Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 10 November 2015
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Measuring Verbal Psychotherapeutic Techniques—A Systematic Review of Intervention Characteristics and Measures

\r\nAntje Gumz,*Antje Gumz1,2*Barbara TreeseBarbara Treese3Christopher MarxChristopher Marx1Bernhard StraussBernhard Strauss3Hanna Wendt&#x;Hanna Wendt2
  • 1Berlin University of Psychology, Berlin, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
  • 3Institute of Psychosocial Medicine and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Jena, Jena, Germany

Language is one of the most important “tools” of psychotherapists. The working mechanisms of verbal therapeutic techniques, however, are still marginally understood. In part, this is due to the lack of a generally acknowledged typology as well as a gold standard for the assessment of verbal techniques, which limits the possibility of conducting studies focusing this topic. The present study reviews measures used in clinical research which assess directly observable dimensions of verbal interventions in a reliable manner. All measures were evaluated with respect to their theoretical foundation, research goals, assessment modes, and various psychometric properties. A systematic search in databases (PubMed, PsycInfo, PsycArticles, PSYNDEX, Web of Science, Embase) followed by an additional “snowballing” search covering the years 1940–2013 yielded n = 179 publications eligible for review. Within these publications, 34 measures were identified showing great heterogeneity regarding the aspects under study. Only two measures reached the highest psychometric standards and can be recommended for clinical use without any reservation. Central problems include deficiencies in the systematization of techniques as well as their partly ambiguous and inconsistent definitions. To promote this field of research, it will be important to achieve a consensus concerning the terminology, conceptions and measures of verbal interventions.

Introduction

The general efficacy of psychotherapy is well established (e.g., Lambert, 2013) based on a variety of reliable and valid measures. Numerous studies demonstrated that psychotherapeutic treatment using different change theories and related to diagnoses is associated with an improvement of symptoms, quality of life and psychological, social and occupational functioning (e.g., Beutler, 2009; Norcross, 2011; Wampold and Imel, 2015). Moreover, there seem to be only minor significant differences of therapeutic effects in different psychotherapy schools (Castonguay and Beutler, 2006; Barber et al., 2013; Wampold and Imel, 2015). However, other findings indicate that there is still a considerable amount of patients who do not benefit from psychotherapy (Lambert, 2013), or even get worse and that the sustainability of therapy effects is limited in certain patient populations (Barlow, 2010).

These findings call for increased research efforts in order to improve mental health care. Accordingly, the focus of psychotherapy research has increasingly shifted from the question “if” psychotherapy is effective to the question what works and how it works (Castonguay, 2013).

The Active Ingredients of Psychotherapy

What are the active ingredients of psychotherapy? In order to answer this question it is important to decide which variables need to be considered in respective research agendas. Psychotherapy processes can be described with regard to different characteristics (e.g., Orlinsky and Howard, 1987). An important topic in this respect is whether the effect of psychotherapeutic interventions is mainly due to common therapeutic factors or to therapeutic techniques.

Common factors are implicitly relevant in any therapeutic interaction. They are not explicitly anchored in the treatment models of the different schools of psychotherapy, nor are they considered in the treatment models for specific mental disorders (Tschacher et al., 2014). In their Taxonomy Project Tschacher et al. (2014) address the topic of definition und conceptualization of common factors of psychotherapy and delineate different terminological problems and logical inconsistencies. Based on a comprehensive literature search the authors specified all constructs discussed as non-specific or common factors of therapeutic change in psychotherapy research literature by at least two authors. The resulting list comprises 22 common factors (Pfammatter and Tschacher, 2012; Tschacher et al., 2014). These factors include, for example, therapeutic alliance, readiness to change, insight or cognitive restructuring (Connolly Gibbons et al., 2009; Tschacher et al., 2014). In accordance with the Generic Model of Psychotherapy (Orlinsky and Howard, 1987) common factors relate to various aspects of the therapeutic process: Interpersonal (e.g., therapeutic alliance), intrapersonal (e.g., instillation of hope), or clinical aspects (e.g., affective catharsis).

In many cases, techniques which are considered to belong to a particular psychotherapy method, and common factors have been treated as alternatives in explaining therapeutic progress. In other words, it was considered that either specific therapeutic interventions or common factors can explain the effects of psychotherapy. This dichotomy, however, needs to be questioned. Techniques and common factors cannot be considered independently. Common factors evolve in the context of the therapeutic relationship, which in turn is influenced to a large extent by the techniques. Having this in mind, common factors can be regarded as active principles implemented by the therapist's specific techniques (Castonguay and Beutler, 2006; Pfammatter and Tschacher, 2012; Gumz et al., 2013).

Knowledge about the common factors provides little guidance for the therapist for what can actually be done in the therapy sessions. Empirical knowledge regarding the therapeutic techniques, i.e., regarding the question what therapists mostly do in successful sessions or successful therapies in general, are of great value to psychotherapy, as they allow recommendations of specific practice. Differentiation of therapist behaviors is crucial for specifying the extent to which techniques actually differ and how this ultimately affects treatment outcome (Bergin and Strupp, 2009). Therefore, it is an important desideratum to analyze therapeutic techniques on the level of single sessions or single units of therapeutic interaction to give profound answers to the question of the how of therapeutic effectiveness (Margison et al., 2000; Mergenthaler, 2015).

What is Specifically Meant by Technique?

To date, there is no general consensus regarding the question what is meant by the term “technique.” Is this what the therapist does or says? Obviously not. The term can also refer to therapeutic attitudes, implicit theories or even patient behavior typical to a therapy method, e.g., warmth and empathy (Bergin and Strupp, 2009), therapeutic abstinence, neutrality, free association technique or regression (Gumz et al., 2014; Tschacher et al., 2014). Some techniques refer to broader descriptions of procedures or settings, e.g., role playing, sculpture work, reflecting team technique and hypnosis. In other cases, “technique” refers to detailed descriptions of single verbal statements such as the verbalization of emotional reactions or transference interpretations (Gumz et al., 2014; Tschacher et al., 2014).

Bergin and Strupp (2009) pointed out that the repertoire of the contemporary psychotherapist includes an impressive list of techniques, which often are employed in combination but more or less intuitively and unsystematically. Schools of therapy differ in their relative emphases upon particular techniques.

In our view, it is possible to draw a distinction between specific and common techniques. The term “specific” means that a certain technique is unique for a particular psychotherapy method. For example, exposure with response prevention is considered as a typical technique of cognitive-behavioral therapists, transference interpretation as a typical technique for psychodynamic therapists and circular questions as a typical technique for systemic therapists. Common techniques are those that are not specific for a respective therapy method. For example, the technique of exploration cannot be related to a specific therapy method. Another example is the verbalization of emotional reactions which is considered as typical technique of humanistic therapists, while psychodynamic therapists use this technique as well (Gumz et al., 2014).

The term “specific” might also mean that a certain technique may be tailored to the treatment of a specific psychiatric disorder. Such a tailored technique is, for example, planning of pleasant activities in the treatment of depressive disorders or keeping a food diary in the treatment of eating disorders (Pfammatter and Tschacher, 2012; Tschacher et al., 2014).

All in all, therapeutic techniques are characterized by numerous very different features, which makes it difficult to establish definite criteria for their assessment (Gumz et al., 2014). One approach to reduce complexity is to focus on single verbal techniques as a first step. On this level, however, an orientation is not easy as well, as there is no generally acknowledged typology of verbal techniques. Types of verbal techniques are described inconsistently with regard to the number and kind of techniques of a certain therapeutic method. Moreover, definitions of categories with the same label partly do not match, or vice versa, identical aspects have differing designations (Brumberg and Gumz, 2012; Gumz et al., 2014).

This can be illustrated using the example of clarification which was analyzed amongst other categories within the framework of a systematic investigation of definitions of psychodynamic intervention techniques in the theoretical literature (Gumz et al., 2014). In most, but not in all of the analyzed sources, clarification was specified as a type of intervention technique of high importance. Different authors consistently specified that the therapeutic aim and a core characteristic of a clarification is to foster the understanding of a phenomenon. This aim is also described as a core characteristic of the concept interpretation. The formal techniques through which this aim is supposed to be achieved, are heterogeneous. Thus, the clarification was described as: (a) The patient is asked to specify and to associate or (b) the patient is asked to describe a phenomenon in more detail or (c) the therapist identifies recurrent topics or themes or (d) the therapist inquires about the patient‘s feelings (e.g., with respect to related associations or parallels) or (e) the therapist describes the effect of the patient's behavior on the therapist or, finally,(f) the therapist rephrases the central ideas of the patient's statement or summarizes these.

In order to clarify what is specifically meant by “therapeutic techniques,” we suggest to systematize on four hierarchical levels:

Level (1) First of all, techniques referring to the therapeutic dialogue need to be distinguished from techniques referring to broader descriptions of procedures or settings or physical exercises, e.g., role playing, sculpture work, reflecting team technique and hypnosis, keeping a food diary in the treatment of eating disorders, or telling the patient to do a breathing exercise.

