Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 August 2016
Sec. Psychopathology

The Origins of Mental Toughness – Prosocial Behavior and Low Internalizing and Externalizing Problems at Age 5 Predict Higher Mental Toughness Scores at Age 14

\r\nDena Sadeghi Bahmani*Dena Sadeghi Bahmani1*Martin HatzingerMartin Hatzinger2Markus GerberMarkus Gerber3Sakari LemolaSakari Lemola4Peter J. CloughPeter J. Clough5Sonja PerrenSonja Perren6Kay von KlitzingKay von Klitzing7Agnes von WylAgnes von Wyl8Edith Holsboer-TrachslerEdith Holsboer-Trachsler1Serge Brand,*Serge Brand1,3*
  • 1Center for Affective, Stress and Sleep Disorders, Psychiatric Clinics of the University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Adult Psychiatry, Psychiatric Services Solothurn, Solothurn, Switzerland
  • 3Department of Sport, Exercise and Health, Division of Sport and Psychosocial Health, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 4Department of Psychology, University of Warwick, Coventry, UK
  • 5Department of Psychology, Manchester Metropolitan University, Manchester, UK
  • 6Department of Teacher Education, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany
  • 7Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
  • 8Institute of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

Background: The concept of mental toughness (MT) has gained increasing importance among groups other than elite athletes by virtue of its psychological importance and explanatory power for a broad range of health-related behaviors. However, no study has focused so far on the psychological origins of MT. Therefore, the aims of the present study were: to explore, to what extent the psychological profiles of preschoolers aged five were associated with both (1) MT scores and (2) sleep disturbances at age 14, and 3) to explore possible gender differences.

Method: Nine years after their first assessment at age five (preschoolers), a total of 77 adolescents (mean age: 14.35 years; SD = 1.22; 42% females) took part in this follow-up study. At baseline, both parents and teachers completed the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), covering internalizing and externalizing problems, hyperactivity, negative peer relationships, and prosocial behavior. At follow-up, participants completed a booklet of questionnaires covering socio-demographic data, MT, and sleep disturbances.

Results: Higher prosocial behavior, lower negative peer relationships, and lower internalizing and externalizing problems at age five, as rated by parents and teachers, were associated with self-reported higher MT and lower sleep disturbances at age 14. At age 14, and relative to males, females had lower MT scores and reported more sleep disturbances.

Conclusion: The pattern of results suggests that MT traits during adolescence may have their origins in the pre-school years.

Introduction

Clough et al. (2002) formulate the concept of Mental toughness (MT) in its present form and since then, the concept of MT has gained increasing interest by virtue of its psychological importance and explanatory power with respect to psychological concepts such as coping with stress, self-esteem, and motivation, and with respect to a broad range of health-related behaviors (Table 1) (Dewhurst et al., 2012; Gerber et al., 2013b; Perry et al., 2013; Crust et al., 2014; Stamp et al., 2015). MT refers to an individual’s capacity to be consistently successful in coping with difficult life circumstances and comprises the following dimensions: Control (own life and emotions), Commitment (to personal aims and achievements), Challenge (considering changes in life not as threats but as challenges), and Confidence (in own abilities and in other people) (Clough et al., 2002; Perry et al., 2013). Thus, MT refers to the tendency to appraise threats and pressure as opportunities to thrive (Thelwell et al., 2005), actively to seek and approach challenges (Crust, 2008), and successfully to overcome setbacks and difficulties (Clough et al., 2002; Dewhurst et al., 2012). Thus, as shown in and apparent from Table 1, MT embodies a range of cognitive-emotional processes closely involved in coping with stress, motivation, self-esteem, unexpected events, and social settings (confidence in other people).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Overview of Mental toughness (MT) traits in relation to other psychological concepts (the list doesn’t claim to be complete).

Initially, studies in this field focused on MT in elite athletes (Loehr, 1994; Fourie and Potgieter, 2001; Jones et al., 2002, 2007; Thelwell et al., 2005; Crust, 2007, 2008; Clough and Strycharczyk, 2012); these studies showed that mentally tough athletes were able to cope with stress during a competition and to remain more focused and confident (Levy et al., 2006; Crust, 2007; Mack and Ragan, 2008; Nicholls et al., 2008; Kaiseler et al., 2009; Sheard, 2009; Crust and Azadi, 2010). However, more recent studies have applied the concept of MT to other groups such as healthy older adolescents (Gerber, 2011; Gerber et al., 2012, 2013a, 2015a,b; Brand et al., 2014a,b), healthy younger adolescents (Brand et al., 2016a,b), university students (Stamp et al., 2015), lower, middle, and senior managers, and clerical/administrative workers in early, middle and late adulthood (Marchant et al., 2009; Perry et al., 2013), as well as those working in education (Crust et al., 2014), and the military (Arthur et al., 2015). All these studies have shown higher MT scores to be associated with better coping with stress (Gerber et al., 2013a,b), with better sleep quality assessed both subjectively (Brand et al., 2014b) and objectively (Brand et al., 2014a), and with better physical performance (Crust and Clough, 2005; Gerber et al., 2012). Further, we showed that patients with multiple sclerosis (MS) at illness onset (mean age = 32.3 years) reported similar MT traits as healthy adolescents and young adults did (Sadeghi Bahmani et al., 2016).

To date, however, no study has considered the origins of MT by asking what psychological dimensions in childhood might predict MT traits in adolescence. The aim of the present study was therefore to address this question. To this end participants from a previous study when they were 5 years old and at kindergarten (Hatzinger et al., 2007, 2008, 2010; Brand et al., 2015) were contacted at age 14 and assessed once again. We believe that the present study has the potential to shed new light on the origins and development of MT, a psychological attribute, which appears to underlie a broad range of positive behaviors. We further hold that with the present study attention in developmental psychology might shift from psychopathology toward salutogenic dimensions.

