Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 January 2016
Sec. Cognition

Preserved Suppression of Salient Irrelevant Stimuli During Visual Search in Age-Associated Memory Impairment

  • 1Gerontology Research Group, Department of Medicine, Faculty of Health Sciences, Universidade da Coruña, A Coruña, Spain
  • 2Research, Development and Innovation Department, Gerontological Complex La Milagrosa, Provincial Association of Pensioners and Retired People (UDP) from A Coruña, A Coruña, Spain
  • 3Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, University of Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain

Previous studies have suggested that older adults with age-associated memory impairment (AAMI) may show a significant decline in attentional resource capacity and inhibitory processes in addition to memory impairment. In the present paper, the potential attentional capture by task-irrelevant stimuli was examined in older adults with AAMI compared to healthy older adults using scalp-recorded event-related brain potentials (ERPs). ERPs were recorded during the execution of a visual search task, in which the participants had to detect the presence of a target stimulus that differed from distractors by orientation. To explore the automatic attentional capture phenomenon, an irrelevant distractor stimulus defined by a different feature (color) was also presented without previous knowledge of the participants. A consistent N2pc, an electrophysiological indicator of attentional deployment, was present for target stimuli but not for task-irrelevant color stimuli, suggesting that these irrelevant distractors did not attract attention in AAMI older adults. Furthermore, the N2pc for targets was significantly delayed in AAMI patients compared to healthy older controls. Together, these findings suggest a specific impairment of the attentional selection process of relevant target stimuli in these individuals and indicate that the mechanism of top-down suppression of entirely task-irrelevant stimuli is preserved, at least when the target and the irrelevant stimuli are perceptually very different.

Introduction

Memory impairment is common in people over the age of 65. When this memory decline has no underlying medical cause, it is known as age-associated memory impairment (AAMI), which has been considered as a non-progressive normal decline due to aging (Crook et al., 1986; Youngjohn and Crook, 1993; Hänninen et al., 1995). Information on the prevalence rates of AAMI is scantly and varies greatly because of different definitions and unstandardized methodology, but in general prevalence has been estimated to range from 3.6 to 38.4% (see Ward et al., 2012, for a recent review).

Previous neurophysiological and neuropsychological studies have concluded that, in addition to memory impairment and in comparison with age-matched controls, AAMI older adults may show a significant decline in attentional resource capacity and executive functions associated with frontal lobe function (Hänninen et al., 1997; Anderer et al., 2003). To our knowledge, electrophysiological studies trying to elucidate the mechanisms that underlie visual selective attention processes in AAMI are inexistent. Nevertheless, this is an interesting point taking into account that the ability to suppress distracting information or resist interference, a critical aspect of selective attention mechanisms, plays an important role in a broad range of cognitive functions including working memory (Gazzaley et al., 2005; Hasher, 2007; Solesio-Jofre et al., 2011, 2012).

In laboratory studies, inhibitory mechanisms of selective attention may be inferred from visual search paradigms that require subjects to search for a predefined target stimulus in arrays containing a variable number of distractor stimuli, and that may reveal deficits of filtering out or suppressing irrelevant distractors during target processing (Luck and Hillyard, 1995; Luck and Ford, 1998).

The widely known age-related deficit in visual search processes has been attributed to a decline in the inhibition of irrelevant stimuli (i.e., the inhibitory deficit hypothesis, Hasher and Zacks, 1988; Colcombe et al., 2003; Madden and Whiting, 2004). In the present study, we specifically explored whether a salient but task-irrelevant color stimulus automatically captures attention in AAMI patients. To this end, we employed a visual search paradigm in which the participants searched for a target defined by its orientation, and an irrelevant color stimulus was presented without prior announcement in separate trials. The task-irrelevant stimulus was a red bar, since red stimuli have shown to be highly salient and to attract attention more efficiently than stimuli of other colors (Pomerleau et al., 2014). Taking into account that the lateralized N2pc event-related brain potentials (ERPs) component has been considered as an index of attentional selection/filtering processes (Luck and Hillyard, 1994a,b; Eimer, 1996; Girelli and Luck, 1997; Luck et al., 1997; Hickey et al., 2009), its appearance was used as a mean to explore whether visuospatial attention is captured by the irrelevant stimulus. In this line, previous studies have reported that the presence of a salient irrelevant stimulus can automatically (bottom-up) attract attention to its spatial location (Theeuwes, 1991, 1992, 1994; Theeuwes et al., 2000; Hickey et al., 2006), significantly slowing performance if the salient stimulus is a distractor and speeding performance if is the target (see Ruz and Lupiáñez, 2002, for a review). In a previous electrophysiological study, we found that healthy older adults were not more susceptible to attentional capture than younger adults, being able to ignore the interference of task-irrelevant color stimuli (Lorenzo-López et al., 2008). With the present study we will extend our previous results using an AAMI older adults group on the same experimental task.

