Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 16 February 2016
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Factor Structure and Psychometric Properties of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 in an Italian Community Sample

  • 1Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 2Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 3Institute of Behavioral and Cognitive Psychology and Psychotherapy (IPSICO), Firenze, Italy
  • 4Department of Health Sciences, University of Florence, Firenze, Italy

Anxiety Sensitivity (AS) is defined as the fear of anxiety and of arousal-related bodily sensations, arising from erroneous beliefs that these sensations will have adverse consequences. AS plays a key role both in the onset and in the maintenance of several disorders, particularly anxiety disorders. To date, only two studies on American samples have examined the bifactor structure of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3); therefore, findings on different cultures are needed. The main purpose of the present study was to assess the factor structure and psychometric properties of the ASI-3 in an Italian community sample. Participants were recruited from the general population (N = 1507). The results of a series of confirmatory factor analyses indicated that the bifactor structure fitted the data better than the most commonly accepted structure for the measure and that it was invariant across gender. Moreover, the current study provided evidence regarding the ASI-3’s reliability and its convergent and divergent validity. Lastly, results pertaining incremental validity of the ASI-3 Physical and Cognitive Concerns subscales above and beyond the total showed that the former was not associated with a measure of physiological anxiety, whereas the latter was weakly associated with a measure of worry. Findings suggest that the ASI-3 is comprised of a dominant general factor and three specific independent factors; given the dominance of the general factor, the use of the ASI-3 total score as a measure of the general fear of anxiety is recommended in both clinical and research settings.

Introduction

Anxiety Sensitivity (AS) is defined as an individual’s fear of anxiety and of arousal-related bodily sensations (“fear of anxiety symptoms”), including those that may occur during normal physiological processes (e.g., heartbeat acceleration, breathing problems, feeling faint), arising from erroneous beliefs that these sensations will have adverse and harmful physical, psychological, or social consequences such as death, insanity, or social rejection (Reiss and McNally, 1985). In particular, AS is considered a dispositional feature that acts as an anxiety amplifier: When individuals with high levels of AS experience anxiety, they start both catastrophically misinterpreting their introceptive sensations and worrying about them, thus increasing the intensity of anxiety.

AS represents, therefore, a cognitive predisposition to general fearfulness, a vulnerability factor (diathesis) for anxiety psychopathology that plays a central role in both the onset and the maintenance of several anxiety disorders (McNally, 2002; Schmidt et al., 2006; Bernstein and Zvolensky, 2007; Li and Zinbarg, 2007; Calkins et al., 2009; Olatunji and Wolitzky-Taylor, 2009). High AS levels have been found in patients suffering from panic disorder (PD) and agoraphobia (Reiss et al., 1986; White et al., 2006), posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Leen-Feldner et al., 2008; Marshall et al., 2010), social and specific phobias (Sandin et al., 1996; Norton et al., 1997), and obsessive compulsive disorder (Calamari et al., 2008). Typically, patients with PD and/or agoraphobia are characterized by the most elevated AS intensity in comparison to clinical groups with other anxiety pathologies or healthy controls (Deacon and Abramowitz, 2006; Taylor et al., 2007; Mantar et al., 2010; Naragon-Gainey, 2010). For instance, in a recent meta-analysis, Olatunji and Wolitzky-Taylor (2009) reported a large effect size indicative of greater AS among patients with anxiety disorders, especially for those with PD and PTSD, compared to healthy controls (d = 1.61 and d = 0.71, respectively). Although AS is concurrently and prospectively related to a variety of anxiety disorders, particularly those characterized by hyperarousal (e.g., Ball et al., 1995; Rector et al., 2007; Feldner et al., 2008; Marshall et al., 2010; Schmidt et al., 2010), it has been demonstrated that people with high AS tend to suffer also from non-anxiety psychopathologies such as depression, health anxiety, nicotine, and alcohol addiction, drug problems, chronic pain, and eating disorders (Reiss et al., 1986; Otto et al., 1995; Asmundson, 1999; Stewart et al., 1999; Taylor, 1999; Watt and Stewart, 2000; Anestis et al., 2008; Esteve and Camacho, 2008; Naragon-Gainey, 2010).

The first conceptualizations of AS implicated a unitary construct (Reiss and McNally, 1985; Reiss, 1991; Taylor et al., 1991, 1992). The most widely used self-report measure specifically developed to assess AS is the Anxiety Sensitivity Index (ASI; Reiss et al., 1986; Peterson and Reiss, 1992), a 16-item questionnaire. Although the conceptualization of AS underwent a refinement to include a multidimensional structure, and the original ASI has demonstrated well-established psychometric properties in several samples (Peterson and Plehn, 1999), there was no consensus in the literature regarding its factor structure (Taylor, 1999). Indeed, factor-analytic studies of the original ASI reported results ranging from a one-factor solution (Reiss et al., 1986; Peterson and Heilbronner, 1987; Taylor et al., 1992; Won et al., 1995; Ayvaşɪk, 2000) to 2- to 4-factor solutions (Telch et al., 1989; Wardle et al., 1990; Cox et al., 1996; Zinbarg et al., 1997; Blais et al., 2001; Cho, 2004). Various studies focusing on the latent dimensionality of the original ASI found a hierarchical and multidimensional structure in adult (Zinbarg et al., 1997; Taylor and Cox, 1998a,b) as well as in young individuals (Silverman et al., 2003). In particular, the original ASI was found to be composed of one higher-order factor (general AS) and three lower-order dimensions, namely: Physical Concerns (fear of somatic sensations); Social Concerns (fear of publicly observable anxiety symptoms that may cause social rejection or ridicule); and Cognitive Concerns (fear of cognitive or psychological dyscontrol; Zinbarg et al., 1997). Overall, the most frequently found factor solution was made up of three interrelated factors loading on a single higher-order factor (Taylor, 1999). Potential explanations for the instability in the original ASI factor structure concerned the different factor selection criteria employed across studies and the recruitment of some small samples (Taylor, 1999). Also, since the original ASI was not developed according to a multidimensional model, the Physical Concerns domain was measured by half of the ASI items, leaving only a few items to assess the other two factors (four items for each dimension, generating an unequal item distribution; Taylor and Cox, 1998a; Deacon and Valentiner, 2001). Lastly, the latter two subscales demonstrated weak content validity due to the fact that several items (e.g., “It scares me when I am nauseous”) were not explicitly linked to a specific AS factor (Blais et al., 2001).

Aiming to improve the measure, Taylor and Cox (1998a,b) developed two instruments: the ASI-Revised (ASI-R; Taylor and Cox, 1998a), a 36-item revised scale characterized by the same instructions and response format of the original ASI (10 of 16 original items were incorporated), and the 60-item AS Profile (Taylor and Cox, 1998b). However, studies of both scales demonstrated that their factor solutions were not fully satisfying (e.g., Mohlman and Zinbarg, 2000; Blais et al., 2001; Zvolensky and Forsyth, 2002; Bouvard et al., 2003; Deacon et al., 2003; Olatunji et al., 2005; Deacon and Abramowitz, 2006; Armstrong et al., 2006; Lim et al., 2007; Schmidt et al., 2008). As such, in order to overcome the AS factorial structure instability, Taylor et al. (2007) proposed a new, further-revised multidimensional instrument: the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3), which consists of 18 items assessing the three factors most frequently found in previous AS research (six items for each factor): Physical, Social, and Cognitive Concerns. Six items were selected from the ASI-R, whereas five items were derived from the original ASI.

