Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 November 2016
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Students at Risk of School Failure View all 55 articles

Understanding Risk-taking Behavior in Bullies, Victims, and Bully Victims Using Cognitive- and Emotion-Focused Approaches

  • Department of Special Education and Counselling, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong

Bullying and risky behavior are two common problems among adolescents and can strongly affect a youth’s overall functioning when both coexist. Some studies suggest that bullying in adolescence may promote risky behavior as a coping strategy to deal with victimization related stress. Other studies consider bullying as an outcome of high-risk behavior. Despite the association between the two is well-established, no study has examined the risk-taking patterns among bullying groups (i.e., bully, victim, and bully victim). This study attempted to elucidate the potential relationships between bullying and risk-taking by addressing the two models: a cognitive-focused model and an emotion-focused model of risk taking, and to clarify how adolescents’ characteristics in risk taking associate with bullying outcomes.

Method: 136 Chinese adolescents (Mean Age = 14.5, M = 65, F = 71) were recruited and grouped according to bullying identity: Bully (n = 27), Victim (n = 20), Bully victim (n = 37) and Control (n = 52). Cognitive Appraisal of Risky Events (CARE) questionnaire was used to measure participants’ expectancies about the risks, benefits and involvement associated with risky activities. Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT) was administered to capture the emotion-laden process in risk taking.

Results: Cognitively, Bully was associated with an overestimation of risk while Victim was associated with an underestimation of risk and overrated benefit. Bully victim exhibited a unique pattern with an overestimation of benefit and risk. All study groups projected higher involvement in risky behavior. Behaviorally, both Bully and Bully victim were associated with high risk modulation whereas Victim was associated with impulsive decision-making. Interestingly, compared with bully, bully victim had significantly higher bullying scores, suggesting a wider range and more frequent bullying activities. In conclusion, Bully maybe a group of adolescents that is vigilant in situational deliberation and risk modulation while Victims with high impulsivity, are more likely to place themselves in risky situations. Bully victims presented the combined pattern of the two pure groups and associated with the highest risk-taking propensity. Better picture of risk taking pattern associated with different groups was illustrated, allowing better matching for future prevention and intervention program for distinct bullying individuals.

Introduction

Bullying and victimization among youth is a serious and complex problem that is receiving increased attention. Bullying has been conceptualized as a distinct type of aggression characterized by a repeated and systematic abuse of power (Ttofi et al., 2012). It encompasses a spectrum of both physical and verbal aggressive actions. It can be direct (e.g., hitting, kicking, threatening, and extortion) or indirect (e.g., spreading rumors and social exclusion) (Karatzias et al., 2002). Because bullying involves a bully and a victim, early research tended to dichotomize children into one of these two mutually exclusive groups. However, there also appears to be a third group of bully victims who both bully and are bullied by others (Haynie, 2001; Veenstra et al., 2005).

Bullying and high-risk activities are two common problems among adolescents worldwide and can strongly affect a youth’s physical, psychological, social, and educational functioning when both coexist (Currie et al., 2012). Recently, the link between bullying and risk-taking behavior is an emergent area of research. To date, studies have found associations between bullying and delinquency (Olweus, 1979), alcohol/substance use (Kaltiala-Heino et al., 2000; Nansel et al., 2001; Carlyle and Steinman, 2007), smoking (Ellickson et al., 1997; Forero et al., 1999), and unprotected sex (Liang et al., 2007). In particular, bullies and bully victims often exhibit the highest rate of wide ranged risk-taking behaviors (Haynie, 2001). On the other hand, there are major inconsistencies in the literature examining the association between victims and risk-taking behaviors. For example, some studies have suggested that adolescent victims are at a higher risk of heavy drinking and substance abuse than non-victim adolescents are (Wills and Filer, 1996; Khantzian, 1997; Maniglio, 2009). Other studies have revealed that victims of bullying had lower levels of smoking (Forero et al., 1999) and alcohol consumption (Nansel et al., 2001; Carlyle and Steinman, 2007) when compared to controls. Studies that explain the association between bullying and risk-taking behavior are bi-directional (e.g., Collier et al., 2013). Some studies suggest that bullying or other forms of peer victimization in adolescence may promote risky behavior such as substance abuse as a coping strategy or self-medication attempting to deal with or anesthetize victimization related stress or negative feelings (Danielson et al., 2010; Durand et al., 2013; Hong et al., 2014). However, other studies consider bullying as an outcome of, rather than a risk factor for, high-risk behavior. Despite the association between bullying and risk-taking behaviors, no study to date has examined risk-taking patterns among the three bully groups.

Risk Taking: The Decision-Making Model and the Social-Neuroscience Model

Both human and animal studies of risk-taking behavior have proposed models that predict risky choice. Recent studies have provided two new perspectives: the decision-making model (e.g., Millstein and Halpern-Felsher, 2001) and the social-neuroscience model (e.g., Steinberg, 2008). Decision-making theorists posit that individuals’ beliefs about the consequences of their actions and perceptions of their vulnerability regarding those consequences play a key role in their behavior (Millstein and Halpern-Felsher, 2001). Researchers are interested in understanding why adolescents make the decisions they do and their competence in making these decisions. In conceptualizing and measuring perceptions of risk, decision-making theorists observe whether people recognize the benefits and risks inherent in a given situation in order to measure their sense of risk and vulnerability (Slovic, 1987; Siegrist et al., 2000). Recent studies have suggested that engagement in risky activities is determined by individual differences in cognitive appraisal (expected benefit minus expected risk) (Fromme et al., 1997, 1999). When a person overestimates the benefit of or underestimates the risk of certain events it increases the likelihood of their participation in a risky activity (Byrnes, 1998; Steinberg, 2004, 2005). In short, decision theorists generally believe that by adopting a risk-benefit analysis, one might obtain the net expectancy of the risky event, which might provide useful information in predicting an individual’s involvement in risk-taking behavior (Steinberg, 2005). Under the decision-making model, risk perceptions play a fundamental role in behavioral intervention programs that try to encourage adolescents to recognize and acknowledge their own vulnerability to negative outcomes.