Prolonged N2pc latencies and decreased amplitudes have been previously reported in healthy older adults in visual search tasks (Lorenzo-López et al., 2008, 2011; Amenedo et al., 2012; Li et al., 2013; Störmer et al., 2013), indicating a decline in the correlates of allocation of attentional resources to a lateralized target stimulus. N2pc amplitude has also been shown to be smaller in patients with multiple-domain-amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI; Cespón et al., 2013a, 2015) than in healthy older adults. Importantly, recent promising evidence has suggested that cognitive training can counteract the age-related decline in visual selective attention, increasing the N2pc amplitude after 10 weeks of a speed of processing training conducted twice a week (O’Brien et al., 2013).

In order to study the allocation of attentional resources to a relevant target in AAMI patients, we will also explore the time course and amplitude of the N2pc component to target arrays. This is a relevant point since, to our knowledge, modulations of the specific early visuospatial attention process indexed by this component have not been studied before in these patients.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Event-related brain potentials were recorded from 10 AAMI older adults (four females; mean age 83.10 ± 9.80 years, age range 64–96 years), recruited from the Gerontological Complex La Milagrosa in A Coruña (Spain). All had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity, and reported normal color vision. They underwent a standardized diagnostic assessment comprising medical history and neuropsychological assessment to examine mental capacity and to rule out the presence of dementia, which includes the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975; Blesa et al., 2001) and the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS; Sheikh and Yesavage, 1986). Participants were considered to have AAMI if they met all the following diagnostic criteria (Crook et al., 1986): (1) Subjective memory complaints affecting routine activities. We specifically assessed subjective memory complaints using one sample question “Do you suffer from forgetfulness?”, (2) Objective evidence of memory loss (a score of at least 1 SD below the mean for younger adults on the Logical Memory subtest from Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, 1945), (3) Absence of intellectual dysfunction, (4) Absence of dementia or any other neurological disease that affects memory (e.g., stroke, depression). Specifically, participants showed absence of dementia as determined by a score above 24 on the MMSE (mean score: 28.30 ± 1.95), and absence of depressive symptoms as determined by a mean score of 2.90 ± 2.38 on the GDS (cut point of 5 or more), (5) No medical disorders that could produce cognitive deterioration (e.g., serious cardiac disease, poorly controlled diabetes mellitus, and cancer not in remission for 2 years or longer). These ERPs were compared with those obtained in 22 healthy older adults (11 females, mean age 68.5 ± 6.0 years, age range 60–84 years), not meeting the AAMI criteria, from our previous study (Lorenzo-López et al., 2008). All participants in this control group were healthy well-functioning adults without a history of neurological or psychiatric disorder, had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity, and reported normal color vision.

Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to their inclusion in the study and their rights were protected. The experimental protocol and procedures were approved by the ethics committee of the University of A Coruña and conformed to the principles embodied in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Stimuli and Procedure

The electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings were made in an electrically shielded and sound attenuated room. Participants sat in a comfortable chair at 100 cm viewing distance from a computer display with a black background and a continuously visible white fixation cross. They were instructed to maintain central fixation and to minimize blinking while searching for a target object in arrays containing non-target distractor objects (we used the same experimental paradigm as Lorenzo-López et al., 2008, which was based on Luck and Hillyard, 1994a). The stimuli used in the experiment are shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Schematic diagram of the visual search arrays used in the event-related brain potential (ERP) experiment. The arrow represents the time passage.

Bilateral multi-element visual search arrays were presented composed of eight colored bars subtending 0.3° × 0.9° visual angle. The bars were placed at random locations within a 9.2° × 6.9° imaginary rectangular region that was centered on fixation cross. There were three types of search arrays that were randomly presented: target arrays comprising one blue-vertical bar and seven blue-horizontal bars (p = 0.2); non-target arrays comprising one red-horizontal bar and seven blue-horizontal bars (p = 0.2); and homogeneous arrays comprising eight blue-horizontal bars (p = 0.6). The location of target and non-target stimuli was unpredictable, and they were equally likely to appear in the right or left visual hemifields. After the fixation cross was presented for 900–1100 ms, each search array was presented for 750 ms (see Figure 1). The target was defined by the same feature (orientation) across all trials and the participants were not informed about the appearance of the irrelevant color distractor. Stimuli and search arrays were created, presented, and controlled using the Presentation software application (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., version 0.76). The experimental session was divided into six blocks of trials. Each block consisted of at least 10 orientation target arrays and at least 10 color non-target arrays presented to each hemifield, and at least 80 homogeneous arrays, to a maximum of 250 arrays in total. The task was to indicate as rapidly and accurately as possible whether the target stimulus was present or absent in each search array, by pressing a button with one hand for target-present trials and another button with the other hand for target-absent trials. Response buttons were counterbalanced across participants.