These lower-order facets of AS, consistently assessed with the ASI-3, seem to be differently related to the etiology and maintenance of particular types of anxiety-related disorders. Specifically, Physical Concerns involve fear of physical arousal sensations (e.g., rapid heartbeat) due to concerns that these signal physical illness outcomes or physical catastrophe (e.g., heart attack) and they are strongly associated with panic attacks and PD and/or agoraphobia, health anxiety, and somatization (Taylor et al., 2007; Escocard et al., 2009; Osman et al., 2010; Kemper et al., 2012; Wheaton et al., 2012). Findings pertaining to Social Concerns revealed that this construct significantly correlates with fear of negative evaluation, social phobia and introversion (Taylor et al., 2007; Kemper et al., 2009, 2012; Wheaton et al., 2012; Olthuis et al., 2014). Indeed, they involve fears of publicly observable anxiety symptoms (e.g., shaking) due to concerns that they may result in negative social consequences (e.g., social rejection). Lastly, less clear-cut results have been reported in relation to Cognitive Concerns, which involve fears of cognitive anxiety-related sensations (e.g., difficulty concentrating) due to concerns that they may lead to adverse psychological consequences (e.g., loss of control): many studies did not find a specific relation to any anxiety disorder, but rather to general distress and depression (Taylor and Cox, 1998a; Olthuis et al., 2014), whereas Wheaton et al. (2012) found that they were associated with generalized anxiety disorder.

Taylor et al. (2007) demonstrated that the ASI-3 was characterized by a three-factor hierarchic structure (three subscales for the first-order level and a Global AS factor for the second-order level) both in clinical and non-clinical samples. Such results have been replicated on a mixed sample of anxiety disorder patients and undergraduate students (Wheaton et al., 2012). In contrast, Osman et al. (2010) and Ebesutani et al. (2014) administered the ASI-3 to undergraduate students and found that a bifactor model, consisting of a general factor and three independent orthogonal group factors, added significant improvement over the three-factor hierarchic model in representing the structure of the ASI-3, thus suggesting that the latent structure of the ASI-3 might be unidimensional. Indeed, in bifactor models each item loads on a general factor reflecting what is common among the items and embodies the individual differences on a target dimension (Brown, 2006). Furthermore, two or more orthogonal “group” factors are specified in such a structure: these are specific factors that are orthogonal to one another evaluated by the items capable of explaining item response variance not accounted for by the general factor (Brown, 2006).

It is noteworthy that in the study by Ebesutani et al. (2014) the ASI-3 bifactor model’s results were invariant across gender. As far as psychometric properties are concerned, the ASI-3 subscales proved to be reliable, showing alpha values ranging from 0.73 to 0.91 in the study by Taylor et al. (2007), and from 0.80 to 0.90 (0.93 for the total score) in the study by Wheaton et al. (2012); similarly, Osman et al. (2010) found good values of the composite reliability (i.e., a latent variable modeling procedure to compute true score reliability; range in ρs = 0.80–0.86; ρ = 0.90 for the ASI-3 total). The ASI-3 also showed good convergent, discriminant, and criterion-related validity (Taylor et al., 2007; Osman et al., 2010). Furthermore, it proved to be a better predictor of anxious response to laboratory challenge than the ASI, thus bolstering the improvement of the ASI-3 over the original version (Carter et al., 2009). Importantly, regarding socio-demographic variables, the ASI-3 was revealed to be not affected by age, education level, and gender (Taylor et al., 2007; Osman et al., 2010).

In light of the promising findings mentioned above, it was necessary to verify the stability of the ASI-3 factor structure and its psychometric properties across different cultures and languages. The cross-cultural invariance of the three-factor hierarchic structure has been confirmed by factor analyses in community samples (Sandin et al., 2007; Kemper et al., 2009; Lim and Kim, 2012), as well as in patients with anxiety or mood disorders (Escocard et al., 2009; Mantar et al., 2010; Kemper et al., 2012) from South American, European, Middle Eastern, and Asian countries. Notably, none of these studies tested the bifactor model. Good internal consistency values for the single second-order level (α = 0.91) and for the three first-order level factors (αs = 0.81–0.89) were confirmed in a Brazilian sample made up of patients with anxiety disorders (Escocard et al., 2009), as well as in a Turkish sample consisting of patients with anxiety and mood disorders (single second-order level factor: α = 0.93; three first-order level factors; αs = 0.82–0.89; Mantar et al., 2010). Also, in a Korean college sample, internal consistency values varied from adequate to good (range in αs = 0.73–0.86 and 0.87 for the global scale; Lim and Kim, 2012). Furthermore, moderate associations between the three subscales (range in rs = 0.41–0.61) and high correlations between subscales’ scores and the total score (range in rs = 0.70–0.86) have been found in this Korean sample (Lim and Kim, 2012). Likewise, in a non-clinical Spanish sample (Sandin et al., 2007) Cronbach’s alpha coefficients ranged from 0.83 to 0.87 (α = 0.91 for the total score); subscales proved to be highly related to the total score (rs ranging from 0.80 to 0.83), and moderately inter-correlated (range in rs = 0.42–0.59). The 1-month test–retest stability in cross-cultural validations ranged from 0.83 to 0.85 (Sandin et al., 2007) and resulted in 0.64 for the total score (Mantar et al., 2010) in the Spanish and Turkish samples, respectively. Furthermore, patients with anxiety disorders have been successfully distinguished (discriminant validity) from patients with non-anxiety disorders by the ASI-3 in Brazilian, German, and Turkish clinical samples (Escocard et al., 2009; Mantar et al., 2010; Kemper et al., 2012), and it also demonstrated good convergent and divergent validity (Escocard et al., 2009; Mantar et al., 2010; Osman et al., 2010; Kemper et al., 2012; Lim and Kim, 2012).

Lastly, regarding socio-demographic variables, in a sample of Brazilian patients Escocard et al. (2009) replicated findings by Taylor et al. (2007) and Osman et al. (2010), demonstrating that the ASI-3 was not affected by age, education level, and gender. In contrast, females showed higher ASI-3 scores compared to males in a Spanish community sample (Sandin et al., 2007).

The Current Study

In consideration of the potential utility of the ASI-3, the main aim of the present study was to provide data on its factorial structure and ascertain its reliability, as well as its validity, in an Italian community sample. It is noteworthy that the ASI-3 was designed to be used in both clinical and non-clinical samples. Indeed, given that AS proved to be predictive of panic or anxious responses to challenge and stress (Joiner et al., 2002), a self-report instrument capable of identifying people who are theoretically at risk for developing anxiety disorders or psychological problems is recommended. Furthermore, since some studies outlined the AS’s dimensional structure (Asmundson et al., 2011) and suggested the use of non-clinical samples to evaluate AS because patients often present comorbidity problems (Noyes et al., 2004), studying the ASI-3 in non-clinical samples, in addition to clinical populations, could be useful.

Our first step was to examine the factor structure of the Italian ASI-3 by performing five CFAs that tested five different models. Then the following hypotheses were tested: (1) The ASI-3 factor model would be invariant in terms of gender, as found by Ebesutani et al. (2014); (2) Since few statistics on gender differences have been reported in the literature, we aimed to further investigate them in the present sample. Overall, the results from the few available studies reported no gender differences; therefore, we did not expect differences across gender on the Italian ASI-3 either; (3) The reliability and temporal stability of the ASI-3 would be good; (4) We would find low correlations between the ASI-3 score and age and education in the present adult sample (>18 years); (5) Correlations of the ASI-3 scores with another anxiety measure (convergent validity) would be moderate to high; (6) Correlations of the ASI-3 scores with a measure of depression would be lower than the correlations with a measure of anxiety (divergent validity). Lastly, since Ebesutani et al. (2014) highlighted the need to further examine the incremental utility of the ASI-3 subscales above and beyond the total we also addressed this issue. In particular, we sought to explore whether physical concerns predicted scores on a measure of physiological anxiety above and beyond the general AS factor, and whether cognitive concerns were predictive of scores on a measure of worry above and beyond the general AS factor. We did not formulate any specific hypothesis in regard to social concerns, since no criterion-measures for social anxiety were administered to participants.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

The present sample consisted of 1507 community individuals (38.9% male) from various Italian towns. The current sample represents a subset of a larger sample (N = 1617). Participants were identified for the current study based on completion of the ASI-3 with no missing data.1 All participants were Caucasian. The mean age of the sample was 38.35 years (SD = 14.72; range = 17–80) and the mean years of education was 13.35 (SD = 3.24; range = 5–28). Participants listed their marital status as follows: 45.7% single, 47.4% married or cohabitating, 4.8% separated or divorced, 1.6% widowed, and 0.5% other conditions. The employment profile of the total sample was as follows: 44.1% full-time job, 30.2% students, 3.1% part-time job, 4.5% unemployed, 2.6% retired, 2.8% full-time homemaker, and 12.7% other. A subgroup of 80 undergraduate students (40% females; mean age = 27.7 years; SD = 5.3) were asked to complete the questionnaires on two occasions 4 weeks apart in order to assess the test–retest reliability of the ASI-3.

