Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 28 January 2016
Sec. Educational Psychology

Quadratic Models May Provide a Useful Set of Models that Detect Combined Effects of Achievement Goals on Academic Attainment

  • Health Psychology and Behavioural Medicine Research Group, Laboratory of Self-Regulation, Faculty of Health Sciences, School of Psychology and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Perth, WA, Australia

There is a consensus of opinion among proponents of theories of achievement goals that people may choose to pursue two different types of goals: mastery goals that focus on developing competence through task mastery and learning and performance goals that focus on demonstrating competence by outperforming others (Ames and Archer, 1987; Dweck and Leggett, 1988; Nicholls, 1989; Ames, 1992; Pintrich, 2000). Research in educational contexts indicates that mastery goals are reliably associated with positive outcomes such as high intrinsic motivation, high task-interest, and use of deep learning strategies (Harackiewicz et al., 2000, 2008). However, performance goals exhibit a stronger relationship with high task performance than mastery goals (Senko et al., 2011; Van Yperen et al., 2015). This is more likely to be observed when tendencies to pursue mastery or performance goals are coupled with approach reactions rather than avoidance reactions (Senko et al., 2011).

The positive effects of performance goals and mastery goals on the same or distinct educational outcomes have also lead researchers to propose that students who pursue both goal types simultaneously may experience more positive outcomes than students who adopt one type of goal only (Harackiewicz et al., 1998). The reason for this is that students pursuing both goals simultaneously may accrue the benefits associated with each goal (Senko et al., 2011). Accordingly, Barron and Harackiewicz (2001) advised researchers to examine the combined effects of achievement goals on outcomes by examining whether the following regression equation explained observations:

A=b0+b1M+b2P+b3MxP+e10    (1)

In Equation 1, A represents individuals' achievement on a task such as the grades that students achieve in an exam. The terms M and P are individuals' responses to instruments measuring mastery goals and performance goals. The term MxP is a product term that represents the interaction between mastery goals and performance goals. The coefficient b0 is the intercept of the regression equation. The term e10 indicates residual variance unexplained by the regression equation. The coefficients b1 to b3 are unstandardized regression coefficients indicating main or interactive effects of mastery goals or performance goals on achievement.

Broadly speaking, Equation 1 supports combined effects of achievement goals on academic attainment if the main effects of mastery goals (i.e., b1 > 0) and performance goals (i.e., b2 > 0) on achievement are positive and statistically significant. Alternatively, the model represented in Equation 1 supports combined effects if the coefficient on the interaction between mastery goals and performance goals is positive and statistically significant (i.e., b3 > 0 ; Senko et al., 2011). However, previous research has rarely observed combined effects of achievement goals on academic attainment. A cursory review of the literature indicates that only six out of 66 studies (9%) have supported combined effects of performance goals and mastery goals on academic attainment by ways of using the model presented in Equation 1 (see Table 1). These studies confirmed an additive model. However, it can be argued that these effects could be obtained by chance, given the rather small number of significant effects. This possibility is reinforced by the fact that some of the studies exhibited borderline significant results (Finney et al., 2004; Bodmann et al., 2008; Pekrun et al., 2009).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of achievement goal effects on performance outcomes.

One reason that the regression model described in Equation 1 might have not been successful in detecting combined effects may be related to the fact that it does not test the hypothesis that individuals who endorse one goal at the highest possible level and the other goal at a marginally lower level exhibit optimal performance (i.e., individuals who adopt a high-mastery/moderate-performance goal profile). This is because the model implied by Equation 1 assumes that the effects of a goal (e.g., performance goal) on academic achievement are monotonically increasing within high or low levels of endorsing the other goal (e.g., mastery goal) (Edwards, 1994, 2001). As a consequence, when Equation 1 supports combined effects, it always “forces” researchers to conclude that a high-mastery/high-performance goal profile is the most adaptive goal profile. However, this statistical assumption of monotonicity may be restrictive and it may not express the ways that students who adopt both achievement goals regulate goal adoption. For example, in educational contexts, students who are inclined to pursue both achievement goals may endorse mastery goals at a slightly higher level than performance goals at the beginning of the semester when learning is important (Pintrich, 2000; Barron and Harackiewicz, 2001). Unfortunately, the model implied by Equation 1 does not test whether students who adopt a high-mastery/moderate-performance goal profile are the best performers. As a consequence, it may mislead researchers to reject a combined effect of achievement goals on academic attainment that corresponds to a high-mastery/moderate-performance profile when in fact observations support a combined effect.