In this regard, a literature search on the search engine Pubmed with the items ‘resilience’, ‘hardiness’, or ‘mental toughness’ in combination with ‘development’ produced very few results (note that the concepts of MT, resilience, and hardiness seem to share a common basis but without being synonyms; see Table 1). For ‘hardiness’, no study could be identified in combination with the term ‘development’. With regard to ‘resilience’ and ‘development’, the following points were identified: resilience is understood as an individual’s skill in successfully adapting to stress and adversity (Skala and Bruckner, 2014). Interpersonal factors associated with resilience include male gender, higher intelligence, aspects of character, temperament, and genes. Family factors include stable and positive relations with an adult, while a broader social environmental factor is being integrated into a community. Concerning the development of resilience, (Masten et al., 1999, 2004; Masten, 2004; Masten and Cicchetti, 2012; Masten and Tellegen, 2012) identified higher IQ and favorable parenting as factors with the potential to protect child development in the context of severe adversity. They also emphasized the relevance of adaptive resources, planfulness/future motivation, autonomy, adult support, and coping skills as possible factors underlying resilience and successful development. Additionally, Masten and Tellegen (2012) found that resilient adults reported high quality relationships with parents and other adults and good cognitive and socio-emotional skills during their childhood, while Sameroff and Rosenblum (2006) identified poor parenting, antisocial peers, low-resource communities, and economic hardship as the main factors impairing resilience.

Thus, while research examining the impact of hardiness and resilience on child development is scarce, and while the concept of MT offers a basis for integrating a broad range of coping literature into a common framework (Table 1), it must be noted that most previous studies have focused on the development of psychopathology. In fact, there is some evidence that psychopathology in preschoolers may persist over time and that childhood psychopathology could predict psychological difficulties in adolescence (Giedd et al., 1999; Paus et al., 1999, 2008; Caye et al., 2016). In particular, attention has been given to whether behavioral problems such as internalizing and externalizing behavior in preschoolers could predict psychiatric problems in adolescence and adulthood. However, externalizing problems have been more frequently investigated than internalizing problems and, in general, results indicated greater stability over time for externalizing than for internalizing behavior problems (Pihlakoski et al., 2006). Pihlakoski et al. (2006) have also shown that externalizing problems in boys and girls at age three strongly predicted both externalizing and internalizing problems at 12 years. Externalizing disorders are characterized by disruptive, disobedient, and harmful behaviors that are often manifested physically (e.g., in aggressive, impulsive, and non-compliant behavior; Weisz and Weiss, 1991), and seemed to exhibit considerable stability over time throughout development (Pihlakoski et al., 2006). Furthermore, externalizing behaviors have been associated with social aggression, disruptive behavior, a perceived lack of constraint, and risky behaviors; in particular, aggressive and destructive behaviors in early childhood predicted later problems (Pihlakoski et al., 2006). In addition, evidence indicated that childhood psychopathology was associated with higher rates of early substance use and problem substance use (King et al., 2004). Externalizing disorders (e.g., conduct problems and ADHD) have been found to have the strongest impact on later tobacco use, and children displaying aggressive behavior at 5 years were more likely to consume tobacco 14 years later (though, surprisingly, no association was found between externalizing problems and a prediction of DSM-IV nicotine dependence at 21-year follow-up; Fischer et al., 2012). However, in general, children with an early onset of conduct problems (onset in preschool) and a high degree of continuity seemed to have a much more negative prognosis than children with a late onset (adolescence) (McMahon, 1999). In conclusion, psychopathology such as externalizing problems in childhood and adolescence appeared to predict unfavorable behaviors such as tobacco consumption in later life (Fischer et al., 2012).

Internalizing disorders are characterized by feelings of sorrow, guilt, worry, and somatization (Weisz and Weiss, 1991), and children with internalizing disorders display reactions such as social withdrawal, a lack of pleasure in enjoyable activities, and a lack of energy (Cicchetti and Toth, 1998). Internalizing problems were linked to social deficits (e.g., submissive and inhibited interaction), poor interaction with the peer-group, social isolation and development of a negative self-concept (Fischer et al., 2012) and might lead to internalizing disorders such as depression and anxiety (McMahon, 1999). About 2–3% of children and 6–8% of adolescents suffer from depression and the lifetime prevalence of depression during adolescence was in the range 15–20% (McMahon, 1999). The estimated prevalence of anxiety disorders in childhood and adolescence varied from 9 to 21%. Girls were twice as likely to experience an anxiety disorder (McMahon, 1999), a trend recently further confirmed, in that internalizing problems had increased among recent cohorts of girls as compared to previous cohorts, but not among boys (Bor et al., 2014). Yet, findings with respect to internalizing behaviors are less consistent than those for externalizing behaviors; this may be due to young children’s limited ability to express anxiety and depression. Moreover, parents seemed to have difficulties in recognizing these emotions in their preschool children (Pihlakoski et al., 2006). Nevertheless, internalizing problems in 2–5 years young children have proved to be relatively stable over a 2-year follow-up period (Pihlakoski et al., 2006).

In addition, there is evidence that peer victimization (e.g., experiencing frequent verbal or physical bullying by peers) in middle childhood was a relevant predictor of internalizing behavior problems and psychological disorders during adolescence (Schwartz et al., 2015). There have been several indications of a moderate link between peer victimization and some form of internalizing behavior problem such as symptoms of depression, anxiety, loneliness, and withdrawal (Schwartz et al., 2015). More specifically, Schwartz et al. (2015) provided evidence that peer victimization in middle childhood could act as a key marker of disorders at later stages of development. These authors found that children who experienced frequent peer victimization in middle childhood were significantly more likely to meet criteria for a major depressive disorder during late adolescence.

Additionally, researchers have examined the long-term influence of sleep disturbances on several psychological problems. Existing research has shown that sleep problems were persistent and that individual differences in sleep problems were highly stable over time (Wong, 2010; Wong et al., 2010). Thus, Wong (2010) and Wong et al. (2010) found that having trouble sleeping at age 3 to 8 years was significantly associated with self-reported sleep problems at age 11 to 17. Indeed those who had trouble sleeping in childhood, compared to those having no trouble sleeping, had an almost 2.5-fold greater likelihood of having trouble sleeping in adolescence (Brand et al., 2015 for extensive overview).

