Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 13 April 2016
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Who Is Making The Decision Matters – Conceptual, Methodological, Empirical and Applied Approaches To Individual Differences in Decision Making View all 8 articles

Crucial Dimensions of Human Altruism. Affective vs. Conceptual Factors Leading to Helping or Reinforcing Others

  • Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland

The aim of this article is to identify factors leading to favorable attitudes toward other people from different social categories. The parts of article reflect diverse levels of altruism regulation from primary affective responses to the environment, through social norms, to abstract moral concepts related to good and evil. The latter allow understanding of the perspective of other people (including those belonging to out-groups), acceptance of their values and engagement not only in helping behavior but also in supporting the development of others.

The Many Facets of Altruism

When he first used the term “altruism” in a scientific context, August Comte could not have predicted the staggering career of the concept in humanities, as well as social and natural science, or the myriad alternative meanings it would spawn. For Comte, altruism denoted a type of social behavior expressed through living selflessly for the sake of others (Campel, 2006).

Today, the “well-being of others” remains the defining criterion of altruism, but the question of selflessness is debated. The dispute whether non-egoistic behavior is possible at all is the bone of contention (Cf. the Cialdini-Batson argument). But even the proponents of the “egoistic” view are using the term (Cialdini et al., 1981). One attempt to resolve the dilemma of selflessness was to look at costs, which are sometimes even interpreted as losses (Hamilton, 1964). The point is that the altruistic actor incurs significant, personal damage when acting for the sake of others. The import of these costs is supposed to invalidate suspected “egoistic” motivation for altruism thus defined. Nevertheless, with the exception of kin altruism (where the stakes can be as high as life or health), reciprocal altruism demands smaller sacrifices, being decidedly less spectacular (as clearly explained by Wilson, 1975). A more inclusive (Tooby and Cosmides, 1996) definition of altruism refers to all manner of actions for the benefit of others, some of which do not necessarily compromise the benefactor’s interests. It is manifested in assistance that carries no costs, in forging transient alliances, cooperation, sharing something of value with others, offering (anonymously) something.

Altruism is not limited to helping the disadvantaged. Other actions that tend to be equated with altruism include expressing positive emotions and empathy. Altruistic motivation is associated with commitment to values aimed at serving the best interest of others (Reykowski, 1989). The concept of altruism is so broad as to include species at the lower rungs of the evolutionary ladder. Some forms of cooperation in social insects as well as birds, are considered the most basic forms of altruism (Wilson, 1975).

The present paper deals with the broad definition of altruism, understood as the dispositional willingness to respond with positive emotions to others and to treat others, including strangers, sympathetically. Such sympathy not always translates to behavioral acts (this is the difference between the concept of “altruism” and that of “prosocial behavior”).

The purpose of the article is to present various possible internal dispositions not only innate to particular species or internalized during the course of socialization, but also shaped through values generated reflexively by subjects and standards determining the criteria of good and evil for others, the world, and even future generations. The paper focus on mechanisms forming direct or indirect premises of altruism.

Reactive Nature of the Premises of Altruism: Primary, Automatic Interrelations with Others

The primary determinants of altruism are innate and universal and have been shaped by evolution (Wilson, 1975; Burnstein, 2005) and early childhood experiences (Warneken and Tomasello, 2006). Our insight into their specifics stems from developmental data and embodiment research. The basic ability to focus attention on others is reflected in automatically following the movement of another person’s eyes. Homo sapiens is the only species to have evolved gaze direction to be easily perceived by others (due to highly visible, white sclera) and only humans (already in infancy) use that information, which, in turn, facilitates the recognition of another person’s perspective – a skill important for the activation of a person’s altruistic engagement (Tomasello, 2014). Data reveal that early, non-verbal, bodily exchange, and communication lead to strong connections between the child and the environment. The abilities to mimic and to have synthonic reactions to the affective states of others have a special importance in the course a lifetime. The neural basis for these processes is known as “mirror neurons” (Iacoboni, 2005).