Level (2) Within the group of techniques referring to the therapeutic dialogue, verbal, and non-verbal techniques can be distinguished. We define verbal techniques as the verbal utterances of the therapist within the therapeutic dialogue. Non-verbal techniques (or behavior, communication) are closely intertwined with verbal information and prosodic features (Madonik, 2001; Pawelczyk, 2011; e.g., mimic signals, affective expressions, movement patterns, see Geißler, 2005).

Level (3) Verbal techniques contain directly observable and latent features. Directly observable features relate to the semantic content (“what is said”) of therapeutic utterances or other semantic units (words, sentences, longer segments) which can be rated based on session transcripts or audio recordings. Latent features characterize the implicit pragmatic content of the utterances (“what is implicated” or “what is meant”). In order to evaluate latent features of verbal techniques a higher degree of subjective inference is necessary, compared to directly observable features. If a whole session is rated (global coding), the semantic or implicit pragmatic content of all therapeutic utterances is usually aggregated. In consequence, global coding methods generally involve a higher degree of subjective inference because larger amounts of data need to be cognitively aggregated and there are no obligatory rules regarding this aggregation (Heaton et al., 1995). Among the latent features there are, e.g., functional characteristics (e.g., therapist's intentions such as “directing the dialog,” “speaking kindly,” “controlling the affects” or therapeutic attitudes like abstinence, neutrality) or qualitative characteristics (e.g., therapist's empathy, warmth or competence, internal coherence of an intervention).

Level (4) Regarding directly observable features we suggest to differentiate three basic characteristics: 1. Form (i.e., the formal and structural manner in which the therapist responds to the patient's experience, behavior or statement, e.g. “closed question,” “interpreting,” “paraphrase”), 2. Temporal focus (i.e., the period of time to which the intervention refers, e.g., “childhood,” “present”), and 3. Thematic content (i.e., the topic of the intervention, e.g., “therapist,” “mother,” “defenses”).

Research on Verbal Therapeutic Techniques

Within psychotherapy process research, varying instruments are used to measure therapeutic techniques, each having its own focus, capturing different features, and showing different psychometric characteristics. The findings of empirical studies are highly variable and often inconsistent, making it difficult to draw firm conclusions regarding the nature, processes, and effects of verbal techniques. There is an overwhelming amount of results concerning different techniques from various psychotherapy schools, naturalistic or manualized, gained by means of observational studies or interventional studies and on the basis of very different sample sizes. These studies examined the association of techniques with different factors such as symptom change, therapeutic relationship, therapeutic gains in single sessions or the relationship of specific techniques with therapy- or therapist-related variables. The diversity of these studies is difficult to systematize.

It can be assumed that all these difficulties contributed to the fact that, so far, there is little systematic knowledge regarding the efficacy of therapeutic techniques. Reviews on the subject are either concerned with selected types of techniques (e.g., transference interpretation; Høglend, 2004; Brumberg and Gumz, 2012), methodologically flawed (e.g., Blagys and Hilsenroth, 2000, 2002), or out-dated (Elliott et al., 1987).

An important first step to advance this field of research should be to establish a more solid theoretical as well as methodological framework for the measurement of verbal techniques. As a starting point we suggest to differentiate four basic features of verbal techniques as explained in the previous section: 1. Form (observable characteristic), 2. Temporal focus (observable characteristic), 3. Thematic content (observable characteristic), and 4. Latent characteristics. Such a typology can be helpful to review and structure the amount of existing theoretical articles and empirical results regarding categories of verbal therapeutic techniques.

The Current Review

Systematic knowledge regarding the question what kind of verbal techniques a therapist should or should not apply in his or her therapeutic work is of major practical importance. Our review attempts an initial clarification of the state of affairs by addressing the question which instruments are available for the assessment of verbal techniques in psychotherapy, which types of techniques are assessed and which psychometric properties the measures have.

We will restrict our review to the directly observable features of verbal techniques (see Section What is Specifically Meant by Technique?). Verbal techniques are the cornerstone of most psychotherapeutic methods. Language is the basic medium through which new information is conveyed, and it is one of the most important “tools” of the therapist (Gumz et al., 2014). Verbal techniques can, in principle, be examined beyond the differences between therapeutic schools. Focusing on verbal techniques, we will concentrate on measures used to assess directly observable dimensions. This means that we will restrict the review on measures which assess the formal, temporal, and thematic dimensions. Measures that exclusively address latent characteristics of therapeutic techniques were not considered in this review, as this would have potentiated the complexity of the research agenda and in the light of the long-term objective to systematically analyze techniques in order to be able to establish the state of the art in this research area. By focusing on explicit features of verbal techniques it is possible to clarify major facets of the research agenda. The next step will then be to extend the focus of research on latent features.

Moreover, an important criterion was that the reviewed measures report reliability.

We hope to contribute to a more consistent understanding of the subject. A more advanced systematization will help researchers to evaluate results as well as to choose existing instruments or to develop new instruments. Moreover, there is a practical benefit of our review: Only if the categories of a measure can be reliably registered by different individuals (interrater reliability), they can later be correctly trained and applied by other clinicians.

Our specific research questions were:

1. What are the characteristics of existing reliable measures assessing the formal, temporal and thematic dimensions of verbal therapeutic techniques?

a. What is the theoretical basis for the measures?

b. How have the measures been developed and which research questions were addressed?

c. What are the scales, categories, or items underlying the measures?

d. Which assessment mode is employed and which rating perspectives are considered?

2. What are the psychometric properties of the measures?

Methods

Eligibility Criteria

Studies were eligible for inclusion if they met the following criteria:

a. The study dealt with verbal therapeutic techniques within a psychotherapeutic setting.

b. Verbal therapeutic techniques were assessed at least partially with regard to formal, temporal or thematic features.

c. The study was based on ratings of specific psychotherapy sessions.

d. The study referred to individual outpatient psychotherapy with adults.

e. The study was published in a peer-reviewed journal.

f. The study was published in English or German.

g. The measure was developed with regard to well-established methods assuring the interrater reliability of the assessment of verbal techniques in order to establish its general applicability, i.e., Intraclass Coefficient Correlations (ICC; Shrout and Fleiss, 1979), (weighted) kappa-values (Cohen, 1960, 1968; Fleiss, 1971; Light, 1971), Finn's r (Finn, 1970, 1972), or Pearson's product-moment correlation coefficient. The procedure of reliability assessment and the reliability values need to be reported in a study published in English or German in a peer-reviewed journal.

Search Procedure and Study Selection Process

A systematic database search (Pubmed, PsycInfo, PsycArticles, PSYINDEX, Web of science, and Embase) for the years from 1940 to 2013 was conducted by one of the authors (B.T.) using the following search terms, their combination and truncation: psychotherapy, psychotherapeutic process, process research, measure, scale, rating, instrument, intervention, therapeutic technique, coding. The resulting records were judged by B.T. regarding titles and abstracts considering the above mentioned eligibility criteria. The 162 resulting studies were judged for eligibility by all authors. In a next step, further relevant articles were identified (by B.T. and H.W.) by screening references of the included articles as well as the excluded reviews (“snowballing,” c.f. Greenhalgh and Peacock, 2005) and by searching identified authors' names and measures (hand search). Additionally, the authors of all measures were contacted to collect coding manuals and further relevant information (non-response to the request was not a criterion for exclusion).

Psychometric Evaluation of Measures

Each measure was evaluated regarding objectivity, reliability, and validity. For each of these criteria, an “A” means that there are no limitations, a “B” indicates some restrictions, and “C” points to severe deficits (see Figure 1 for evaluation criteria). An existing coding manual with clear and unambiguous item/category definitions enables the user to conduct objective ratings. Therefore, we assumed high objectivity when a detailed coding manual was published or available from the authors. Since all included measures report reliability, this aspect is differentiated into two levels with ICC values being the best because they are in line with the highest statistical standards (Bartko and Carpenter, 1975; Tinsley and Weiss, 1975).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Criteria for evaluation of the included measures.

Results

Study Selection

Based on the systematic literature research, 1568 records were found. After screening the titles and abstracts, 162 records were selected for assessment of eligibility. Ninety-nine records had to be excluded: 31 reviews and 26 records were not peer-reviewed (11 book chapters, 15 dissertations), 23 records did not refer to verbal therapeutic techniques, but to other aspects of psychotherapy. Further six records had to be excluded, because the measure did not refer to the intervention form or content characteristics of verbal techniques. Five further records were excluded because they were not used for rating therapy sessions but the general activity of therapists in psychotherapy. Seven measures had to be excluded because of insufficient interrater reliability (e.g., percentages of agreements) or because reliability was not reported at all (Adler and Enelow, 1966; Holzman and Forman, 1966; Karl and Abeles, 1969; Gedo and Schaffer, 1989; Winston et al., 1991; Bucci and Maski, 2007; Hepner et al., 2010). Only one record was published in another language than English or German so that it could not be included. The following snowballing research revealed 116 further relevant records (see Figure 2 for study selection process). In total, 179 articles were included in the present systematic review. They comprise 168 peer-reviewed articles and 11 published coding manuals or scale descriptions and comprise a total of 34 measures. These are presented in Tables 15 in an alphabetical order, separated according to the theoretical orientation and the assessment mode (global measures referring to complete therapy sessions vs. microanalytic measures referring to therapists' single utterances). To maintain a good readability we will refer to the respective measure using its acronym (e.g., YACS for the Yale Adherence and Competence Scale, see Tables 15 for the references of respective acronyms).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Flow diagram of study selection process.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Global measures referring to psychodynamic therapy.