In summary, previous research has focused on the predictive value of internalizing and externalizing problems, and sleep disturbances during childhood for psychopathology traits in adolescence. However, no evidence is available as regards the predictive value of internalizing and externalizing problems during childhood for psychological constructs such as MT in adolescence. Therefore, the aim of the present longitudinal study was to shed some light into this issue. We hold that the present data have the potential to add to the existing literature in important ways: first, compared to other psychological constructs as listed in Table 1, the concept of MT has been established only 1.5 decades ago, and to the best of our knowledge, no research has focused on psychological constructs as precursors of adolescent MT. Second, in our opinion, the concept of MT deserves further research, as it has the potential to cover a broad range of cognitive-emotional concepts such as coping, self-esteem, motivation, and social confidence in one single construct (see Table 1).

Given the lack of previous research, we drew upon findings relating to psychopathology in formulating our hypotheses. Thus, following others (Wong et al., 2010; Fischer et al., 2012; Settles et al., 2012), we anticipated that positive psychological traits evident in childhood (low externalizing and internalizing problems, high prosocial behavior) would predict higher MT scores and also lower sleep disturbances at age 14. Furthermore, based on previous research (Brand et al., 2014a,b), we expected that higher MT scores would be associated with lower sleep disturbances at age 14. Finally, we expected that compared to boys, girls would report lower MT scores (Brand et al., 2016a,b) and more sleep disturbances (Armitage and Hoffmann, 2001; Brand et al., 2016b; Mong and Cusmano, 2016).

Materials and Methods

Procedure

As described elsewhere (Brand et al., 2015), children participating in this study were assessed during their 1st year in kindergarten (when they were 5 years old; Hatzinger et al., 2007, 2008, 2010). These children were contacted again at age 14. Participants completed self-rating questionnaires covering socio-demographic information, MT and sleep (see below). The general purpose of the follow-up study was explained to the adolescents and their parents. Prior to entry to the study both adolescents and their parents were asked to sign an informed consent form. The study protocol was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the local ethics committee.

Parts of the ongoing longitudinal study have been already published. Specifically, Brand et al. (2015) showed that sleep quality at the age of five predicted psychological traits in areas such as peer relationships and success at coping with stress while, surprisingly, sleep at the age of 14 years was unrelated. In the present study, we focused on the associations between participants’ psychological traits at age five (SDQ, parents’ and teachers’ ratings; see below) and participants’ self-rated MT and sleep disturbances at age 14. This pattern of associations has not been examined so far. Thus, the present data are novel.

Sample

The core sample has been described in detail elsewhere (Perren et al., 2006; Brand et al., 2015). Briefly, preschoolers of kindergartens of Basel (Basel, Switzerland) were assessed as regards their subjective and objective sleep parameters, the level of saliva cortisol under baseline [cortisol awakening response (CAR)] and under challenge conditions (a modified social stress test), and their psychological functioning [Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ, see below for more details; (Goodman, 1997): internalizing and externalizing problems, hyperactivity, negative peer relationship, prosocial behavior], as assessed via parents’ and teachers’ ratings. Of the 95 children at age five (M = 5.4 years, SD = 0.44) for whom parents and kindergarten teachers completed the SDQ, 77 (81.05%) agreed to participate in the follow-up study at age 14, that is, about 9 years later. Mean age at follow-up was 14.25 years (SD = 1.21; 32 females and 45 males). As stated in Brand et al. (2015), participants and non-participants at follow-up did not significantly differ as regards age, gender, sleep profiles or psychological traits (internalizing and externalizing problems, hyperactivity, negative peer-relationship, and prosocial behavior) at baseline. Accordingly, age, gender, sleep profiles, or psychological traits at baseline were not entered as covariates in all statistical equations of the present study.

Tools Employed at Age Five

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 1997).

Parents and teachers completed the SDQ, which consists of 25 items covering the following five dimensions: internalizing problems, externalizing problems, hyperactivity, peer problems, and prosocial behavior. An overall score can also be derived, with higher scores reflecting more negative psychological functioning. Each scale consists of five items that are rated on a three-point scale ranging from 0 (= not true) to 2 (= certainly true). The sum is calculated to generate subscale scores. In the present study, internal consistency was moderate to high (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87).

Tools Participants Employed at Age 14

Mental Toughness

Participants were asked to fill in the 18-item Mental Toughness Questionnaire (MTQ18; Clough et al., 2002; German version: Gerber et al., 2012, 2013a,b). The MTQ18 is the short version of the MTQ48 questionnaire (Clough et al., 2002), which has proved to be a valid and reliable instrument in previous research (Gerber et al., 2013b; Perry et al., 2013). Very high correlations exist between the MTQ18 and MTQ48 (Clough et al., 2002; Gerber et al., 2014, 2015a). Answers on the MTQ18 are given on five-point Likert-type scales ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Responses across items are summed, with higher scores reflecting greater MT (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.92).

Sleep Disturbances

To assess sleep disturbances, the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI; (Bastien et al., 2001) was employed; this is a 7-item screening measure for insomnia and an outcome measure for use in treatment research. The items, answered on 5-point rating scales (0 = not at all, 4 = very much), refer in part to DSM-IV (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders) criteria for insomnia (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) by measuring difficulty in falling asleep, difficulties remaining asleep, early morning awakenings, increased daytime sleepiness, impaired daytime sleepiness, impaired daytime performance, low satisfaction with sleep, and worrying about sleep. Evidence for the validity and reliability of this instrument has been presented previously (Fernandez-Mendoza et al., 2012; Gerber et al., 2016). The higher the overall score, the more the respondent is assumed to suffer from sleep disturbances (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.92).