The study was carried on in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the institutional review board of the University of Firenze. All participants were recruited on a voluntary basis, by means of a snowball sampling procedure, and gave their written consent before entering the study. The initial recipients were individuals selected and contacted among the acquaintances of several members of our laboratory staff (trainees and post graduate studies born in different regions of Italy). Initial recipients were then invited to select and contact further individuals among the acquaintances. No incentives for participation were given. Eligible participants completed a battery of self-report measures individually administered and rotated in their sequence to control for order effects.

Materials

All participants completed a background information questionnaire and the following measures:

The Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3; Taylor et al., 2007)

It is an 18-item, self-report measure developed to assess AS. Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (“not at all”) to 4 (“very much”); the higher the score, the more severe the AS level.

The standard steps outlined in the psychology literature guided the Italian translation process used in the present study (e.g., Brislin, 1986). In the first step, three independent researchers translated the questionnaire from English to Italian and then reached agreement on a common version. Idiomatic Italian at the sixth-grade level was used for this step. Furthermore, the researchers reviewed the common version to ensure that there were no colloquialisms or esoteric sentences that would make interpretations difficult. The shared form was then back-translated by a bilingual individual with extensive knowledge of psychological topics. The back-translation was nearly identical to the original one. As a final step, the Italian ASI-3 items were rated by five experts in anxiety and depressive disorders. Each expert rated the items on a 5-point scale (1 = “not at all,” 5 = “extremely”) for clarity (the extent to which the item is clearly described). The experts’ ratings indicated excellent clarity (mean across all items = 4.4; SD = 0.4), indicating that further item refinement was unnecessary.

The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck et al., 1988)

It is a 21-item, self-report inventory assessing the severity of anxiety. The Italian version of the BAI was administered to 654 undergraduates, 831 community controls, and 64 anxious patients. Excellent psychometric properties were observed in both the original (internal consistency: α = 0.92; 1-week test–retest reliability: r = 0.75 in a community sample) and the Italian version (internal consistency: α = 0.89, α = 0.87, and α = 0.81 in undergraduates, community and anxious patients, respectively; 1-month test–retest reliability: r = 0.62 in a student sample; Beck et al., 1988; Sica et al., 2006; Sica and Ghisi, 2007). Good Cronbach’s alpha was also observed in the present study (α = 0.87).

The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck et al., 1996)

It is a 21-item, self-report scale measuring the severity of affective, cognitive, motivational, vegetative, and psychomotor components of depression. Excellent psychometric properties were observed in both the original (internal consistency: α = 0.92; 1-week test–retest reliability: r = 0.75 in a community sample) and the Italian version (internal consistency: α = 0.89, α = 0.87, and α = 0.81 in undergraduates, community and anxious patients, respectively; 1-month test–retest reliability: r = 0.62 in a student sample; Beck et al., 1996; Ghisi et al., 2006; Sica and Ghisi, 2007). Internal consistency was also good in the sample employed in the present study (α = 0.85).

The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21; Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995)

It consists of 21 items organized into three scales: depression, referring to lack of incentive, low self-esteem, and dysphoria; anxiety, assessing somatic and subjective symptoms of anxiety, as well as acute responses of fear; and stress, measuring irritability, impatience, tension, and persistent arousal (Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995). Findings on the Italian version suggested that use of the total score, measuring a “general distress” factor, could be more appropriate than calculating the three subscale scores separately (Bottesi et al., 2015). The total score of the Italian version showed excellent internal consistency values (α = 0.90 and α = 0.92 in a community and in a mixed clinical sample, respectively), good 2-week test–retest reliability in an undergraduate sample (r = 0.74), large convergent/divergent validity coefficients and good criterion-oriented validity (Bottesi et al., 2015). Good Cronbach’s alpha for the total score was also observed in the present study (α = 0.88).

The Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer et al., 1990)

It is a 16-item inventory assessing trait worry and, in particular, the generality, excessiveness, and uncontrollability features of pathological worry. The internal consistency of the Italian version of the PSWQ, measured on a community sample was good (α = 0.85; Morani et al., 1999). In the present study, the alpha coefficient was an acceptable 0.70.

Data Analysis

In order to identify the best factor structure of the ASI-3 in our community sample, and following the recommendations by Reise et al. (2010), we conducted five different confirmatory factor analysis (CFAs) that tested five respective theoretical models: (a) a one-dimensional model (all 18 items loading on a single factor); (b) a 2-factor “Physical concerns + Social and Cognitive concerns” correlated traits model; (c) a 2-factor “Physical and Cognitive concerns + Social concerns” correlated traits model; (d) a 3-factor “Physical, Social, and Cognitive concerns” correlated traits model; and (e) a bifactor model in which each of the 18 items is constrained to load on a general factor and on one out of the three (uncorrelated) domain-specific factors (see Figure 1). The solutions (b), (c), and (d) were tested since data from the current literature about the contribution of the ASI-3 subscales to the overall internal structure of the questionnaire are inconsistent. Please note that we decided not to test the fit of a second-order model (three dimensions plus a common higher-order AS factor) in light of the fact that this model would have produced identical fit to the three-factor correlated traits model (Brown, 2006).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Factor structure of the ASI-3.

As suggested by Rhemtulla et al. (2012), given that the data were ordinal and strongly skewed (see Table 1), the Weighted Least Squares Mean and Variance (WLSMV) robust estimator was employed in all CFAs. Assessment of the fit of each model was based on several indices. Since the χ2 statistic is extremely sensitive to sample size, two relative fit indices have been considered: the Tucker Lewis index (TLI) and the comparative fit index (CFI), as they both perform well with small and large samples. For these indices, values >0.95 and >0.97 are associated with acceptable and good fit, respectively (Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003). The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was also used. This is an absolute fit index assessing approximation of parameter estimates to true parameters in the population. RMSEA values <0.05 can be considered as a good fit, whereas values between 0.05 and 0.08 as an adequate fit (Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003). Models were compared according to multiple criteria. First, a qualitative evaluation of the fit indices of each model was considered. Second, the fit of the bifactor model relative to the four competing models was evaluated using scaled χ2 difference tests (Satorra, 2000) and the ΔCFI criterion (Cheung and Rensvold, 2002). Specifically, if the difference in the CFIs between two nested models (ΔCFI) is smaller than |0.01|, the hypothesis of no difference in fit between the two competing models should not be rejected and the more parsimonious model should be retained. Given that the scaled χ2 difference test is very sensitive to sample size, the interpretation of the results was primarily based on ΔCFI. In order to assess reliability, the Omega Hierarchical coefficient2 was calculated; standard convention for acceptable reliability is ω > 0.70 (Nunnally, 1978).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics for ASI-3 items (N = 1507).

As second step, following the Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analysis (MG-CFA) approach of Ebesutani et al. (2014), the measurement invariance of the best ASI-3 factor solution across gender was assessed. We first examined the fit of the single-sample best factor solution within the male and female samples separately. Next, we examined configural invariance across males and females. As configural invariance requires that both genders display the same number of factors as well as identical corresponding items, a baseline model implying the same factorial structure across gender was carried out. In this case, a good-fitting model suggests configural invariance. After testing configural invariance, we constrained item factor loadings and item thresholds to be equal across groups to simultaneously assess metric and scalar invariance (Muthén and Muthén, 2010). In this case, metric and scalar invariance imply that the meaning of the constructs (the factor loadings) and the levels of the underlying items are equal in both groups. Consequently, groups can be compared on their scores on the latent variables. The model implying metric and scalar invariance was evaluated according to 2 criteria: (a) a qualitative analysis of model fit indices and (b) the difference of CFI (ΔCFI) against the configural invariance model (Cheung and Rensvold, 2002). A t-test comparing males and females on the ASI total score was then performed.