Theoretical Orientation

Eighteen of the included measures refer to a specific theoretical orientation. Most of these are derived from psychodynamic therapy (see Table 1 for global and Table 2 for microanalytic measures). Within these, two measures are specially developed for use in analytic therapy, two for supportive-expressive therapy and one for time-limited psychodynamic therapy, the other seven measures can be generally used in dynamic psychotherapy. Only three measures refer to cognitive-behavioral orientated therapy or to a specific setting (drug counseling, treatment for substance use disorders, motivational interview), respectively (see Table 3). Sixteen measures were classified as “pantheoretical,” because they refer to several different theoretical orientations (see Table 4 for global and Table 5 for microanalytic measures).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Microanalytic measures referring to psychodynamic therapy.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Measures referring to cognitive-behavioral orientated therapy or to a specific setting.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Pantheoretical global measures.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Pantheoretical microanalytic measures.

Scale Development and Research Questions

All measures have been developed using a top down approach based on theoretical considerations, therapy manuals, literature research, clinical experience, and expert discussion as well as already existing scales. Three measures (APS, see Table 2; YACS, see Table 3; and PQS, see Table 4) included bottom-up analyses comprising analyses of therapist's utterances and audio- or videotaped therapy sessions among other strategies within the scale development process but none of the measures was developed exclusively in a bottom-up procedure (see Tables 15, column 3).

The measures have been developed to address various research questions. These can be classified into five groups: 1. Six measures aim to assess the adherence and competence of the therapist regarding specific therapeutic orientations (e.g., ACS-SEC, see Table 1; CTS-R, see Table 3; SPRS, see Table 4; YACS, see Table 3). 2. Among the pantheoretical measures, five measures aim to distinguish between two or more therapeutic orientations (e.g., CPPS, see Table 4; CSPRS, see Table 4; CSTF, see Table 5; SATC, see Table 5). 3. Two measures aim to investigate microprocesses in psychotherapy such as patient therapist interaction (e.g., PIRS, see Table 2; and CIP, see Table 3), or to analyze the relationship between therapeutic techniques and therapy outcome (four measures, i.e., APS, see Table 2; CAPS, see Table 1; MIT, see Table 2; PIC see Table 2). 4. Eleven measures try to assess all possible verbal techniques in general (e.g., ITS, see Table 5; PQS, see Table 4) or within a therapeutic orientation (e.g., Coding of therapist statement, see Table 2; CTACS, see Table 3). 5. Finally, six measures focus on very specific aspects of psychotherapy, e.g., techniques used within the psychodynamic therapy of patients with borderline personality disorder (TVII, see Table 1), or focusing theoretically important constructs such as “interpretations” (TIRS, see Table 2).

Description of Subscales, Categories, and Items

According to the research questions, the measures assess verbal therapeutic techniques on various differentiation levels. The number of categories ranges from four categories without further differentiation (Coding of therapist statement; see Table 2; Connolly et al., 1998) to 100 items without further categorization (PQS; see Table 4; Jones, 1985) or 96 items, categorized into eight scales with several subscales (CSPRS; see Table 4; Hollon et al., unpublished work, cited from Hill et al., 1992; see Figure 3 for details). Apart from this differentiation level, the focus is partly limited to only one or two aspects of verbal techniques. The MIT (see Table 2) for example differentiates between “interpretation” vs. “non-interpretation.” The ISTS (see Table 1) and the ITS (see Table 5) distinguish the scales “supportive techniques” from “interpretative techniques” which include further items, respectively. The category or item that is assessed by the majority of measures is “interpretation.”

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Number of items per measure.

All measures assess techniques concerning their form (e.g., “question,” “interpretation,” “agenda setting”). Moreover, ten measures refer to the content of an intervention. Among those which address the thematic content or the temporal focus are for example the PIC (see Table 2; four content codes: defenses, anxieties, impulse/feeling, no content), the TAS (see Table 1; “future plans of patient”), and Response modes coding system (see Table 5; three categories: statement categories, time frames, and person code). But 12 measures also refer to the latent dimension, because they include scales or items which address the “climate or atmosphere” (PQS, see Table 4) or the “Therapist's warmth and friendliness” (VPPS, see Table 4). Some measures even mix the formal and latent dimension on item levels (e.g., VTSS, see Table 1: “Therapist shows evidence of listening respectively,” ISTS, see Table 1: “gratify the patient, i.e., make the patient feel good rather than anxious in the session”).

Assessment Mode and Rating Perspective

The assessment mode of the included measures can be generally classified into global vs. microanalytic. Global measures refer to whole therapy sessions or larger segments of sessions. Microanalytic measures refer to more fine-grained units like utterances or sentences. Furthermore, judged units can be assessed on metric vs. dichotomous scales and can be coded as manyfold vs. mutually exclusive.

Within the theoretical orientations, the amount of global and microanalytic measures is approximately counterbalanced. Global measures generally use metric scales, microanalytic measures prefer dichotomous items which are mutually exclusive. The only measures that assess verbal therapeutic techniques in a metric way on a microanalytic level are: 1. the APS (where ratings refer to segments of session so that it is not purely microanalytic, see Table 2) and one scale (“degree of initiative”) of the SATC (see Table 5), which does not belong to the observable formal or content dimension of verbal techniques. All in all, no measure purely measures verbal techniques in a metric way on a microanalytic level.

The PQS (see Table 4) is the one measure that uses a completely different rating method (Q-sort; Jones, 1985). The 100 items have to be sorted for each session on a continuum from 1 = least characteristic to 9 = most characteristic so that the distribution of items yield a normal distribution.

In general, all measures are based upon an external observer's rating; the only exception is the TIQI (see Table 4) which is rated by the therapist himself. Two measures (CPPS and MULTI, see Table 4) can be additionally rated by the therapist and the patient. These two measures therefore allow simultaneous ratings from three different perspectives.

Psychometric Aspects

Objectivity

Of the 34 included measures, only 14 report a detailed coding manual which is published and/or available in the World Wide Web or from the authors and therefore fulfill the A-criterion within this category. Within these coding manuals, the definitions of the categories or items are described in detail and rater instructions are given. Only two manuals are less exact, one gives general information about the structure and use of the measure (APS, see Table 2) and one manual only gives short definitions of the categories (PIRS, see Table 2). For eight measures, the coding manual exists as an unpublished manuscript. For 13 measures, no coding instruction is reported, but a description of the scales or categories can mostly be found in the published articles.

Reliability

There are large differences regarding the reliability of the included measures (see Tables 15, column 9 and 10; see Supplementary Material for procedure and results of reliability calculation). First, the procedures of reliability calculation differ widely. The number of sessions and patients on which the values are based ranges from three sessions with three patients (total of 9 sessions; Silberschatz et al., 1986) to eight sessions with 33 clients (total of 264 sessions; Stiles et al., 1989). Some studies report reliability scores based on consensus ratings (e.g., Butler et al., 1995) or ratings achieved by regular meetings to prevent rater drift (e.g., Gaston and Ring, 1992; Connolly Gibbons et al., 2002). Some reliability values are based on ratings of students, others on ratings of the measures' authors. In most cases, raters have been trained extensively before using the measure (e.g., Goldfried et al., 1997), but sometimes not (e.g., McCarthy and Barber, 2009). Some studies describe the rating procedure incompletely so that information about the number of rated sessions is missing.

Secondly, the reliability gold standard by calculating ICC's is fulfilled by n = 16 of the included measures. Within these, ICC (2,2) and ICC (2,1) are mostly used. For two measures, it remains unclear which form of ICC was calculated (VTSS; see Table 1, Butler et al., 1995; CTS-R; see Table 3, Blackburn et al., 2001). Overall, ICC values range from acceptable to high levels. Scales, categories or items with ICC values below the acceptable level are found in measures with discrete categories, so none of the measures had to be excluded because of lacking reliability, although ICC values are presented.

All other measures (n = 18) report Finn's r correlation coefficients, Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients or kappa values. Similar to ICC values, it is often not clearly specified which kind of kappa has been applied (e.g., “Light's kappa,” Light, 1971; “Fleiss kappa,” Fleiss, 1971), whether it is the median or the average kappa). One measure failed to report reliability values for all subscales. Since available values were good, the measure was not excluded from the review.

Validity

Six of the included measures report no information about the validity of the measure and three further measures report insufficient information. For three measures, studies are available which report a broad examination and evidence for validity for the respective measure: CPPS (see Table 4; Hilsenroth et al., 2005), ITS (see Table 5; Gaston and Ring, 1992), and YACS (see Table 3; Carroll et al., 2000). All other measures report validity on a minimal level, especially with regard to criterion validity (which means that scales reflect and distinguish between different therapeutic orientations) and convergent validity (which means that the scale has been compared with another measure).