Mimicry as an automatic imitation of a partner’s behavior (this is known as “social glue”) connects people. Empirical findings have confirmed that mimicry leads to favorable treatment of the mimicker [e.g., giving larger tips to barmen who mimic patrons’ language and facial expressions: (van Baaren et al., 2003)]. The mechanism is reciprocal: the mimicker is more positively inclined toward the person mimicked. Mimicry increases the sense of interpersonal closeness, reciprocal similarity, and facilitates the flow of interaction. Mimicry increases the willingness to help, e.g., to spontaneously pick up coins dropped “by accident”. Help was offered both to the mimicker and to bystanders. One of the most common ways to perform an altruistic act is to donate to charity, either with your time, your money, or your material goods. The people being mimicked were more likely to donate to charities (van Baaren et al., 2004). Further experiments (Stel et al., 2008) have demonstrated the same mechanism for mimickers. In our studies we noticed increased donations to charity accompanied mimicry, even when the mimicker was in no way associated with the collection (Szuster, 2005).

Peripheral mechanisms triggered automatically during interaction also allow us to decode others’ facial expressions and to communicate our own affective states (Dimberg et al., 2000). The primary, automatic mechanism of affective contagion (e.g., as in the experiment where infants exposed to crying responded by crying themselves – see Simner, 1971) is considered to be the base for subsequent emotional empathy. It is reactive, instinctive and it triggers a circular reaction (Hoffman, 2000), in which a person’s own state is no longer distinguishable from the suffering of another person. Empathy, based on the affective mechanism (Hoffman, 1975, 2000), results from the ability to respond automatically to other people expressing emotions. Its distinctive feature is the psychological separation of the perceiver and recipient of someone’s emotion from the individual experiencing that emotion. The active nature of this process creates a new quality of altruism responding, introducing certain “filters” between one’s own and other people’s emotional states (Baron-Cohen, 2011) and the harm and well-being of others is not equivalent to one’s own.

Numerous data confirm the relationship between empathy and altruism, cooperation and just distribution of goods (Hoffman, 1975, 2000; Eisenberg and Morris, 2001). Priming with empathy was shown to increase sensitivity to the needs of others and to promote positive attitudes toward members of stigmatized groups: AIDS sufferers, homeless people, criminals (Batson, 1997) and minorities (Vescio et al., 2003). Our own findings on cyberbullying supported the effectiveness of empathy activation in virtual reality. Priming with empathy reduced the frequency by adolescents sending comments that compromised their peers. However, these effects were transient. Empathy was successful in reducing cyberbullying only when triggered immediately before the measurement of the behavior (Barlińska et al., 2013).

Empathy does not always strengthen altruistic behaviors. This is particularly true of the affective empathy. Discomfort generated as a reaction to the suffering of another person produces the reaction of avoidance, depreciation of the other person (the concept of emphatic anger – Hoffman, 2000) and even an attack or an act of aggression. The latter occurs when the sharing of emotions concerns aggression. In short, the specifics of the primary mechanisms of altruism are their reflexive, involuntary, and automatic nature. These mechanisms have limitations. They require direct contact. However, the memory of such experiences enables individuals (through mental images) to also respond with empathy to other persons, irrespective of how distant in space and time they may be (Hoffman, 2000).

Socialization as the Fundament of Normative Altruism

The external world triggers reflexive approach or avoidance responses. Seeking interaction with others is fostered by the biological craving for sensory stimulation, without which it is impossible to maintain homeostasis. Mere exposure to neutral stimuli (i.e., those that carry no threat) later trigger elementary positive responses (Zajonc, 1968). Lasting contact encourages increasing familiarity with the environment and enhanced involuntary attachment. However, the main social foundations of altruism are rooted in seeking contact with others in order to obtain reinforcement for the purposes of safety, the sense of belonging and affiliation (Schachter, 1959; Baumeister and Leary, 1995). A characteristic aspect for this category of altruism is that the individual’s responses oriented towards other people are instrumental to that subject’s own needs. Reinforcements provide a platform for creating a psychological dependence, which induces the subject to meet external expectations (despite punishment) and to conform to social norms including those that require altruism and condemn egoism.