A number of researchers have suggested that problems associated with Equation 1 can be overcome by applying the following quadratic equation to explain effects of goal orientations on attainment (Krantz and Tversky, 1971; Lubinski and Humphreys, 1990; Aiken and West, 1991; Cortina, 1993; Edwards and Parry, 1993; Ganzach, 1997):

A=b0+b1M+b2P+b3M2+b4MxP+b5P2+e10    (2)

In this equation, M2 and P2 are quadratic terms that represent non-linear relationships between achievement goals and academic attainment. The coefficients b3 and b5 are unstandardized regression coefficients that represent effects associated with the quadratic terms. The model implied by Equation 2 can test a broader set of hypothesis about combined effects because the quadratic terms can examine whether achievement goals yield higher levels of academic attainment when they are endorsed at some moderate level (Griffin et al., 1999; Edwards, 2001). For example, the quadratic model may indicate that a high-mastery/moderate-performance goal profile is the most optimal profile if, in Equation 2, the main and quadratic effects of mastery and performance goals on academic achievement are linear and concave, respectively. The reason for this is that linear functions return higher levels of attainment when achievement goals are endorsed at the highest possible levels whereas concave functions indicate that achievement goals yield higher performance levels when they are endorsed at moderate levels (Edwards and Parry, 1993).

As it stands in the current literature on achievement goals, it is difficult to determine whether the quadratic model is more effective in detecting combined effects than the model implied by Equation 1. This is because the majority of studies have not included quadratic terms in their regression analyses. Most importantly, we need to explain what a curvilinear relationship in this context means. In particular, performance approach goals produce a mix of simultaneously positive (e.g., Elliot and Church, 1997; Harackiewicz et al., 1997; Senko and Harackiewicz, 2005; Hulleman et al., 2010; Senko and Hulleman, 2013) and negative or null effects on academic achievement (e.g., Ames and Archer, 1988; Greene et al., 2004; Long et al., 2007; Matos et al., 2007). On the one hand, performances goals promote academic attainment because they motivate students to actively attend to teachers' instructions, guides and hints that allow them predicting content of examinations (learning agenda hypothesis; Senko et al., 2013). These goals orient students to use studying strategies that increase probability to do well at exams. For example, allocating the studying time and effort to materials evenly ensures attainment of a high academic grade (Senko et al., 2013). On the other hand, performance goals may undermine academic achievement by increasing anxiety and perceived social pressure. For example, Eisenbarth and Petrichkoff (2012) observed a convex relationship between these variables whereby worry reached lowest levels when participants moderately endorsed performance goals. Therefore, it is possible that a moderate endorsement of performance goals yields the highest achievement, whereas any deviation (above or below a moderate level) undermines it. There is some evidence to support this relationship (Sideridis, 2007; Sideridis et al., 2013; Sideridis and Stamovlasis, 2014; Stamovlasis and Sideridis, 2014). For example, Sideridis et al. (2015) demonstrated that a mild performance class-climate increased reading performance, but more active instigation of the performance goals beyond a moderate level was maladaptive for reading performance. In addition, Sideridis et al. (2015) also observed a linear relationship between reading performance and class-climates in which teachers strongly encouraged adoption of mastery goals. Building on these findings and Senko et al.'s (2013) theoretical framework, a high-mastery/moderate-performance goal profile is expected to be the most optimal goal profile. Hence, the quadratic model may prove to be a viable data analytic model that assists researchers in detecting combined effects of achievement goals on academic attainment. We look to future research on the effects of achievement goals on academic attainment to routinely apply the quadratic model to analyse their data. This will provide definitive evidence as to whether the quadratic model is the most effective in describing interactive effects of mastery and performance goals on academic attainment.