All in all, only two measures fulfill the A-criterion for all three categories (CPPS, see Table 4, and YACS, see Table 3; see Supplementary Material for procedure and results of validity calculation).

Discussion

The objective of the present study was to review available measures designed to assess directly observable (i.e., formal, temporal, and thematic) dimensions of verbal therapeutic techniques in a reliable and valid manner. All in all, 34 measures were identified which show a great heterogeneity regarding purposes, theoretical foundations and assessment procedures. Whereas there is much more consensus related to outcome measures in psychotherapy (e.g., Strupp et al., 1997). In general, it appears that most research groups rather created their own new measure than using existing ones in order to address their research questions. Explanations of this approach include that available measures are too specific in focus (McCarthy and Barber, 2009), do not cover the necessary constructs under investigation (Milbrath et al., 1999), require too much effort, or show insufficient psychometric properties (Hilsenroth et al., 2005). This practice, however, generated a number of problems in the assessment of verbal techniques.

Issues Regarding the Systematization of Techniques

The reviewed measures refer to a specific theoretical orientation or to different theoretical orientations. They have been developed to address different research questions such as assessing adherence and competence of the therapist regarding specific therapeutic orientations, distinguishing between therapeutic orientations, investigating microprocesses in psychotherapy, or analyzing the relationship between techniques and therapy outcome.

Some of them assess all possible verbal techniques (again within a therapeutic orientation or in general), whereas others focus on selected theoretically important constructs such as “interpretation.” “Interpretation” is the category or item that is assessed by the majority of measures. The focus is partly limited to only one or two aspects of verbal techniques (e.g., differentiation between “interpretation” vs. “non-interpretation” or “supportive techniques” vs. “interpretative techniques”). There are heterogeneous levels on which techniques are differentiated. The number of categories ranges from four categories without further differentiation to 100 items without further categorization.

A potential problem is that the hierarchical structures of the measures are different, although the same or similar techniques are assessed. For example, the ISTS (see Table 1) contains the scale “interpretative techniques” (level 1) which includes further subordinated items like the “impression of the therapist” (level 2). The HSS (see Table 5), on the other hand, subsumes items as “interpretation” (level 2) within the scale “insight skills” (level 1). Thus, it is difficult to specify the relationship of scales and items in both measures (notably the intervention “interpretation”) as well as results derived from using them.

The definitions of verbal techniques are also very different across measures. In some cases, the same categories are defined in different ways. For example, the category “interpretation” was defined as a therapeutic utterance which goes beyond the perceptions of the patient (Hill, 1978), an utterance which refers to one or several dynamic components (e.g., wishes, fears, defense mechanisms; Piper et al., 1998) or a theory-derived response of the analyst, rated according to the degree to which it transforms meaning by bringing aspects outside of awareness into full awareness (Waldron et al., 2004b). In other cases, intervention categories show overlaps and can hardly be distinguished from one another. This situation is certainly due to and aggravated by the fact that—in the theoretical literature—specific intervention techniques are described and defined inconsistently, abstractly and in part ambiguously, which limits their operationalizability and investigation in evidence-based therapy research (Fonagy, 2000; Paniagua, 2003; Gumz et al., 2014).

Another problem derives from the different facets of the assessment of verbal techniques. One problem in this respect is that many items or subscales of available measures simultaneously assess different features of therapeutic techniques (e.g., formal and qualitative features, thematic content, and therapist's intentions), prohibiting the possibility to analyze these features separately (e.g., VTSS, see Table 1: “Therapist encourages the patient's expression and/or exploration of feelings in relation to a significant other (including therapist.”)

From our point of view it is helpful to clearly specify on which level techniques are to be described. There is a difference between settings in which more global descriptions are needed (e.g., role play technique, sculpture work, hypnosis or transference regarding a more comprehensive topic) and settings in which a focus on the microanalytic level of single verbal interventions is more important (e.g., the verbalization of emotional reactions or a specific transference interpretation; Gumz et al., 2014) and where there is greater overlap between different therapeutic schools. Furthermore, it would be beneficial to separate the basic features of verbal techniques (Form, Temporal focus, Thematic content, Latent characteristics). This relates specifically to the latent characteristics. This would provide the opportunity to evaluate different aspects of therapeutic techniques, i.e., to differentiate between what the therapist said and what his utterance implied. Even if this strict systematic may be difficult to realize in research it may be useful to compare results and conduct meta-analyses.

Issues Regarding Assessment Mode and Rating Perspective

Half of the measures reviewed (n = 17) use global assessment (i.e., rating of complete sessions) while the other 17 measures use a microanalytic assessment referring to different units like utterances, sentences or speaking turns. The advantage of global assessment is that they allow for individual weighting of information (Bøgwald et al., 1999) and that they are time-saving, thereby allowing the analysis of larger sets of data. On the other hand, global methods deliver rather crude assessments of complex processes and are, moreover, prone to cognitive biases, e.g., anchoring effects and availability heuristics (Tversky and Kahneman, 1974; see also Heaton et al., 2010). Microanalytic assessments, on the other hand, are more labor-intensive which limits the amount of data to be analyzed. However, microanalytic methods allow for the analysis of the effects of specific intervention techniques on a micro-level which is to date hardly known and would help to clarify the processes involved in psychotherapeutic change. In summary, both assessment modes have advantages as well as disadvantages that need to be considered before conducting a study. A general desideratum in this respect will be to define what “a verbal intervention” actually is, i.e., which unit (single sentence vs. speaking turn vs. therapy session) needs to be assessed.

Another issue refers to the rating perspective. Except for three measures (TIQI, CPPS, MULTI, see Table 4), all included measures have to be used by an external observer. This perspective may be sufficient to answer many research questions regarding verbal techniques. Some topics, however, cannot be covered by analyzing external ratings, e.g., differences in the perception of verbal techniques by therapist vs. patient. Accordingly, it would be helpful to conceive measures which allow the assessment of different perspectives on the therapy process to increase the range of analytic possibilities.

Issues Regarding Psychometric Aspects

A final set of problems relates to the psychometric properties of the measures reviewed. Objectivity is the least problematic aspect in this respect: For most measures (n = 22) the authors reported the availability of explicit coding manuals. Some of these manuals were published (n = 14) which makes it possible to reconstruct the rating process in detail (A-criterion). In unpublished manuals (n = 7), it is more difficult to draw firm conclusions about the objectivity of the rating process. However, it can be assumed that the ratings within published studies are based on manuscripts describing the use of the measures so that objectivity is given on a minimal level (B-criterion). For the remaining measures (n = 13), no manuals were reported so that the objectivity of the rating process is not guaranteed.

The reliability of the measures reviewed is much more difficult to evaluate, since different authors used a wide range of different methods of reliability assessment. For n = 16 measures ICC values were reported and classified as sufficiently reliable (A-criterion). The remaining measures (n = 18) show a satisfying reliability reporting kappa, Fleiss' r or product-moment correlations. It was observed that reliability values in some cases should be treated with caution, because the rating procedures were described incompletely, the exact method of reliability assessment was not specified (e.g., “kappa” instead of “Cohen's kappa” or “Fleiss” “kappa”), or the occurrence of interrater discussions which may lead to a falsifying increase of reliability over time. The evaluation of the validity of results proved to be the most problematic aspect in the assessment of verbal techniques. Only three measures (CPPS, see Table 4; ITS, see Table 5; YACS, see Table 3) revealed good values for different fields of validity while for all other measures there is only minimal or hardly any information related to validity. The question remains, why the important validation criterion is not considered sufficiently within the scale development process.

All in all, only two measures fulfilled the A-criterion in matters of objectivity, reliability and validity, i.e., the CPPS (see Table 4, which allows to discriminate between two or more therapeutic orientations) and the YACS (see Table 3, which aims to assess the adherence and competence of the therapist in treatments for substance use disorders). From a methodological perspective, these are the measures which can be recommended for clinical use regarding the respective research questions without reservation. All other measures show more or less severe psychometric deficits regarding psychometric aspects. From a practical perspective, some of these measures might nevertheless be helpful to address specific research questions, particularly to analyze the relationship between therapeutic techniques and therapy outcome or to investigate microprocesses in psychotherapy. However, in general it needs to be stated that further research has to operate with more refined methods ensuring the objective, reliable and valid rating of verbal therapeutic techniques.

Limitations

It could be possible that the used keywords did not identify all instruments that assess verbal techniques. Another limitation is that only papers in English and German language were considered which excludes measures in other languages. Investigating the effect of techniques on therapeutic outcome was outside the scope of this review, and would be a valuable contribution for future research.

Are the Proposed Standards Reasonable or a Pie in the Sky?

Our systematic review revealed a number of problems regarding the measures available for the assessment of verbal techniques. Due to these problems of assessing verbal techniques, it is almost impossible to compare the scales and categories of different measures as well as results derived from studies, even if they examine the same outcome variable. Accordingly, no definite conclusions regarding their efficacy can be drawn.