School bullying involves repeated aggression toward those who are perceived as weaker and less dominant and it has negative consequences for both bullies and victims (Ttofi et al., 2012). Many factors play an important role in school aggression: individual differences on how one encodes and emotionally processes evocative situations, interpretation and response to the behaviors of others, and motivation toward obtaining reward and avoiding punishment (e.g., Crick and Dodge, 1996). Another view of risk-taking behavior highlights the role of affective intensity and sensitivity to reward. According to this perspective, risky behavior cannot be fully explained by a deficiency in comprehending the potential consequences of these actions (Reyna and Farley, 2006). Moreover, leading environmental cues may “win” over cognitive control in emotionally charged circumstances. This model is supported by epidemiological reports of heightened affective responsiveness and incentive-based behavior changes when cognitive-based measures are hampered. Steinberg (2010) examined the social-neuroscience model of risk taking using behavioral measures. Steinberg used a gambling task to capture the reward-related decision-making process of participants and concluded that a heightened reward sensitivity and impulsivity were seen consistently and clearly in high-risk takers.

Study Objectives

In sum, while many studies on bullying and its associations with a variety of risk-taking behaviors have been performed, it is surprising that no study has examined and compared the risk-taking pattern among bullying groups (i.e., bully, victim, and bully victim). This study investigated the link between risk taking and bullying in adolescents in hopes to elucidate the potential relationship between bullying and risk-taking behavior by addressing the two models of risk taking. Within the framework of these two models, this study hopes to offer a holistic model for understanding and differentiating a risk-taking pattern among the three bullying groups. Particularly, this study adopted a risk-appraisal questionnaire to address a cognitively focused process and a computerized gambling task to measure a motivational, emotion-focused process. It was hypothesized that bullies and victims would exhibit different patterns of risk appraisal and propensity. Moreover, bully victims were expected to present co-morbid patterns of both bullies and victims.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The study sampled students from Hong Kong public secondary schools. A multi-stage sampling procedure was used to obtain the sample. Mass invitation has been sent to all public secondary schools in Hong Kong. Schools to be studied were then randomly selected such that the proportion of students in each selected district represented the number of students in that area. This selection procedure resulted in the selection of 4 schools respectively with one from Hong Kong Island, one from Kowloon Peninsula, and two from the New Territories. As schools were given the option to withdraw from participation, a further four schools were selected by the same procedure to act as a back-up. Schools that withdrew from participation were replaced by schools on the back-up list from within the same district. Of the originally selected schools, none withdrew from participation. From each school, six students were randomly selected from grade 7 to grade 12, such that a total of 36 students were selected per school for participation. With eight students dropped out in the middle of the study, 136 adolescents were recruited and tested. Participants ranged in age from 12 to 17 years (M = 14.45, SD = 1.614) and comprised of 65 males (M) (47.8%) and 71 females (F) (52.2%). Based on self-reported experience of school bullying and victimization in the last 4 months, the participants were categorized into four distinct groups: control group (n = 52, M = 19, F = 33), bully group (n = 27, M = 10, F = 17), victim group (n = 20, M = 10, F = 10), and bully victim group (n = 37, M = 26, F = 11). All participants had a clean medical history; spoke Cantonese as their first language; had normal intelligence; and had no suspected brain damage or neurological, sensory, or psychiatric problems.

Materials

This study consisted of a screening phase and an assessment phase. During the screening phase, participants were administered a standard Raven Progressive Matrices (RPM) as a proxy of intelligence in order to rule out low intellectual functioning. Only students with normal intelligence were invited to the assessment phase. During the assessment phase, the Computerized Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT) was administered individually to each participant. Participants then completed a demographic questionnaire including questions about age, educational level, family income, and the Cognitive Appraisal of Risky Events Questionnaire (CARE). The assessment phrase lasted approximately 1 h. All assessments were conducted by well-trained research assistants with undergraduate degrees in psychology who had been trained for approximately 2 h in test administration. All parents and adolescents provided informed consent to participate in this study. Ethical approval for the research was gained through the Human Research Ethics Committee at the Education University of Hong Kong.

Measures

Non-verbal Intelligence

A traditional RPM (Raven et al., 2000) was employed in this study for screening purposes. It is a non-verbal, intellect measure that captures both analytic reasoning and visual-spatial reasoning ability. It comprises 60 items. Participants first see one implicitly meaningful diagram with one missing piece and six choices. The goal is to complete the diagram by identifying the correct piece. As the test proceeds, participants will face tougher decisions with additional implicit rulings, diagrams, and similar answers introduced. Only participants who score in the 80th percentile or above re selected and proceed to the assessment phase. Since items serves as simple, perceptual-motor control it is capable to provide a pure measure of “g factor” (deductive and reproductive ability) regardless of cultural and knowledge influence (Prabhakaran et al., 1997).

School-Bullying

A school-bullying questionnaire (Ng and Tsang, 2008; Wong et al., 2008) was used in this study for grouping and measuring the frequency of being bullied or victimized. The questionnaire comprised three subscales: (a) the witness, (b) the bully, and (c) the victim, which include identical items with only a difference in perspective wordings (i.e., witnessed, bullied, or victimized). Only the bully and victim subscales were administrated in this study (see Appendix A). Each subscale measured the rate of bullying with five categories: physical, verbal, relational, extortion, and intimidation on a 4-point frequency scale consisting of 0 (never), 1 (at least once per month), 2 (once per week), and 3 (everyday). Participants were placed into corresponding groups according to features of bullying identity. The control group comprised participants who did not indicate any bullying-related experience, the bully group comprised participants who indicated experience in bullying only, the victim group comprised participants who indicated experience in being victimized only, and the bully victim group comprised participants who indicated experience with both bullying and victimization. The bully and victim subscales had an internal reliability of α = 0.72 and 0.73, respectively.