ERP Methodology

Electroencephalography was continuously recorded using BrainAmp amplifiers (Brain Vision recorder software, Brain Products) with a 32-channel electro-cap (ECI Inc; 10–20 International System). All the active electrodes were referred to the nose tip and grounded with an electrode placed at nasion. Vertical and horizontal EOG activities were recorded bipolarly from above and below the left eye and from the outer canthi of both eyes. Electrode impedances were kept below 10 kΩ. The EEG signals were amplified (10 K) and digitized at a rate of 500 Hz/channel, and filtered on-line with a band pass of 0.05–100 Hz.

Data Analyses

Reaction times (RTs) were recorded for the three types of search arrays. Only RTs associated with correct responses were included in the statistical analyses. Accuracy was measured as the percentage of correct responses (hit rates) to orientation targets with responses no longer than 1100 ms. A mixed design analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to analyze mean correct RTs, considering the Search Array (target, non-target, homogeneous) as the within-subject factor and Group (healthy controls, AAMI patients) as the between-subject factor. A one-way ANOVA with Group (healthy controls, AAMI patients) as the between-subjects factor was performed to analyze hit rates.

Electroencephalographic data were analyzed using BrainVision Analyzer 2.0 software (Brain Products). First, a notch filter (>50 Hz) was applied to the data. EEG was averaged off-line for epochs of 500 ms post-stimulus and 100 ms pre-stimulus. Epochs exceeding ±100 μV and those containing blinks, and horizontal or vertical eye movements were excluded from averaging, as well as epochs associated with incorrect or no responses. EEG was averaged separately for target and non-target pop-outs occurring in the right visual field (RVF) and in the left visual field (LVF) and for homogeneous arrays, resulting in five waveforms for each participant. After trial rejection, the mean number of averaged free-artifact epochs for each experimental condition was 89.3 ± 17.9 for RVF target arrays (range 68–117), 89.4 ± 7.1 for LVF target arrays (range 82–102), 61.9 ± 11.8 for RVF non-targets arrays (range 45–84), and 61.2 ± 12.3 for LVF non-targets arrays (range 44–90). As a whole, the average number of epochs analyzed was 75.4 ± 8.5, ranging from 66 to 99.5. Mean amplitude values of N2 posterior component were measured at P3/P4, PO3/PO4, O1/O2, and T5/T6 sites in each patient from 250 to 410 ms (this time window was defined based on visual inspection of ERP grand-averaged waveforms). These data were entered into an initial overall model ANOVA in which Array Type (target, non-target), Electrode Location (parietal, parieto-occipital, occipital, temporal), Hemisphere (left or right hemisphere electrode site), and Laterality (ipsilateral or contralateral relative to the electrode location) were entered as within-subject factors, and Group (healthy controls, AAMI patients) as the between-subjects factor. Separate ANOVAs were also performed for each search array type, with Electrode Location, Hemisphere, and Laterality as the within-subject factors, and Group as the between-subjects factor.

To isolate N2pc, difference waveforms were constructed by subtracting the ERPs for ipsilateral (relative to the electrode location) target arrays from those for contralateral target arrays. N2pc latencies and mean amplitudes were quantified in these contralateral-minus-ipsilateral waveforms from the largest negative peak and mean voltage between 250 and 410 ms after the target array onset in the AAMI participants, and between 205 and 375 ms in the healthy controls (see Lorenzo-López et al., 2008). The resulting N2pc amplitude and latency values were entered into mixed model ANOVAs with Electrode Location (parietal, parieto-occipital, occipital, temporal) as the within-subject factor, and Group (healthy controls, AAMI patients) as the between-subjects factor. Voltage maps were also computed from the difference waveforms at the time point of the maximum N2pc peak indicated by the ANOVAs, to examine potential changes in the N2pc scalp distribution between the groups. Whenever appropriate, degrees of freedom were corrected by the conservative Greenhouse–Geisser estimate. When necessary, post hoc comparisons were performed using the Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons. An alpha level of 0.05 was used for all statistical tests. Effect sizes for ANOVAs are reported as partial eta-squared (ηp2).

Results

Behavioral Results

Mean RTs in healthy older adults and AAMI patients are summarized in Table 1 as a function of array type. AAMI participants showed a lower performance level on the visual search task [F(1,30) = 32.695, p < 0.0001; healthy older mean hit rates: 91.2 ± 12.5%; AAMI: 64.5 ± 11.6%], and slower RTs [F(1,30) = 7.496, p < 0.010, ηp2 = 0.200; healthy older: 600.5 ± 90.3 ms; AAMI: 683.3 ± 44.1 ms]. Importantly, there was a significant Array Type by Group interaction on the RTs [F(2,60) = 5.240, 𝜀 = 0.587, p < 0.023, ηp2 = 0.149]. As can be seen in Table 1, RTs were slowest for the orientation targets, intermediate for the color non-targets, and fastest for the homogeneous arrays [Array Type: F(2,42) = 16.036, 𝜀 = 0.554, p < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.433; with p < 0.05 for all the post hoc pairwise comparisons among array types] in healthy older controls. However, AAMI patients did not show significant RT differences among the three types of arrays [Array Type: F(2,18) = 0.325, 𝜀 = 0.643, p = 0.635, ηp2 = 0.035].