Where does this leave us? In this review, we have certainly used very high standards for measures of psychotherapy research. It must be discussed if these standards are really reasonable or if they are “pie in the sky,” since they are difficult to accomplish and may be too rigorous. Certainly, these issues might be painfully obvious to many investigators. On the other hand, it is important and useful to reiterate these criteria to enhance progress in research and to reach standards common in psychotherapy outcome research (e.g., Strupp et al., 1997). Some of the authors of the studies we rated as deficient, undoubtedly might have provided interesting and reasonable justifications as to why they did not attain what we refer to as the “gold” standard. Moreover, we have also listed some problems which cannot be avoided or need to be tolerated. Nevertheless, a solid theoretical as well as a methodological framework for measuring verbal techniques using clear terminology and accurate conception is of paramount importance to advance this field of research.

Language is the cornerstone of most psychotherapy sessions. Knowing which specific verbal techniques “good” therapists use in their practical work has a high practical relevance for psychotherapist training and clinical routine. Many questions remain in the domain of studying verbal techniques. We need more sophisticated theoretical knowledge regarding the question which techniques really matter and which should be classified as “clinical lore” (Barber, 2015, p. 325). Moreover it is important to know what the specific features of a particular technique actually are. And it will be important to achieve a consensus concerning the definition and operationalization of verbal techniques. Regarding these topics more discussions between clinicians and researchers are necessary. An optimal solution would be to define techniques without reference to therapeutic methods and to find clear unambiguous designations for specific techniques so that the same thing is not called by different names. Authors of measures need to clarify the specific features of verbal techniques that are to be assessed. Although the four basic features which we suggested (Form, Temporal focus, Thematic content, Latent characteristics) are not always easily distinguished, it may be worth the effort, as this would finally help to disentangle the complex mechanisms involved in the efficacy of verbal techniques.

What is the importance of “specific” techniques which are considered to belong to a particular psychotherapy method compared to other techniques which are used without reference to a particular method? In order to answer this question the creation and use of multitheoretical scales is of great theoretical and practical utility (Barber, 2015, p. 232). The question can be broadened: To what extent is the outcome due to unintended or even non-theoretically relevant techniques (Barber, 2015). Answering this latter question it would be useful to describe what therapists do without any reference to a specific theory. For this purpose, bottom-up, qualitative approaches could be helpful to develop scales in order to be able to gather theoretically unbiased, non-predetermined and comprehensive information.

Another interesting research question would be how the latent features of verbal techniques complement and interact with the directly observable features in explaining good outcome, or how the use of different therapeutic techniques changes during the course of therapy.

A significant proportion of the outcome variance is explained by a therapist effect (Wampold, 2001). Our understanding of the variables responsible for these effects of the therapist appears to be lacking (Castonguay, 2011). It may be particularly interesting to study the possible interaction between personal characteristics and therapeutic techniques. Apart from focusing on who the therapist is, what he/she should do in therapy to facilitate change is likely to be of great interest to clinicians (Castonguay, 2011).

A final important question would be how the techniques interact with alliance or other common factors to bring about patient improvement. Castonguay (2011) named the investigation of the interaction between participants, relationship, and technique variables for different clinical disorders as one of the two most important directions of future research.

Answering these clinically relevant questions can be facilitated by establishing a theoretically clear system and accurate conceptions as well as a methodically solid basis.

Conclusions

To date, the results regarding verbal interventions gathered with available instruments can hardly be compared. This has mainly three reasons: 1. Different objectives for the use of the instruments (e.g., measuring competence and adherence vs. differentiation between therapy methods vs. analyzing specific theoretical constructs), 2. Insufficient systematization (e.g., analysis of different or different numbers of interventions, partly on different hierarchical levels), and 3. Inconsistencies in the definition of categories (definitions with the same label often do not match, or vice versa, identical aspects have differing designations).

In conclusion, the overview over the status quo of assessment methods allows for the formulation of important desiderata of future research. Firstly, it will be important to achieve a consensus concerning the definition and operationalization of verbal techniques.

Secondly, we suggest that authors of measures need to clarify the specific features of verbal techniques that are to be assessed. In our review, we applied a typology comprising four features, i.e., the Form, the Temporal focus, the Thematic content, and Latent characteristics. Although these four features are not always easily distinguished, it may be worth the effort, as this would result in a more focused approach toward verbal techniques which would finally help to disentangle the complex mechanisms involved in the efficacy of verbal techniques. It would reduce the chaos and complexities of definitions and make it easier to find an orientation among concepts and empirical findings.

Thirdly, it will be important to put more emphasis on the psychometric properties of measures. Our review revealed that only two of the available measures fulfill the highest standard of the three central validation criteria. In general, authors of future measures need to provide explicit coding manuals ensuring the objectivity of the rating process and demonstrate the reliability and validity of the assessment. Finally, our review revealed that to date there is no measure which was primarily developed in the course of a qualitative bottom-up approach, i.e., starting from empirical data of therapeutic utterances in therapy sessions which are then explicitly described and classified. For future research, we recommend that this approach should be granted more attention, as it allows the gathering of comprehensive information (Schreier, 2012).

Given that future research meets these basic desiderata, it should be possible to establish a firmer ground for the assessment of verbal therapeutic techniques. This would, as a consequence, advance the state of knowledge about the question which techniques can help whom with specific sets of symptoms in certain situations, and finally help to specify empirically based guidelines for psychotherapeutic practice.

Author Contributions

AG conceived and coordinated the study. BT and HW participated in the design of the study. All authors (AG, BT, BS, CM, HW) substantially contributed to the acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data for the work. AG, HW, and CM drafted the manuscript. All authors revised it critically for important intellectual content. All authors gave their final approval of the version to be published. All authors gave their agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01705

References

Ablon, J., and Jones, E. E. (1999). Psychotherapy process in the National Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 67, 64–75. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.67.1.64

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adler, L. M., and Enelow, A. J. (1966). An instrument to measure skill in diagnostic interviewing: a teaching and evaluation tool. J. Med. Educ. 41, 281–288. doi: 10.1097/00001888-196603000-00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amrhein, P. C., Miller, W. R., Yahne, C. E., Palmer, M., and Fulcher, L. (2003). Client commitment language during motivational interviewing predicts drug use outcomes. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 71, 862–878. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.71.5.862

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banon, E., Evan-Grenier, M., and Bond, M. (2001). Early transference interventions with male patients in psychotherapy. J. Psychother. Pract. Res. 10, 79–92.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Barber, J. P. (2015). “Core conflicts in psychotherapy research,” in Visions in Psychotherapy Research and Practice: Reflections from the Presidents of the Society for Psychotherapy Research, eds B. M. Strauss, J. B. Barber, and L. G. Castonguay (New York, NY: Routledge), 316–328.

Google Scholar

Barber, J. P., and Crits-Christoph, P. (1996). Development of a therapist adherence/competence rating scale for supportive-expressive dynamic psychotherapy: a preliminary report. Psychothe. Res. 6, 81–94. doi: 10.1080/10503309612331331608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, J. P., Foltz, C., Crits-Christoph, P., and Chittams, J. (2004). Therapists' adherence and competence and treatment discrimination in the NIDA Collaborative Cocaine Treatment Study. J. Clin. Psychol. 60, 29–41. doi: 10.1002/jclp.10186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, J. P., Krakauer, I., Calvo, N., Badgio, P. C., and Faude, J. (1997). Measuring adherence and competence of dynamic therapists in the treatment of cocaine dependence. J. Psychother. Pract. Res. 6, 12–24.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Barber, J. P., Liese, B. S., and Abrams, M. J. (2003). Development of the cognitive therapy adherence and competence scale. Psychother. Res. 13, 205–221. doi: 10.1093/ptr/kpg019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, J. P., Muran, J. C., McCarthy, K. S., and Keefe, R. J. (2013). “Research on psychodynamic therapies,” in Bergin and Garfield's Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change, 6th Edn., ed M. J. Lambert (John Wiley & Sons, Inc), 443–494.

Barber, J. P., Mercer, D., Krakauer, I., and Calvo, N. (1996). Development of an adherence/competence rating scale for individual drug counseling. Drug Alcohol Depend. 43, 125–132. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(96)01305-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkham, M., and Shapiro, D. (1986). Counselor verbal response modes and experienced empathy. J. Couns. Psychol. 33, 3–10. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.33.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlow, D. H. (2010). Negative effects from psychological treatments: a perspective. Am. Psychol. 65, 13–20. doi: 10.1037/a0015643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1962). Dimensions of therapist response as causal factors in therapeutic change. Psychol. Monogr. 76, 1–36. doi: 10.1037/h0093918

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartko, J. J., and Carpenter, W. T. (1975). On the methods and theory of reliability. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 163, 307–317. doi: 10.1097/00005053-197611000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., and Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression: A Treatment Manual. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Bergin, A. E., and Strupp, H. H. (2009). Changing Frontiers in the Science of Psychotherapy. Chicago, IL: Aldine-Atherton.