Cognitive Appraisal in Risky Behavior

The CARE questionnaire was administered to measure adolescents’ outcome expectancies about the risks and benefits associated with involvement in risky activities. These outcome expectancies were measured by three CARE subscales: expected benefits (CARE_EB, Appendix B), expected risks (CARE_ER, Appendix C), and expected involvements (CARE_EI, Appendix D) (Fromme et al., 1997). The CARE_EB and CARE_ER scales capture the extent that participants anticipate positive or negative outcomes from their participation in 30 risky activities on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not likely) to 7 (very likely). The CARE_EI scale uses the same Likert scale; however, it measures the likelihood of participation in these risky activities. The CARE questionnaire covers activities of illicit drug use (e.g., smoking marijuana), aggressive illegal behavior (e.g., slapping someone), risky sexual activities (e.g., sex with multiple partners), heavy drinking (e.g., drinking more than 5 alcoholic beverages), high-risk sports (e.g., mountain climbing), and negative academic work behavior (e.g., missing class). The goal of the CARE questionnaire is to predict behavioral problem tendencies associated with cognitive factors. Its reliability is excellent (Cronbach’s α for CARE_EB, CARE_ER, and CARE_EI are 0.90, 0.90, and 0.89, respectively).

Behavioral Measure in Risky Behavior

The CGT was administered to capture risk-taking propensity and reward-related sensitivity under uncertainty (Edwards, 1957; Andrew and Cronin, 1997; Rogers et al., 1999; Greene et al., 2000; Ladouceur et al., 2000). It aims to minimize both the learning and executive/working memory demands on participants, which can confound the interpretation of test scores. In the CGT, participants see 10 boxes colored either red or blue on the top of the screen. Display of the boxes represents the probability of winning (e.g., 9B:1R, 5B:5R). The goal is to bet on the color with a higher probability in later trials staring with 100 points to earn more points. At the beginning of the first trial, all participants were informed that they were playing for a joint school competition and those who scored in the top ten would receive a souvenir as a reward. Four trials were administered and the first and third trial was for practice/instruction for the upcoming trial. An ascending rule was implemented on the first two trials as the amount-to-bet was determined progressively by the points the participant held: 5, 25, 50, 75, and 95%. A descending rule was also implemented as participants could decrease their bet progressively. Points available to bet would grow larger or shrink smaller, respectively. A high/low pitched sound was prompted informing participants if they won or lost at the end of every round. When the participant finished a trial, his/her final points were presented allowing him/her to compare their current score with their previous score and motivate him/herself to perform better in the next trial.

Verbal instruction

Participants were instructed that they could see a row of boxes across the top of the screen with “X” red boxes and “Y” blue boxes. The computer had hidden a yellow token under one of these boxes. Participants had to decide whether they thought it was hidden under a red box or a blue box.

Ascending stage

Participants were instructed that they were provided with 100 points to start. After they chose red or blue, they had to bet a certain amount of points that they could win. The first bet they were shown was small; however, as they waited, the bets grew larger, which allowed them to choose the size of your bet. The size of the bets also depended on how many points they had: the smallest bet as always 5% of the total and the largest bet was always 95% of the total. They were encouraged to try to make as much as they could. At the final score screen in between the blocks, several prompts were used depending on how well the participants’ score was increasing. For example, “Well done! That was good. Now you are going to start off with 100 points again and you need to try to build up as many points as you can again.” If the final score got too low participants received a prompt such as “hard luck!”

Descending training and test

This time, the way participants’ selected their bets was slightly different as the first bet offered was large and then they gradually got smaller. Participants were instructed to practice and to make as much as they could.

Propensity outcome measures

The CGT consisted of six propensity outcome measures: (a) Deliberation time is the mean decision latency of participants choosing what color to bet on after presentation of the colored boxes. Higher scores represented longer deliberation time measured in milliseconds. (b) Delay aversion measures the difference in the bet’s percentage in the ascending verses the descending condition. If participants were unwilling or unable to wait to make a decision then they would be more likely to bet larger amounts when the possible bets were displayed in descending rather than ascending order. Higher scores indicated greater impulsivity. (c) Quality of decision-making is the proportion of trials that the participant chose to gamble on the more likely outcome. Higher scores meant that more choices were made on the likely outcome. (d) Risk taking is the mean proportion of points that the participant makes on each trial when they had chosen the more likely outcome. Higher score indicated that more points were placed on the likely outcome. (e) Risk adjustment is the degree that a participant varies their risk taking in response to the ratio of red to blue boxes on each trial. Higher score represented more likelihood to modify his/her response when the outcome probability changed. (f) Overall proportion bet measured the mean proportion of points bet across all trials. Higher scores represented a larger overall bet amount.

Data were analyzed with the SPSS version 21 for Windows, and were inspected for normality to ensure that the assumptions of parametric statistics were met before analyses were performed. The significance level was set at p < 0.05.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Table 1 summarizes the descriptive statistics for each group. Close to a marginally significant level of group differences were found in general intellectual ability [F(3,125) = 2.58, p = 0.06, ηp2 = 0.06]. Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that the control group had significantly outperformed the victim group (p < 0.05). No significant difference was found between the control and the bully victim group (p = 0.68). The four groups did not differ in age, education level (self, father, and mother), and average family income (all ps > 0.10). Total bully scores and victim scores captured from the school bullying questionnaire are also summarized in Table 1 with higher scores representing more frequent bullying or victimization behavior. The bully victim group showed significant higher bullying frequency than the bully group (p < 0.001); however, there was no difference in victim score between the victim and bully victim groups (p > 0.10). For sex, a chi-square analysis was applied and a statistical significance was found [see Table 2, χ2(3, N = 136) = 9.72, p = 0.02].

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Characteristics of controls, bullies, victims, and bully victims.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Cross-tabulation of sex according to bully groups.

