Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 27 September 2016
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Students at Risk of School Failure View all 55 articles

Sensation-Seeking and Impulsivity as Predictors of Reactive and Proactive Aggression in Adolescents

\r\nMaría Del Carmen Prez Fuentes*María Del Carmen Pérez Fuentes*Maria del Mar Molero JuradoMaria del Mar Molero JuradoJos J. Carrin MartínezJosé J. Carrión MartínezIsabel Mercader RubioIsabel Mercader RubioJos J. Gzquez\r\nJosé J. Gázquez
  • University of Almería, Almería, Spain

In adolescence, such matters as substance use and impulsiveness may give rise to problematic behavior repertoires. This study was therefore done to analyze the predictive value of sensation-seeking and impulsiveness dimensions related to the functions of aggression (reactive/proactive) and types of expression (physical/relational). A total of 822 high school students in Almeria (Spain) aged 13–18, were administered the Sensation-Seeking Scale, the State Impulsiveness Scale and Peer Conflict Scale. The results show the existence of a positive correlation of the majority of factors analyzed, both in impulsiveness and sensation-seeking, with respect to the different types of aggression. Furthermore, aggressive behavior is explained by the combination of a sensation-seeking factor (Disinhibition) and two impulsiveness factors (Gratification and Automatism). This study shows the need to analyze aggression as a multidimensional construct.

Introduction

Adolescence is a stage of change in which the individual must make decisions and respond to a diversity of situations. Such matters as substance use (Gázquez et al., 2015a) or how to interact with others (Inglés et al., 2014) could thus become repertoires of problem behavior in adolescents. In addition to individual factors (Gázquez et al., 2015b; Valle et al., 2015), adopting certain risk and/or problem behaviors also depends on other family (Martínez-Loredo et al., 2016) or peer group (Monahan et al., 2009) factors which are determining for the construction of self-concept and personal wellbeing (Nacimiento and Mora-Merchán, 2014; Álvarez et al., 2015; Azpiazua et al., 2015; Goñi et al., 2015).

Adolescence is also characterized by premature experimentation of new experiences and sensations. Jessor et al. (1998) argue that sensation-seeking can interfere with healthy adolescent development, and has been shown to be one of the most important risk factors in behavior problems. For many adolescents, the social setting inhibits imprudence, but for others it promotes risk-taking and emotion-seeking. These experiences sometimes include drug use, with negative consequences for their development that later become evident (Pérez-Fuentes et al., 2015). MacPherson et al. (2010) found that assuming risks was an important predictor of adolescent drinking. Curran et al. (2010) observed that adolescents who drive while under the effects of alcohol were strongly correlated with sensation-seeking factors, specifically with emotions and excitement, disinhibition and susceptibility to boredom.

An important matter related to the increase in sensation-seeking during adolescence and also aggressive behavior is impulsivity (Archer and Webb, 2006). This is defined as an expression of uninhibited behavior characterized by lack of control of behavior (Cardoso-Moreno et al., 2015). Given the role of impulsivity in involvement in risk behaviors by adolescents, the positive effect of interventions during childhood to prevent the first forms of impulsivity, which continue into adolescence if not treated, is clear.

Impulsivity and aggression maintain a pattern of consistent relationship. However, not everyone who is impulsive has aggressive behavior, nor is it manifested in the same way. For example, Hatfield and Dula (2014) found that high scores on impulsivity were associated with higher levels of physical or direct aggression. Grimaldi et al. (2014) suggest that relational aggressors may be more exposed to negative consequences related to alcohol when they respond impulsively to negative emotions.

