perception of Bodily Sensations

Truth is a matter of definition. Different perspectives have been suggested such as correspondence, consensus, and pragmatic truth (Kruglanski, 1989) or criterion and construct validity (Cronbach and Meehl, 1955). The correspondence theory of truth defines accuracy as correlation between a percept and an external criterion. The consensus theory of truth regards perception as validated if it is socially shared, for example, if patients agree with other patients or with health care professionals about symptoms and symptom severity. Correspondence and consensus validity are usually low in the perception of bodily sensations (e.g., Macquart-Moulin et al., 1997; Fallone et al., 2004; Hiller et al., 2006; Janssens et al., 2009; Riegel et al., 2010) and this constitutes a problem for the diagnostic process. In a biomedical model, symptoms need to be “proven” by physiological measures.

This emphasis on external criteria contrasts with a ‘pragmatic’ psychophysics perspective (Thurstone, 1927; Killian, 1985), with neural models of information processing (Friston, 2005; Barrett and Simmons, 2015), and with philosophical models of perception (Hume, 1739/2007; James, 1909/1974; Hohwy, 2012). These models agree that the brain cannot access the world—including the peripheral body- directly, but needs to rely on patterns of brain activity (Hohwy, 2012). Working with proximal stimulation, the brain creates a statistical model of the body in the world that allows to make inferences about causes and predictions about future stimulations (Friston, 2005; Hohwy, 2012). In this perspective “reason must be considered as a kind of cause of which truth is the natural effect” (Hume, 1739/2007, p. 121) and not vice versa. The brain relies on pragmatic truth which optimizes the “truth’s cash-value in experiential terms” (James, 1909/1974, p. 5). This inference process does not necessarily reach conscious awareness, but is considered an automatic process running on different hierarchical levels of information processing (e.g., Friston, 2005; Edwards et al., 2012).

Perception of bodily sensations can become gradually independent from physiological changes, creating interoceptive “illusions” such as nocebo or placebo effects (Van den Bergh et al., 2002; Büchel et al., 2014; Constantinou et al., 2015). To assume that perception of placebo and nocebo effects is flawed is a misconception resulting from confusing cause and effect (Hume, 1739/2007). Despite low correspondence/consensus validity, pragmatic validity can be high (Cronbach and Meehl, 1955) and related to an overall optimization of perception and coping in terms of a cost/benefit strategy (Huttenlocher et al., 2000). Only if statistical models of the body are not updated in response to error feedback, suboptimal models may lead to suboptimal perception and coping, as it has been suggested for medically unexplained symptoms (Brown, 2004).

To summarize, perception is not measurement and the impact of the emotions and contextual variables on perception is not measurement error nor a flaw in perception. Keeping perception and perceptual bias context dependent and tuned-in to situational shifts in costs and benefits is an important skill, not a defect (Lynn and Barrett, 2014; Petersen et al., 2015).

Anxiety as “Uneasiness of the Mind”

Predictive coding models give up the notion of an agent or self on a superordinate level that supervises perception, but conceptualize perception as representation of statistical regularities in stimulation (Hohwy, 2012). This idea can be traced back to Hume (1739/2007) who proposed that the mind constructs perceiver and percept at the same time. If perception is not coordinated by a self, but the self is a consequence of perception, affective states such as anxiety need to be considered not as something a self “has,” but as part of the data processing algorithms. In this process, the uneasiness of the mind is a reaction to the challenge to integrate unspecific, disconnected, and often inconsistent experiences. Hume (1739/2007, Book I Part IV) suggests that in response to this challenge “the mind is bound to be uneasy and to seek relief from that uneasiness” by constructing a self that is the illusion of a continuous existence. This self -in an illusion of agency- can feel and try to evade threats to its existence.

Hume’s uneasiness of the mind has been rediscovered by recent theory development. Negative affect has been conceptualized as resulting from a mismatch between predicted stimulation and experienced stimulation (Joffily and Coricelli, 2013) or as a response to reduction of prediction error at a rate that is lower than expected (Van de Cruys, 2014). We suggest that another important way in which anxiety may be inherent to algorithms of data processing is as decision rule for solving the infinity problem (e.g., Chater and Vitányi, 2003). From the limited data provided by our senses, the brain could create an infinite number of patterns of experiences. Thus, a central problem in experience is which rules the brain applies to choose those experiential categories that are most useful in a pragmatic sense, that is, condensed enough to facilitate perception and not too condensed to miss important details (Reed, 1972; Anderson, 1991).