Cognitive Appraisal of Risky Events

To examine whether bullying identity (control, bully, victim, bully victim) was significantly associated with the three outcome expectancies measured by CARE, a series of 2 (bully vs. not bully) × 2 (victim vs. not victim) between subject ANCOVAs with general intellectual ability as covariate were computed and summarized in Table 3. A post hoc Fisher’s Least Significant Difference (LSD) for group comparison within each sub-questionnaire was also conducted. Based on the framework of the decision-making model, a hierarchical regression analysis was conducted to examine the expected net (CARE_EN) (CARE_EN = CARE_EB – CARE_ER) in predicting CARE_EI (see Table 4). Results showed that the aggregate effect of expected benefit minus expected risk significantly predicted the expected involvement of risky events (p < 0.001).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Cognitive appraisal of controls, bullies, victims, and bully victims: expected benefits, risks, and involvement.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Hierarchical regressions predicting expected involvement in the cognitive appraisal of risky events.

Expected Benefits

The positive expectancy for risky activities was measured by the CARE_EB subscale. Higher scores represented a greater degree of positive outcome anticipation. There was significant main effect of victim [F(1,120) = 24.49, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.17] meaning that victims anticipated risky events significantly more beneficial than the other groups did. Neither the main effect of being a bully [F(1,120) = 2.98, p = 0.09, ηp2 = 0.02] nor the interaction between being a bully and victim [F(1,120) = 0.04, p = 0.83, ηp2 = 0.00] was significant. Inspection of means showed bully victims and victims scored significantly higher than controls and bullies, who confirmed a main effect of victims overestimating the benefits of risky events (see Table 3).

Expected Risks

The negative expectancy of risk activities was captured through the CARE_ER subscale. Higher scores represented a greater degree of anticipating negative outcomes. There were significant main effects of bully [F(1,120) = 4.41, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.03] and victim [F(1,120) = 14.12, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.10] meaning that participants with a bully or a victim identity anticipated risky events as significantly more or less risky. There was no significant interaction effect (p = 0.15). Mean scores of victims were significantly lower than the other groups suggesting a main effect of victims underestimating risks and bullies overestimated risks (Table 3).

Expected Involvements

Anticipation of participation for risky events was measured using CARE Expected Involvement subscale. The higher score suggests the higher probability of participation in potentially harmful activities. There were significant main effects of Victim [F(1,120) = 14.27, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.10] and Bully [F(1,120) = 5.79, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.05]. There was no significant interaction effect (p = 0.16). Mean scores on Bully, Victim, and Bully Victim were significantly higher than Control group (see Table 3). The pattern suggested participants with Bully or Victim identity anticipate higher expected involvement in risky events.

Predicting Bully and Victim Scores

A hierarchical regression analysis was conducted to examine what CARE variables best predicted the severity of bullying and victimization. Table 5 shows that CARE accounted for an additional 16.9% of the variance in Bullying severity, and CARE_EI was the only significant predictor of Bullying severity (β = 0.40, t = 4.30, p < 0.001). Regarding the severity of victimization, Table 6 shows that CARE accounted for only an additional 2.5% of the variance in victimization severity, and none of the CARE variables significantly predicted the frequency of being bullied.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Hierarchical regressions predicting cognitive appraisal in risky events variables among bully total.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 6. Hierarchical regressions predicting cognitive appraisal in risky events variables among victim total.

Behavioral Measure in Risky Behavior: the Cambridge Gambling Task

In order to capture behavior differences in adolescents’ reward-related sensitivity and risk-taking propensity, the six CGT factors previously listed were analyzed with general intellectual ability as a covariate using a 2 (bully vs. not bully) × 2 (victim vs. not victim) between-subjects ANCOVA (see Table 7). A one-way ANCOVA followed by a post hoc LSD were conducted to examine significant differences between groups within each factor. Correlations between the CGT factors are shown in Table 8.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 7. Cognitive appraisal of controls, bullies, victims, and bully victims using the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT).

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 8. Correlations among CGT variables.

Deliberation Time

For mean decision latency, there was a significant main effect of victim [F(1,120) = 5.56, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.04], and a marginal main effect of bully [F(1,120) = 3.27, p = 0.07, ηp2 = 0.03]. An interaction effect was not found [F(1,120) = 1.25, p = 0.27, ηp2 = 0.01]. Inspection of means revealed that victims might use shortened decision times than bullies (see Table 7).

Delay Aversion

Delay aversion captured differences between the scores in the descending and ascending conditions. There was a marginal main effect for bully [F(1,120) = 3.30, p = 0.07, ηp2 = 0.03] and a significant main effect for victim [F(1,120) = 7.41, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.06]. An interaction effect was not found [F(1,120) = 0.85, p = 0.36, ηp2 = 0.01]. Inspection of mean scores on delay aversion revealed that victims had higher scores than controls, bullies, and bully victims, suggesting that victims might be more impulsive than the other groups.

Decision-Making Quality

Neither a main effect of bully [F(1,120) = 1.75, p = 0.19, ηp2 = 0.01] nor a main effect of victim [F(1,120) = 0.68, p = 0.41, ηp2 = 0.01] were significant. There was a significant interaction effect of bully and victim [F(1,120) = 5.64, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.04]. The interaction effect revealed that with the absence of bullies, victims alone incurred significant impact to the proportion of trials participants gambled on the more favorite outcome, leaving the victim group as the only group that made more choices on the likely outcome. A subsequent means scores comparison revealed that victims scored higher than bully victims and controls (Table 7).

Risk Taking

Neither a main effect of bully (p = 0.46) or victim (p = 0.85) nor any interactions were significant (p > 0.10).

Risk Adjustment

There was a significant main effect of Bully [F(1,120) = 9.66, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.07]. Neither main effects of Victim [F(1,120) = 1.50, p = 0.22, ηp2 = 0.01] nor interaction effect [F(1,120) = 0.45, p = 0.50, ηp2 = 0.00] were significant. Inspection of mean scores revealed that Bully had higher risk adjustment scores than Victim (Table 7) suggesting Bully identity exhibited higher risk modulation.

Overall Bet Proportion

There was no significant main effect of bully [F(1,120) = 0.48, p = 0.49, ηp2 = 0.00], victim [F(1,120) = 1.39, p = 0.24, ηp2 = 0.001], or interaction [F(1,120) = 0.42, p = 0.52, ηp2 = 0.00].