Social norms are instilled through the educational process, whereby social actors (parents, teachers, peers, in-group members) verbalize their expectations toward individuals. However, socialization is also affected by a wide variety of non-verbal factors operating in the social environment (Hoffman, 2000). They include ways of responding to others, feeling sympathetic, or antipathetic toward them. These messages may take the form of microexpressions, and other subtle reflections of behavior (e.g., distance, physical, and psychological closeness). Thus, some attitudes, both explicit, and implicit are formed as a result of socialization (Greenwald and Banaji, 1995). The environment offers behavioral patterns (algorithms) approved in a given community and the culture prevalent in that community. The subject repeats them with no need for verbalization. Thus morality, including the imperative of being helpful, can have an intuitive nature, associated with the sense of good without the ability to understand the essence of the principles of acting properly (Jarymowicz, 2015). Gradually, the range of individual attitudes depends on what is typical of the groups one belongs to. Younger children spontaneously express aversion to others, while older children try to hide it (cf. the Pollyanna hypothesis: Drozda-Senkowska, 1990). Internalized social norms become the source for standards of “normal states” (Reykowski, 1989). Failure to meet them becomes a reason for internal punishments. More or less conscious realization that helping others is highly valued in society makes that strategy a norm whose fulfillment provides gratification and maintains or enhances positive mood, and reduces the negative one. A series of experiments proved that following the helpfulness norm can be instrumental in the affective well-being of the individual (Schwartz, 1977; Staub, 1979; Cialdini et al., 1981). This, in turn, perpetuates the motivation to engage in helping others.

Social norms, associated (unconsciously or reflexively) with self-esteem become personal, which significantly enhances their control over behavior (Schwartz, 1977). There have been numerous empirical findings confirming the regulatory role of self-esteem in inspiring altruistic behavior (Berkowitz and Daniels, 1963; Karyłowski, 1982; Schwartz and Howard, 1982). This rule also concerns volunteers who, apart from having higher self-esteem, are happier, more optimistic and generally more content with their lives (Piliavin, 2003).

People are usually not consciously aware of these relationships. Breaking a norm (punishing partner’s mistakes with electric shocks in an experiment, or lying) increased helpful behavior in the next task both towards the same person in a different context (Berkowitz, 1972). In our research, feedback about participants’ high social competence increased the amount of memorized data about a person in need and the number of ideas generated for helping (Szuster, 2005).

In summary: in the course of social development, human behaviors, including the ones which do not benefit the helper, are regulated by certain norms. People learn quickly that helping others is a highly regarded value. Compliance of behavior with the norm stabilizes affective well-being and is also a means of maintaining a positive self-image.

Concepts, Reflections, and Moral Values as Bases for Altruism

As a result of socialization, the individual gains knowledge (intuitive or verbalized) of what is right and wrong – in terms of culture-specific prescriptions. The in-group standard contents of norms such as social responsibility (Berkowitz and Daniels, 1963), sharing and giving, justice or reciprocity (Walster et al., 1978) are all the factors leading to altruism. Their regulatory function is associated with anticipated reward or punishment (sometimes as delayed as salvation after death as a result of living according to religious teachings). Social approval or disapproval is the main basis of altruistic acts.

Such normative altruism is often limited to the norms of one’s reference group, which may be the source of altruism toward the in-group members, but at the same time a source of prejudice and hostility toward the out-group. The understanding of norms may be limited to one’s environment. In the experiments of Piliavin (see: Reykowski, 1979), donations to single mothers decreased as a function of physical distance: the most generous donations were given to mothers from the same town, the lowest to those living in another state. However, even a much smaller distance can mean that help is not extended to the person in need lying on the other side of the street (Staub, 1974).