We propose that negative affect may lead to an over-emphasis on simplicity and reduction of redundancy to the point of loss of unique data. This can be adaptive in situations in which fast information processing is crucial. If excessive simplification becomes a habitual processing style, this may lead to serious reduction of predictive power.

More specifically, we propose that negative affect and anxiety impact on perceived homogeneity of sensation categories (Petersen et al., 2014b, 2015). Categories are defined by both (1) a central tendency and (2) dispersion of stimuli around this point. The dispersion determines how much confidence the brain can have in inferences based on a category prototype. Animal studies suggest that confidence estimates (i.e., estimates of stimulus dispersion) do not require meta-cognition (e.g., Hampton, 2001; Foote and Crystal, 2007; Kepecs et al., 2008).

A homogeneous category acts as precise prior, that is, a likelihood distribution with small dispersion. Lower perceived variability within a category may lead to more extensive generalization of information about the category to new stimuli (e.g., Shepard, 1987; Zaman et al., 2015). We suggest that negative affect is related to a tendency to process the broad picture in categorical terms and less on a single stimulus basis (Robinson and Clore, 2002), resulting in more extensive generalization of category-information, and in the case of aversive categories, fear-generalization.

Biological history has shaped the structure and function of the brain to give priority to deriving valence from stimulation, dividing sensations very broadly in “good” or “bad” to make affective predictions (Barrett and Bar, 2009). In the case of aversive interoceptive stimuli such as pain or breathlessness, highly homogeneous categories allow nearly instant predictions and affective evaluations with high confidence. This allows fast activation of coping behavior with little or no sensory analysis (Petersen et al., 2015). Thus, processing data at a highly condensed category level and ignoring seemingly redundant information may provide a subjective feeling of relief, at least in the short-term, because it allows avoidance of direct stimulus processing. It may help to create and defend a feeling of certainty in the face of ambiguous information (Quellar et al., 2006; Petersen et al., 2015). To summarize, we propose that affect does not only guide data collection in terms of quantity, attention, and speed (Thayer and Lane, 2000; Koster et al., 2004; Cisler and Koster, 2010), but that affect sets a threshold for the degree to which information is condensed and how strongly it is edited to fit into categories.

As threshold mechanism, affect may determine the extent to which information is eliminated as redundant or irrelevant and it may determine when information seeking is aborted in favor of a decision based on priors and prototypes. Thus, we propose a modified vigilance avoidance model (e.g., Mogg and Bradley, 1998) in which negative affect is not related to absolute avoidance of stimulation, but to a shift from more detailed processing to highly condensed prototypes. Thus, high processing priority for interoceptive stimuli in more anxious individuals (e.g., Domschke et al., 2010) may not lead to more rigorous testing of incoming sensory stimulation. In threat-sensitive persons negative affective predictions may initiate the application of overly condensed category-related information and more aggressive stimulus disambiguation by using category information. Focus on condensed category prototypes in turn will make detection of details less likely that would be needed to update categories with regard to central tendency and dispersion.

Taking Inferential Leaps Between Branches of the Perceptual Tree

Predictive coding approaches place a particular emphasis on brain hierarchies involved in building a representation of the world (e.g., Friston, 2005; Edwards et al., 2012). Lower levels of this hierarchy predict basic sensory attributes in the form of fast and automatic inferences, which are usually gated out of awareness (e.g., breathing sensations at rest). More complex inferences are suggested to be passed on to higher brain structures and—passing the gate of conscious processing—implicit priors may become explicit hypotheses (Hohwy, 2012).

Categorization models equally emphasize hierarchy (Kemp et al., 2007). We propose a continuum ranging from sensory grouping to perceptual grouping to explicit categorization. On the lowest level, at the interface between world and senses, anatomy of the sensory system will group stimulation and process it in a limited number of afferent systems. This processing will be further subjected to perceptual grouping (patterns of stimulation in space and time) and categorization at a central level leading to conscious perception of bodily sensations.