Predicting Bullying and Victim Scores

A hierarchical regression analysis was conducted to examine what CGT variables best predicted the severity of bullying and victimization. Table 9 shows that CGT accounted for an additional 10.2% of the variance in bullying severity and risk adjustment was the only significant predictor of bullying severity (β = 0.25, t = 2.77, p < 0.01). Regarding the severity of victimization, Table 10 shows that CGT only accounted for an additional 3.5% of the variance in the frequency of victimization and none of the CGT variables significantly predicted the severity of victimization.

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 9. Hierarchical regressions predicting CGT variables among bully total.

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 10. Hierarchical regressions predicting CGT variables among victim total.

Discussion

Risk Taking in Bullies

Bullying and risk-taking behavior are two common problems among adolescents worldwide and can cause serious impact to adolescents’ physical, psychological, social, and educational functioning when both coexist in the same youth (Currie et al., 2012). Despite the associations between bullying and risk-taking behaviors, no study thus far has examined and compared the risk-taking pattern among bullying groups (i.e., bully, victim and bully victim). This study elucidated the risk-taking pattern in these three bullying groups by addressing two risk-taking models (i.e., the decision-making model and the social-neuroscience model) and clarifying how adolescents’ characteristics in risk taking may associate with outcomes in bullying or victimization.

These findings suggest that being a bully or victim is associated with distinct results on the two approaches, and these results contribute several novel findings to the current literature. Under the decision-making framework, bullying was associated with higher negative expectancy on risky activities and more expected involvement in them. In other words, being a bully meant more anticipated negative outcomes in risky activities. Interestingly, despite the higher anticipation of negative outcomes, bullies expected themselves to have higher involvement in these activities compared to the non-bully groups. In CGT, bullying was associated with positive risk-taking adjustment, an increased amount of gambling their points when the odds were in their favor, and vice versa. These results indicated that being a bully was vigilant in situational deliberation and risk modulation, and contradicted the general agreement that there was a positive link between bullying and impulsivity (Olweus, 1995; Schwartz et al., 2001; O’Brennan et al., 2009). Perhaps bullying is a unique, complex form of interpersonal aggression or group phenomenon (e.g., Olweus, 2001; Salmivalli, 2001; Rodkin and Hodges, 2003) and bullies tend to be hyper-vigilant to social cues and attribute negative intentions to others (Vaillancourt et al., 2003). The paradox of aggression is that it is both adaptive and maladaptive. Aggressive adolescents are at risk for a host of possible negative consequences (e.g., disciplinary punishment, school suspension, etc.) (Dodge et al., 2006). However, aggression is often a successful means in changing other’s behavior and can be used to acquire resources and maintain group boundaries. Moreover, researchers generally agree that bullies tend to exhibit high levels of social intelligence and the ability to manipulate peers (e.g., Peeters et al., 2010). In other words, bullies may be a group of adolescents who are sensitive and respond quickly to external cues through peer manipulation, resulting in potential risk for themselves and victims.

Risk Taking in Victims

The results of the CARE questionnaire suggested that victims were associated with an overestimation of benefits, an underestimation of risks, and higher expected involvement in risky events. According to the decision-making model, these patterns suggested the lowest sense of risk and the highest vulnerability to it (Hunter and Boyle, 2004; Hunter et al., 2004; Wachs et al., 2012). Motivationally, victims were associated with less deliberation time and more delay aversion in CGT. In CGT, even though delay response does not increase the available information for decision making, shorter deliberation time may indicate impulsive decision-making. This assumption was further supported by their poor performance in delay aversion and that victims were more likely to bet larger amounts when bets were displayed in a descending rather than an ascending way. It is well established that impulsivity is particularly relevant to peer victimization. For example, children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder who share common features of impulsivity and high risk-taking propensity, show marked impairment in peer relationships and are significantly bullied by peers (e.g., Hoza, 2007). Researchers argued that individuals who are low on self-control or high on impulsivity are unable to see the consequences of their actions and more likely to place themselves in risky situations without regard for their long-term outcome (e.g., Higgins et al., 2009).

Risk Taking in Bully Victims

Consistent with our hypothesis, bully victims presented the combined pattern of the two pure groups. Cognitively, bully victims showed similar, but not identical patterns of the two pure groups. Bully victims overestimated the benefits of risk as well as benefit and projected the highest possibility of engaging in risky behavior. Motivationally, this unique group shared identical patterns with bullies in terms of positive risk adjustment. In other words, bully victims exhibited similar risk tendencies to victims in terms of cognitive-focused processes and to bullies on emotion-focused processes. Importantly, compared with bullies, bully victims had significantly higher bullying scores, showing that they engaged in a wider range of more frequent bullying activities. In fact, previous studies that focused on the frequency and forms of bullying also reported that bully victims used wider and more aggressive strategies than the pure groups did (Olweus, 1993; Schwartz, 1999; Olafsen and Viemerö, 2000). Additional evidence has suggested that bully victims may exhibit the highest aggression level of all three groups as well as the poorest emotion regulation (e.g., Nansel et al., 2004). Olweus (1978) suggested that bullies were generally more functional, more likely to use proactive aggression, and more likely to have an extensive social network than bully victims, who were more likely to react aggressively and show troubling risk patterns across virtually all adjustment indicators.

Implications

Concern about the frequency and effects of adolescent bullying was reflected in the increase in research aimed at understanding its causes and consequences in order to develop appropriate policy and intervention strategies. Nonetheless, the success of intervention programs to prevent or mitigate bullying in adolescence has been limited (e.g., Olweus, 1999; Merrell et al., 2008). Even when programs have an impact, the improvement appears to be in changing adolescents’ knowledge and perceptions on bullying, but not the behavior. This study extended the existing literature on bullying and victimization by addressing the co-occurrence of bullying and victimization and identifying a range of risk-taking patterns associated with the three groups. This study suggested that there were significant group differences on how bullies, victims, and bully victims appraised risky events and emotionally processed evocative risky situations. Moreover, this study identified different predictors of bullying and victimization. In accordance with the decision-making model, this study provided further support that participants’ perceived benefits and risk may play a key role in predicting their expected involvement in risky behaviors. Given that expected involvement in risky events is a significant predictor to the severity of bullying, future intervention programs should include cognitive training in risk evaluation (Steinberg, 2007). For instance, presenting strategies for effective risk and benefit evaluation will encourage less risky and healthier choices. Moreover, given the high vigilance of bullies to external cues, future interventions should target the enhancement of social skills and coping strategies on peer conflicts. Victims with marked impulsivity, on the other hand, may be targeted to deliver interventions on self-control that strengthen their ability to fit in with their peers and reduce the likelihood of rejection and victimization, or to provide supportive interpersonal relationships to reduce their isolation (Andreou et al., 2005). Effective and comprehensive evaluation of risk is another intervention focus for victims to help increase their awareness on the consequences of their risky choices.