Aggression, currently conceived as a multidimensional construct, can take many forms (García-Sancho et al., 2016). The functions of aggression refer to the aggressor’s motivation, and historically, two types have been distinguished, proactive and reactive (Hartup, 1974; Dodge and Coie, 1987). Proactive aggression refers to deliberate actions directed at achieving a goal, while reactive aggression refers to emotional response to attack. Thus different theoretical approaches postulate the intervention of different cognitive and social processes for each function (Gifford-Smith and Rabiner, 2004). The problem in detecting the relationships of the different aspects of proactive and reactive aggression is that the measures of these two functions are often intercorrelated (Dodge et al., 1990). However, later studies have shown that the two functions of aggression behave like two independent constructs (Poulin and Boivin, 2000), although they often occur at the same time (Bushman and Anderson, 2001). For example, reactive aggression is often related to problems with emotional regulation, internalization symptoms, rejection by classmates or victimization (Card and Little, 2006). In some cases this type of effect is seen in youth who reject school (Inglés et al., 2015). Subjects who show proactive aggression overestimate the positive consequences of aggression and minimize the probability of being punished for it (Marsee and Frick, 2007). In this respect, scientific evidence suggests that there is a relationship between proactive aggression and certain traits of insensitivity and lack of empathy or guilt (Frick and Dickens, 2006).

Thus both reactive and proactive aggression have been associated with negative effects for development of the individual (Hubbard et al., 2010), even with consequences in later stages (Cleverley et al., 2012). For example, such consequences as anxiety and depression in reactive aggressors (Fite et al., 2014) or the proliferation of antisocial/delinquent behavior with impulse control in proactive aggressors (Scarpa et al., 2010) have been observed.

Sensation-seeking has been related to the development of aggressive behavior (Wilson and Scarpa, 2011). Risk-taking, as proneness to acting impulsively to achieve reward even though there are negative consequences, would also be associated with aggression and delinquency (Romer, 2010). Previous studies on the relationship between personality and antisocial behavior have shown that both failure to control impulses and sensation-seeking are related to aggression and rule-breaking (Newcomb and McGee, 1991). Similarly, little inhibition in childhood leads to rule-breaking and becomes a risk factor for aggression in adolescents (Moeller et al., 2001).

Raine et al. (2006) found that low levels of inhibition and high sensation-seeking were present in adolescents with both reactive and proactive aggression. Findings such as these report the association between impulsive tendencies and the reactive and proactive forms of aggression. However, proactive aggressive individuals can show a stronger ability to regulate immediate aggressive impulses, channeling them into planned aggression (Dodge et al., 2006). Other authors, such as Steinberg et al. (2008) observed specific effects (not general) of sensation-seeking on deviant behavior and attitudes in adolescents who years before has used drugs and/or had participated in delinquent activities.

Recently, authors like Cui et al. (2016) have concluded that children whose high levels of reactive and proactive aggression persisted over time also had high levels of sensation-seeking and risk-taking, as well as low levels of moral reasoning.

Moreover, two forms of aggression related to interpersonal relationships may be distinguished (Buss, 1961; Valzelli, 1983; Grotpeter and Crick, 1996), physical (direct) and relational (indirect) aggression. Some studies (Stickle et al., 2012) mention that males and females show signs of relational aggression in different ways. Centifanti et al. (2015) suggested that individual factors are associated with participation in relational aggression by girls and may therefore be an indicator of problem behavior.

The purpose of this study was to find out the predictive value of variables related to sensation-seeking and impulsivity related to the functions of aggression (reactive/proactive) and its forms of expression (physical/relational) in adolescents.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A sample of 822 high school students was selected by cluster sampling from eight high schools in the province of Almeria (Spain). The participants were aged 13–18 with a mean of 14.84 years (SD = 0.87). Of the total simple, 51.8% (n = 426) were males and 48.2% (n = 396) females, with mean ages of 14.85 (SD = 0.87) and 14.82 years (SD = 0.86), respectively. The distribution of our sample by academic year was: 43.7% were 3rd year high school students (n = 359) and the remaining 56.3% were in 4th year (n = 463).

Instruments

Escala de Búsqueda de Sensaciones [Sensation-Seeking Scale] (Pérez and Torrubia, 1986). This scale measures the tendency to seek new risky experiences. It consists of a total of 40 items with dichotomous answers (yes/no) on four subscales: Emotion-seeking (BEM), Excitement-seeking (BEX), Disinhibtion (DES) and Susceptibility to Boredom (SAB). The authors found reliability coefficients over 0.87 for the whole questionnaire scale and Cronbach’s alpha of 0.70–0.87 on the subscales.