All of the above-mentioned analyses were performed via the open-source software R (R Development Core Team, 2013). For structural equation models, the package lavaan (Rosseel, 2012) was used.

Product-moment correlations were performed to evaluate the temporal stability of the ASI-3 scores, as well as convergent and divergent validity, by means of associations between the ASI-3 scores and anxious (BAI score) and depressive (BDI-II score) symptoms. Partial correlations were utilized to establish the specificity of associations when controlling for worry (PSWQ score) and general distress (DASS-21 total score), as well as to test incremental validity of the ASI-3 Physical and Cognitive Concerns subscales (correlations between the ASI-3 Physical and Cognitive Concerns subscales and scores on the BAI and the PSWQ, respectively, controlling for the ASI-3 Total score). Overall, due to the large sample size, the results were interpreted using a significance level of 1% (Simmons et al., 2011).

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for the ASI-3 items in the whole sample. Overall, items were strongly skewed in the positive direction (i.e., low frequencies for high values of the ASI-3 scale). It is to note that the mean percentage of non-response per item was.13% (SD = 0.12). The three items with the highest percentage of non-response were item 3 (0.40%), item 5 (0.32%), and item 7 (0.26%). In light of these low percentages, it is possible to conclude that none of the ASI-3 items can be considered as problematic.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis Models

Table 2 reports fit indices of the bifactor model and all of the competing models. Notably, both the 3-factor correlated traits model and the bifactor model fitted the data well. Nonetheless, the bifactor model resulted in the best factor solution and provided a good fit to observed data [χ2(117, n = 1507) = 317.7, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.975; TLI = 0.968; RMSEA = 0.045]. Scaled χ2 difference tests showed that the bifactor model fitted significantly better than the one-dimensional model [χ2DIFF(18) = 415.0, p < 0.001], the 2-factor (“Physical concerns + Social and Cognitive concerns”) correlated traits model [χ2DIFF(17) = 184.0, p < 0.001], the 2-factor (“Physical and Cognitive concerns + Social concerns”) correlated traits model [χ2DIFF(17) = 237.0, p < 0.001], and the 3-factor correlated traits model [χ2DIFF(15) = 82.7, p < 0.001]. ΔCFIs between the bifactor model and the competing models also supported these results (all ΔCFIs were larger than 0.022, see Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Fit statistics for the confirmatory factor analysis models (N = 1507).

As shown in Table 3, all loadings associated with the general factor were significant at the 1% level (with all ps < 0.001) and had a satisfactory size. Loadings associated with specific group factors were characterized by a generally smaller size. In particular, four loadings associated with the specific group factor Cognitive Concerns were not statistically significant at the 1% level. Notably, Table 3 also shows that, as emerged in the study by Ebesutani et al. (2014), the decrease in the loadings on the specific factor after accounting for the general factor (i.e., the difference between loadings on the specific factor in the correlated 3-factor model vs. the bifactor model) was greater in the case of the Cognitive Concerns factor.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. The estimated bifactor model (N = 1507).

In terms of explained variance (see Reise et al., 2010), the general factor explained 33% of the total variance, whereas the Physical, Social, and Cognitive Concerns specific factors explained 8, 8, and 4%, respectively, with 47% error. Thus, the general factor accounted for nearly 62% of the common variance extracted. The Omega Hierarchical coefficient for the total score based on our bifactor solution was 0.79. The Omega Hierarchical coefficients for the Physical, Social, and Cognitive Concerns specific factors were 0.36, 0.46, and 0.07, respectively. Taken together, these results support the presence of a relatively strong general ASI-3 factor. In other words, if a composite were formed based on summing the ASI-3 items, we could conclude that 79% of the variance of this composite could be attributable to variance on the general factor (Reise et al., 2010).

Measurement Invariance of the ASI-3 Bifactor Model Across Gender

Before conducting the MGCFA for ordinal data to evaluate measurement invariance across gender, we examined the distribution of item scores separately for males and females. As for the whole sample (see Table 1), the distributions of item scores appeared strongly skewed in both groups. In particular, males did not use the rating “4” in five items (i.e., items 3, 4, 7, 10, and 14) and females did not use the rating “4” in two items (i.e., items 14 and 16; see Table 1). Since groups must have the same values on observed variables to perform a multi-group confirmatory analysis with ordinal data, we collapsed scores “3” and “4” into the same category, i.e., “3”.

All steps conducted to verify the measurement invariance of the bifactor model across gender are summarized in Table 4. First, single-sample solutions of the ASI-3 bifactor model fitted well in both males (N = 582) and females (N = 925). Next, our baseline model, which tested to verify the configural invariance of the bifactor model in the whole sample, showed a good fit to the observed data (CFI = 0.974, TLI = 0.966, RMSEA = 0.045). Thus, the same number of factors, reflected by the same set of indicators, was present across gender groups. Our test of equal factor loadings and equal item thresholds also supported invariance of these parameters across males and females, as evidenced by the good fit indices and a ΔCFI < 0.01 (see Table 4). None of the 18 ASI-3 items were associated with any differential item functioning across gender, allowing for meaningful and interpretable raw score comparisons across males and females.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Fit statistics for the bifactor model tested for invariance across gender.

In the present sample, a t-test comparing males and females on the ASI-3 total score showed a non-significant difference at the 1% level (MMales = 9.97, SDMales = 7.52, MFemales = 10.81, SDFemales = 7.64, t(1505) = -2.10, p = 0.036). Nonetheless, according to Cohen’s guidelines (Cohen, 1988), a difference between genders is present (higher scores in women than in men) although the magnitude the difference is relatively small (dCohen = -0.11)3.

Association of the ASI-3 Scores with Age and Education

Since, based on the CFAs, ASI-3 resulted in a relatively strong general factor, Pearson’s correlations were calculated only for the ASI-3 total score. In the present sample, neither age (r = 0.01) nor education (r = -0.03) was associated with the ASI-3 total score (all ps > 0.05).

Temporal Stability, Convergent and Divergent Validity, Incremental Validity

As stated above, Pearson’s correlations were calculated only for the ASI-3 total score. One-month test–retest reliability for the ASI-3 total score was high (Pearson’s r = 0.76; p < 0.001). In regards to convergent and divergent validities of the ASI-3 total score, it correlated positively and moderately both with the BAI (r = 0.41; p < 0.001) and the BDI-II (r = 0.37; p < 0.001). Importantly, only the association with the BAI remained significant after controlling for the PSWQ and for the DASS-21 total score (partial r = 0.31; p = 0.002), whereas the association with the BDI-II was no longer significant (partial r = 0.17; p = 0.10).

Lastly, the examination of the incremental utility of the ASI-3 subscales above and beyond the total score of the ASI-3 highlighted that the ASI-3 Physical Concerns subscale was not related to the BAI (r = -0.03, p = 0.22), whereas the ASI-3 Cognitive Concerns subscale was only weakly related, in terms of effect size, to the PSWQ (r = 0.16, p < 0.001) after controlling for the general ASI factor.