Limitations and Future Directions

This study has several limitations that should be considered when interpreting our findings. First, the sample size was relatively small. Future studies should include a larger sample size and a power calculation to determine what sample size would be needed to detect differences between groups. Second, this study was cross-sectional in nature. Therefore, causation cannot be determined. A temporal relationship between bullying and a risk-taking pattern could not be inferred. Questions of causation can only be answered in a longitudinal research design, which should be implemented in the future. Third, this study was localized to Chinese adolescents. Even though assessment tools were standardized, a comprehensive assessment tool on risk appraisal may be needed to elicit cultural differences on risk perception. In China, school bullying is often regarded as a collective act (Cheng et al., 2011; Huang et al., 2013; Chui and Chan, 2015), and whether this is a form of “collective bullying” that may cause an impact to the risk-taking pattern is yet to be determined. Fourth, although this study examined emotional aroused stimulation (i.e., the gambling task), it might not present an authentic context where bullying is most likely to occur. Lastly, this study was limited by its use of self-report measures (e.g., the bully or the victim). Biases such as social desirability and retrospective recall issues may occur. Moreover, the likelihood of under-reporting adolescents’ school bullying behavior was also possible. Future research should include other bullying behavioral assessments such as peer and teacher nominations and behavioral observations as supplementary measures to validate self-reported findings (e.g., Espelage and Holt, 2001; Espelage and Swearer, 2003).

With growing recognition that bullying is a complex phenomenon that is influenced by multiple factors, past research has been examined within a sociological framework. This study enhanced the understanding of this social phenomenon from a psychological perspective by examining the risk-taking pattern associated with bullies, victims, and bully victims. This analysis will aid in not only comprehending the mechanism of how these groups respond to risk taking, but also lead to improved practice for prevention and intervention. Overall, bullying behavior is the result of the interaction of multiple causes and factors, and it is too early to believe that the identification of various risk-taking patterns or specific intervention that target their unique risk-taking processes would be a cure-all solution for the elimination of bullying or victimization in these groups. However, this study provides a basis for future studies to adopt a holistic approach in investigating the link between bullying and risk-taking behavior.

Author Contributions

KP is the sole author of this research article and her task involves conception or design of the work, data analysis and interpretation, drafting the article and final approval of the version to be published.

Acknowledgment

The work described in this paper was partially supported by the Research Support Scheme 2016/2017 of the Department of Special Education and Counselling at the Education University of Hong Kong.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Andreou, E., Vlachou, A., and Didaskalou, E. (2005). The roles of self-efficacy, peer interactions and attitudes in bully-victim incidents implications for intervention policy-practices. Sch. Psychol. Int. 26, 545–562.

Google Scholar

Andrew, M., and Cronin, C. (1997). Two measures of sensation seeking as predictors of alcohol use among high school males. Pers. Individ. Dif. 22, 393–401. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00214-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrnes, J. P. (1998). The Nature and Development of Decision-Making: A Self-Regulation Model. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Carlyle, K. E., and Steinman, K. J. (2007). Demographic differences in the prevalence, co-occurrence, and correlates of adolescent bullying at school. J. Sch. Health 77, 623–629. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2007.00242.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, Y. Y., Chen, L. M., Liu, K. S., and Chen, Y. L. (2011). Development and psychometric evaluation of the school bullying scales: a Rasch measurement approach. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 71, 200–216. doi: 10.1177/0013164410387387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chui, W. H., and Chan, H. C. O. (2015). Self-control, school bullying perpetration, and victimization among Macanese adolescents. J. Child Fam. Stud. 24, 1751–1761. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collier, K. L., van Beusekom, G., Bos, H. M., and Sandfort, T. G. (2013). Sexual orientation and gender identity/expression related peer victimization in adolescence: a systematic review of associated psychosocial and health outcomes. J. Sex Res. 50, 299–317. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2012.750639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crick, N. R., and Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Dev. 67, 993–1002. doi: 10.2307/1131875

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Currie, C., Zanotti, C., Morgan, A., Currie, D., de Looze, M., Roberts, C., et al. (2012). Social Determinants of Health and Well-Being among Young People. Copenhagen: World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe.

Google Scholar

Danielson, C. K., McCart, M., de Arellano, M. A., Macdonald, A., Doherty, L., and Resnick, H. (2010). Risk reduction for substance use and trauma-related psychopathology in adolescent sexual assault victims: findings from an open trial. Child Maltreat. 15, 261–268. doi: 10.1177/1077559510367939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., and Lynam, D. (2006). “Aggression and antisocial behavior in youth,” in Handbook of Child Psychology, eds W. Damon and R. M. Lerner (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.).

Google Scholar

Durand, G., Belacel, N., and LaPlante, F. (2013). Graph theory based model for learning path recommendation. Inf. Sci. 251, 10–21. doi: 10.1016/j.ins.2013.04.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, A. L. (1957). The Social Desirability Variable in Personality Assessment and Research. New York, NY: Dryden Press.