Google Scholar

Beutler, L. E. (2009). Making science matter in clinical practice: redefining psychotherapy. Clin. Psychol. 16, 301–317. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2009.01168.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bischoff, M. M., and Tracey, T. J. G. (1995). Client resistance as predicted by therapist behavior: a study of sequential dependence. J. Consult. Psychol. 42, 487–495. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.42.4.487

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blackburn, I.-M., James, I. A., Milne, D. L., Baker, C., Standart, S., Garland, A., et al. (2001). The Revised Cognitive Therapy Scale (CTS-R): psychometric properties. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 29, 431–446. doi: 10.1017/s1352465801004040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blagys, M. D., and Hilsenroth, M. J. (2000). Distinctive features of short-term psychodynamic-interpersonal psychotherapy: a review of the comparative psychotherapy process literature. Clin. Psychol. 7, 167–188. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.7.2.167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blagys, M. D., and Hilsenroth, M. J. (2002). Distinctive activities of cognitive-behavioral therapy - A review of the comparative psychotherapy process literature. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 22, 671–706. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00117-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bøgwald, K.-P. (2001). Do patients and their therapists agree on the content of treatment? J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 189, 830–837. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200112000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bøgwald, K.-P., Høglend, P., and Sørbye, O. (1999). Measurement of transference interpretations. J. Psychother. Pract. Res. 8, 264–273.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bordin, E. S. (1966). “Free association: an experimental analogue of the psychoanalytic situation,” in Methods of Research in Psychotherapy, eds L. A. Gottschalk and A. H. Auerbach (New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts), 189–208.

Brumberg, J., and Gumz, A. (2012). Transference interpretations and how they work: a systematic review. Z. Psychosom. Med. Psychother. 58, 219–235. doi: 10.13109/zptm.2012.58.3.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Brunink, S. A., and Schroeder, H. E. (1979). Verbal therapeutic behavior of expert psychoanalytically oriented, gestalt, and behavior therapists. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 47, 567–574. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.47.3.567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bucci, W., and Maski, B. (2007). Beneath the surface of the therapeutic interaction: the psychoanalytic method in modern dress. J. Am. Psychoanal. Assoc. 55, 1355–1397. doi: 10.1177/000306510705500412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, S. F., Henry, W. P., and Strupp, H. H. (1995). Measuring adherence in time-limited dynamic psychotherapy. Psychotherapy 32, 629–638. doi: 10.1037/0033-3204.32.4.629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carkhuff, R. R. (1969). Helping and Human Relations. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Google Scholar

Carroll, K. M., Nich, C., Sifry, R. L., Nuro, K. F., Frankforter, T. L., Ball, S. A., et al. (2000). A general system for evaluating therapist adherence and competence in psychotherapy research in the addictions. Drug Alcohol Depend. 57, 225–238. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(99)00049-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castonguay, L. G. (2011). Psychotherapy, psychopathology, research and practice: pathways of connections and integration. Psychother. Res. 21, 125–140. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2011.563250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castonguay, L. G. (2013). Psychotherapy outcome: an issue worth Re-Revisiting 50 Years Later. Psychotherapy 50, 52–67. doi: 10.1037/a0030898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castonguay, L. G., and Beutler, L. E. (2006). Principles of therapeutic change: a task force on participants, relationships, and techniques factors. J. Clin. Psychol. 62, 631–638. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 20, 37–46. doi: 10.1177/001316446002000104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1968). Weighted kappa - nominal scale agreement with provision for scaled disagreement or partial credit. Psychol. Bull. 70, 213–220. doi: 10.1037/h0026256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connolly, M. B., Crits-Christoph, P., Shappell, S., Barber, J., Luborsky, L., and Shaffer, C. (1999). Relation of transference interpretations to outcome in the early sessions of brief supportive- expressive psychotherapy. Psychother. Res. 9, 485–495.

Google Scholar

Connolly, M. B., Crits-Christoph, P., Shappell, S., Barber, J. P., and Luborsky, L. (1998). Therapist interventions in early sessions of brief supportive-expressive psychotherapy for depression. J. Psychother. Pract. Res. 7, 290–300.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Connolly Gibbons, M. B., Crits-Christopf, P., Levinson, J., Gladis, M., Siqueland, L., Barber, J. P., et al. (2002). Therapist interventions in the interpersonal and cognitive therapy sessions of the Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program. Am. J. Psychother. 56, 3–26.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Connolly Gibbons, M. B., Crits-Christoph, P., Barber, J., Stirman, S. W., Gallop, J., and Goldstein, L. A. (2009). Unique and common mechanisms of change across cognitive and dynamic psychotherapies. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 77, 801–813. doi: 10.1037/a0016596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, M., and Bond, S. (1992). The Psychodynamic Intervention Rating Scale. San Francisco, CA: University of California.

DeRubeis, R. J., Hollon, S. D., Evans, M. D., and Bemis, K. M. (1982). Can psychotherapies for depression be discriminated? A systematic investigation of cognitive therapy and interpersonal therapy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 50, 744–756. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.50.5.744

CrossRef Full Text

Elliott, R. (1979). How clients perceive helper behaviors. J. Couns. Psychol. 26, 285–294. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.26.4.285

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliott, R., Barker, C., Caskey, N., and Pistrang, N. (1982). Differential helpfulness of counselor verbal response modes. J. Consult. Psychol. 29, 354–361. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.29.4.354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliott, R., Stiles, W. B., Mahrer, A. R., Hill, C. E., Friedlander, M. L., and Margison, F. R. (1987). Primary therapist response-modes - comparison of 6 rating systems. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 55, 218–223. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.55.2.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, M. D., Piasecki, J. M., Kriss, M. R., and Hollon, S. D. (1984). Rater's Manual for the Collaborative Study Psychotherapy Rating Scale - Form 6. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota and the St. Paul-Ramsey Medical Center.

Google Scholar

Fiedler, F. E. (1950). The concept of the ideal therapeutic relationship. J. Consult. Psychol. 14, 239–245. doi: 10.1037/h0058122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiedler, F. E. (1951). Factor analyses of psychoanalytic, nondirective, and Adlerian therapeutic relationships. J. Consult. Psychol. 15, 32–38. doi: 10.1037/h0054601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finn, R. H. (1970). A note on estimating reliablity of categorical data. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 30, 71–76. doi: 10.1177/001316447003000106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finn, R. H. (1972). Effects of some variations in rating scale characteristics on the means and reliabilities of ratings. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 32, 255–265. doi: 10.1177/001316447203200203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleiss, J. L. (1971). Measuring nominal scale agreement among many raters. Psychol. Bull. 76, 378–382. doi: 10.1037/h0031619

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P. (ed.). (2000). “Section B: reflections on psychoanalytic research problems – an anglo-saxon view,” in An Open Door Review of Outcome Studies in Psychoanalysis, 2nd Edn (London: International Psychoanalytic Association), 20–55.

Forgatch, M. S., and Chamberlain, P. (1982). The Therapist Behavior Code. Unpublished instrument and technical report, Eugene, OR: Oregon Social Learning Center.

Friedlander, M. L. (1982). Counseling discourse as a speech event: revision and extension of the Hill Counselor Verbal Response Category System. J. Couns. Psychol. 29, 425–429. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.29.4.425

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaston, L. (1988). Manual for the Inventory of Therapeutic Strategies - ITS. Unpublished manuscript. Montreal, QC: McGill University, Departement of Psychiatry.

Gaston, L., and Ring, J. M. (1992). Preliminary results on the inventory of therapeutic strategies. J. Psychother. Pract. Res. 1, 135–146.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gedo, P. M., and Schaffer, N. D. (1989). An empirical approach to studying psychoanalytic process. Psychoanal. Psychol. 6, 277–291. doi: 10.1037/h0085124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geißler, P. (2005). Nonverbale Interaktion in der Psychotherapie Forschung und Praxis im Prozess. Gießen: Psychosozial-Verlag.

Google Scholar

Gibbons, M. B., Crits-Christoph, P., Barber, J. P., Wiltsey Stirman, S., Gallop, R., Goldstein Lizabeth, A., et al. (2009). Unique and common mechanisms of change across cognitive and dynamic psychotherapies. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 77, 801–813. doi: 10.1037/a0016596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldfried, M. R., Hayes, A. M., Castonguay, L. G., Drozd, J. E., and Shapiro, D. A. (1997). A comparative analysis of the therapeutic focus in cognitive-behavioral and psychodynamic-interpersonal sessions. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 65, 740–748. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.65.5.740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldfried, M. R., Newman, C. F., and Hayes, A. M. (1989). The Coding System of Therapeutic Focus. Unpublished manuscript. New York, NY: State University of New York at Stony Brook.