Discussion

The ASI-3 was developed to overcome many of the concerns that characterized its previous versions. It has proved to be a valuable and useful comprehensive measure of both clinical and sub-clinical AS symptoms. Indeed, several studies provided support for the ASI-3’s reliability and its convergent, discriminant, criterion-related, and construct validity (Sandin et al., 2007; Taylor et al., 2007; Escocard et al., 2009; Mantar et al., 2010; Osman et al., 2010; Kemper et al., 2012; Lim and Kim, 2012; Wheaton et al., 2012). Nonetheless, inconsistent data about its factor structure has emerged (Sandin et al., 2007; Taylor et al., 2007; Escocard et al., 2009; Kemper et al., 2009, 2012; Mantar et al., 2010; Osman et al., 2010; Lim and Kim, 2012; Ebesutani et al., 2014). As a consequence, the aim of the present study was to assess the factor structure and the psychometric properties of the ASI-3 when applied on a large Italian community sample. Given that environmental factors may vary across cultures, cross-cultural studies in AS manifestations and in instruments devised to assess AS are recommended. Indeed, even though the influence of genetic factors in the etiology of AS has been confirmed (Stein et al., 1999; Taylor et al., 2008), both empirical (Taylor et al., 2008) and retrospective (Stewart et al., 2001; Scher and Stein, 2003) studies have bolstered the role of environmental factors in AS development. For example, although a few studies found that AS is associated in the same way with anxiety and related disorders across socio-cultural contexts (Zvolensky et al., 2001, 2003; Bernstein et al., 2006; Taylor et al., 2007), symptom perception and expression may be affected by cultural variability (Kirmayer et al., 1995). Furthermore, employing a cross-culturally validated instrument allows to compare international research results and to perform international research projects (Van Widenfelt et al., 2005). Finally, the assessment of the ASI-3 factor structure across different cultures represents an important issue because when a measure is used in diverse cultures, it might be interpreted differently (Irvine and Carroll, 1980).

Five CFAs performed on an Italian community sample suggested that, although the original three-factor hierarchic structure (Taylor et al., 2007) evidenced good fit indices, the best factor solution was a bifactor model. Moreover, the general factor accounted for nearly 62% of the common variance extracted, and the Omega Hierarchical coefficient based on our bifactor solution was 0.79, which suggests the presence of a relatively strong general ASI-3 factor. The present findings are consistent with data by Osman et al. (2010) and Ebesutani et al. (2014) and suggest that the ASI-3 consists of a dominant general factor (i.e., general fear of anxiety-related sensations) and three specific, independent, and orthogonal factors. This might mean, as stated by Ebesutani et al. (2014), that “ […] the fear of anxiety in general and the fears of physical, cognitive, and social anxiety-related events appear to be distinct, unrelated fears (once accounting for the general AS factor; Ebesutani et al., 2014, p. 461)” and, consistently with Reise et al. (2010)’s considerations, encourages future research to further explore whether the three AS factors might be independent sub-systems associated to fear event processing. Thus, although the current results are not in contrast to the broadly accepted hierarchical model (i.e., a higher-order factor with three correlated lower-order factors) tested in clinical and community samples (Sandin et al., 2007; Taylor et al., 2007; Escocard et al., 2009; Kemper et al., 2009; Mantar et al., 2010; Lim and Kim, 2012; Wheaton et al., 2012), as a whole our results support the unidimensionality of the latent structure of the ASI-3 and that the general AS factor affects ASI-3 score variation more than the specific anxiety-related fear subscales; therefore, the use of the total score could be more appropriate and informative than calculating the three subscale scores separately. Further support to this argument is that the majority of the loadings associated with the specific Cognitive Concerns factor were not statistically significant (and also the Omega Hierarchical coefficient for the Cognitive Concerns factor was very low), whereas all loadings associated with the general factor were significant at p < 0.001 and had a satisfactory size. Interestingly, only two items on the Cognitive Concerns factor in the bifactor model showed satisfying loadings in the current Italian sample, specifically item 2 “When I cannot keep my mind on a task, I worry that I might be going crazy” and item 4 “It scares me when I am unable to keep my mind on a task,” both depicting the interference of anxiety on task execution; in other words, the negative impact of cognitive symptoms is concrete. Therefore, there might be a difference in how cognitive concerns present in the Italian culture as compared to U.S. culture (e.g., Osman et al., 2010; Ebesutani et al., 2014) and that the ASI-3 Cognitive Concerns items may not adequately capture this dimension of AS concerns. Present evidence overall suggest that the ASI-3 can be useful in research and clinical settings, as it represents a cost-effective instrument to evaluate general AS: As a matter of fact, the bifactor model allows a direct exploration of “the extent to which items reflect a common target trait and the extent to which they reflect a primary or subtrait” (Brown, 2006, p. 546).

A further purpose of the present study was to assess measurement invariance of the ASI-3 across gender. In our sample, the bifactor model evidenced equal form, factor loadings, and item thresholds across gender; moreover, none of the 18 ASI-3 items were associated with any differential item functioning across gender. This allowed for meaningful and interpretable raw score comparisons across gender, which prevented potential bias in the score interpretations. These findings confirm previous results from Ebesutani et al. (2014), revealing that scores from the bifactor model are invariant in terms of gender. A comparison analysis demonstrated the existence of small differences between males and females in the AS levels. Therefore, the ASI-3 seems to be quite sensitive to gender differences, in line with findings with a Spanish community sample which showed higher AS levels in women than men (Sandin et al., 2007) but in contrast with a number of other findings in literature which failed in detecting gender differences in the AS levels (e.g., Taylor et al., 2007; Escocard et al., 2009; Osman et al., 2010). From a clinical perspective this could be relevant: indeed, since the prevalence of anxiety disorders varies by gender (F > M), it is reasonable to hypothesize that AS, which is a factor that contributes to anxiety disorders development, could be higher in women than men. Furthermore, both age and education were not associated with the ASI-3 total score. The present results are in line with findings by Taylor et al. (2007), Escocard et al. (2009), and Osman et al. (2010) and further support the notion that the ASI-3 is insensitive to most socio-demographic variables: overall, these findings are consistent with our initial hypotheses. Mean total scores on the ASI-3 characterizing our community sample (females: M = 10.81, SD = 7.64; males: M = 9.97, SD = 7.52) are slightly lower than those observed in other non-clinical samples (e.g., Sandin et al., 2007; Taylor et al., 2007; Mantar et al., 2010; Osman et al., 2010; Lim and Kim, 2012; Wheaton et al., 2012; Ebesutani et al., 2014; mean values for the total score ranged between 10.7 ± 8.1 and 16.74 ± 11.03); to note, all previous studies were conducted on undergraduate samples, whereas our study sample was made up only in part of students (30.2%) and mainly of people from Italian general population.

Regarding psychometric properties, the Omega Hierarchical coefficient for the total score was 0.79, thus suggesting a good reliability for the general factor of the Italian ASI-3 (Nunnally, 1978); these results are consistent with those found by Osman et al. (2010) and Ebesutani et al. (2014). In addition, the 1-month test–retest reliability was good for the ASI-3 total score and in line with results from other cross-cultural studies (Sandin et al., 2007; Mantar et al., 2010). The convergent validity of the Italian version of the ASI-3 was adequate. Overall, the total score evidenced a pattern of specific associations with another anxiety symptom-related measure (i.e., the BAI). Regarding divergent validity, the ASI-3 total score was positively correlated with a measure of depression (i.e., the BDI-II). This result is not surprising, since it is well known that anxiety and depression are characterized by overlapping features and that individuals with depressive symptoms may show high levels of AS (i.e., Taylor et al., 1996; Cox et al., 2001; Armstrong et al., 2006). However, it is noteworthy that the correlations between the ASI-3 and the BAI remained significant even when the effect of worry (assessed by the PSWQ) and general distress (measured by the DASS-21) was controlled; on the other hand, the association between the ASI-3 and the BDI-II did not remain significant when controlling for the effect of worry and general distress after ASI total scores were controlled. Therefore, although there is some shared variability, results from partial correlations highlighted the specificity of the ASI-3 total score, indicating satisfactory convergent and divergent validity. Lastly, an in-depth investigation of the incremental validity of the ASI-3 Physical and Cognitive Concerns subscales above and beyond the total provided only partial support to our hypotheses. Indeed, only the ASI-3 Cognitive Concerns subscale showed an association, albeit weak, with a measure of worry; a similar finding was reported also by Ebesutani et al. (2014). On the other hand, the ASI-3 Physical Concerns subscale did not emerged to be associated with a measure of physiological anxiety. Such results provide further support to the notion that the general AS factor is the more informative score from the ASI-3 as previously observed by Ebesutani et al. (2014).