Google Scholar

Ellickson, P., Saner, H., and McGuigan, K. A. (1997). Profiles of violent youth: substance use and other concurrent problems. Am. J. Public Health 87, 985–991. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.87.6.985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espelage, D. L., and Holt, M. K. (2001). Bullying and victimization during early adolescence: peer influences and psychosocial correlates. J. Emot. Abuse 2, 123–142. doi: 10.1300/J135v02n02_08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espelage, D. L., and Swearer, S. M. (2003). Research on school bullying and victimization: What have we learned and where do we go from here? Sch. Psychol. Rev. 32, 365–384.

Google Scholar

Forero, R., McLellan, L., Rissel, C., and Bauman, A. (1999). Bullying behaviour and psychosocial health among school students in New South Wales, Australia: cross sectional survey. BMJ 319, 344–348. doi: 10.1136/bmj.319.7206.344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fromme, K., D’Amico, E. J., and Katz, E. C. (1999). Intoxicated sexual risk taking: an expectancy or cognitive impairment explanation? J. Stud. Alcohol 60, 54–63. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1999.60.54

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fromme, K., Katz, E. C., and Rivet, K. (1997). Outcome expectancies and risk-taking behavior. Cogn. Ther. Res. 21, 421–442. doi: 10.1023/A:1021932326716

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, K., Krcmar, M., Walters, L. H., Rubin, D. L., and Hale, L. (2000). Targeting adolescent risk-taking behaviors: the contributions of egocentrism and sensation-seeking. J. Adolesc. 23, 439–461. doi: 10.1006/jado.2000.0330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haynie, D. L. (2001). Delinquent peers revisited: Does network structure matter? 1. Am. J. Soc. 106, 1013–1057. doi: 10.1086/320298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, F. Y., Huang, D. H., Lin, H. Y., and Chiu, S. L. (2014). Analysis of the psychological traits, Facebook usage, and Facebook addiction model of Taiwanese university students. Telemat. Informat. 31, 597–606. doi: 10.1016/j.tele.2014.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoza, B. (2007). Peer functioning in children with ADHD. Ambul. Pediatr. 7, 101–106. doi: 10.1016/j.ambp.2006.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, G. E., Jennings, W. G., Tewksbury, R., and Gibson, C. L. (2009). Exploring the link between low self-control and violent victimization trajectories in adolescents. Crim. Justice Behav. 36, 1070–1084. doi: 10.1177/0093854809344046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, H., Hong, J. S., and Espelage, D. L. (2013). Understanding factors associated with bullying and peer victimization in Chinese schools within ecological contexts. J. Child Fam. Stud. 22, 881–892. doi: 10.1007/s10826-012-9647-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, S. C., and Boyle, J. M. (2004). Appraisal and coping strategy use in victims of school bullying. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 74, 83–107. doi: 10.1348/000709904322848833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, S. C., Boyle, J. M., and Warden, D. (2004). Help seeking amongst child and adolescent victims of peer-aggression and bullying: the influence of school-stage, gender, victimisation, appraisal, and emotion. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 74, 375–390. doi: 10.1348/0007099041552378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaltiala-Heino, R., Rimpelä, M., Rantanen, P., and Rimpelä, A. (2000). Bullying at school—an indicator of adolescents at risk for mental disorders. J. Adolesc. 23, 661–674. doi: 10.1006/jado.2000.0351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karatzias, A., Power, K. G., and Swanson, V. (2002). Bullying and victimisation in Scottish secondary schools: Same or separate entities? Aggress. Behav. 28, 45–61. doi: 10.1002/ab.90005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khantzian, E. J. (1997). The self-medication hypothesis of substance use disorders: a reconsideration and recent applications. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 4, 231–244. doi: 10.3109/10673229709030550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladouceur, R., Dugas, M. J., Freeston, M. H., Léger, E., Gagnon, F., and Thibodeau, N. (2000). Efficacy of a cognitive–behavioral treatment for generalized anxiety disorder: evaluation in a controlled clinical trial. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68:957. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.6.957

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, H., Flisher, A. J., and Lombard, C. J. (2007). Bullying, violence, and risk behavior in South African school students. Child Abuse Neglect 31, 161–171. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maniglio, R. (2009). The impact of child sexual abuse on health: a systematic review of reviews. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 647–657. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merrell, K. W., Gueldner, B. A., Ross, S. W., and Isava, D. M. (2008). How effective are school bullying intervention programs? A meta-analysis of intervention research. Sch. Psychol. Q. 23:26. doi: 10.1037/1045-3830.23.1.26

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millstein, S. G., and Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2001). “Perceptions of risk and vulnerability,” in Adolescent Risk and Vulnerability: Concepts and Measurement, Vol. 15, eds B. Fischhoff, E. O. Nightingale, and J. G. Iannotta (Washington, DC: National Academies Press).

Google Scholar

Nansel, T. R., Craig, W., Overpeck, M. D., Saluja, G., and Ruan, W. J. (2004). Cross-national consistency in the relationship between bullying behaviors and psychosocial adjustment. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 158, 730–736. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.158.8.730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W. J., Simons-Morton, B., and Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying behaviors among US youth: prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment. JAMA 285, 2094–2100. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.16.2094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, J. W., and Tsang, S. K. (2008). School bullying and the mental health of junior secondary school students in Hong Kong. J. Sch. Violence 7, 3–20. doi: 10.1300/J202v07n02_02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brennan, L. M., Bradshaw, C. P., and Sawyer, A. L. (2009). Examining developmental differences in the social-emotional problems among frequent bullies, victims, and bully/victims. Psychol. Sch. 46, 100–115. doi: 10.1002/pits.20357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olafsen, R. N., and Viemerö, V. (2000). Bully/victim problems and coping with stress in school among 10-to 12-year-old pupils in Åland, Finland. Aggress. Behav. 26, 57–65. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337 (2000)26:1<57::AID-AB5>3.0.CO;2-I

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in the Schools: Bullies and Whipping Boys. Washington, DC: Hemisphere Publishing Corporation.

Google Scholar

Olweus, D. (1979). Stability of aggressive reaction patterns in males: a review. Psychol. Bull. 86:852. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.4.852

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olweus, D. (1993). “Victimization by peers: antecedents and long-term outcomes,” in Social Withdrawal, Inhibition, and Shyness in Childhood, eds K. H. Rubin, and J. B. Asendorpf (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 315–341.