Goldsamt, L. A., Goldfried, M. R., Hayes, A. M., and Kerr, S. (1992). Beck, Meichenbaum, and Strupp: a comparison of three therapies on the dimension of therapist feedback. Psychotherapy 29, 167–176. doi: 10.1037/0033-3204.29.2.167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomes-Schwartz, B., and Schwartz, J. M. (1978). Psychotherapy process variables distinguishing the “inherently helpful” person from the professional psychotherapist. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 46, 196–197. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.46.1.196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenhalgh, T., and Peacock, R. (2005). Effectiveness and efficiency of search methods in systematic reviews of complex evidence: audit of primary sources. Br. Med. J. 331, 1064–1065. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38636.593461.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gumz, A., Horstkotte, J. K., and Kästner, D. (2014). The techniques of the psychodynamic therapist. Types of verbal interventions from a theoretical and practice-oriented perspective. Z. Psychosom. Med. Psychother. 60, 219–237. doi: 10.13109/zptm.2014.60.3.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gumz, A. M., Geyer, M., and Brähler, E. (2013). Psychodynamic therapy from the perspective of self-organization. a concept of change and a methodological approach for empirical examination. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 21, 299–310. doi: 10.1002/cpp.1840

Hahlweg, K., Revenstorf, D., and Schindler, L. (1984). Effects of behavioral marital therapy on couples' communication and problem-solving skills. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 52, 553–566. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.52.4.553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hannöver, W., Blaut, C., Kniehase, C., Martin, T., and Hannich, H.-J. (2013). Interobserver agreement of the German translation of the Motivational Interviewing Sequential Code for Observing Process Exchanges (MI-SCOPE;D). Psychol. Add. Behav. 27, 1196–1200. doi: 10.1037/a0033041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Heaton, K., Hill, C. E., and Edwards, L. (2010). Comparing molecular and molar methods of judging therapist techniques. Psychother. Res. 5, 141–153. doi: 10.1080/10503309512331331266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heaton, K. J., Hill, C. E., and Edwards, L. A. (1995). Comparing molecular and molar methods of judging therapist techniques. Psychother. Res. 5, 141–153. doi: 10.1080/10503309512331331266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hepner, K. A., Azocar, F., Greenwood, G. L., Miranda, J., and Burnam, M. A. (2010). Development of a clinician report measure to assess psychotherapy for depression in usual care settings. Adm. Policy Ment. Health 37, 221–229. doi: 10.1007/s10488-009-0249-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hersoug, A. G., Bøgwald, K.-P., and Høglend, P. (2005). Changes of defensive functioning. Does interpretation contribute to change? Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 288–296. doi: 10.1002/cpp.444

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, S. A., Knox, S., and Hill, C. E. (2006). Teaching graduate trainees how to manage client anger: a comparison of three types of training. Psychother. Res. 16, 282–292. doi: 10.1080/10503300500264838

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, C. E. (1978). Development of a counselor verbal response category system. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 25, 461–468.

Google Scholar

Hill, C. E., Greenwald, C., Reed, K. G., Charles, D., O'Farrell, M. K., and Carter, J. A. (1981). Manual for the Counselor and Client Verbal Response Category System. Columbus, OH: Marathon.

Hill, C. E., and O'Brien, K. M. (1999). Helping Skills: Facilitating Exploration, Insight, and Action. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Hill, C. E., O'Gradey, K. E., and Elkin, I. (1992). Applying the Collaborative study psychotherapy rating scale to rate therapist adherence in cognitive-behavior therapy, interpersonal therapy, and clinical management. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 60, 73–79. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.60.1.73

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilsenroth, M. J., Bonge, D. R., Blagys, M. D., Ackerman, S. J., and Blais, M. A. (2005). Measuring psychodynamic-interpersonal and cognitive-behavioral techniques: development of the comparative psychotherapy process scale. Psychother. Theory Res. Pract. Train. 42, 340–356. doi: 10.1037/0033-3204.42.3.340

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Høglend, P. (2004). Analysis of transference in psychodynamic psychotherapy: a review of empirical research. Can. J. Psychoanal. 12, 280–300.

Google Scholar

Hollon, S. D., Waskow, I. E., Evans, M., and Lowery, H. A. (1984). System for Rating Therapies for Depression. Los Angeles, CA: Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychiatric Association.

Holzman, M. S., and Forman, V. P. (1966). A multidimensional content-analysis system applied to the analysis of therapeutic technique in psychotherapy with schizophrenic patients. Psychol. Bull. 66, 263–281. doi: 10.1037/h0023825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoyt, M. F., Marmar, C. R., Horowitz, M. J., and Alvarez, W. F. (1981). The therapist action scale and the patient action scale: instruments for the assessment of activities during dynamic psychotherapy. Psychther. Theory Res. Pract. 18, 109–116. doi: 10.1037/h0085950

CrossRef Full Text

Jones, E. E. (1985). Manual for the Psychotherapy Process Q-set. Berkeley, CA: University of California.

Jones, E. E., Krupnick, J. L., and Kerig, P. K. (1987). Some gender effects in a brief psychotherapy. Psychotherapy 24, 336–352. doi: 10.1037/h0085724

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, E. E., and Windholz, M. (1990). The psychoanalytic case study: toward a method for systematic inquiry. J. Am. Psychoanal. Assoc. 38, 985–1015. doi: 10.1177/000306519003800405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karl, N. J., and Abeles, N. (1969). Psychotherapy process as a function of the time segment sampled. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 33, 207–212. doi: 10.1037/h0027173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr, S., Goldfried, M. R., Hayes, A. M., Castonguay, L. G., and Goldsamt, L. A. (1992). Interpersonal and intrapersonal focus in cognitive-behavioral and psychodynamic-interpersonal therapies: a preliminary analysis of the Sheffield Project. Psychother. Res. 2, 266–276. doi: 10.1080/10503309212331333024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenigsberg, H. W., Kernberg, O. F., Haas, G., Lotterman, A., Rockland, L., and Selzer, M. (1985). Development of a scale for measuring techniques in the psychotherapy of borderline patients. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 173, 424–431. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198507000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenigsberg, H. W., Kernberg, O. F., Rockland, L., Appelbaum, A., Carr, A., and Kernberg, P. (1988). “Developing an instrument for characterizing psychotherapy techniques in studies of the psychotherapy of borderline patients,” in Psychoanalytic Process Research Strategies, eds H. Dahl, H. Kächele and H. Thomä (New York: Springer-Verlag), 147–62.

Lambert, M. J. (2013). “The efficacy and effectiveness of psychotherapy,” in Bergin and Garfield's Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change, Vol. 6, ed M. J. Lambert (New Jersey: NJ; Wiley), 169–218.

Liese, B. S., Shepherd, D. D., Cameron, C. L., and Ojeleye, A. E. (1995). Teaching psychological knowledge and skills to family physicians. J. Clin. Psychol. Med. Settings 2, 21–38. doi: 10.1007/BF01988625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Light, R. J. (1971). Measures of response agreement for qualitative data: some generalizations and alternatives. Psychol. Bull. 76, 365–377. doi: 10.1037/h0031643

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luborsky, L. (1984). Principles of Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy: A Manual for Supportive-Expressive (SE) Treatment. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Luborsky, L., Bachrach, H., Graff, H., Pulver, S., and Christoph, P. (1979). Preconditions and consequences of transference interpretations. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 167, 391–401. doi: 10.1097/00005053-197907000-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madonik, B. G. (2001). I Hear What You Say, but What Are You Telling Me? The Strategic Use of Nonverbal Communication in Mediation. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Malan, D. H. (1963). A Study of Brief Psychotherapy. London: Tavistock.

Google Scholar

Malan, D. H. (1976). Toward the validation of dynamic psychotherapy. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Margison, F., Barkham, M., Evans, C., McGrath, G., Mellor-Clark, J., Audin, K., et al. (2000). Measurement and psychotherapy: evidence based practice and practice based evidence. Br. J. Psychiatry 177, 123–130. doi: 10.1192/bjp.177.2.123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mark, D., and Faude, J. (1995). “Supportive-expressive therapy for cocaine abuse,” in Dynamic Therapies for Psychiatric Disorders (Axis 1), eds J. P. Barber and P. Crits-Christoph (New York, NY: Basic Books), 294–331.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Marziali, E. A., and Sullivan, J. M. (1980). Methodological issues in the content analysis of brief psychotherapy. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 53, 19–27. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1980.tb02864.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, K. S., and Barber, J. P. (2009). The Multitheoretical List of Therapeutic Interventions (MULTI): initial report. Psychother. Res. 19, 96–113. doi: 10.1080/10503300802524343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCullough, L. (1988). Psychotherapy interaction coding system manual: the PIC system. Soc. Behav. Sci. Doc. 18.

Mercer, D., and Woddy, G. (1992). Addiction Counseling. University of Pennsylvania: VAMC Center for Studies of Addiction.

Mergenthaler, E. (2015). “Resonating minds: a school-independent theoretical conception and its empirical application to psychotherapeutic processes, in Visions in Psychotherapy Research and Practice: Reflections from Presidents of the Society for Psychotherapy Research, eds B. Strauss, J. Barber and L. Castonguay (New York, NY: Taylor & Francis), 292–314.

Milbrath, C., Bond, M., Cooper, S., Znoj, H. J., Horowitz, M. J., and Perry, J. C. (1999). Sequential consequences of therapists' interventions. J. Psychother. Pract. Res. 8, 40–54.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Miller, W. R., Moyers, T. B., Ernst, D., and Amrhein, P. (2008). Manual for the Motivational Interviewing Skill Code (MISC). The University of New Mexico; Center on Alcoholism, Substance Abuse, and Addictions.