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Mean reaction times (RTs, ms) and their corresponding standard deviations as a function of array type in healthy older adults and AAMI patients.

ERPs Results

In the overall ANOVA where target and irrelevant non-target arrays were included, a significant Electrode Location by Array Type interaction [F(3,90) = 7.049, p < 0.002, 𝜀 = 0.693, ηp2 = 0.190] revealed that the distribution of voltage over the scalp was significantly different for target and non-target arrays. Subsequent ANOVAs were conducted separately for each search array type, because a significant main effect of Laterality [F(1,30) = 43.857, p < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.594], and a significant Laterality by Array Type interaction [F(1,30) = 31.373, p < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.511] were found.

For target arrays, the ANOVAs reflected the presence of the N2pc component in both groups by a significant main effect of Laterality [F(1,30) = 43.457, p < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.529]. Figure 2A shows the grand-average ERP waveforms for target arrays in AAMI patients, revealing a higher negativity in the N2 latency range contralateral to the target location. Visual inspection of the waveforms showed an apparent hemispheric asymmetry in its magnitude, with the amplitude difference between ipsilateral and contralateral waveforms being lower in the right hemisphere. However, the Hemisphere factor and the Hemisphere by Laterality interaction were not significant [Hemisphere: F(1,30) = 2.935, p = 0.097, ηp2 = 0.089; Hemisphere by Laterality: F(1,30) = 1.010, p = 0.323, ηp2 = 0.033], suggesting that the N2pc component was present in all the bilateral posterior scalp electrodes analyzed.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. (A) Grand-average ERPs for targets. Superimposed are responses to targets presented in the contralateral (solid lines) and ipsilateral (dashed lines) visual field to electrode locations. (B) Grand-average ERPs for irrelevant non-targets. Superimposed are responses to non-targets presented in the contralateral (solid lines) or ipsilateral (dashed lines) to electrode locations. The N2pc component is absent.

Difference waveforms are shown in Figure 3A. The N2pc latency was significantly delayed [F(1,30) = 19.979, p < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.400] in AAMI patients (347.7 ± 20.4 ms) compared to healthy older adults (308.3 ± 24.1 ms). However, mean N2pc amplitude did not significantly differ between the groups [F(1,30) = 0.446, p = 0.509, ηp2 = 0.015; healthy controls: –0.9 ± 0.6 μV; AAMI patients: –1.0 ± 0.7 μV]. Topographical voltage maps of the contralateral-minus-ipsilateral difference waveforms are shown in Figure 3B, revealing similar N2pc scalp distribution over lateral visual cortical areas in both groups.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. (A) Grand-average contralateral-minus-ipsilateral difference waveforms for healthy older adults (blue lines) and AAMI patients (red lines). (B) Voltage topographic maps showing the N2pc amplitude distribution in healthy older adults and AAMI patients at the latency of the peak amplitude. The N2pc topographical scalp distribution was maximal at occipital sites in both groups.

For non-target arrays, the ANOVAs revealed the absence of the N2pc component in both groups [Laterality: F(1,30) = 0.655, p = 0.425, ηp2 = 0.021]. Figure 2B illustrates grand-average ERP waveforms for non-target arrays, showing no differences in mean amplitude between the ipsilateral and contralateral waveforms in the N2 latency range. This result suggests that the color irrelevant stimuli did not produce an automatic orienting of attention to their location in AAMI patients.

Discussion

One of the hypotheses accounting for the normal changes in working memory processes in elderly is based on inefficient inhibitory mechanisms and diminished attentional capacity (Levitt et al., 2006). According to this point of view, inhibitory processes would significantly decline with age, so older adults would be less able to ignore or maintain out of working memory salient irrelevant information, becoming more distractible (Solesio-Jofre et al., 2011, 2012), or susceptible to attentional capture (Kramer et al., 2000). Thus, the literature provides evidence of an age-related decline in the ability to maintain an inhibitory set (Kramer et al., 2000; Colcombe et al., 2003; Greenwood and Parasuraman, 2004). In order to explore the distractor suppression mechanisms in AAMI, describing a non-disease aging decline in memory showing attentional problems, ERPs were recorded while AAMI participants searched for a simple target defined by an orientation difference to the surrounding distractors. To specifically explore the potential capture of attention to distractor locations, a task-irrelevant stimulus defined by a different unique feature (color) was also presented without their knowledge. In order to detect the possible attentional capture by these irrelevant color stimuli, analysis were focused on the N2pc ERP component reflecting brain activity specifically related to the allocation of visuospatial attention (Luck and Hillyard, 1994a,b; Woodman and Luck, 1999, 2003), and that has been previously used as a valid tool to investigate the ability of irrelevant salient stimuli to capture attention (Girelli and Luck, 1997; Eimer and Kiss, 2006; Hickey et al., 2006; Lorenzo-López et al., 2008; Rodríguez-Holguín et al., 2009; Burra and Kerzel, 2013).