Escala de Impulsividad Estado [State Impulsivity Scale] (EIE; Iribarren et al., 2011). This scale was developed to evaluate impulsive behavior defined as a state, that is, impulsivity as a manifest behavior that can vary in the short term. It consists of 20 items, with a response format based on a four-point Likert-type scale in which the subject is asked to evaluate the frequency with which each of the statements is true. The items that make up the scale are grouped into three subscales: Gratification (urgency in satisfying impulses, preference for immediate reward, intolerance to frustration and tendency to act without thinking of negative consequences); Automatism (repeated, rigidly expressed behavior, with no attention to contextual variables); and Attentional (presence of unplanned behavior which takes place too soon without considering all the information available). The authors found high reliability for the complete scale (α = 0.88) and each of its dimensions: Gratification (α = 0.84), Automatism (α = 0.80) and Attentional (α = 0.75).

Peer Conflict Scale (PCS; Marsee et al., 2004). This is a self-report scale developed to evaluate the forms and functions of aggression. It consists of 40 items distributed among four subscales: Physical Proactive Aggression, Physical Reactive Aggression, Relational Proactive Aggression and Relational Reactive Aggression. The response format is based on a four-point Likert-type scale (0 = not at all true, 1 = somewhat true, 2 = very true and 3 = definitely true), and the elements are grouped in four subscales, scoring 0–30 on each. The authors (Marsee et al., 2011), found satisfactory internal consistency for each of the subscales (Physical Reactive α = 0.87; Reactive Relational α = 0.77; Physical Proactive α = 0.79; Relational Proactive α = 0.76).

Procedure

This study was exempt from ethical approval, because the study did not involve any potential risk for the participants. All participants provided written consent. Before collecting data, a meeting was held with the school directors/counselors where they were informed of the purposes, procedure and use of research data. When the tests were administered, the participants were guaranteed confidential data processing and given instructions for their completion. They were also informed that they were voluntary, anonymous and that their data were protected by applicable legislation. Two members of the research team traveled to the high schools to administer the tests.

Data Analysis

First, to identify the variables related to the different forms of aggression analyzed (Physical Proactive, Physical Reactive, Relational Proactive, and Relational Reactive), the Pearson’s correlation coefficient was calculated as well as the corresponding descriptive statistics.

Stepwise multiple linear regression analysis was performed to find out how the predictor variables (Sensation-seeking: Gratification, Automatism and Attentional; Impulsivity: Emotion-seeking, Excitement-seeking, Disinhibition and Susceptibility to boredom) were related to each of the criterion variables.

Results

Sensation-Seeking and Impulsivity Factors Related to Forms of Aggression

The correlation coefficients found show the existence of positive correlations between impulsivity (Gratification, Automatism and Attentional) and sensation-seeking (Emotion-seeking, Excitement-seeking, Disinhibition and Susceptibility to boredom) factors and the forms of aggression analyzed.

As observed in Table 1, adolescents with high scores in physical proactive aggression (AgPA) showed high levels of Gratification (r = 0.31; p < 0.001), Automatism (r = 0.26; p < 0.001), Attentional impulsivity (r = 0.24; p < 0.001), Emotion-seeking (r = 0.06; p < 0.05), Disinhibition (r = 0.35; p < 0.001) and Susceptibility to boredom (r = 0.17; p < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics of impulsivity and sensation-seeking factors and correlation coefficients with form of aggression (N = 822).

Furthermore, high scores on Relational Proactive Aggression (AgPR) correlated positively with all the Impulsivity factors (Gratification: r = 0.30; p < 0.001; Automatism: r = 0.26; p < 0.001; Attentional: r = 0.24; p < 0.001), Disinhibition (r = 0.31; p < 0.001) and Susceptibility to boredom (r = 0.16; p < 0.001). Physical Reactive Aggression (AgRA) as a component of aggression may be seen in Table 1 to be correlated with all the components of Impulsivity (Gratification: r = 0.35; p < 0.001; Automatism: r = 0.32; p < 0.001; Attentional: r = 0.32; p < 0.001 and sensation-seeking (Emotion-seeking: r = 0.13; p < 0.001; Excitement-seeking: r = 0.09; p < 0.01; Desinhibition: r = 0.40; p < 0.001; Susceptibility to boredom: r = 0.22; p < 0.001).