Google Scholar

Moyers, T. B., and Martin, T. (2006). Therapist influence on client language during motivational interviewing sessions. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 30, 245–251. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moyers, T., Martin, T., Catley, D., Harris, K. J., and Ahluwalia, J. S. (2003). Assessing the integrity of motivational interviewing interventions: reliability of the Motivational Interviewing Skills Code. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 31, 177–184. doi: 10.1017/S1352465803002054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norcross, J. C. (2011). (Ed.). Psychotherapy Relationships that Work: Evidence-based Responsiveness, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Ogrodniczuk, J. S., and Piper, W. E. (1999). Measuring therapist technique in psychodynamic psychotherapies development and use of a new scale. J. Psychother. Pract. Res. 8, 142–154.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

O'Malley, S. S., Suh, C. S., and Strupp, H. H. (1983). The Vanderbilt psychotherapy process scale: a report on the scale development and a process-outcome study. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 51, 581–586. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.51.4.581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orlinsky, D. E., and Howard, K. I. (1967). The good therapy hour: experiential correlates of patients' and therapists' evaluations of therapy sessions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 16, 621–632. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1967.01730230105013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orlinsky, D. E., and Howard, K. I. (1975). Varieties of Psychotherapeutic Experience: Multivariate Analyses of Patients' and Therapists' Reports. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.

Orlinsky, D. E., and Howard, K. I. (1987). A generic model of psychotherapy. J. Integr. Eclectic Psychother. 6, 6–27.

Google Scholar

Paniagua, C. (2003). Problems with the concept ‘interpretation’. Int. J. Psychoanal. 84, 1105–1123. doi: 10.1516/17U4-KBU0-NFNB-2E8K

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pawelczyk, J. (2011). Talk as Therapy: Psychotherapy in a Linguistic Perspective. Berlin; Boston: Mouton de Gruyter.

Google Scholar

Pfammatter, M., and Tschacher, W. (2012). Wirkfaktoren der Psychotherapie – eine Übersicht und Standortbestimmung. Z. Psychiatr. Psychol. Psychother. 60, 67–76. doi: 10.1024/1661-4747/a000099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piper, W. E., Debbane, E. G., de Carufel, F. L., and Bienvenu, J. P. (1987). A system for differentiating therapist interpretations from other interventions. Bull. Menninger Clin. 51, 532–550.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Piper, W. E., Joyce, A. S., McCallum, M., and Azim, H. F. (1998). Interpretive and supportive forms of psychotherapy and patient personality variables. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 66, 558–567. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.66.3.558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samoilov, A., Goldfried, M. R., and Shapiro, D. (2000). Coding system of therapeutic focus on action and insight. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68, 513–514. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.3.513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schindler, L., Hohenberger-Sieber, E., and Hahlweg, K. (1989). Observing client-therapist interaction in behaviour therapy: development and first application of an observational system. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 28, 213–226.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schreier, M. (2012). Qualitative Content Analysis in Practice. Los Angeles, CA; London: Sage.

Shapiro, D. A., Barkham, D. A., and Irving, D. L. (1980). A Modified Helper Behavior Rating Manual (Social and Applied Psychology Unit Memo No. 415). Sheffield: Departement of Psychology; University of Sheffield.

Shapiro, D. A., Barkham, M., and Irving, D. L. (1984). The reliability of a modified Helper Behavior Rating System. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 57, 45–48. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1984.tb01579.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, D. A., and Startup, M. J. (1990). Raters' Manual for the Sheffield Psychotherapy Rating Scale (Memo No. 1154). Sheffield: University of Sheffield; MRC/ESRC Social and Applied Psychology Unit.

Shrout, P., and Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlation: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol. Bull. 86, 420–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silberschatz, G., Fretter, P. B., and Curtis, J. T. (1986). How do interpretations influence the process of psychotherapy? J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 54, 646–652. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.54.5.646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, W. U. (1945). An investigation of the nature of non-directive psychotherapy. J. Gen. Psychol. 33, 193–223. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1945.10544506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Startup, M., and Shapiro, D. A. (1993). Dimensions of cognitive therapy for depression: a confirmatory analysis of session ratings. Cognit. Ther. Res. 17, 139–151. doi: 10.1007/BF01172962

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stiles, W. B. (1978). Manual for a Taxonomy of Verbal Response Modes. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Institute for Research in Social Science.

Stiles, W. B. (1992). Describing Talk: A Taxonomy of Verbal Response Modes. Newbury Park: Sage.

Google Scholar

Stiles, W. B., Hardy, G. E., Barkharn, M., Rees, A., Shapiro, D. A., Startup, M., et al. (1996). Therapist session intentions in cognitive-behavioral and psychodynamic-interpersonal psychotherapy. J. Couns. Psychol. 43, 402–414. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.43.4.402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stiles, W. B., Shapiro, D. A., and Firthcozens, J. A. (1989). Therapist differences in the use of verbal response mode forms and intents. Psychotherapy 26, 314–322. doi: 10.1037/h0085441

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, B., Strupp, H. H., Burgmeier-Lohse, M., Wille, H., and Storm, S. (1992). Deutschsprachige Version der Vanderbilt-Psychotherapie-Skalen: beschreibung und Anwendung in zwei Kurztherapien. Z. Klin. Psychol. Psychopathol. Psychother. 40, 411–430.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Strupp, H. H. (1957). A multidimensional system for analyzing psychotherapeutic techniques. Psychiatry 20, 293–312.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Strupp, H. H. (1966). A Multidimensional System für Analyzing Psychotherapeutic Communications: Manual, 2nd Edn. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina.

Google Scholar

Strupp, H. H., Horowitz, L. M., and Lambert, M. J. (1997). Measuring Patient Changes in Mood, Anxiety, and Personality Disorders. Toward a Core Battery. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Tinsley, H. E. A., and Weiss, D. J. (1975). Interrater reliability and agreement of subjective judgments. Res. Methodol. 22, 358–376. doi: 10.1037/h0076640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Town, J. M., Hardy, G. E., McCullough, L., and Stride, C. (2012a). Patient affect experiencing following therapist interventions in short-term dynamic psychotherapy. Psychother. Res. 22, 208–219. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2011.637243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Town, J. M., McCullough, L., and Hardy, G. E. (2012b). Therapist interventions in short-term dynamic psychotherapy: six expert treatments. Br. J. Guid. Couns. 40, 31–42. doi: 10.1080/03069885.2011.621518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tschacher, W., Junghan, U. M., and Pfammatter, M. (2014). Towards a taxonomy of common factors in psychotherapy - results of an expert survey. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 21, 82–96. doi: 10.1002/cpp.1822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 185, 1124–1131. doi: 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaughan, S. C., Spitzer, R., Davies, M., and Roose, S. (1997). The definition and assessment of analytic process: can analysts agree? Int. J. Psychoanal. 78, 959–973.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Waldron, S., Scharf, R., Crouse, J., Firestein, S., Burton, A., and Hurst, D. (2004a). Saying the right thing at the right time: a view through the lens of the Analytic Process Scales (APS). Psychoanal. Q. 73, 1079–1125. doi: 10.1002/j.2167-4086.2004.tb00193.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldron, S., Scharf, R. D., Hurst, D., Firestein, S. K., and Burton, A. (2004b). What happens in a psychoanalysis? A view through the lens of the analytic process scale (APS). Int. J. Psychoanal. 85, 443–466. doi: 10.1516/5PPV-Q9WL-JKA9-DRCK

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wampold, B. E. (2001). The Great Psychotherapy Debate: Models, Methods, and Findings. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Wampold, B. E., and Imel, Z. E. (2015). The Great Psychotherapy Debate: The Evidence for What Makes Psychotherapy Work. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Winston, A., Pollack, J., McCullough, L., Flegenheimer, W., Kestenbaum, R., and Trujillo, M. (1991). Brief psychotherapy of personality disorders. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 179, 188–193. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199104000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, J. E., and Beck, A. T. (1980). Cognitive Therapy Scale: Rating Manual. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania.

Google Scholar

Keywords: verbal intervention, technique, skills, measure, psychometric property, systematic review

Citation: Gumz A, Treese B, Marx C, Strauss B and Wendt H (2015) Measuring Verbal Psychotherapeutic Techniques—A Systematic Review of Intervention Characteristics and Measures. Front. Psychol. 6:1705. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01705

Received: 21 July 2015; Accepted: 22 October 2015;
Published: 10 November 2015.

Edited by:

Wolfgang Tschacher, University of Bern, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Alemka Tomicic, Universidad Diego Portales, Chile
Agnes Von Wyl, Zurich University of Applied Sciences, Switzerland
Louis Castonguay, Pennsylvania State University, USA

Copyright © 2015 Gumz, Treese, Marx, Strauss and Wendt. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Antje Gumz, a.gumz@psychologische-hochschule.de

Present Address: Hanna Wendt, Schön Klinik Bad Bramstedt, Bad Bramstedt, Germany

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.