Behavioral results showed a significant delay in mean RTs for both relevant target selection and successful distractor rejection, and a significant decrement in performance in AAMI patients compared to healthy controls. A reduced task-related attentional selection has been also previously reported in amnestic MCI and AD patients (Redel et al., 2012). The fact that the AAMI patients did not show RT differences between the three search array types suggests that there are differences in the processing of distractors between the groups. In the present paper, no EEG sign of capture by the irrelevant non-targets at the level of visuospatial attention (reflected by the absence of the N2pc component) was observed regardless of group, indicating a preserved top-down suppression of irrelevant information. Thus, the observed slowing in distractor rejection in the AAMI group may be explained by differences in the time course of response-related processes (selection of the appropriate motor response, and execution of the required response) but not by delays in the distractor suppression process reflected by N2pc. In this regard, potential changes in motor-related ERP activity between AAMI patients and healthy older controls should be explored in future studies with large samples. Previous studies have reported age-related deficits in motor-response production in visual search (Amenedo et al., 2012; Wiegand et al., 2013) and Simon (Van der Lubbe and Verleger, 2002; Cespón et al., 2013b) tasks.

From an electrophysiological point of view, a robust N2pc component was observed when the AAMI participants were presented with orientation target stimuli. Visual inspection of the waveforms showed an apparent hemispheric asymmetry in the magnitude of the N2pc component, which seems lower in the electrodes placed on the right hemisphere. A similar effect was observed in healthy older adults (see Figure 1, right panel in Lorenzo-López et al., 2008). It is important to note that spatial selective attention is indeed right hemispheric dominant (Mesulam, 1981; Shulman et al., 2010). In this regard, in a recent magnetoencephalographic study (Lorenzo-López et al., 2011) we observed a significant age-related hypoactivation of the occipito-temporal sources of the magnetic counterpart of the N2pc component (mN2pc) that was more pronounced in the right hemisphere and that could partly explain the observed hemispheric asymmetry.

Importantly, although a clear N2pc was observed in both groups for target arrays, its peak latency was significantly delayed in AAMI patients (on average 39.4 ms longer) compared to healthy controls, suggesting that they require more time to shift their visuospatial attention onto the relevant target. This slowing in N2pc latency may contribute to the general slowing observed in RTs to targets in these patients. However, no significant differences between the groups in N2pc mean amplitude and scalp distribution were found, suggesting that the amount of attention that is allocated to the target did not differ. The differential time course of the N2pc between the groups suggests that the AAMI condition involves a significant slowing of the allocation of visuospatial attention, besides their typical subjective sense of gradual memory decline in everyday situations. However, this finding must be interpreted with caution because, given the high difference in the mean age between the groups, the observed delay in RTs and N2pc latency might partially correspond to the expected latency shifts with advancing age. In this way, our results could not be specifically attributed to the AAMI process, and suggest that the previously reported age-related impairment in the allocation of visuospatial attention (Lorenzo-López et al., 2008, 2011; Amenedo et al., 2012; Wiegand et al., 2013) is clearly exacerbated as the age advances. It is important to note, however, that if the difference in age between the groups completely explains the results, a delay in N2pc latency and a reduction in its amplitude (related to slower and less effective allocation of attentional resources to the processing of the target stimulus, respectively) should be expected in the AAMI patients, which were older. However, taking into account that a non-linear relationship between N2pc peak latency and age has been recently demonstrated (Cespón et al., 2013b), and that no differences were found in the N2pc amplitude, we consider that the healthy older group was valid as a control for the present manipulation.

As previously stated, the N2pc component was absent for task-irrelevant distractors in both groups, suggesting that the color stimulus did not automatically capture the attention of participants when it was completely irrelevant for the task. In the same line, some studies have found that irrelevant stimuli in visual search do not capture attention (Leber and Egeth, 2006; Wykowska and Schubo, 2011), and that older adults demonstrate the same resistance to capture by irrelevant color stimuli than younger adults (Lien et al., 2011). We failed to find evidence of attentional capture by color in both AAMI and healthy older adults.