Some limits of the current study need to be pointed out. First, results from community members were based on a sample that may differ qualitatively from a clinical one; observed findings from the CFA may not be generalizable to patients with anxiety-related disorders and will require thoughtful interpretation. In addition, the recruitment process we employed might not allow one to consider our participants as an accurate representation of the Italian general population. However, we tried to reduce the impact of this limitation by recruiting a large number of participants. A further shortcoming that deserves to be mentioned refers to the small sample size of participants who completed the ASI-3 twice in order to test its temporal stability. Finally, only a few measures were employed to assess convergent, divergent, and incremental validity.

This study represents the first step for studying the bifactor structure of the ASI-3 in the Italian context; next steps will be conducting further studies in clinical Italian samples in order to assess the invariance of the scale structure according to clinical groups and to evaluate how AS is associated with different psychological disorders, as well as to better clarify whether and how the AS subdimensions are related to the general AS dimension. Moreover, additional studies are needed to address the sensitivity of the ASI-3 (as an outcome measure) to treatment effects when applied to Italian patients. Lastly, future study of additional properties of the bifactor model across cultural groups is encouraged.

Conclusion

The present study sheds light on the factor structure of the ASI-3 and gives robust evidence that the ASI-3 is a brief, reliable, and valid measure to evaluate AS in the Italian population. The present findings support suggestions by Osman et al. (2010) and Ebesutani et al. (2014) to employ the ASI-3 total score to evaluate the general fear of anxiety in both clinical and research settings.

Author Contributions

MG: Performed literature review and wrote the manuscript. GB: Performed literature review and contributed in writing up the manuscript. GA: Performed statistical analyses. ER: Performed data collection, data entry and support to data analyses. GM: Perfomed data collection. CS: Supervised the entire project.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ From the original 1617 participants, 27 (1.7%) participants were excluded due to presence of missing data on each item of the socio-demographic section and on each item of the ASI-3. Next, 49 (3%) participants were excluded due to incomplete values on the socio-demographic section schedule. Of the remaining 1541 participants, only 33 (2%) were excluded due to the presence of at least one missing answer on the ASI-3 items (31 participants with one missing value, and 2 participants with two missing values).
  2. ^ The Omega Hierarchical coefficient estimates the proportion of variance in raw scores attributable to a single general trait (i.e., general AS). The extent to which the ASI-3 scores reflect a single general dimension of AS common to all items is expressed by the Total Score Omega Hierarchical coefficient. The degree to which the subscale scores provide reliable variance after accounting for the general factor is expressed by the Omega Hierarchical coefficient for each subscale.
  3. ^ The same conclusions hold for the ASI-3 total score on the “collapsed” scale (i.e., 4 points from “0” to “3”): MMales = 9.87, SDMales = 7.41, MFemales = 10.67, SDFemales = 7.46, t(1505) = -2.04, p = 0.042, dCohen = -0.11.

References

Anestis, M. D., Holm-Denoma, J. M., Gordon, K. H., Schmidt, N. B., and Joiner, T. E. (2008). The role of anxiety sensitivity in eating pathology. Cogn. Ther. Res. 32, 370–385. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9085-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, K. A., Khawaja, N. G., and Oei, T. P. S. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis and psychometric properties of the anxiety sensitivity index - revised in clinical and normative populations. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 22, 116–125. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.22.2.116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asmundson, G. J. G. (1999). “Anxiety sensitivity and chronic pain: empirical findings, clinical implications, and future directions,” in Anxiety Sensitivity: Theory, Research and Treatment of the Fear of Anxiety, ed. S. Taylor (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 269–285.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Asmundson, G. J. G., Weeks, J. W., Carleton, N. R., Thibodeau, M. A., and Fetzner, M. G. (2011). Revisiting the latent structure of the anxiety sensitivity construct: more evidence of dimensionality. J. Anxiety Disord. 25, 138–147. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ayvaşɪk, H. B. (2000). Kaygɪ duyarlɪǧɪ indeksi: geçerlik ve güvenirlik çalışması. Türk. Psikol. Derg. 15, 43–57.

Google Scholar

Ball, S. G., Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., Uccello, R., and Rosenbaum, J. F. (1995). Differentiating social phobia and panic disorder: a test of core beliefs. Cogn. Ther. Res. 19, 473–482. doi: 10.1007/BF02230413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., and Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 56, 893–897. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Bernstein, A., and Zvolensky, M. J. (2007). Anxiety sensitivity: selective review of promising research and future directions. Exp. Rev. Neurother. 7, 97–101. doi: 10.1586/14737175.7.2.97

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, A., Zvolensky, M. J., Kotov, R., Arrindell, W. A., Taylor, S., Sandin, B., et al. (2006). Taxonicity of anxiety sensitivity: a multi-national analysis. J. Anxiety Disord. 20, 1–22. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blais, M. A., Otto, M. W., Zucker, B. G., McNally, R. J., Schmidt, N. B., Fava, M., et al. (2001). The anxiety sensitivity index: item analysis and suggestions for refinement. J. Pers. Assess. 77, 272–294. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA7702_10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bottesi, G., Ghisi, M., Altoè, G., Conforti, E., Melli, G., and Sica, C. (2015). The italian version of the depression anxiety stress scales-21: factor structure and psychometric properties on community and clinical samples. Compr. Psychiatry 60, 170–181. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouvard, M., Ayxères-Vighetto, A., Dupont, H., Aupetit, J., Portalier, S., and Arrindell, W. (2003). [Preliminary validation of the French translation of anxiety sensibility index-revised (ASI-R)]. L’Encéphale 29, 157–164.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Brislin, R. W. (1986). “The wording and translation of research instruments,” in Field Methods in Cross-Cultural Research. Cross-Cultural Research and Methodology Series, eds W. J. Lonner and J. W. Berry (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.), 137–164.

Google Scholar

Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Calamari, J. E., Rector, N. A., Woodard, J. L., Cohen, R. J., and Chik, H. M. (2008). Anxiety sensitivity and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Assessment 15, 351–363. doi: 10.1177/1073191107312611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calkins, A. W., Otto, M. W., Cohen, L. S., Soares, C. N., Vitonis, A. F., Hearon, B. A., et al. (2009). Psychosocial predictors of the onset of anxiety disorders in women: results from a prospective 3-year longitudinal study. J. Anxiety Disord. 23, 1165–1169. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.07.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, M. M., Sbrocco, T., and Ayati, F. (2009). Predicting anxious response to a social challenge and hyperventilation: comparison of the ASI and ASI-3. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 40, 434–442. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, G. W., and Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 9, 233–255. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, Y. R. (2004). Factor structure of the korean version of the anxiety sensitivity index: confirmatory evidence for a hierarchical model. Korean J. Clin. Psychol. 23, 207–220.