In conclusion, the present article suggests that previous research might have not been successful in detecting combined effects of achievement goals on academic attainment because they did not include quadratic terms in their regression models. Quadratic terms, which test for non-linear relationship between achievement goals and academic attainment, enable researchers to examine a broader set of hypothesis about combined effects such as whether a high mastery-moderate performance goal profile is the most optimal. Therefore, by applying the quadratic model future research may become more consistent in detecting combined effects of achievement goals on academic attainment and provide a precise description of which goal profile is most optimal in terms of yielding highest levels of academic performance.

Author Contributions

The current paper is a part of the Ph.D. of SK. SK, NC, and MH conceived the ideas presented in the article and drafted the article. The final approval was obtained from all co-authors prior to submission.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Aiken, L. S., and West, S. G. (1991). Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms goals structures and student motivation. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 261–271. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.3.261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ames, C., and Archer, J. (1987). Mothers' beliefs about the role of ability and effort in school learning. J. Educ. Psychol. 79, 409–414. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.79.4.409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ames, C., and Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: students' learning strategies and motivation processes. J. Educ. Psychol. 80, 260–267. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barron, K. E., and Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement goals and optimal motivation: testing multiple goal models. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 706–722. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodmann, S., Hulleman, C. S., and Harackiewicz, J. M. (2008). Achievement goal systems: an application of goal systems theory to achievement goal research. Int. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 21, 71–96.

Google Scholar

Church, M. A., Elliot, A. J., and Gable, S. L. (2001). Perceptions of classroom environment, achievement goals, and achievement outcomes. J. Educ. Psychol. 93, 43–54. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.43

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortina, J. M. (1993). Interaction, nonlinearity and multicollinearity: implications for multiple regression. J. Manage. 19, 915–922. doi: 10.1177/014920639301900411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dweck, C. S., and Leggett, E. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychol. Rev. 95, 256–273. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, J. R. (1994). The study of congruence in organizational behavior research: critique and a proposed alternative. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 58, 51–100.

Google Scholar

Edwards, J. R. (2001). Ten difference score myths. Organ. Res. Methods 4, 264–286. doi: 10.1177/109442810143005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, J. R., and Parry, M. E. (1993). On the use of polynomial regression equations as an alternative to difference scores in organizational research. Acad. Manage. J. 36, 1577–1613.

Google Scholar

Eisenbarth, C. A., and Petrichkoff, L. M. (2012). Independent and interactive effects of task and ego orientations in predicting competitive trait anxiety among college-age athletes. J. Sport Behav. 35, 387–405.

Google Scholar

Elliot, A. J., and Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 1218–1232. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finney, S. J., Pieper, S. L., and Barron, K. E. (2004). Examining the psychometric properties of the achievement goal questionnaire in a more general academic context. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 64, 365–382. doi: 10.1177/0013164403258465

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganzach, Y. (1997). Misleading interaction and curvilinear terms. Psychol. Methods 3, 235–247. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.2.3.235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, B. A., Miller, R. B., Crowson, H. M., Duke, B. L., and Akey, K. L. (2004). Predicting high school students' cognitive engagement and achievement: contributions of classroom perceptions and motivation. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 29, 462–482. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2004.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffin, D., Murray, S. L., and Gonzalez, R. (1999). A friendly note on difference scores: a conceptual primer. Pers. Relatsh. 6, 505–518. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.1999.tb00206.x

CrossRef Full Text

Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Carter, S. M., Lehto, A. T., and Elliot, A. J. (1997). Predictors and consequences of achievement goals in the college classroom: maintaining interest and making the grade. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73, 1284–1295. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., and Elliot, A. J. (1998). Rethinking achievement goals: when are they adaptive for college students and why? Educ. Psychol. 33, 1–21. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep3301_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., Carter, S. M., and Elliot, A. J. (2000). Short-term and long-term consequances of achievement goals: predicting interest and performance over time. J. Educ. Psychol. 92, 316–330. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.2.316

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harackiewicz, J. M., Durik, A. M., Barron, K. E., Linnenbrink, E. A., and Tauer, J. M. (2008). The role of achievement goals in the development of interest: reciprocal relations between achievement goals, interest and performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 100, 105–122. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hulleman, C. S., Schrager, S. M., Bodmann, S. M., and Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). A meta-analytic review of achievement goal measures: different labels for the same constructs or different constructs with similar labels? Psychol. Bull. 136, 422–449. doi: 10.1037/a0018947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krantz, D. H., and Tversky, A. (1971). Conjoint measurement analysis of composition rules in psychology. Psychol. Rev. 78, 151–169. doi: 10.1037/h0030637