Due to the fact that the color irrelevant stimulus was never the target in our study and that target and distractor features remained constant across trials and were perceptually easily distinguishable (i.e., they differed only in color), it is likely that older adults (both healthy and AAMI) efficiently ignore or suppress the color dimension adopting a specific attentional set to search for the relevant dimension (orientation). In this line, it has been recently demonstrated that when the target was predictable, the N2pc to salient color distracters was abolished (Burra and Kerzel, 2013). Our results suggest that attentional capture by a simple feature is not purely stimulus-driven and can be modulated or suppressed by the top-down knowledge of the observers (Bacon and Egeth, 1994; Folk and Remington, 1998; Theeuwes and Burger, 1998; Peterson and Kramer, 2001; Connor et al., 2004; Rodríguez-Holguín et al., 2009; Hilimire and Corballis, 2014). Thus, it is possible that preserved voluntary goal-directed (top-down) mechanisms or search strategies enabled AAMI subjects to efficiently suppress processing of distracting information in the present study, avoiding attentional capture. It is also possible that given that color feature was completely irrelevant across the whole task and that participants had prior knowledge of task-relevant target features, non-targets do not require active processing to be rejected in the present experimental task (Luck and Hillyard, 1994b; Rodríguez-Holguín et al., 2009). In order to further explore the potential attentional capture by irrelevant stimuli in AAMI patients, future studies should explore other ERP components (as contralateral positivities) reflecting distractor-related processes.

A limitation of the present study is that target and task-irrelevant stimuli were presented successively making difficult generalizations to real-world perception of natural visual scenes, in which simultaneous presentation is common, causing considerably more interference than in laboratory visual search experiments. Admittedly, it cannot be completely ruled out that age differences between the groups had an influence in the present findings. Future studies with age-matched groups are needed to establish reliable conclusions.

In summary, our results provide evidence for declined visuospatial abilities in AAMI patients, who showed a reduced speed of selective attentional shifts to the task-relevant target stimulus and prolonged RTs regardless of search array. Furthermore, our findings suggest that the mechanisms of top-down suppression of irrelevant color non-target stimuli are preserved for AAMI older adults in a simple visual search task, since they were able to successfully inhibit automatic shifting of attention to their location in the visual scene. Similar interference resistance in healthy and AAMI older adults suggest preserved inhibitory function in normal aging, at least in the context of simple feature visual search, with easily distinguishable target and irrelevant non-target features. The possibility that AAMI individuals demonstrate difficult with top-down suppression under more demanding and interfering conditions or with high-valued stimuli should be explored in future studies.

Author Contributions

All authors meet all four criteria for authorship recommended by the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors. LL-L and JC contributed to the conception and design of the work, analysis, and interpretation. LL-L, AB, and CL contributed to the data acquisition. All authors collaborated in the interpretation of data for the work. LL-L drafted the article. All authors participated in revising it critically and gave final approval of the version to be published.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

The authors sincerely thank the Gerontological Complex La Milagrosa in A Coruña (Spain) for providing access to their AAMI patients.

References

Amenedo, E., Lorenzo-López, L., and Pazo-Álvarez, P. (2012). Response processing during visual search task in normal aging: the need for more time to prevent the cross talk between spatial attention and manual response selection. Biol. Psychol. 91, 201–211. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderer, P., Saletu, B., Semlitsch, H. V., and Pascual-Marqui, R. D. (2003). Non-invasive localization of P300 sources in normal aging and age-associated memory impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 24, 463–479. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(02)00058-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bacon, W. F., and Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Percept. Psychophys. 55, 485–496. doi: 10.3758/BF03205306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blesa, R., Pujol, M., Aguilar, M., Santacruz, P., Bertran-Serra, I., Hernández, G., et al. (2001). Clinical validity of the ‘mini-mental state’ for Spanish speaking communities. Neuropsychologia 39, 1150–1157. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00055-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burra, N., and Kerzel, D. (2013). Attentional capture during visual search is attenuated by target predictability: evidence from the N2pc, Pd, and topographic segmentation. Psychophysiology 50, 422–430. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cespón, J., Galdo-Álvarez, S., and Díaz, F. (2013a). Electrophysiological correlates of amnestic mild cognitive impairment in a Simon task. PLoS ONE 8:e81506. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cespón, J., Galdo-Álvarez, S., and Díaz, F. (2013b). Age-related changes in ERP correlates of visuospatial and motor processes. Psychophysiology 50, 743–757. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cespón, J., Galdo-Álvarez, S., Pereiro, A. X., and Díaz, F. (2015). Differences between mild cognitive impairment subtypes as indicated by event-related potential correlates of cognitive and motor processes in a Simon task. J. Alzheimers Dis. 43, 631–647. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colcombe, A. M., Kramer, A. F., Irwin, D. E., Peterson, M. S., Colcombe, S., and Hahn, S. (2003). Age-related effects of attentional and oculomotor capture by onsets and color singletons as a function of experience. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 113, 205–225. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(03)00019-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connor, C. E., Egeth, H. E., and Yantis, S. (2004). Visual attention: bottom-up versus top-down. Curr. Biol. 14, R850–R852. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crook, T., Bartus, R. T., Ferris, S. H., Whitehouse, P., Cohen, G. D., and Gershon, S. (1986). Age-associated memory impairment: proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change. Report of a National Institute of Mental Health Work Group. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2, 261–276. doi: 10.1080/87565648609540348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimer, M. (1996). The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 99, 225–234. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimer, M., and Kiss, M. (2006). Attentional capture by task-irrelevant fearful faces is revealed by the N2pc component. Biol. Psychol. 74, 108–112. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folk, C. L., and Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 847–858. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gazzaley, A., Cooney, J. W., Rissman, J., and D’Esposito, M. (2005). Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1298–1300. doi: 10.1038/nn1543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girelli, M., and Luck, S. J. (1997). Are the same attentional mechanisms used to detect visual search targets defined by color, orientation and motion? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 238–253. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.2.238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwood, P. M., and Parasuraman, R. (2004). The scaling of spatial attention in visual search and its modifications in healthy aging. Percept. Psychophys. 66, 3–22. doi: 10.3758/BF03194857