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd Edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., and Taylor, S. (2001). The effect of rumination as a mediator of elevated anxiety sensitivity in major depression. Cogn. Ther. Res. 25, 525–534. doi: 10.1023/A:1005580518671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, B. J., Parker, J. D. A., and Swinson, R. P. (1996). Anxiety sensitivity: confirmatory evidence for a multidimensional construct. Behav. Res. Ther. 34, 591–598. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(96)00006-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deacon, B., and Abramowitz, J. (2006). Anxiety sensitivity and its dimensions across the anxiety disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 20, 837–857. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deacon, B. J., Abramowitz, J. S., Woods, C. M., and Tolin, D. F. (2003). The anxiety sensitivity index - revised: psychometric properties and factor structure in two nonclinical samples. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 1427–1449. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00065-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deacon, B. J., and Valentiner, D. P. (2001). Dimensions of anxiety sensitivity and their relationship to nonclinical panic. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 23, 25–33. doi: 10.1023/A:1011087322899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebesutani, C., McLeish, A. C., Luberto, C. M., Young, J., and Maack, D. J. (2014). A bifactor model of anxiety sensitivity: analysis of the anxiety sensitivity index-3. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 36, 452–464. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013-9400-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Escocard, M. R. P. G., Fioravanti-Bastos, A. C. M., and Landeira-Fernandez, J. (2009). Anxiety sensitivity factor structure among brazilian patients with anxiety disorders. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 31, 246–255. doi: 10.1007/s10862-008-9103-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteve, M. R., and Camacho, L. (2008). Anxiety sensitivity, body vigilance and fear of pain. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 715–727. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldner, M. T., Zvolensky, M. J., Schmidt, N. B., and Smith, R. C. (2008). A prospective test of anxiety sensitivity as a moderator of the relation between gender and posttraumatic symptom maintenance among high anxiety sensitive young adults. Depress. Anxiety 25, 190–199. doi: 10.1002/da.20281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghisi, M., Flebus, G. B., Montano, A., Sanavio, E., and Sica, C. (2006). Beck Depression Inventory-II. Manuale. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.

Google Scholar

Irvine, S. H., and Carroll, W. K. (1980). “Testing and assessment across cultures,” in Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology, eds H. C. Triandis and J. W. Berry (Boston, MA: Allyn& Bacon).

Google Scholar

Joiner, T. E., Schmidt, N. B., Schmidt, K. L., Laurent, J., Catanzaro, S. J., Perez, M., et al. (2002). Anxiety sensitivity as a specific and unique marker of anxious symptoms in youth psychiatric inpatients. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 30, 167–175. doi: 10.1023/A:1014757300294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemper, C. J., Lutz, J., Bähr, T., Rddel, H., and Hock, M. (2012). Construct validity of the anxiety sensitivity index–3 in clinical samples. Assessment 19, 89–100. doi: 10.1177/1073191111429389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemper, C. J., Ziegler, M., and Taylor, S. (2009). Überprüfung der psychometrischen qualität der deutschen version des angstsensitivitätsindex-3. Diagnostica 55, 223–233. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.55.4.223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirmayer, L. J., Young, A., and Hayton, B. C. (1995). The cultural context of anxiety disorders. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 18, 503–521.

Google Scholar

Leen-Feldner, E. W., Feldner, M. T., Reardon, L. E., Babson, K. A., and Dixon, L. (2008). Anxiety sensitivity and posttraumatic stress among traumatic event-exposed youth. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 548–556. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W., and Zinbarg, R. E. (2007). Anxiety sensitivity and panic attacks: a 1-year longitudinal study. Behav. Modif. 31, 145–161. doi: 10.1177/0145445506296969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, Y.-J., and Kim, J.-H. (2012). Korean anxiety sensitivity index-3: its factor structure, reliability, and validity in non-clinical samples. Psychiatry Investig. 9, 45–53. doi: 10.4306/pi.2012.9.1.45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, Y.-J., Yu, B.-H., and Kim, J.-H. (2007). Korean anxiety sensitivity index—revised: its factor structure, reliability, and validity in clinical and nonclinical samples. Depress. Anxiety 24, 331–341. doi: 10.1002/da.20210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, S. H., and Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. Sydney: Psychology Foundation of Australia.

Google Scholar

Mantar, A., Yemez, B., and Alkin, T. (2010). The validity and reliability of the turkish version of the anxiety sensitivity index-3. Turk. J. Psychiatry 21, 225–234.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Marshall, G. N., Miles, J. N. V., and Stewart, S. H. (2010). Anxiety sensitivity and PTSD symptom severity are reciprocally related: evidence from a longitudinal study of physical trauma survivors. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 119, 143–150. doi: 10.1037/a0018009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNally, R. J. (2002). Anxiety sensitivity and panic disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 938–946. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01475-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., and Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behav. Res. Ther. 28, 487–495. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohlman, J., and Zinbarg, R. E. (2000). The structure and correlates of anxiety sensitivity in older adults. Psychol. Assess. 12, 440–446. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.12.4.440

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morani, S., Pricci, D., and Sanavio, E. (1999). Penn state worry questionnaire e worry domains questionnaire. Presentazione delle versioni italiane ed analisi della fedeltà. Psicoter. Cogn. E Comportamentale 5, 195–209.

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (2010). Mplus User’s Guide, 4th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Author.

Google Scholar

Naragon-Gainey, K. (2010). Meta-analysis of the relations of anxiety sensitivity to the depressive and anxiety disorders. Psychol. Bull. 136, 128–150. doi: 10.1037/a0018055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norton, G. R., Cox, B. J., Hewitt, P. L., and McLeod, L. (1997). Personality factors associated with generalized and non-generalized social anxiety. Pers. Individ. Dif. 22, 655–660. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00243-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noyes, R., Carney, C. P., and Langbehn, D. R. (2004). Specific phobia of illness: search for a new subtype. J. Anxiety Disord. 18, 531–545. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(03)00041-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric Theory. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Olatunji, B. O., Sawchuk, C. N., Deacon, B. J., Tolin, D. F., Lilienfeld, S. O., Williams, N. L., et al. (2005). The anxiety sensitivity profile revisited: factor structure and psychometric properties in two nonclinical samples. J. Anxiety Disord. 19, 603–625. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olatunji, B. O., and Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B. (2009). Anxiety sensitivity and the anxiety disorders: a meta-analytic review and synthesis. Psychol. Bull. 135, 974–999. doi: 10.1037/a0017428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olthuis, J. V., Watt, M. C., and Stewart, S. H. (2014). Anxiety sensitivity index (ASI-3) subscales predict unique variance in anxiety and depressive symptoms. J. Anxiety Disord. 28, 115–124. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osman, A., Gutierrez, P. M., Smith, K., Fang, Q., Lozano, G., and Devine, A. (2010). The anxiety sensitivity index–3: analyses of dimensions, reliability estimates, and correlates in nonclinical samples. J. Pers. Assess. 92, 45–52. doi: 10.1080/00223890903379332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., Fava, M., Uccello, R., and Rosenbaum, J. F. (1995). Elevated anxiety sensitivity index scores in patients with major depression: correlates and changes with antidepressant treatment. J. Anxiety Disord. 9, 117–123. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(94)00035-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, R. A., and Heilbronner, R. L. (1987). The anxiety sensitivity index: construct validity and factor analytic structure. J. Anxiety Disord. 1, 117–121. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(87)90002-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, R. A., and Plehn, K. (1999). “Measuring anxiety sensitivity,” in Anxiety Sensitivity: Theory, Research, and Treatment in the Fear of Anxiety, ed. S. Taylor (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 61–68.

Google Scholar

Peterson, R. A., and Reiss, S. (1992). Anxiety Sensitivity Index Manual, 2nd Edn. Worthington, OH: International Diagnostic Services.

Google Scholar

R Development Core Team (2013). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Available at: http://www.R-project.org/

Google Scholar

Rector, N. A., Szacun-Shimizu, K., and Leybman, M. (2007). Anxiety sensitivity within the anxiety disorders: disorder-specific sensitivities and depression comorbidity. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 1967–1975. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiss, S. (1991). Expectancy model of fear, anxiety, and panic. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 11, 141–153. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(91)90092-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiss, S., and McNally, R. J. (1985). “The expectancy model of fear,” in Theoretical Issues in Behavior Therapy, eds S. Reiss and R. R. Bootzin (New York, NY: Academic Press), 107–121.