Intellectual development contributes to increased cognitive complexity including self and social awareness (Piaget, 1965). The result is an improved understanding of the world and a multiplicity of perspectives. Intellectual skills pave the way for decentration: changing the cognitive perspective from egocentric to external. This, in turn, may lead to voluntary shifting and focusing of attention, and to conscious separation of one’s own and other people’s perspectives. Whether developmental or situational, shortage of decentration leads to egocentric biases in the perception of others and the world. The ability to take the perspective of others significantly increases the potential of human altruism (Batson et al., 1997). Understanding of social norms changes and the subject gradually becomes independent of algorithms imposed by the environment. Filled with individual content and bound to the Self, norms present one of the most common sources of Self standards: Ought or Ideal (Schwartz, 1977; Higgins, 1987). This leads to the emergence of new, internal imperatives for altruistic behavior.

Some mechanisms regulating altruistic behavior are stimulated when the so-called Ought Self standards (Higgins, 1987) are activated. In many cultures, obligations are limited to the requirement of helping others in trouble. This category of mechanisms should be treated separately from those that lead to supporting others in their development and improving their quality of life. They key to their comprehension appears to be the concept of the Ideal Self, related to moral values. While social norms and their internalization (in the form of the Ought Self standards) are essentially “local” and promote altruism predominantly toward the in-group, moral values are universal, and, as such, extend the scope of altruism to out-groups. When intellectual development allows reflective thinking and understanding of moral values, the subject becomes able to perceive the state of another person (regardless of social status and own interests). Cognitive complexity, and cognitive categories width (Pettigrew, 1979) facilitate openness to others, reduce prejudice, and promote positive attitudes. Jarymowicz (2015) argues that cognitive complexity becomes the determinant of a truly moral behavior if it leads to the so-called axiological complexity that is the understanding of the sense of such abstract concepts as human rights or social justice. The understanding of values expressed in abstract language requires the involvement of the intellect in reflective thinking. Once that is achieved, the standards of ideal states become the point of reference for the states of others, triggering altruistic motivation, and others are perceived as autonomous individuals having a value in and of themselves (Reykowski, 1979; Rutkowska and Szuster, 2011). Bolstered by ideals, altruistic involvement goes beyond restoring what a given society considers a balance (relieving hardship) and opens up to supporting development and expanding other people’s opportunities.

In short: the system of reflective thinking and evaluation alters the quality of human functioning (Deutsch and Strack, 2006; Jarymowicz and Imbir, 2015) with regard to understanding the meaning of abstract concepts referring to moral values in one’s own life. It increases the accessibility of various criteria of evaluating the situation of others, including what is generally understood as goodness. This, in turn, can cause the subject to get engaged more involved in further altruistic deeds.

Conclusion

The article presents three group altruism-building mechanisms and focuses on the role of their biological, social and personal (conceptive, reflective) factors. They engage the body, as well as various parts of the human mind. Each type of mechanism generates reactions of a different nature.

The first basic mechanisms yield reactive and automatic reactions. These are universal, bodily mechanisms, which are the very basis of every type of reaction. Individual differentiation among them is slight. They constitute the “manufacturer’s equipment” of the person.

The second type of mechanism is the result of the process of learning the patterns of behaviors and understanding norms. They are the source of one’s engagement mostly in the well-being of one’s “in-group” members.

The last type of mechanism represents the outcome of intellectual involvement and reflective thinking. They lead to forming abstract evaluative concepts, which give rise to the emergence of entirely new standards of world evaluation.

The evolutionary processes to which man is subject are the very source of the mechanisms’ diversity. In the course of evolution, human beings have been equipped with automatic, affective mechanisms allowing them to approach others. This is the basis of altruism. However, the specificity of human altruism depends on the development of concepts enabling one to transcend the face-to-face context, to understand the perspective of others and to accept their values. Also, through intellect, anticipation of the interests of future generation is feasible. The differentiation between ought and ideal evaluative standards creates a chance to become involved in the development of another person, and not only to be content with charitable acts.