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, J., Monoi, S., Harper, B., Knoblauch, D., and Murphy, P. (2007). Academic motivation and achievement among urban adolescents. Urban Educ. 42, 196–222. doi: 10.1177/0042085907300447

CrossRef Full Text

Lubinski, D., and Humphreys, L. G. (1990). Assessing spurious “moderator” effects: illustrated substantively with the hypothesized (“synergistic”) relation between spatial and mathematical ability. Psychol. Bull. 107, 385–393. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.3.385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matos, L., Lens, W., and Vansteenkiste, M. (2007). Achievement goals, learning strategies and Language achievement among Peruvian high school students. Psychol. Belg. 47, 51–70. doi: 10.5334/pb-47-1-51

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The Competitive Ethos and Democratic Education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., and Maehr, M. A. (2009). Achievement goals and achievement emotions: testing a model of their joint relations with academic performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 115–135. doi: 10.1037/a0013383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pintrich, P. R. (2000). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: the role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 3, 544–555. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.3.544

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senko, C., Hama, H., and Belmonte, K. (2013). Achievement goals, study strategies, and achievement: a test of the “learning agenda” framework. Learn. Individ. Dif. 24, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senko, C., and Harackiewicz, J. M. (2005). Regulation of achievement goals: the role of competence feedback. J. Educ. Psychol. 97, 320–336. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.320

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senko, C., Hulleman, C., and Harackiewitz, J. (2011). Achievement goal theory at the crossroads: old and controversies, current challenges, and new directions. Educ. Psychol. 46, 26–47. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2011.538646

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senko, C., and Hulleman, C. S. (2013). The role of goal attainment expectancies in achievement goal pursuit. J. Educ. Psychol. 105, 504–521. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0031136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sideridis, G. D. (2007). Why are students with learning disabilities depressed? A goal orientation model of depression vulnerability. J. Learn. Disabil. 40, 526–539. doi: 10.1177/00222194070400060401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sideridis, G. D., Antoniou, F., and Simos, P. (2013). The physiological effects of goal orientations on the reading performance of students with dyslexia: a pilot study. Soc. Behav. Sci. 93, 1546–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.10.080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sideridis, G. D., and Stamovlasis, D. (2014). The role of goal orientations in explaining academic cheating in students with learning disabilities: an application of the cusp catastrophe. Ethics Behav. 24, 444–466. doi: 10.1080/10508422.2013.877393

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sideridis, G. D., Stamovlasis, D., and Antoniou, F. (2015). Reading achievement, mastery, and performance goal structures among students with learning disabilities: a nonlinear perspective. J. Learn. Disabil. doi: 10.1177/0022219415576524. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stamovlasis, D., and Sideridis, G. (2014). Ought approach—Ought avoidance: nonlinear effects under achievement situations. Nonlinear Dynamics Psychol. Life Sci. 18, 67–90.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Van Yperen, N. W., Blaga, M., and Potmes, T. (2015). A meta-analysis of the impact of situationally induced achievement goals on task performance. Hum. Perform. 28, 165–182. doi: 10.1080/08959285.2015.1006772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: achievement goals, multiple goals effect, quadratic model, combined effect, non-linear relationship

Citation: Kamarova S, Chatzisarantis NLD and Hagger MS (2016) Quadratic Models May Provide a Useful Set of Models that Detect Combined Effects of Achievement Goals on Academic Attainment. Front. Psychol. 7:29. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00029

Received: 13 November 2015; Accepted: 07 January 2016;
Published: 28 January 2016.

Edited by:

Stephen Wee Hun Lim, National University of Singapore, Singapore

Reviewed by:

Kep Kee Loh, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, France
William R. Aue, Purdue University, USA

Copyright © 2016 Kamarova, Chatzisarantis and Hagger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sviatlana Kamarova, sviatlana.kamarova@postgrad.curtin.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.