Statistical Analysis

First, a series of Pearson’s correlations was performed between SDQ scores (parents’ and teachers’ ratings of children at age five) and participants’ MT and sleep disturbances scores at age 14. Second, two multiple regression analyses (stepwise backward) were performed with MT scores and sleep disturbances (age 14) as dependent variables and SDQ scores (age 5) as predictors. Third, possible gender differences in MT and sleep disturbances at age 14 were calculated with two t-tests. Fourth, the bivariate association between MT and sleep disturbances scores at age 14 was examined with a Pearson’s correlation. The nominal level of significance was set at alpha <0.05. Statistics was performed with SPSS® 23.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA) for Apple Mac®.

Results

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations between strengths and difficulties (SDQ) at 5 years and MT and sleep disturbances at 14 years.

All statistical indices are reported in Table 2 and therefore not repeated in the text again.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations between MT and sleep disturbances at age 14 and teachers’ and parents’ rating of children’s psychological functioning at age 5.

Lower internalizing and externalizing problems, and higher prosocial behavior, as rated by parents and teachers, were associated with higher MT scores and lower sleep disturbances. Better peer relations and lower overall scores, as rated by teachers, were associated with higher MT scores and lower sleep disturbances. No significant associations were found for hyperactivity (parents’ and teachers’ ratings) or for negative peer relations, as rated by parents.

As regards sleep disturbances, higher sleep disturbances were associated with higher internalizing and externalizing problems, lower prosocial behavior, as rated by parents and teachers, and with more negative peer relationships and higher overall scores, as rated by teachers. No significant associations were found for hyperactivity (parents’ or teachers’ ratings), or for negative peer relationships and overall scores, as rated by parents.

MT Scores and Sleep Disturbances

The correlation coefficient was r = -0.45 (p < 0.05); higher MT scores were related to lower sleep disturbances.

Predicting Mental Toughness and Sleep Disturbances (at 14 Years) from Strengths and Difficulties (SDQ; at 5 Years)

Table 3 reports the results from the two multiple regression analyses (stepwise backward) with MT scores and sleep disturbances as dependent variables and the strengths and difficulties as predictors (to avoid redundancy and biased calculations, SDQ Total scores were not entered in the equations).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Overview of the multiple regression analyses (stepwise backward) with MT and sleep disturbances at age 14 as dependent variables and parents’ and teachers’ ratings of the children’s strengths and difficulties (SDQ) at children’s age five as independent variables.

Higher Mental toughness scores were associated with lower internalizing problems (parents, teachers), lower externalizing problems, more positive peer relationships, and higher prosocial behavior (teachers). The following variables were excluded from the equation: teachers’ ratings of hyperactivity; parents’ ratings of internalizing problems, prosocial behavior, hyperactivity, and negative peer relationships.

Higher sleep disturbances were associated with higher externalizing problems (parents, teachers), and more negative peer relationships (teachers). The following variables were excluded from the equation: teachers’ ratings of internalizing problems, prosocial behavior, and hyperactivity; parents’ ratings of internalizing problems, negative peer relationships, prosocial behavior, and hyperactivity.

Gender Differences in MT and Sleep Disturbances

Two t-tests (Table 4) revealed that females at 14 years, compared to males, had lower MT scores and reported more sleep disturbances.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Mental toughness scores and sleep disturbances, separated by gender.

Discussion

The key findings of the present study were that lower scores of internalizing and externalizing problems and negative peer-relationships and higher prosocial behavior scores at age five, as rated by parents and teachers, were associated with higher MT scores and lower sleep disturbances scores at age 14. The pattern of results adds to the current literature in an important way in that we were able to shed some light on the origins of adolescent MT by relating this to favorable psychological traits at the age of 5 years.

Three hypotheses were formulated and each of these is considered now in turn.

Our first hypothesis was that positive psychological traits during childhood (lower internalizing and externalizing problems, lower negative peer-relationships, higher pro-social behavior) would be associated with greater MT at 14 years, and this was confirmed. We hold that the present study expands upon previous research in being the first to associate adolescent MT from favorable childhood psychological traits. As shown in Tables 2 and 3, both parents’ and teachers’ ratings of children’s lower externalizing, internalizing problems, negative peer relationships and higher prosocial behavior during preschool was associated with higher MT scores during adolescence.

Our second hypothesis was that childhood psychological traits would be associated with lower sleep disturbances scores in adolescence, and this hypothesis also received support. We believe that this pattern of results confirms both the assumed association between positive psychological traits and sleep quality. In this view, there is evidence from longitudinal studies that sleep quality impacts on psychological functioning (Kaneita et al., 2009; Roberts et al., 2009; Hatzinger et al., 2013b, 2014; Roberts and Duong, 2014, 2015; Brand et al., 2015); the results of a meta-analysis indicated that poor sleep predicted symptoms of depression among adolescents, and not vice versa (Lovato and Gradisar, 2014). On the other hand, psychological traits also impacted on sleep patterns (see Brand et al., 2015 for extensive overview), a direction of influence also confirmed in the present study.

Our third hypothesis was that, cross-sectionally, higher MT scores would be related to fewer sleep disturbances, and again data confirmed this. Therefore, the present pattern of results is also consistent with previous findings (Brand et al., 2014a,b, 2016a,b), and underscores the bi-directionality of sleep and psychological functioning.

The data available do not shed any light on why positive psychological traits such as lower internalizing and externalizing problems, lower negative peer-relationships and higher prosocial behavior at 5 years, and as rated by parents and teachers, should be associated with both self-rated increased MT and lower sleep disturbances 9 years later. We know from previous studies (Wong et al., 2010; Fischer et al., 2012; Settles et al., 2012; Shin et al., 2012) that increased psychological issues during childhood also increased the risk of increased psychological issues in adolescence and early adulthood. We also know that personality traits remained fairly stable from childhood to adolescence (Roberts et al., 2001; Moffitt et al., 2011; Shin et al., 2012; Keefer et al., 2013), and in this view, we also know that higher IQ and positive parenting favor the development of resilience in childhood and provide protection under conditions of severe adversity (Masten et al., 1999). In this context, Masten and Tellegen (2012) reported that resilience increased as a function of high quality relationships with parents and other adults (see also Skala and Bruckner, 2014), cognitive quality, social-emotional skills, adaptive resources, planfulness, future motivation, autonomy, adult support, and coping skills (Masten, 2004; Masten et al., 2004), and that resilience often emerged in childhood and endured, but that there were also late bloomers. Sameroff and Rosenblum (2006) emphasized that, in addition to the behavioral and emotional self-regulation characteristic of good mental health and the cognitive self-regulation characteristic of high intelligence, environmental factors such as parenting, peers, and economic conditions may independently and bi-directionally contribute to a child’s resilience.