The knowledge about the sources of altruism mentioned above falls within the domain of biology and psychology alike. However, the emerging theories should not be treated as competitive. Described in terms of various scientific disciplines, the sources of altruism should be regarded as complementary theories aimed as explaining diverse forms of engagement in helping others. From the psychological point of view such an approach to altruism could be viewed as consistent with the dual mind theories developed by many researchers (Deutsch and Strack, 2006; Jarymowicz and Imbir, 2015).

Author Contributions

My role in creating the manuscript included: (1) Substantial contributions to the conception or design of the work; and (2) Drafting the work or revising it critically for important intellectual content; and (3) Final approval of the version to be published; and (4) Agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Barlińska, J., Szuster, A., and Winiewski, M. (2013). Cyberbullying among adolescent bystanders: role of the communication medium, form of violence and empathy. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 23, 37–51. doi: 10.1002/casp.2137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). Zero Degrees of Empathy. London: Penguin Group.

Google Scholar

Batson, C. D. (1997). Self-other merging and empathy-altruism hypothesis: replay to Neurberg et al. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73, 517–522. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.517

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, D., Early, S., and Salvarani, G. (1997). Perspective taking: imagining how another feels vs. imagining how you would feel. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 23, 751–758. doi: 10.1177/0146167297237008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkowitz, L. (1972). Social norms, feelings, and other factors affecting helping and altruism. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 6, 63–108. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60025-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkowitz, L., and Daniels, L. R. (1963). Responsibility and dependency. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 66, 429–437. doi: 10.1037/h0049250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burnstein, E. (2005). “Altruism and genetic relatedness,” in The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology ed. D. M. Buss (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.), 528–551.

Google Scholar

Campel, R. L. (2006). Altruism in auguste comte and ayn rand. J. Ayn Rand Stud. 7, 357–369.

Google Scholar

Cialdini, R. B., Baumann, D. J., and Kenrick, D. T. (1981). Insights from sadness: a three-step model of the development of altruism as hedonism. Dev. Rev. 1, 207–223. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(81)90018-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deutsch, R., and Strack, F. (2006). Duality models in social psychology: from dual processes to interacting systems. Psychol. Inq. 17, 166–172. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1703_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., and Elmehed, K. (2000). Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expression. Psychol. Sci. 11, 86–89. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drozda-Senkowska, E. (1990). Zjawisko Pollyanny w ocenach społecznych dzieci. Przegląd Psychologiczny 33, 183–192.

Eisenberg, N., and Morris, A. S. (2001). The origin and social significance of empathy- related responding. Soc. Justice 14, 94–120.

Greenwald, A. G., and Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol. Rev. 102, 4–27. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 1–52. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(64)90039-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. Psychol. Rev. 94, 319–340. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.3.319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, M. (1975). Developmental synthesis of affect and cognition and its implications for the altruistic motivation. Dev. Psychol. 11, 607–622. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.11.5.607

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, M. (2000). Empathy and Moral Development. Implication for Caring and Justice. Cambridge: University Press.

Google Scholar

Iacoboni, M. (2005). “Understanding others: imitation, language, empathy,” in Perspectives on Imitation: From Cognitive Neuroscience to Social Science, eds S. Hurley and N. Chater (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 77–99.

Google Scholar

Jarymowicz, M. (2015). “Fear and hope in intractable conflicts: automatic vs. reflective attributes of collective emotional orientations,” in The Social Psychology of Intractable Conflicts. Celebrating the Legacy of Daniel Bar-Tal eds E. Halperin and K. Sharvit (Berlin: Springer).

Google Scholar

Jarymowicz, M., and Imbir, K. (2015). Towards a human emotions taxonomy (based on their automatic vs. reflective origin). Emot. Rev. 7, 183–188. doi: 10.1177/1754073914555923

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karyłowski, J. (1982). “Two types of altruistic behavior: doing good to feel good or to make the other feel good,” in Cooperation and Helping Behavior Theories and Research, eds V. Derlega and J. Grzelak (New York, NY: Academic Press), 387–396.