Interoception, just as exteroception, means taking “inductive leaps given very sparse data” (Kemp et al., 2007). Leaps may be made in both directions, from sensation levels to conclusion about categories, or from categories to sensations. Interoceptive categories are seldom fully distinct and misclassification (leaps to the wrong branch) can easily occur. Figure 1 illustrates overlap between interoceptive categories using the example of asthma and sports. Mistaking breathlessness during exercise as a symptom of respiratory disease may lead to exercise avoidance, reduced physical fitness, and disease aggravation (Wanrooij et al., 2014).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Level 4 presents the concept dyspnea as superordinate category, Level 3 presents examples of subordinate categories that can define the superordinate category on Level 4. Level 2 and 1 represent the variability of experience within each subordinate category. Level y presents the level of sensory data. Please note that for the sake of simplicity, we present only three of the multiple experiential categories which may be included in the experience of dyspnea. Please note also that this example represents the experience reported by one single person and is not intended to be a general model of dyspnea perception.

Asthma and panic are another example of two distinct categories that overlap strongly at the symptom level. Breathing sensations such as a tight chest and feelings of suffocation occur during asthma and panic attacks (Lehrer et al., 2002). While taking asthma medication (short-acting beta agonists) during an asthma attack alleviates symptoms, sensations induced by this medication such as increased heartbeat and tremor (Salpeter et al., 2004) may aggravate panic. If etiologically distinct symptom categories that require distinct courses of action overlap experientially, inferences based on the “wrong” priors/prototypes may be useless or even harmful. Again, we suggest that this applies to priors on all levels.

Indices of Perceived Category Homogeneity and Complexity

We propose that affect increases (1) perceived homogeneity of interoceptive categories and confidence in information inferred from these categories, (2) their subjective accentuation, and (3) the impact of these precise priors on perception. Testing dispersion of categories of bodily sensations rather than mean values is a new methodological approach that allows testing innovative hypotheses brought forward by recent predictive coding models. Placing our model in a categorization framework has the advantage that these as yet untested hypotheses can be targeted with established paradigms and statistics developed in the long-standing tradition of categorization research.

To calculate homogeneity of a sensation category, parameters such as standard deviation (SD), coefficient of variation, or range can be computed (Park and Judd, 1990; Boldry et al., 2007; Petersen et al., 2014b). SDs are usually higher for higher means and the coefficient of variation -as the ratio of SD divided by the mean—is recommended if mean values underlying SDs differ widely.

Furthermore, individual slopes across categories allow estimating perceived category variability (for a tutorial on individual slopes analysis, see Pfister et al., 2013). In this analysis, the location of the stimulus on the dimension underlying the category is used to predict, for example, self-report of stimulus intensity or unpleasantness. Steeper slopes indicate stronger differentiation between stimuli (Petersen et al., 2015). Studies are missing that test the effect of trait and state affect on perceived homogeneity within sensation categories, but we expect negative affect and anxiety to be related to lower dispersion (less steep slopes) and to stronger generalization.

Direct similarity ratings or confusion frequencies between stimuli within categories (e.g., Shepard, 1987) allow to compute category homogeneity without explicitly defining dimensions of experience (e.g., Borg and Groenen, 2005; Petersen et al., 2008). Similarity ratings and confusion frequencies can be analyzed in a signal detection approach calculating sensitivity for differences within and between categories and decision criteria. These approaches have been used in studies on interoceptive categorization using respiratory stimuli. Results suggest that individuals high in habitual symptom report and negative affect accentuate stronger between-category differences and show more liberal decision strategies (Petersen et al., 2015).

Furthermore, perceived overlap of sensation categories can be assessed using overlap-indices (Rafaeli-Mor et al., 1999). As an example, we calculated overlap, defined as average communality, for the data given in Figure 1. In this example, one person has selected twelve descriptors to describe dyspnea during “exercise” and 10 descriptors for dyspnea related to “asthma.” Five descriptors were used to describe both situations. Mean overlap is calculated as OL = (5/12+5/10)/2 = 0.46 indicating a perceived overlap of 46%. Depression has been suggested to be related to lower complexity in self-perception (Rafaeli-Mor et al., 1999), but we are not aware of research testing the relationship of anxiety, negative affect, or depression with blurred category boundaries in the perception of bodily sensations.