Finally, as shown by the correlation coefficients found for Relational Reactive Aggression (AgRR), adolescents who showed this form of aggression were also observed to have high scores on Gratification (r = 0.31; p < 0.001), Automatism (r = 0.26; p < 0.001), Attentional (r = 0.25; p < 0.001), Emotion-seeking (r = 0.07; p < 0.05), Excitement-seeking (r = 0.05; p < 0.05), Desinhibition (r = 0.31; p < 0.001) and Susceptibility to boredom (r = 0.19; p < 0.001).

Proactive Aggression (Physical/Relational) Predictor Variables

Regression analysis yielded three models for Physical Proactive Aggression (Table 2) of which Model 3 has the most explanatory power with 16.2% (R2 = 0.16) of the variance explained by the factors included in the model.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Stepwise multiple linear regression (Physical Proactive Aggression).

To confirm model validity, independence of residuals was analyzed. The Durbin–Watson D statistic found was D = 1.78, which confirms the absence of positive and negative autocorrelation.

As shown in Table 2, the T was associated with a probability of error below 0.05 in all the variables included in the model. Furthermore, the standardized coefficients revealed that the variables with the most explanatory weight were Disinhibition, Automatism and Gratification, and the first of them (Disinhibition) was the strongest predictor of Physical Proactive Aggression. Finally, the absence of collinearity between variables included in the model may be assumed as tolerance is high and VIF is low.

The three Relational Proactive Aggression models resulting from the regression analysis are shown in Table 3, where Model 3 found 14.1% explained variance (R2 = 0.14). In this case, the Dubin–Watson D confirmed no correlation of residuals (D = 1.68).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Stepwise multiple linear regression (Relational Proactive Aggression).

The T statistic shows an association with a probability of error below 0.05 for all the variables included in the model, Gratification, Desinhibition and Automatism. According to the standardized coefficients found in this case, the Disinhibition variable is the strongest predictor of relational proactive aggression.

In view of the values found for the Tolerance and VIF indicators, in this case, collinearity of variables is assumed to be absent, since tolerance is high and VIF is low.

Reactive Aggression (Physical/Relational) Predictor Variables

As shown in Table 4, three models were found when Physical Reactive Aggression was the variable entered, the third of which had the most explanatory power. The Automatism, Gratification and Disinhibition variables included in the model explained 21.5% of the variance (R2 = 0.21) in physical reactive aggression. The validity of the model is also reflected in the independence of the residuals with a Durbin–Watson D = 1.85.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Stepwise multiple linear regression (Physical Reactive Aggression).

The T statistic is associated with a probability of error of less than 0.05 in the five variables in the model (Table 4). And according to the standardized coefficients, the variables with the highest explanatory weight are Disinhibition, Automatism and Gratification. These coefficients show the Disinhibition variable to be the strongest predictor of physical reactive aggression. In this case, collinearity of variables is assumed to be absent as indicated by high tolerance and low VIF.

As the result of multiple regression analysis, three models are also found for Relational Reactive Aggression, of which Model 3 is the one with the highest explanatory power, with 14.4% (R2 = 0.14) of the variance explained by the factors included in the model (Table 5).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Stepwise multiple linear regression (Relacional Reactiva Agresión).

To confirm the validity of the model, the independence of residuals was analyzed. The Durbin–Watson D was D = 1.65, confirming absence of positive and negative autocorrelation.

Table 5 shows that the T value is associated with a probability of error of less than 0.05 in all the variables included in the model. Furthermore, the standardized coefficients reveal that the Disinhibition factor is the strongest predictor of relational reactive aggression.

Finally, collinearity of the variables included in the model is assumed to be absent since tolerance indicators are high and VIF is low.