Google Scholar

Pettigrew, T. F. (1979). The ultimate attribution terror: extending cognitive analysis of prejudice. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 5, 461–476. doi: 10.1177/014616727900500407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piaget, J. (1965). The Moral Judgment of the Child. New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Piliavin, J. (2003). “Doing well by doing good: benefits for the benefactor,” in Flourishing: The Positive Psychology and The Life Well Lived, eds C. L. M. Keyes and J. Haidt (Washington, DC: APA), 227–247.

Google Scholar

Reykowski, J. (1979). Motywacja, postawy prospołeczne a osobowość. Warszawa: Państwowe Wydawnictwo Naukowe.

Google Scholar

Reykowski, J. (1989). “Dimension of development in moral values,” in Social and Moral Values, eds J. Reykowski, N. Eisenberg, and E. Staub (Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 33–58.

Rutkowska, D., and Szuster, A. (2011). Choosing an interaction partner: the role of the representations of other people and coding systems in the preference pattern for the selection criteria. Stud. Psychol. 53, 185–199.

Google Scholar

Schachter, S. (1959). Psychology of affiliation. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.

Google Scholar

Schwartz, S., and Howard, J. A. (1982). “Internalized values as motivators of altruism,” in Development and Maintenance of Prosocial Behavior: International Perspectives, eds D. Bar-Tal, J. Karyłowski, J. Reykowski, and E. Staub (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 229–255.

Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H. (1977). “Normative influences on altruism,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 10, ed. L. Berkowitz (New York, NY: Random House), 221–279.

Google Scholar

Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn’s response to the cry of another infant. Dev. Psychol. 5, 136–150. doi: 10.1037/h0031066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staub, E. (1974). “Helping a distressed person: social, personality and stimulus determinants,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 7, ed. L. Berkowitz (New York: Academic Press), 293–341.

Google Scholar

Staub, E. (1979). Positive Social Behavior and Morality. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Stel, M., van Baaren, R. B., and Vonk, R. (2008). Effects of mimicking: acting prosocially by being emotionally moved. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 965–976. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.472

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szuster, A. (2005). W poszukiwaniu źródeł i uwarunkowań ludzkiego altruizmu. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Instytutu PAN.

Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (2014). A Natural History of Human Thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. (1996). Friendship and the banker,s paradox: other patwaysto the evolution of adaptaions for altruism. Proc. Br. Acad. 88, 119–143.

Google Scholar

van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami, K., and Van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychol. Sci. 15, 71–74. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501012.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Steenaert, B., and van Knippenberg, A. (2003). Mimicry for money: behavioral consequences of imitation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 39, 393–398. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00014-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vescio, T. K., Sechrist, G. B., and Paolucci, M. P. (2003). Perspective taking and prejudice reduction: the mediatonal role of empathy arousal and situational attributions. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 33, 455–472. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walster, E., Walster, G. W., and Berscheid, E. (1978). Equity: Theory and Research. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

Google Scholar

Warneken, F., and Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science 31, 1301–1303. doi: 10.1126/science.1121448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology. The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harward University Press.

Google Scholar

Zajonc, R. (1968). Attitudinal effect of mere exposure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 9, 1–27. doi: 10.1037/h0025848

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: automatic interactions, affiliation, socialization, normative vs. moral standards

Citation: Szuster A (2016) Crucial Dimensions of Human Altruism. Affective vs. Conceptual Factors Leading to Helping or Reinforcing Others. Front. Psychol. 7:519. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00519

Received: 17 November 2015; Accepted: 29 March 2016;
Published: 13 April 2016.

Edited by:

Richard Rende, Social Behavioral Research Applications, USA

Reviewed by:

Jennifer Joy-Gaba, Virginia Commonwealth University, USA
Mark Hallahan, College of the Holy Cross, USA

Copyright © 2016 Szuster. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anna Szuster, hanka@psych.uw.edu.pl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.