Other analytical strategies to test for differentiation between sensation categories include contingency tables. Taking the example in Figure 1, a 2 × 2 table can be drawn including in its cells the number of attributes that have been assigned only to one of the two categories or are shared by both categories. Chi2 for this 2 × 2 table is X2 = 4.96, p = 0.026 indicating that the attribution of descriptors to the two situations is significantly different from a random distribution. Other indices such as Cramer’s V or Phi allow to analyze distributions of attributes across contingency tables larger than 2 × 2 (Showers, 1992). They range between 0 and 1 with 1 indicating complete compartmentalization of attributes. In the example of Figure 1, Cramer’s V = 0.54, indicating moderate differentiation between exercise and asthma in the choice of descriptors.

Conclusion

Theories developed over the last centuries are in agreement about the relative and autonomous character of perception. We propose that this new perspective can be fruitfully applied to empirical paradigms using category homogeneity as estimate of confidence of the brain in perceptual inferences and predictions. This perspective may allow testing how affect is inherent to the perceptual process.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

This work was funded by a grant from the Fonds National de la Recherche, Luxembourg (6805115).

References

Anderson, J. R. (1991). The adaptive nature of human categorization. Psychol. Rev. 98, 409–429. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.3.409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Bull. 133, 1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., and Bar, M. (2009). See it with feeling: affective predictions during object perception. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 1325–1334. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., and Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 419–429. doi: 10.1038/nrn3950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boldry, J. G., Gaertner, L., and Quinn, J. (2007). Measuring the measures. A meta-analytic investigation of the measures of outgroup homogeneity. Group Processes Intergroup Relat. 10, 157–178. doi: 10.1177/1368430207075153

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borg, I., and Groenen, P. J. (2005). Modern Multidimensional Scaling: Theory and Applications. New York: Springer.

Google Scholar

Brown, R. J. (2004). Psychological mechanisms of medically unexplained symptoms: an integrative conceptual model. Psychol. Bull. 130, 793–812. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browning, M., Behrens, T. E., Jocham, G., O’Reilly, J. X., and Bishop, S. J. (2015). Anxious individuals have difficulty learning the causal statistics of aversive environments. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 590–596. doi: 10.1038/nn.3961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Büchel, C., Geuter, S., Sprenger, C., and Eippert, F. (2014). Placebo analgesia: a predictive coding perspective. Neuron 81, 1223–1239. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chater, N., and Vitányi, P. (2003). Simplicity: a unifying principle in cognitive science? Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 19–22. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00005-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cisler, J. M., and Koster, E. H. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 203–216. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantinou, E., Van Den Houte, M., Bogaerts, K., Van Diest, I., and Van den Bergh, O. (2015). Can words heal? Using labeling to reduce the effects of unpleasant cues on symptom reporting. Front. Psychol. 5:807. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cronbach, L. J., and Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychol. Bull. 52, 281–302. doi: 10.1037/h0040957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domschke, K., Stevens, S., Pfleiderer, B., and Gerlach, A. L. (2010). Interoceptive sensitivity in anxiety and anxiety disorders: an overview and integration of neurobiological findings. Clin. Psychol. Rev 30, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. D., Galton, H. C., Morgan, R., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Meyer, M., et al. (2010). Listening to your heart: How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1835–1844. doi: 10.1177/0956797610389191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, M. J., Adams, R. A., Brown, H., Pareés, I., and Friston, K. J. (2012). A Bayesian account of ‘hysteria’. Brain 135, 3495–3512. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, T. M., Stern, A., Clarke, D. D., Ivbijaro, G., and Kasney, L. M. (2010). The treatment of patients with medically unexplained symptoms in primary care: a review of the literature. Mental Health Family Med. 7, 209–221.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fallone, C. A., Guyatt, G. H., Armstrong, D., Wiklund, I., Degl’Innocenti, A., Heels-Ansdell, D., et al. (2004). Do physicians correctly assess patient symptom severity in gastro-oesophageal reflux disease? Alimentary Pharmacol. Ther. 20, 1161–1169. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2036.2004.02257.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foote, A. L., and Crystal, J. D. (2007). Metacognition in the rat. Curr. Biol. 17, 551–555. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.01.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 360, 815–836. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagliese, L. (2009). Pain and aging: the emergence of a new subfield of pain research. J. Pain 10, 343–353. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampton, R. R. (2001). Rhesus monkeys know when they remember. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 5359–5362. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071600998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hiller, W., Rief, W., and Brähler, E. (2006). Somatization in the population: from mild bodily misperceptions to disabling symptoms. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 41, 704–712. doi: 10.1007/s00127-006-0082-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hohwy, J. (2012). Attention and conscious perception in the hypothesis testing brain. Front. Psychol. 3:96. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hume, D. A. (1739). A Treatise of Human Nature. Reprint Edition, eds D. F. Norton and M. J. Norton (2007). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., and Vevea, J. L. (2000). Why do categories affect stimulus judgment? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 129, 220–241. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1909). The Meaning of Truth. New York: Longmans-Green. Reprint Edition, ed. W. James. (1975). The Meaning of Truth, Vol. 2. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Janssens, T., Verleden, G., De Peuter, S., Van Diest, I., and Van den Bergh, O. (2009). Inaccurate perception of asthma symptoms: a cognitive–affective framework and implications for asthma treatment. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 317–327. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joffily, M., and Coricelli, G. (2013). Emotional valence and the free-energy principle. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9:e1003094. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemp, C., Perfors, A., and Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Learning overhypotheses with hierarchical Bayesian models. Dev. Sci. 10, 307–321. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00585.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kepecs, A., Uchida, N., Zariwala, H. A., and Mainen, Z. F. (2008). Neural correlates, computation and behavioural impact of decision confidence. Nature 455, 227–231. doi: 10.1038/nature07200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Killian, K. J. (1985). The objective measurement of breathlessness. CHEST J. 88(2_Suppl. ), 84S–90S. doi: 10.1378/chest.88.2_Supplement.84S