Discussion and Conclusion

In the first place, results show the existence of a positive correlation of most of the impulsivity and sensation-seeking factors analyzed with the different forms of aggression (Newcomb and McGee, 1991; Raine et al., 2006). The exceptional cases in which there is no correlation refer to proactive aggression and emotion/excitement-seeking. It is reasonable to argue that sensation-seeking and risk-taking become more evident in children with reactive aggression, since they tend to seek excitement and act on their impulses in the rush of the moment, while children with proactive aggression are able to channel their aggressive behavior in a more calculated manner (Dodge et al., 2006). According to Steinberg et al. (2008), sensation-seeking could have specific effects on deviant behavior. Thus the expression of a certain type of aggression could be mediated by individual factors (Centifanti et al., 2015; Gázquez et al., 2015b), cognitive and social processes (Gifford-Smith and Rabiner, 2004; Monahan et al., 2009; Martínez-Loredo et al., 2016) and even characterization of the various expressions of aggression itself (Stickle et al., 2012).

The purpose of this study was to find out the predictive value of sensation seeking and impulsivity for the functions of aggression (reactive/proactive) and its forms of expression (physical/relational) in adolescents. The results of multiple regression analysis reveal that in all cases, aggressive behavior is explained by the combination of a sensation-seeking factor (Disinhibition) and two impulsivity factors (Gratification and Automatism). The fact that they are the same components that combine to construct the model of each of the forms of aggression analyzed (García-Sancho et al., 2016) supports the idea of interrelation of the functions of aggression (Dodge et al., 1990).

The presence of the Disinhibition component as one of the aggression predictor variables (Wilson and Scarpa, 2011) demonstrates the tendency to experience new sensations during adolescence (Jessor et al., 1998). This orientation toward sensation-seeking is present not only in the development of aggressive behavior, but is also found as a predictor in other adolescent problem behavior such as substance use (Curran et al., 2010). The inclusion of impulsivity factors, which are also present in other adolescent risk behaviors (MacPherson et al., 2010), in the models reflects their association with aggression in all its forms (Moeller et al., 2001; Romer, 2010).

Relatively few studies have considered sensation-seeking and impulsivity in relation to reactive and proactive aggression (Cui et al., 2016). Findings such as these show the need to give the analysis of aggression as a multidimensional construct more attention (García-Sancho et al., 2016) to the extent that its different functions and forms of expression have been identified as responsible for a variety of effects in adolescent development (Hubbard et al., 2010; Scarpa et al., 2010; Fite et al., 2014), and even afterward (Cleverley et al., 2012).

The main limitations of the study are that: (1) The sample, although representative, is comprised only of high school students and cannot be generalized to other grade levels, and therefore, one of the future lines of research is the replication of this study in other years; and, (2) the biases typical of self-report techniques, for example, the associations found with the effects of social desirability, which with age show positive relationships to certain desirable characteristics in the self-report.

However, although this study does have some limitations which should be kept in mind for future research, it may be considered a precursor, and is of great interest for the relevant data it contributes to the design of interventions which make it possible to work on reducing risk factors and strengthening those which protect against aggressive behavior.

Therefore, progress made in research along this line requires development of future analyses based on causal models and the analysis of mediating factors in aggressive behavior in all of its forms. In other words, adolescent intervention must be able to counteract the tendency to sensation-seeking and any other form of impulsivity related to the origin and/or maintenance of aggressive behavior.

Author Contributions

MP and MJ (Drafting and analysis of data). JM and IR (bibliographic search). JG (Reviewers made changes).

Funding

This study was done with the collaboration of the Almeria Provincial Government.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Álvarez, A., Suárez, N., Tuero, E., Núñez, J., Valle, A., and Regueiro, B. (2015). Implicación familiar, autoconcepto del adolescente y rendimiento académico [Family involvement, adolescent self-concept and academic achievement]. Eur. J. Invest. 5, 293–311. doi: 10.1989/ejihpe.v5i3.133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archer, J., and Webb, I. A. (2006). The relation between scores on the buss-perry aggression questionnaire and aggressive acts, impulsiveness, competitiveness, dominance, and sexual jealousy. Aggress. Behav. 32, 464–473. doi: 10.1002/ab.20146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azpiazua, L., Esnaola, I., and Sarasa, M. (2015). Capacidad predictiva del apoyo social en la inteligencia emocional de adolescentes [Predictive capacity of social support on emotional intelligence in adolescence]. Eur. J. Educ. Psychol. 8, 23–29. doi: 10.1016/j.ejeps.2015.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bushman, B. J., and Anderson, C. A. (2001). Is it Time to pull the plug on the hostile versus instrumental aggression dichotomy? Psychol. Rev. 108, 273–279. doi: 10.1037/1076-8971.13.2.143