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hänninen, T., Hallikainen, M., Koivisto, K., Helkala, E. L., Reinikainen, K. J., Soininen, H., et al. (1995). A follow-up study of age-associated memory impairment: neuropsychological predictors of dementia. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 43, 1007–1015. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1995.tb05565.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hänninen, T., Hallikainen, M., Koivisto, K., Partanen, K., Laakso, M., Reikkinen, P., et al. (1997). Decline of frontal lobe functions in subjects with age-associated memory impairment. Neurology 48, 148–153. doi: 10.1212/WNL.48.1.148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasher, L. (2007). “Inhibition: attentional regulation of cognition,” in Science of Memory Concepts, eds H. L. Roediger, Y. Dudai, and S. M. Fitzpatrick (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 291–294.

Google Scholar

Hasher, L., and Zacks, R. T. (1988). “Working memory, comprehension, and aging: a review and a new view,” in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, ed. G. H. Bower (New York, NY: Academic Press), 193–225.

Google Scholar

Hickey, C., Di Lollo, V., and McDonald, J. J. (2009). Electrophysiological indices of target and distractor processing in visual search. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 760–775. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickey, C., McDonald, J. J., and Theeuwes, J. (2006). Electrophysiological evidence of the capture of visual attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 604–613. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilimire, M. R., and Corballis, P. M. (2014). Event-related potentials reveal the effect of prior knowledge on competition for representation and attentional capture. Psychophysiology 51, 22–35. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Irwin, D. E., and Theeuwes, J. (2000). Age differences in the control of looking behavior: do you know where your eyes have been? Psychol. Sci. 11, 210–217. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leber, A. B., and Egeth, H. E. (2006). It’s under control: top-down search strategies can override attentional capture. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13, 132–138. doi: 10.3758/BF03193824

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levitt, T., Fugelsang, J., and Crossley, M. (2006). Processing speed, attentional capacity, and age-related memory change. Exp. Aging Res. 32, 263–295. doi: 10.1080/03610730600699118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L., Gratton, C., Fabiani, M., and Knight, R. T. (2013). Age-related frontoparietal changes during the control of bottom-up and top-down attention: an ERP study. Neurobiol. Aging 34, 477–488. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lien, M. C., Gemperle, A., and Ruthruff, E. (2011). Aging and involuntary attention capture: electrophysiological evidence for preserved attentional control with advanced age. Psychol. Aging 21, 188–202. doi: 10.1037/a0021073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzo-López, L., Amenedo, E., and Cadaveira, F. (2008). Feature processing during visual search in normal aging. Neurobiol. Aging 29, 1101–1110. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzo-López, L., Gutiérrez, R., Moratti, S., Maestú, F., Cadaveira, F., and Amenedo, E. (2011). Age-related occipito-temporal hypoactivation during visual search: relationships between mN2pc sources and performance. Neuropsychologia 49, 858–865. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., and Ford, M. A. (1998). On the role of selective attention in visual perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 825–830. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.825

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., Girelli, M., McDermott, M. T., and Ford, M. A. (1997). Bridging the gap between monkey neurophysiology and human perception: an ambiguity resolution theory of visual selective attention. Cogn. Psychol. 33, 64–87. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1997.0660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., and Hillyard, S. A. (1994a). Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology 31, 291–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02218.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., and Hillyard, S. A. (1994b). Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 20, 1000–1014. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.5.1000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., and Hillyard, S. A. (1995). The role of attention in feature detection and conjunction discrimination: an electrophysiological analysis. Int. J. Neurosci. 80, 281–297. doi: 10.3109/00207459508986105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madden, D. J., and Whiting, W. L. (2004). “Age-related changes in visual attention,” in Recent Advances in Psychology and Aging, eds P. T. Costa and I. C. Siegler (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 41–88.