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Van Damme, S., Verschuere, B., and De Houwer, J. (2004). Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotion 4, 312–317. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.3.312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). The psychology of being “right”: the problem of accuracy in social perception and cognition. Psychol. Bull. 106, 395–409. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.106.3.395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacy, C. R., Suh, D. C., Bueno, M., and Kostis, J. B. (2001). Delay in presentation and evaluation for acute stroke time registry for outcomes knowledge and epidemiology (STROKE). Stroke 32, 63–69. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.32.1.63

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leeuw, M., Goossens, M. E., Linton, S. J., Crombez, G., Boersma, K., and Vlaeyen, J. W. (2007). The fear-avoidance model of musculoskeletal pain: current state of scientific evidence. J. Behav. Med. 30, 77–94. doi: 10.1007/s10865-006-9085-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehrer, P., Feldman, J., Giardino, N., Song, H. S., and Schmaling, K. (2002). Psychological aspects of asthma. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 70, 691. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.70.3.691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynn, S. K., and Barrett, L. F. (2014). “Utilizing” signal detection theory. Psychol. Sci. 25, 1663–1673. doi: 10.1177/0956797614541991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macquart-Moulin, G., Viens, P., Bouscary, M. L., Genre, D., Resbeut, M., Gravis, G., et al. (1997). Discordance between physicians’ estimations and breast cancer patients’ self-assessment of side-effects of chemotherapy: an issue for quality of care. Br. J. Cancer 76, 1640–1645. doi: 10.1038/bjc.1997.610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogg, K., and Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 36, 809–848. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00063-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, B., and Judd, C. M. (1990). Measures and models of perceived group variability. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 59, 173–191. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parshall, M. B., Schwartzstein, R. M., Adams, L., Banzett, R. B., Manning, H. L., Bourbeau, J., et al. (2012). An official American Thoracic Society statement: update on the mechanisms, assessment, and management of dyspnea. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 185, 436–452. doi: 10.1164/rccm.201111-2042ST

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfister, R., Schwarz, K., Carson, R., and Jancyzk, M. (2013). Easy methods for extracting individual regression slopes: comparing SPSS, R, and Excel. Tutor. Quant. Methods Psychol. 9, 72–78.