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, A. H. (1961). The Psychology of Aggression. Oxford: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Card, N. A., and Little, T. D. (2006). Proactive and reactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: a meta-analysis of differential relations with psychosocial adjustment. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 30, 466–480. doi: 10.1177/0165025406071904

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardoso-Moreno, M. J., Tomás-Aragonés, L., and Rodríguez-Ledo, C. (2015). Socio-emotional intervention in attention déficit hyperactive disorder. Eur. J. Educ. Psychol. 8, 53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.ejeps.2015.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Centifanti, L. C. M., Fanti, K. A., Thomson, N. D., Demetriou, V., and Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous, X. (2015). Types of relational aggression in girls are differentiated by callous-unemotional traits, peers and parental overcontrol. Behav. Sci. 5, 518–536. doi: 10.3390/bs5040518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleverley, K., Szatmari, P., Vaillancourt, T., Boyle, M., and Lipman, E. (2012). Developmental trajectories of physical and indirect aggression from late childhood to adolescence: sex differences and outcomes in emerging adulthood. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51, 1037–1051. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.07.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, L., Colasante, T., Malti, T., Ribeaud, D., and Eisner, M. P. (2016). Dual trajectories of reactive and proactive aggression from mid-childhood to early adolescence: relations to sensation seeking, risk taking, and moral reasoning. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 44, 663–675. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0079-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curran, M. F., Fuertes, J. N., Alfonso, V. C., and Hennessy, J. J. (2010). The association of sensation seeking and impulsivity to driving while under the influence of alcohol. J. Addict. Offender Couns. 30, 84–98. doi: 10.1002/j.2161-1874.2010.tb00059.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodge, K. A., and Coie, J. D. (1987). Social information-processing factors in reactive and instrumental aggression in children’s playgroups. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 53, 1146–1158. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., and Lynam, D. (2006). “Aggression and antisocial behavior in youth,” in Handbook of Child Psychology. Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, Vol. 3, eds N. Eisenberg, D. William, and R.M. Lerner (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 719–788.

Google Scholar

Dodge, K. A., Price, J. M., Bachorowski, J. A., and Newman, J. P. (1990). Hostile attributional biases in severely aggressive adolescents. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 99, 385–392. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.99.4.385

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fite, P. J., Rubens, S. L., Preddy, T. M., Raine, A., and Pardini, D. A. (2014). Reactive/proactive aggression and the development of internalizing problems in males: the moderating effect of parent and peer relationships. Aggress. Behav. 40, 69–78. doi: 10.1002/ab.21498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frick, P. J., and Dickens, C. (2006). Current perspectives on conduct disorder. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 8, 59–72. doi: 10.1007/s11920-006-0082-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Sancho, E., Salguero, J. M., Vasquez, E. A., and Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2016). Validity and reliability of the Spanish version of the displaced aggression questionnaire. Psicothem 28, 96–101. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2015.222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gázquez, J. J., Pérez-Fuentes, M. C., Carrión, J. J., Luque, A., and Molero, M. M. (2015b). Interpersonal value profiles and analysis to adolescent behavior and social attitudes. J. Psychodidact. 20, 321–337. doi: 10.1387/RevPsicodidact.12978

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gázquez, J. J., Pérez-Fuentes, M. C., Molero, M. M., Martos, A., Cardila, F., Barragán, A. B., et al. (2015a). Adaptación española del cuestionario de expectativas del alcohol en adolescentes [Spanish adaptation of the alcohol expectancy-adolescent questionnaire, brief]. Eur. J. Invest. 5, 357–369. doi: 10.1989/ejihpe.v5i3.138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gifford-Smith, M. E., and Rabiner, D. L. (2004). “Social information processing an children’s social adjustment,” in Children’s Peer Relations: From Development to Intervention, eds J. B. Kupersmidt and K. A. Dodge (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 61–79.