How should these findings be related to the present study? Our proposal is that lower internalizing and lower externalizing problems, lower negative peer-relationships and higher prosocial behavior might be understood as the behavioral and emotional self-regulation that is characteristic of good mental health, the cognitive self-regulation element as suggested by Sameroff and Rosenblum (2006; Keefer et al., 2013), as well as aspects of the adaptive resources, autonomy and coping skills, as suggested by Masten (2004) and Masten et al. (2004). Additionally, prosocial behavior might be understood as reflecting high quality relationships and stable socio-emotional skills (Masten et al., 2004; Masten and Tellegen, 2012; Skala and Bruckner, 2014). Importantly, in the present study lower internalizing and externalizing problems and higher prosocial behavior were associated with greater MT 9 years later, suggesting therefore considerable stability in level of psychological traits from childhood to mid adolescence (Caspi et al., 2005; Moffitt et al., 2011; Shin et al., 2012).

As regards gender differences, our findings confirmed previous results, in that relative to males, females at age 14 had lower MT scores (Brand et al., 2016a,b) and more pronounced sleep difficulties (Armitage and Hoffmann, 2001; Mong and Cusmano, 2016).

Despite the novelty of the findings, several limitations warrant against their overgeneralization. First, the sample size was small, and a larger sample would have provided greater statistical power and may therefore have revealed other significant associations. Second, the pattern of results might have emerged due to further latent, but unassessed dimensions, which might have biased two or more variables in the same or opposite directions. This holds particularly true, as it is conceivable that latent MT traits and sleep patterns at the age of five might have conferred to the MT and sleep disturbances scores at the age of 14. Further, for instance, parenting style was not assessed at both time points. In this regards, there is evidence that children’s and also adolescents’ behavior and sleep are not independent of family functioning, parenting style (Brand et al., 2009), or parents’ sleep patterns (Brand et al., 2009; Kalak et al., 2012; Bajoghli et al., 2013). Third, no neurophysiological data were gathered at the second time point; previous studies have shown that, for instance, cortisol secretion remained stable over time (Hatzinger et al., 2013a), while cortisol secretion is not related to sleep patterns (sleep-EEG; actigraphy) 12 months later, suggesting therefore that cortisol secretion may also vary as a function of current physical and psychological processes. Fourth, sleep at age 14 was only assessed subjectively. Fifth, only the MT overall score was applied (MTQ18); employing the long version (MTQ48) would have allowed a more fine-grained analysis of the associations between childhood and adolescent psychological functioning. In this regard, we underscore that the current interpretations rely on the assumption that psychometric properties of the MTQ18 used with adolescents are acceptable. Sixth, it is conceivable that the strengths and difficulties at the age of 5 years are the result of psychosocial development from very first infanthood to preschool-age, suggesting therefore that the origins of adolescents’ MT might be routed even earlier in development. Seventh and last, given that the concept of MT has gained increasing attention for its interest utility among elite and non-elite athletes to explaining a broad range of coping, motivation, self-esteem and health-related behavior (see Introduction and Table 1), future research might focus on the causal relation between MT traits and such constructs as reported in Table 1.

Conclusion

Positive psychological traits as reflected in lower internalizing and externalizing problems and higher prosocial behavior at age five was associated with higher MT scores and lower sleep disturbances scores at age 14. The pattern of results suggests that positive psychological traits in childhood seemed to lay the foundation for adolescent MT. This is important because MT has proved to be a stress resilience factor during both adolescence and young adulthood.

Author Contributions

Study design: DS, MH, MG, SL, PC, SP, KK, AW, EH, and SB; data gathering: DS, MH, SP, AW, and SB; data analysis: DS, MG, SL, and SB; interpretation of the data: DS, MH, MG, SL, PC, SP, KK, AW, EH, and SB; writing the first draft: DS, MG, EH, and SB; integration of authors’ comments: DS, MG, SL, and SB; final manuscript: DS, MH, MG, SL, PC, SP, KK, AW, EH, and SB.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The handling Editor declared a past co-authorship with one of the authors SB and states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.

Acknowledgments

We thank Christina Stadler and Margarete Bolten (University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland) for data gathering. Further, we thank Nick Emler (University of Surrey, UK) for proofreading the manuscript.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Armitage, R., and Hoffmann, R. F. (2001). Sleep EEG, depression and gender. Sleep Med. Rev. 5, 237–246. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2000.0144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arthur, C., Fitzwater, J., Hardy, L., Beattie, S., and Bell, J. (2015). Development and validation of a military training mental toughness inventory. Mil. Psychol. 27, 232–241. doi: 10.1037/mil0000074

CrossRef Full Text

Bajoghli, H., Alipouri, A., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., and Brand, S. (2013). Sleep patterns and psychological functioning in families in northeastern Iran; evidence for similarities between adolescent children and their parents. J. Adolesc. 36, 1103–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.08.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 84, 191–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastien, C. H., Vallieres, A., and Morin, C. M. (2001). Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. 2, 297–307. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00065-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bor, W., Dean, A. J., Najman, J., and Hayatbakhsh, R. (2014). Are child and adolescent mental health problems increasing in the 21st century? A systematic review. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 48, 606–616. doi: 10.1177/0004867414533834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, S., Gerber, M., Hatzinger, M., Beck, J., and Holsboer-Trachsler, E. (2009). Evidence for similarities between adolescents and parents in sleep patterns. Sleep Med. 10, 1124–1131. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.12.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, S., Gerber, M., Kalak, N., Kirov, R., Lemola, S., Clough, P. J., et al. (2014a). Adolescents with greater mental toughness show higher sleep efficiency, more deep sleep and fewer awakenings after sleep onset. J. Adolesc. Health 54, 109–113. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.07.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, S., Gerber, M., Kalak, N., Kirov, R., Lemola, S., Clough, P. J., et al. (2014b). “Sleep well, our tough heroes!”–in adolescence, greater mental toughness is related to better sleep schedules. Behav. Sleep Med. 12, 444–454. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2013.825839