Google Scholar

Reise, S. P., Moore, T. M., and Haviland, M. G. (2010). Bifactor models and rotations: exploring the extent to which multidimensional data yield univocal scale scores. J. Pers. Assess. 92, 544–559. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2010.496477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiss, S., Peterson, R. A., Gursky, D. M., and McNally, R. J. (1986). Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behav. Res. Ther. 24, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(86)90143-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. É., and Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychol. Methods 17, 354–373. doi: 10.1037/a0029315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: an r package for structural equation modeling - google scholar. J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandin, B., Chorot, P., and McNally, R. J. (1996). Validation of the spanish version of the anxiety sensitivity index in a clinical sample. Behav. Res. Ther. 34, 283–290.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sandin, B., García, R. M. V., Chorot, P., and Germán, M. A. S. (2007). ASI-3: nueva escala para la evaluación de la sensibilidad a la ansiedad. Rev. Psicopatol. Psicol. Clín. 12, 91–104.

Satorra, A. (2000). “Scaled and adjusted restricted tests in multi-sample analysis of moment structures,” in Innovations in Multivariate Statistical Analysis Advanced Studies in Theoretical and Applied Econometrics, eds R. D. H. Heijmans, D. S. G. Pollock, and A. Satorra (New York, NY: Springer), 233–247.

Google Scholar

Scher, C. D., and Stein, M. B. (2003). Developmental antecedents of anxiety sensitivity. J. Anxiety Disord. 17, 253–269. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00202-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., and Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptivegoodness-of-fitmeasures. Methods Psychol. Res. Online 8, 23–74.

Google Scholar

Schmidt, N. B., Keough, M. E., Mitchell, M. A., Reynolds, E. K., MacPherson, L., Zvolensky, M. J., et al. (2010). Anxiety sensitivity: prospective prediction of anxiety among early adolescents. J. Anxiety Disord. 24, 503–508. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, N. B., Keough, M. E., Timpano, K. R., and Richey, J. A. (2008). Anxiety sensitivity profile: predictive and incremental validity. J. Anxiety Disord. 22, 1180–1189. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, N. B., Zvolensky, M. J., and Maner, J. K. (2006). Anxiety sensitivity: prospective prediction of panic attacks and Axis I pathology. J. Psychiatr. Res. 40, 691–699. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sica, C., Coradeschi, D., Ghisi, M., and Sanavio, E. (2006). Beck Anxiety Inventory. Manuale. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.

Sica, C., and Ghisi, M. (2007). “The italian versions of the beck anxiety inventory and the beck depression inventory-ii: psychometric properties and discriminant power,” in Leading-Edge Psychological Tests and Testing, ed. M. A. Lange (Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers), 27–50.

Google Scholar

Silverman, W. K., Goedhart, A. W., Barrett, P., and Turner, C. (2003). The facets of anxiety sensitivity represented in the childhood anxiety sensitivity index: confirmatory analyses of factor models from past studies. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 112, 364–374. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., and Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1359–1366. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, M. B., Jang, K. L., and Livesley, W. J. (1999). Heritability of anxiety sensitivity: a twin study. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 246–251. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.2.246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, S. H., Samoluk, S. B., MacDonald, A. B., and Taylor, S. (1999). “Anxiety sensitivity and substance use and abuse,” in Anxiety Sensitivity: Theory, Research and Treatment of the Fear of Anxiety, ed. S. Taylor (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 287–319.

Google Scholar

Stewart, S. H., Taylor, S., Jang, K. L., Cox, B. J., Watt, M. C., Fedoroff, I. C., et al. (2001). Causal modeling of relations among learning history, anxiety sensitivity, and panic attacks. Behav. Res. Ther. 39, 443–456. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00023-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. (1999). Anxiety Sensitivity: Theory, Research, and Treatment of the Fear of Anxiety. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Taylor, S., and Cox, B. J. (1998a). An expanded anxiety sensitivity index: evidence for a hierarchic structure in a clinical sample. J. Anxiety Disord. 12, 463–483. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(98)00028-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S., and Cox, B. J. (1998b). Anxiety sensitivity: multiple dimensions and hierarchic structure. Behav. Res. Ther. 36, 37–51. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00071-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S., Jang, K. L., Stewart, S. H., and Stein, M. B. (2008). Etiology of the dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: a behavioral–genetic analysis. J. Anxiety Disord. 22, 899–914. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S., Koch, W. J., and Crockett, D. J. (1991). Anxiety sensitivity, trait anxiety, and the anxiety disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 5, 293–311. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(91)90030-W

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S., Koch, W. J., McNally, R. J., and Crockett, D. J. (1992). Conceptualizations of anxiety sensitivity. Psychol. Assess. 4, 245–250. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.4.2.245

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S., Koch, W. J., Woody, S., and McLean, P. (1996). Anxiety sensitivity and depression: how are they related? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 105, 474–479. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.3.474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S., Zvolensky, M. J., Cox, B. J., Deacon, B., Heimberg, R. G., Ledley, D. R., et al. (2007). Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initial validation of the anxiety sensitivity index-3. Psychol. Assess. 19, 176–188. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.2.176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Telch, M. J., Shermis, M. D., and Lucas, J. A. (1989). Anxiety sensitivity: unitary personality trait or domain-specific appraisals? J. Anxiety Disord. 3, 25–32. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(89)90026-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Widenfelt, B. M., Treffers, P. D. A., Beurs, E., de Siebelink, B. M., and Koudijs, E. (2005). Translation and cross-cultural adaptation of assessment instruments used in psychological research with children and families. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 8, 135–147. doi: 10.1007/s10567-005-4752-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wardle, J., Ahmad, T., and Hayward, P. (1990). Anxiety sensitivity in agoraphobia. J. Anxiety Disord. 4, 325–333. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(90)90029-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watt, M. C., and Stewart, S. H. (2000). Anxiety sensitivity mediates the relationships between childhood learning experiences and elevated hypochondriacal concerns in young adulthood. J. Psychosom. Res. 49, 107–118. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(00)00097-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheaton, M. G., Deacon, B. J., McGrath, P. B., Berman, N. C., and Abramowitz, J. S. (2012). Dimensions of anxiety sensitivity in the anxiety disorders: evaluation of the ASI-3. J. Anxiety Disord. 26, 401–408. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, K. S., Brown, T. A., Somers, T. J., and Barlow, D. H. (2006). Avoidance behavior in panic disorder: the moderating influence of perceived control. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 147–157. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Won, H. T., Park, H. S., and Kwon, S. M. (1995). A study on the development of the Korean versions of Panic Scales. Korean J. Clin. Psychol. 14, 95–110.

Google Scholar

Zinbarg, R. E., Barlow, D. H., and Brown, T. A. (1997). Hierarchical structure and general factor saturation of the anxiety sensitivity index: evidence and implications. Psychol. Assess. 9, 277–284. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.9.3.277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zvolensky, M. J., Arrindell, W. A., Taylor, S., Bouvard, M., Cox, B. J., Stewart, S. H., et al. (2003). Anxiety sensitivity in six countries. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 841–859. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00187-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zvolensky, M. J., Feldner, M. T., Eifert, G. H., and Stewart, S. H. (2001). Evaluating differential predictions of emotional reactivity during repeated 20% carbon dioxide-enriched air challenge. Cogn. Emot. 15, 767–786. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zvolensky, M. J., and Forsyth, J. P. (2002). Anxiety sensitivity dimensions in the prediction of body vigilance and emotional avoidance. Cogn. Ther. Res. 26, 449–460. doi: 10.1023/A:1016223716132

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3, Italian community sample, factorial structure, bifactor model, psychometric properties

Citation: Ghisi M, Bottesi G, Altoè G, Razzetti E, Melli G and Sica C (2016) Factor Structure and Psychometric Properties of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 in an Italian Community Sample. Front. Psychol. 7:160. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00160

Received: 02 November 2015; Accepted: 28 January 2016;
Published: 16 February 2016.

Edited by:

Pietro Cipresso, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Italy

Reviewed by:

Sherry Stewart, Dalhousie University, Canada
Andreas Dinkel, Klinikum rechts der Isar, Technische Universität München, Germany

Copyright © 2016 Ghisi, Bottesi, Altoè, Razzetti, Melli and Sica. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marta Ghisi, marta.ghisi@unipd.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.