Google Scholar

Goñi, E., Esnaola, I., Rodríguez, A., and Ortiz de Barrón, I. (2015). Personal self-concept and satisfaction with life in adolescence, youth and adulthood. Psicothema 27, 52–58. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2014.105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grimaldi, E. M., Napper, L. E., and LaBrie, J. W. (2014). Relational aggression, positive urgency and negative urgency: predicting alcohol use and consequences among college students. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 28, 893–898. doi: 10.1037/a0037354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grotpeter, J. K., and Crick, N. R. (1996). Relational aggression, overt aggression, and friendship. Child Dev. 67, 2328–2338. doi: 10.2307/1131626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartup, W. W. (1974). Aggression in childhood: developmental perspectives. Am. Psychol. 29, 336–341. doi: 10.1037/h0037622

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatfield, J., and Dula, C. S. (2014). Impulsivity and physical aggression: examining the moderating role of anxiety. Am. J. Psychol. 127, 233–243. doi: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.127.2.0233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubbard, J. A., McAuliffe, M. D., Morrow, M. T., and Romano, L. J. (2010). Reactive and proactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: precursors, outcomes, processes, experiences, and measurement. J. Pers. 78, 95–118. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00610.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inglés, C. J., Gonzálvez-Maciá, C., García-Fernández, J. M., Vicent, M., and Martínez-Monteagudo, M. C. (2015). Current status of research on school refusal. Eur. J. Educ. Psychol. 8, 37–52. doi: 10.1016/j.ejeps.2015.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inglés, C. J., Torregrosa, M. S., García-Fernández, J. M., Martínez-Monteagudo, M. C., Estévez, E., and Delgado, B. (2014). Conducta agresiva e inteligencia emocional en la adolescencia [Aggressive behavior and Emotional Intelligence in adolescence]. Eur. J. Educ. Psychol. 7, 29–41. doi: 10.1989/ejep.v7i1.150

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iribarren, M. M., Jiménez-Giménez, M., García-de Cecilia, J. M., and Rubio-Valladolid, G. (2011). Validación y propiedades psicométricas de la escala de impulsividad estado (EIE) [Validation and psychometric properties of the state impulsivity scale (SIS)]. Actas Esp. Psiquiatr. 39, 49–60.

Google Scholar

Jessor, R., Turbin, M. S., and Costa, F. M. (1998). Protective factors in adolescent health behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75, 788–800. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.788

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacPherson, L., Magidson, J. F., Reynolds, E. K., Kahler, C. W., and Lejuez, C. W. (2010). Changes in sensation seeking and risk-taking propensity predict increases in alcohol use among early adolescents. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 34, 1400–1408. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01223.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsee, M. A., Barry, C. T., Childs, K. K., Frick, P. J., Kimonis, E. R., Muñoz, L. C., et al. (2011). Assessing the forms and functions of aggression using self-report: factor structure and invariance of the Peer Conflict Scale in youths. Psychol. Assess. 23, 792–804. doi: 10.1037/a0023369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsee, M. A., and Frick, P. J. (2007). Exploring the cognitive and emotional correlates to proactive and reactive aggression in a sample of detained girls. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 35, 969–981. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9147-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsee, M. A., Kimonis, E. R., and Frick, P. J. (2004). Peer Conflict Scale. New Orleans, LA: University of New Orleans.

Google Scholar

Martínez-Loredo, V., Fernández-Artamendi, S., Weidberg, S., Pericot, I., López-Núñez, C., Fernández-Hermida, J., et al. (2016). Parenting styles and alcohol use among adolescents: a longitudinal study. Eur. J. Invest. 6, 27–36.