Google Scholar

Mesulam, M. M. (1981). A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Ann. Neurol. 10, 309–325. doi: 10.1002/ana.410100402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, J. L., Edwards, J. D., Maxfield, N. D., Peronto, C. L., Williams, V. A., and Lister, J. J. (2013). Cognitive training and selective attention in the aging brain: an electrophysiological study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 2198–2208. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, M. S., and Kramer, A. F. (2001). Attentional guidance of the eyes by contextual information and abrupt onsets. Percept. Psychophys. 63, 1239–1249. doi: 10.3758/BF03194537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pomerleau, V. T., Fortier-Gauthier, U., Corriveau, I., Dell’Acqua, R., and Jolicoeur, P. (2014). Colour-specific differences in attentional deployment for equiluminant pop-out colours: evidence from lateralised potentials. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 91, 194–205. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redel, P., Bublak, P., Sorg, C., Kurz, A., Förstl, H., Müller, H. J., et al. (2012). Deficits of spatial and task-related attentional selection in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 33, .e27–.e42. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Holguín, S., Doallo, S., Vizoso, C., and Cadaveira, F. (2009). N2pc and attentional capture by color and orientation-singletons in pure and mixed visual search tasks. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 73, 279–286. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruz, M., and Lupiáñez, J. (2002). A review of attentional capture: on its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control. Psicologica 23, 283–309.

Google Scholar

Sheikh, J. I., and Yesavage, J. A. (1986). Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin. Gerontol. 5, 165–173. doi: 10.1300/J018v05n01_09

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shulman, G. L., Pope, D. L. W., Astafiev, S. V., McAvoy, M. P., Snyder, A. Z., and Corbetta, M. (2010). Right hemisphere dominance during spatial selective attention and target detection occurs outside the dorsal frontoparietal network. J. Neurosci. 30, 3640–3651. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4085-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solesio-Jofre, E., Lorenzo-López, L., Gutiérrez, R., López-Frutos, J. M., Ruiz-Vargas, J. M., and Maestú, F. (2011). Age effects on retroactive interference during working memory maintenance. Biol. Psychol. 88, 72–82. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solesio-Jofre, E., Lorenzo-López, L., Gutiérrez, R., López-Frutos, J. M., Ruiz-Vargas, J. M., and Maestú, F. (2012). Age-related effects in working memory recognition modulated by retroactive interference. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 67, 565–572. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Störmer, V. S., Li, S. C., Heekeren, H. R., and Lindenberger, U. (2013). Normative shifts of cortical mechanisms of encoding contribute to adult age differences in visual-spatial working memory. Neuroimage 73, 167–175. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theeuwes, J. (1991). Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity. Percept. Psychophys. 50, 184–193. doi: 10.3758/BF03212219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Percept. Psychophys. 51, 599–606. doi: 10.3758/BF03211656

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theeuwes, J. (1994). Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 20, 799–806. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., and Kramer, A. F. (2000). “On the time course of top-down and bottom up control of visual attention,” in Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance, eds S. Monsell and J. Driver (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 71–208.

Google Scholar

Theeuwes, J., and Burger, R. (1998). Attentional control during visual search: the effect of irrelevant singletons. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 1342–1353. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1342

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Lubbe, R. H., and Verleger, R. (2002). Aging and the Simon task. Psychophysiology 39, 100–110. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3910100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, A., Arrighi, H. M., Michels, S., and Cedarbaum, J. M. (2012). Mild cognitive impairment: disparity of incidence and prevalence estimates. Alzheimers Dement. 8, 14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1945). A standardized memory scale for clinical use. J. Psychol. 19, 87–95. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1945.9917223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiegand, I., Finke, K., Müller, H. J., and Töllner, T. (2013). Event-related potentials dissociate perceptual from response-related age effects in visual search. Neurobiol. Aging 34, 973–985. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodman, G. F., and Luck, S. J. (1999). Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search. Nature 400, 867–869. doi: 10.1038/23698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodman, G. F., and Luck, S. J. (2003). Serial deployment of attention during visual search. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 29, 121–138. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wykowska, A., and Schubo, A. (2011). Irrelevant singletons in visual search do not capture attention but can produce nonspatial filtering costs. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 645–660. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Youngjohn, J. R., and Crook, T. H. (1993). Stability of everyday memory in age-associated memory impairment: a longitudinal study. Neuropsychology 7, 406–416. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.7.3.406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: AAMI, automatic attentional capture, ERPs, N2pc, visual search

Citation: Lorenzo-López L, Maseda A, Buján A, de Labra C, Amenedo E and Millán-Calenti JC (2016) Preserved Suppression of Salient Irrelevant Stimuli During Visual Search in Age-Associated Memory Impairment. Front. Psychol. 6:2033. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02033

Received: 22 October 2015; Accepted: 21 December 2015;
Published: 12 January 2016.

Edited by:

Roberta Sellaro, Leiden University, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Silvia Pagano, Universitã di Trento, Italy
Iris Wiegand, University of Copenhagen, Denmark

Copyright © 2016 Lorenzo-López, Maseda, Buján, de Labra, Amenedo and Millán-Calenti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Laura Lorenzo-López, laura.lorenzo.lopez@udc.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.