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, S., Hatzinger, M., Stadler, C., Bolten, M., von Wyl, A., Perren, S., et al. (2015). Does objectively assessed sleep at five years predict sleep and psychological functioning at 14 years? – Hmm, yes and no! J. Psychiatr. Res. 60, 148–155. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, S., Kalak, N., Gerber, M., Clough, P. J., Lemola, S., Puhse, U., et al. (2016a). During early and mid-adolescence, greater mental toughness is related to increased sleep quality and quality of life. J. Health Psychol. 21, 905–915. doi: 10.1177/1359105314542816

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, S., Kalak, N., Gerber, M., Clough, P. J., Lemola, S., Sadeghi Bahmani, D., et al. (2016b). During early to mid adolescence, moderate to vigorous physical activity is associated with restoring sleep, psychological functioning, mental toughness and male gender. J. Sports Sci. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1167936 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., and Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: stability and change. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 453–484. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caye, A., Rocha, T. B., Anselmi, L., Murray, J., Menezes, A. M., Barros, F. C., et al. (2016). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder trajectories from childhood to young adulthood: evidence from a birth cohort supporting a late-onset syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 705–712. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cicchetti, D., and Toth, S. L. (1998). The development of depression in children and adolescents. Am. Psychol. 53, 221–241. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clough, P., Earle, K., and Sewell, D. (2002). “Mental toughness: the concept and its measurement,” in Solutions in Sport Psychology, ed. I. Cockerill (London: Thomson Learning), 32–46.

Google Scholar

Clough, P., and Strycharczyk, P. (2012). Developing Mental Toughness: Improving Performance, Wellbeing and Positive Behaviour in Others. London: Kogan.

Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., and Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 24, 385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crust, L. (2007). Mental toughness in sport: a review. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 5, 270–290.

Google Scholar

Crust, L. (2008). A review and conceptual re-examination of mental toughness: implications for future researchers. Pers. Ind. Diff. 45, 576–583. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crust, L., and Azadi, K. (2010). Mental toughness and athletes’ use of psychological strategies. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 10, 43–51. doi: 10.1080/17461390903049972

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crust, L., and Clough, P. J. (2005). Relationship between mental toughness and physical endurance. Percept. Mot. Skills 100, 192–194. doi: 10.2466/pms.100.1.192-194

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crust, L., Earle, K., Perry, J., Earle, F., Clough, A., and Clough, P. (2014). Mental toughness in higher education: relationships with achievement and progression in first-year university sports students. Pers. Ind. Diff. 69, 87–91. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewhurst, S. A., Anderson, R. J., Cotter, G., Crust, L., and Clough, P. J. (2012). Identifying the cognitive basis of mental toughness: evidence from the directed forgetting paradigm. Pers. Ind. Diff. 53, 587–590. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez-Mendoza, J., Rodriguez-Munoz, A., Vela-Bueno, A., Olevarrieta-Bernardino, S., Calhoun, S. L., Bixler, E. O., et al. (2012). The Spanish version of the Insomnia Severity Index: a confirmatory factor analysis. Sleep Med. 13, 207–210. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2011.06.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, J. A., Najman, J. M., Williams, G. M., and Clavarino, A. M. (2012). Childhood and adolescent psychopathology and subsequent tobacco smoking in young adults: findings from an Australian birth cohort. Addiction 107, 1669–1676. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03846.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fourie, S., and Potgieter, J. R. (2001). The nature of mental toughness in sport. S. Afr. Res. Sport Phys. Educ Recr. 23, 63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.bjoms.2012.04.263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerber, M. (2011). Mentale Toughness im Sport. Sportwissenschaft 41, 283–299. doi: 10.1007/s12662-011-0202-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerber, M., Brand, S., Feldmeth, A. K., Lang, C., Elliot, C., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., et al. (2013a). Adolescents with high mental toughness adapt better to perceived stress: a longitudinal study with Swiss vocational students. Pers. Ind. Diff. 54, 808–814. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerber, M., Feldmeth, A. K., Elliot, C., Brand, S., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., and Pühse, U. (2014). Mental health in Swiss vocational students: the moderating role of physical activity. J. Res. Adol. 25, 63–74. doi: 10.1111/jora.12097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerber, M., Feldmeth, A. K., Lang, C., Brand, S., Elliot, C., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., et al. (2015a). The relationship between mental toughness, stress, and burnout among adolescents: a longitudinal study with Swiss vocational students. Psychol. Rep. 117, 703–723. doi: 10.2466/14.02.PR0.117c29z6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerber, M., Kalak, N., Lemola, S., Clough, P. J., Perry, J. L., Pühse, U., et al. (2013b). Are adolescents with high mental toughness levels more resilient against stress? Stress Health 29, 164–171. doi: 10.1002/smi.2447