Google Scholar

Olweus, D. (1995). Bullying or peer abuse at school: facts and intervention. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 4, 196–200. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olweus, D. (1999). “Sweden,” in The Nature of School Bullying: A Cross-National Perspective, eds P. K. Smith, Y. Morita, J. Junger-Tas, D. Olweus, R. Catalano, and P. Slee (New York, NY: Routledge), 2–27.

Google Scholar

Olweus, D. (2001). Bullying at school: tackling the problem. OECD Observer 3, 24–26.

Google Scholar

Peeters, M., Price, T. J., Cervantes, A., Sobrero, A. F., Ducreux, M., Hotko, Y., et al. (2010). Randomized phase III study of panitumumab with fluorouracil, leucovorin, and irinotecan (FOLFIRI) compared with FOLFIRI alone as second-line treatment in patients with metastatic colorectal cancer. J. Clin. Oncol. 28, 4706–4713. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2009.27.6055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prabhakaran, V., Smith, J. A., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., and Gabrieli, J. D. (1997). Neural substrates of fluid reasoning: an fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance of the Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test. Cogn. Psychol. 33, 43–63. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1997.0659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raven, J., Raven, J. C., and Court, J. H. (2000). Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Reyna, V. F., and Farley, F. (2006). Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 7, 1–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1529-1006.2006.00026.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodkin, P. C., and Hodges, E. V. (2003). Bullies and victims in the peer ecology: four questions for psychologists and school professionals. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 32, 384–400.

Google Scholar

Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Middleton, H. C., Williams, E. J., Pickard, J. D., Sahakian, B. J., et al. (1999). Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 19, 9029–9038.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Salmivalli, C. (2001). “Group view on victimization,” in Peer Harassment in School, eds J. Juvonen and S. Graham (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 398–417.

Google Scholar

Schwartz, D., Proctor, L. J., and Chien, D. H. (2001). “The aggressive victim of bullying,” in Peer Harassment in School: The Plight of the Vulnerable and Victimized, eds J. Jovonen and S. Graham (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 147–174.

Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H. (1999). A theory of cultural values and some implications for work. Appl. Psychol. 48, 23–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.1999.tb00047.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegrist, M., Cvetkovich, G., and Roth, C. (2000). Salient value similarity, social trust, and risk/benefit perception. Risk Anal. 20, 353–362. doi: 10.1111/0272-4332.203034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slovic, P. (1987). Perception of risk. Science 236, 280–285. doi: 10.1126/science.3563507

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L. (2004). Risk taking in adolescence: What changes, and why? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1021, 51–58. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence new perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 55–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00475.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Dev. Rev. 28, 78–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L. (2010). A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Dev. Psychobiol. 52, 216–224.

Google Scholar

Ttofi, M. M., Farrington, D. P., and Lösel, F. (2012). School bullying as a predictor of violence later in life: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. Aggress. Violent Behav. 17, 405–418. doi: 10.1002/yd.20009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaillancourt, T., Hymel, S., and McDougall, P. (2003). Bullying is power: implications for school-based intervention strategies. J. Appl. Sch. Psychol. 19, 157–176. doi: 10.1300/J008v19n02_10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Oldehinkel, A. J., De Winter, A. F., Verhulst, F. C., and Ormel, J. (2005). Bullying and victimization in elementary schools: a comparison of bullies, victims, bully/victims, and uninvolved preadolescents. Dev. Psychol. 41:672. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wachs, S., Wolf, K. D., and Pan, C. C. (2012). Cybergrooming: risk factors, coping strategies and associations with cyberbullying. Psicothema 24, 628–633.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wills, T. A., and Filer, M. (1996). “Stress—coping model of adolescent substance use,” in Advances in Clinical Child Psychology, Vol. 18, eds T. H. Ollendick and R. J. Prinz (New York, NY: Springer), 91–132.

Google Scholar

Wong, D. S., Lok, D. P., Lo, T. W., and Ma, S. K. (2008). School bullying among Hong Kong Chinese primary schoolchildren. Youth Society 40, 35–54. doi: 10.1177/0044118X07310134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix A

School Bullying Questionnaire

APPENDIX A

Appendix B

Cognitive Appraisal of Risky Event Questionnaire (CARE) – Expected Benefits

On a scale of 1 (not at all likely) to 7 (extremely likely), HOW LIKELY IS IT THAT YOU WOULD EXPERIENCE SOME POSITIVE CONSEQUENCE (e.g., pleasure, win money, feel good about yourself, etc.) if you were to engage in these activities?

APPENDIX B

Appendix C

Cognitive Appraisal of Risky Event Questionnaire (CARE) – Expected Risks

On a scale of 1 (not at all likely) to 7 (extremely likely), HOW LIKELY IS IT THAT YOU WOULD EXPERIENCE SOME NEGATIVE CONSEQUENCE (e.g., become sick, be injured, embarrassed, lose money, suffer legal consequences, fail a class, or feel bad about yourself) if you engaged in these activities?

APPENDIX C

Appendix D

Cognitive Appraisal of Risky Event Questionnaire (CARE) – Expected Involvements

On a scale of 1 (not at all likely) to 7 (extremely likely), HOW LIKELY IS IT THAT YOU WILL ENGAGE IN EACH OF THESE ACTIVITIES in the next 6 months?

APPENDIX D

Keywords: risk assessment, bullying, adolescents, peer victimization, risk taking

Citation: Poon K (2016) Understanding Risk-taking Behavior in Bullies, Victims, and Bully Victims Using Cognitive- and Emotion-Focused Approaches. Front. Psychol. 7:1838. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01838

Received: 08 September 2016; Accepted: 07 November 2016;
Published: 29 November 2016.

Edited by:

José Jesús Gázquez, University of Almería, Spain

Reviewed by:

Shaljan Areepattamannil, Emirates College for Advanced Education, UAE
Ken Cramer, University of Windsor, Canada

Copyright © 2016 Poon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kean Poon, keanpoon@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.