Google Scholar

Moeller, F. G., Barratt, E. S., Dougherty, D. M., Schmitz, J. M., and Swann, A. C. (2001). Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 1783–1793. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monahan, K. C., Steinberg, L., and Cauffman, E. (2009). Affiliation with antisocial peers, susceptibility to peer influence, and antisocial behavior during the transition to adulthood. Dev. Psychol. 45, 1520–1530. doi: 10.1037/a0017417

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nacimiento, R. L., and Mora-Merchán, J. A. (2014). El uso de estrategias de afrontamiento y habilidades metacognitivas ante situaciones de bullying y cyberbullying [The use of coping strategies and metacognitive skills in situations of bullying and cyberbullying]. Eur. J. Educ. Psychol. 7, 121–129. doi: 10.1989/ejep.v7i2.184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newcomb, M. D., and McGee, L. (1991). Influence of sensation seeking on general deviance and specific problem behaviors from adolescence to young adulthood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 614–628. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.4.614

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez, J., and Torrubia, R. (1986). Fiabilidad y validez de la versión española de la escala de búsqueda de sensaciones (Forma V) [Reliability and validity of the Spanish version of the sensation seeking scale (Form V)]. Rev. Latinoam. Psicol. 18, 7–22.

Google Scholar

Pérez-Fuentes, M., Gázquez, J., Molero, M., Cardila, F., Martos, A., Barragán, A., et al. (2015). Impulsividad y consumo de alcohol y tabaco en adolescentes [Adolescent impulsiveness and use of alcohol and tobacco]. Eur. J. Invest. 5, 371–382. doi: 10.1989/ejihpe.v5i3.139

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poulin, F., and Boivin, M. (2000). Reactive and proactive aggression: evidence of a two-factor model. Psychol. Assess. 12, 115–122. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.12.2.115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raine, A., Dodge, K., Loeber, R., Gatzke-Kopp, L., Lynam, D., Reynolds, C., et al. (2006). The reactive-proactive aggression questionnaire: differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggress. Behav. 32, 159–171. doi: 10.1002/ab.20115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romer, D. (2010). Adolescent risk taking, impulsivity, and brain development: implications for prevention. Dev. Psychobiol. 52, 263–276. doi: 10.1002/dev.20442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarpa, A., Haden, S. C., and Tanaka, A. (2010). Being hot-tempered: autonomic, emotional, and behavioral distinctions between childhood reactive and proactive aggression. Biol. Psychol. 84, 488–496. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., and Graham, S. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1764–1778. doi: 10.1037/a0012955

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stickle, T. R., Marini, V. A., and Thomas, J. N. (2012). Gender differences in psychopathic traits, types, and correlates of aggression among adjudicated youth. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 40, 513–525. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9588-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valle, A., Regueiro, B., Rodríguez, S., Piñeiro, I., Freire, C., Ferradás, M. M., et al. (2015). Perfiles motivacionales como combinación de expectativas de autoeficacia y metas académicas en estudiantes universitarios [Motivational profiles as a combination of self-efficacy expectations and academic goals in university students]. Eur. J. Educ. Psychol. 8, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ejeps.2015.10.001

CrossRef Full Text

Valzelli, L. (1983). Psicobiología de la Agresión y la Violencia [Psychobiology of Aggression and Violence]. Madrid: Alhambra.

Wilson, L. C., and Scarpa, A. (2011). The link between sensation seeking and aggression: a meta-analytic review. Aggress. Behav. 37, 81–90. doi: 10.1002/ab.20369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sensation-seeking, impulsivity, proactive aggression, reactive aggression, adolescent

Citation: Pérez Fuentes MDC, Molero Jurado MdM, Carrión Martínez JJ, Mercader Rubio I and Gázquez JJ (2016) Sensation-Seeking and Impulsivity as Predictors of Reactive and Proactive Aggression in Adolescents. Front. Psychol. 7:1447. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01447

Received: 03 August 2016; Accepted: 09 September 2016;
Published: 27 September 2016.

Edited by:

José Carlos Núñez, University of Oviedo, Spain

Reviewed by:

Alejandro Veas, University of Alicante, Spain
Antonio Valle, University of A Coruña, Spain

Copyright © 2016 Pérez Fuentes, Molero Jurado, Carrión Martínez, Mercader Rubio and Gázquez. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: María Del Carmen Pérez Fuentes, mpf421@ual.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.