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerber, M., Kalak, N., Lemola, S., Clough, P. J., Pühse, U., Elliot, C., et al. (2012). Adolescents’ exercise and physical activity are associated with mental toughness. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 5, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2012.02.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerber, M., Lang, C., Feldmeth, A. K., Elliot, C., Brand, S., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., et al. (2015b). Burnout and mental health in Swiss vocational students: the moderating role of physical activity. J. Res. Adol. 1, 63–74. doi: 10.1111/jora.12097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerber, M., Lang, C., Lemola, S., Colledge, F., Kalak, N., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., et al. (2016). Validation of the German version of the insomnia severity index in adolescents, young adults and adult workers: results from three cross-sectional studies. BMC Psychiatry 16:174. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-0876-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 38, 581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatzinger, M., Brand, S., Perren, S., Stadelmann, S., von Wyl, A., von Klitzing, K., et al. (2008). Electroencephalographic sleep profiles and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA)-activity in kindergarten children: early indication of poor sleep quality associated with increased cortisol secretion. J. Psych. Res. 42, 532–543. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.05.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatzinger, M., Brand, S., Perren, S., Stadelmann, S., von Wyl, A., von Klitzing, K., et al. (2010). Sleep actigraphy pattern and behavioral/emotional difficulties in kindergarten children: association with hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) activity. J. Psych. Res. 44, 253–261. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.08.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatzinger, M., Brand, S., Perren, S., Von Wyl, A., Stadelmann, S., von Klitzing, K., et al. (2013a). In pre-school children, cortisol secretion remains stable over 12 months and is related to psychological functioning and gender. J. Psych. Res. 47, 1409–1416. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.05.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatzinger, M., Brand, S., Perren, S., Von Wyl, A., Stadelmann, S., von Klitzing, K., et al. (2013b). In pre-school children, sleep objectively assessed via sleep-EEGs remains stable over 12 months and is related to psychological functioning, but not to cortisol secretion. J. Psych. Res. 47, 1809–1814. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.05.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatzinger, M., Brand, S., Perren, S., Von Wyl, A., Stadelmann, S., von Klitzing, K., et al. (2014). In pre-school children, sleep objectively assessed via actigraphy remains stable over 12 months and is related to psychological functioning, but not to cortisol secretion. J. Psych. Res. 55, 22–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatzinger, M., Brand, S., Perren, S., von Wyl, A., von Klitzing, K., and Holsboer-Trachsler, E. (2007). Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) activity in kindergarten children: importance of gender and associations with behavioral/emotional difficulties. J. Psych. Res. 41, 861–870. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.07.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, G., Hanton, S., and Connaughton, D. (2002). What is this thing called mental toughness? An investigation of elite sport performers. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 14, 205–218. doi: 10.1080/10413200290103509

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, G., Hanton, S., and Connaughton, D. (2007). A framework of mental toughness in the world’s best performers. Sport Psychol. 21, 243–264.

Google Scholar

Kaiseler, M., Polman, R. C. J., and Nicholls, A. R. (2009). Mental toughness, stress, stress appraisal, coping and coping effectiveness in sport. Pers. Ind. Diff. 47, 728–733. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalak, N., Gerber, M., Kirov, R., Mikoteit, T., Pühse, U., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., et al. (2012). The relation of objective sleep patterns, depressive symptoms, and sleep disturbances in adolescent children and their parents: a sleep-EEG study with 47 families. J. Psych. Res. 46, 1374–1382. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaneita, Y., Yokoyama, E., Harano, S., Tamaki, T., Suzuki, H., Munezawa, T., et al. (2009). Associations between sleep disturbance and mental health status: a longitudinal study of Japanese junior high school students. Sleep Med. 10, 780–786. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.06.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keefer, K. V., Holden, R. R., and Parker, J. D. (2013). Longitudinal assessment of trait emotional intelligence: measurement invariance and construct continuity from late childhood to adolescence. Psychol. Assess. 25, 1255–1272. doi: 10.1037/a0033903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, S. M., Iacono, W. G., and McGue, M. (2004). Childhood externalizing and internalizing psychopathology in the prediction of early substance use. Addiction 99, 1548–1559. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00893.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobasa, S. C., Maddi, S. R., and Kahn, S. (1982). Hardiness and health: a prospective study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 42, 168–177. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.168

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Levy, A. R., Polman, R. C., Clough, P. J., Marchant, D. C., and Earle, K. (2006). Mental toughness as a determinant of beliefs, pain, and adherence in sport injury rehabilitation. J. Sport Rehab. 15, 246–254.

Google Scholar

Loehr, J. E. (1994). The New Toughness Training for Sports: Mental, Emotional, and Physical Conditioning from One of the World’s Premier Sports Psychologists. New York, NY: Penguin Putnam.

Google Scholar

Lovato, N., and Gradisar, M. (2014). A meta-analysis and model of the relationship between sleep and depression in adolescents: recommendations for future research and clinical practice. Sleep Med. Rev. 18, 521–529. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mack, M. G., and Ragan, B. G. (2008). Development of the mental, emotional, and bodily toughness inventory in collegiate athletes and nonathletes. J. Athl. Train 43, 125–132. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-43.2.125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchant, D., Polman, R., Clough, J. P., Jackson, J., Levy, A., and Nicholls, A. (2009). Mental toughness: managerial and age differences. J. Manag. Psychol. 24, 428–437. doi: 10.1108/02683940910959753

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, A. S. (2004). Regulatory processes, risk, and resilience in adolescent development. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1021, 310–319. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, A. S., Burt, K. B., Roisman, G. I., Obradovic, J., Long, J. D., and Tellegen, A. (2004). Resources and resilience in the transition to adulthood: continuity and change. Dev. Psychopathol. 16, 1071–1094. doi: 10.1017/S0954579404040143

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, A. S., and Cicchetti, D. (2012). Risk and resilience in development and psychopathology: the legacy of Norman Garmezy. Dev. Psychopathol. 24, 333–334. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, A. S., Hubbard, J. J., Gest, S. D., Tellegen, A., Garmezy, N., and Ramirez, M. (1999). Competence in the context of adversity: pathways to resilience and maladaptation from childhood to late adolescence. Dev. Psychopathol. 11, 143–169. doi: 10.1017/S0954579499001996