Google Scholar

Petersen, S., Orth, B., and Ritz, T. (2008). Awareness of breathing: the structure of language descriptors of respiratory sensations. Health Psychol. 27, 122–127. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.1.122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S., van den Berg, R., Janssens, T., and Van den Bergh, O. (2011). Illness and symptom perception: a theoretical approach towards an integrative measurement model. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 428–439. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S., Van Stayen, K., Vögele, C., Von Leupoldt, A., and Van den Bergh, O. (2015). Disentangling accuracy and bias in interoception. Front. Psychol. 6:732. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S., Von Leupoldt, A., and Van den Bergh, O. (2014a). Geriatric dyspnea: doing worse, feeling better. Ageing Res. Rev. 15, 94–99. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2014.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S., Schroijen, M., Mölders, C., Zenker, S., and Van den Bergh, O. (2014b). Categorical interoception perceptual organization of sensations from inside. Psychol. Sci. 25, 1059–1066. doi: 10.1177/0956797613519110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quellar, S., Schell, T., and Mason, W. (2006). A novel view of between-categories contrast and within-category assimilation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 406–422. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rafaeli-Mor, E., Gotlib, I. H., and Revelle, W. (1999). The meaning and measurement of self-complexity. Pers. Indiv. Dif. 27, 341–356. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00247-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, S. K. (1972). Pattern recognition and categorization. Cogn. Psychol. 3, 382–407. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(72)90014-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riegel, B., Dickson, V. V., Cameron, J., Johnson, J. C., Bunker, S., Page, K., et al. (2010). Symptom recognition in elders with heart failure. J. Nurs. Scholarsh. 42, 92–100. doi: 10.1111/j.1547-5069.2010.01333.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, M. D., and Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychol. Bull. 128, 934–360. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salpeter, S. R., Ormiston, T. M., and Salpeter, E. E. (2004). Cardiovascular effects of β-agonists in patients with asthma and COPDA meta-analysis. CHEST J. 125, 2309–2321. doi: 10.1378/chest.125.6.2309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidel, E. M., Pfabigan, D. M., Hahn, A., Sladky, R., Grahl, A., Paul, K., et al. (2015). Uncertainty during pain anticipation: the adaptive value of preparatory processes. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 744–755. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepard, R. N. (1987). Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science 237, 1317–1323. doi: 10.1126/science.3629243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Showers, C. (1992). Compartmentalization of positive and negative self-knowledge: keeping bad apples out of the bunch. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 62, 1036–1049. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.62.6.1036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., and Lane, R. D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J. Affect. Disord. 61, 201–216. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thurstone, L. L. (1927). A law of comparative judgment. Psychol. Rev. 34, 273–286. doi: 10.1037/h0070288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van de Cruys, S. (2014). To Err and Err, but Less and Less. Predictive Coding and Affective Value in Perception, Art, and Autism. Ph.D thesis, KU Leuven, Leuven.

Google Scholar

Van den Bergh, O., Winters, W., Devriese, S., and Van Diest, I. (2002). Learning subjective health complaints. Scand. J. Psychol. 43, 147–152. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van den Bergh, O., Winters, W., Devriese, S., Van Diest, I., Vos, G., and De Peuter, S. (2004). Accuracy of respiratory symptom perception in persons with high and low negative affectivity. Psychol. Health 19, 213–222. doi: 10.1080/08870440410001675627

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wanrooij, V. H., Willeboordse, M., Dompeling, E., and van de Kant, K. D. (2014). Exercise training in children with asthma: a systematic review. Br. J. Sports Med. 48, 1024–1031. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., and Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and distress: exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychol. Rev. 96, 234–254. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.2.234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiebe, D. J., Alderfer, M. A., Palmer, S. C., Lindsay, R., and Jarrett, L. (1994). Behavioral self-regulation in adolescents with type I diabetes: negative affectivity and blood glucose symptom perception. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 62, 1204–1212. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.62.6.1204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yong, H. H., Gibson, S. J., David, J. D. L., and Helme, R. D. (2001). Development of a pain attitudes questionnaire to assess stoicism and cautiousness for possible age differences. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 56, P279–P284. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.5.p279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaman, J., Vlaeyen, J. W., Van Oudenhove, L., Wiech, K., and Van Diest, I. (2015). Associative fear learning and perceptual discrimination: a perceptual pathway in the development of chronic pain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 51, 118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: interoception, predictive coding, affect, categorization, symptom perception

Citation: Petersen S, von Leupoldt A and Van den Bergh O (2015) Interoception and the uneasiness of the mind: affect as perceptual style. Front. Psychol. 6:1408. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01408

Received: 05 July 2015; Accepted: 03 September 2015;
Published: 17 September 2015.

Edited by:

Wolfgang Tschacher, University of Bern, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Annette Horstmann, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Germany
Omar C. G. Gelo, Universita del Salento/Sigmund Freud University, Italy

Copyright © 2015 Petersen, von Leupoldt and van den Bergh. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sibylle Petersen, Institute for Health and Behaviour, University of Luxembourg, 2 Avenue de l’Université, L-4365 Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg, sibylle.petersen@uni.lu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.