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, A. S., and Tellegen, A. (2012). Resilience in developmental psychopathology: contributions of the project competence longitudinal study. Dev. Psychopathol. 24, 345–361. doi: 10.1017/S095457941200003X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMahon, R. J. (1999). Child and adolescent psychopathology as risk factors for subsequent tobacco use. Nicotine Tob. Res. 1(Suppl. 2), S45–S50. doi: 10.1080/14622299050011801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mong, J. A., and Cusmano, D. M. (2016). Sex differences in sleep: impact of biological sex and sex steroids. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371, 20150110. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholls, A. R., Polman, R. C. J., Levy, A. R., and Backhouse, S. H. (2008). Mental toughness, optimism, pessimism, and coping among athletes. Pers. Ind. Diff. 44, 1182–1192. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T., Keshavan, M., and Giedd, J. N. (2008). Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 947–957. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T., Zijdenbos, A., Worsley, K., Collins, D. L., Blumenthal, J., Giedd, J. N., et al. (1999). Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. Science 283, 1908–1911. doi: 10.1126/science.283.5409.1908

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perren, S., von Wyl, A., Stadelmann, S., Burgin, D., and von Klitzing, K. (2006). Associations between behavioral/emotional difficulties in kindergarten children and the quality of their peer relationships. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 45, 867–876. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000220853.71521.cb

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, J. L., Clough, P. J., Crust, L., Earle, K., and Nicholls, A. (2013). Factorial validity of the mental toughness questionnaire-48. Pers. Ind. Diff. 54, 587–592. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pihlakoski, L., Sourander, A., Aromaa, M., Rautava, P., Helenius, H., and Sillanpaa, M. (2006). The continuity of psychopathology from early childhood to preadolescence: a prospective cohort study of 3-12-year-old children. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 15, 409–417. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-0548-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, B. W., Caspi, A., and Moffitt, T. E. (2001). The kids are alright: growth and stability in personality development from adolescence to adulthood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 670–683. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.670

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, R. E., and Duong, H. T. (2014). The prospective association between sleep deprivation and depression among adolescents. Sleep 37, 239–244. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3388

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, R. E., and Duong, H. T. (2015). Is there an association between adolescent sleep restriction and obesity. J. Psychosom. Res. 79, 651–656. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.05.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, R. E., Roberts, C. R., and Duong, H. T. (2009). Sleepless in adolescence: prospective data on sleep deprivation, health and functioning. J. Adolesc. 32, 1045–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadeghi Bahmani, D., Gerber, M., Kalak, N., Lemola, S., Clough, P. J., Calabrese, P., et al. (2016). Mental toughness, sleep disturbances and physical activity in patients with multiple sclerosis (MS) compared to healthy adolescents and young adults. Neuropsychiatri. Dis. Treat. 12, 1571–1579. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S111208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sameroff, A. J., and Rosenblum, K. L. (2006). Psychosocial constraints on the development of resilience. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1094, 116–124. doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, D., Lansford, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., and Bates, J. E. (2015). Peer victimization during middle childhood as a lead indicator of internalizing problems and diagnostic outcomes in late adolescence. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 44, 393–404. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.881293

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Settles, R. E., Fischer, S., Cyders, M. A., Combs, J. L., Gunn, R. L., and Smith, G. T. (2012). Negative urgency: a personality predictor of externalizing behavior characterized by neuroticism, low conscientiousness, and disagreeableness. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121, 160–172. doi: 10.1037/a0024948

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheard, M. (2009). A cross-national analysis of mental toughness and hardiness in elite university rugby league teams. Percept. Mot. Skills 109, 213–223. doi: 10.2466/pms.109.1.213-223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, Y. M., Sung, M. J., Lim, K. Y., Park, K. S., and Cho, S. M. (2012). The pathway of internalizing and externalizing problems from childhood to adolescence: a prospective study from age 7 to 14-16 in Korea. Community Ment. Health J. 48, 384–391. doi: 10.1007/s10597-011-9468-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skala, K., and Bruckner, T. (2014). Beating the odds: an approach to the topic of resilience in children and adolescents. Neuropsychiatrie 28, 208–217. doi: 10.1007/s40211-014-0125-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stamp, E., Crust, L., Swann, C., Perry, J., Clough, P., and Marchant, D. (2015). Relationships between mental toughness and psychological wellbeing in undergraduate students. Pers. Ind. Diff. 75, 170–174. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thelwell, R., Weston, N., and Greenless, I. (2005). Defining and understanding mental toughness within soccer. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 17, 326–332. doi: 10.1080/10413200500313636

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisz, J. R., and Weiss, B. (1991). Studying the “referability” of child clinical problems. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 59, 266–273. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.59.2.266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wigfield, A., and Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 25, 68–81. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, M. M. (2010). Pubertal development, sleep problems, and alcohol use: a commentary. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 34, 2019–2021. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01299.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, M. M., Brower, K. J., Nigg, J. T., and Zucker, R. A. (2010). Childhood sleep problems, response inhibition, and alcohol and drug outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 34, 1033–1044. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01178.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mental toughness, sleep, origins, long-term, pro-social behavior, internalizing problems, externalizing problems

Citation: Sadeghi Bahmani D, Hatzinger M, Gerber M, Lemola S, Clough PJ, Perren S, von Klitzing K, von Wyl A, Holsboer-Trachsler E and Brand S (2016) The Origins of Mental Toughness – Prosocial Behavior and Low Internalizing and Externalizing Problems at Age 5 Predict Higher Mental Toughness Scores at Age 14. Front. Psychol. 7:1221. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01221

Received: 17 June 2016; Accepted: 02 August 2016;
Published: 24 August 2016.

Edited by:

Roumen Kirov, Bulgarian Academy of Sciences, Bulgaria

Reviewed by:

Éric Laurent, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, France
Peter Norton, Monash University, Australia

Copyright © 2016 Sadeghi Bahmani, Hatzinger, Gerber, Lemola, Clough, Perren, von Klitzing, von Wyl, Holsboer-Trachsler and Brand. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Serge Brand, serge.brand@upkbs.ch Dena Sadeghi Bahmani, dena.sadeghibahmani@upkbs.ch

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.