Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 14 December 2016
Sec. Eating Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Research in Restrained Eating: Charting the Future View all 6 articles

Dieting and Food Cue-Related Working Memory Performance

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria
  • 2Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria

Executive functioning (e.g., working memory) is tightly intertwined with self-regulation. For example, food cue-elicited craving has been found to impair working memory performance. Furthermore, current dieters have been found to show lower working memory performance than non-dieters. Recent research, however, suggests that it is crucial to consider dieting success in addition to current dieting status or restrained eating in order to reveal cognitive mechanisms that are associated with successful eating-related self-regulation. The current study investigated food cue-related working memory performance as a function of dieting status and dieting success in female students. Participants performed an n-back task with pictures of food and neutral objects. Reaction time in response to food pictures was slower than in response to neutral pictures, whereas omission errors did not differ between picture types. Current food craving was increased after performing the food block, but not after the neutral block. There was an indirect effect of current dieting status on higher food craving after the food block, which was mediated by slower reaction time to food vs. neutral pictures. Furthermore, higher dieting success was associated with fewer omission errors in the food vs. neutral block in current dieters. There were no relationships of restrained eating with current food craving and task performance. Results further highlight the need to differentiate between successful and unsuccessful dieting in addition to current dieting status or restrained eating when examining possible mechanisms of overeating or successful restraint. Although palatable food cues induce food craving regardless of dieting success, they may boost executive functioning in successful dieters, which helps them to overcome these temptations.

Introduction

Restrained eating refers to the intention to restrict food intake deliberately in order to prevent weight gain or to promote weight loss (Tuschl, 1990). Higher scores on restrained eating (particularly when assessed with the Restraint Scale, RS) are associated with a tendency to overeat and higher body mass index (BMI; Stroebe, 2008). This has led to the proposition that dieting (i.e., caloric restriction) may increase the incentive salience of palatable foods (e.g., Fedoroff et al., 2003), which may in turn threaten dieting success. Restraint scores alone, however, do not provide information about if a person is currently dieting or not and, if a person is currently dieting, if that person is successful or unsuccessful in restricting food intake (Lowe, 1995).

Increasing evidence suggests that incorporating perceived self-regulatory success in dieting appears to be an important predictor of cognitive processing of and responses to palatable food cues. For example, self-perceived dieting success has been found to be differentially related to activation and inhibition of dieting goals in response to palatable food cues (Papies et al., 2008; Stroebe et al., 2008; Van Koningsbruggen et al., 2011a,b, 2012), to facets of food craving experiences (Meule et al., 2012a), to the use of dietary control strategies (Meule et al., 2011), and to food intake in the laboratory (Houben et al., 2012; Friese et al., 2015). Differential associations have also been reported with general measures of self-regulatory ability such as impulsivity or cardiac autonomic regulation (Meule et al., 2012b; van Koningsbruggen et al., 2013). Moreover, successful dieters exhibited better executive functioning as measured by motor response inhibition in response to high-calorie food cues (Houben et al., 2012; Meule et al., 2014a).

Response inhibition refers to controlling one’s behavior to override a strong internal predisposition or external lure (Diamond, 2013) and, thus, its relevance for dietary self-control is apparent (Hall, 2016). However, other domains of executive functioning have been identified as crucial for self-regulatory processes as well (Hofmann et al., 2012). Working memory, for example, refers to holding information in mind and mentally working with it (Diamond, 2013). Therefore, working memory and inhibition support one another and rarely is one needed but not the other (Diamond, 2013). High working memory capacity has been suggested to contribute to more effective self-regulation in several self-regulatory domains (Hofmann et al., 2011). Considerable evidence suggests that experiencing craving (e.g., for food), consumes cognitive resources and, thus, impairs performance on tasks that require working memory (Kemps et al., 2008; Tiggemann et al., 2010; May et al., 2012). Moreover, it has been found that introducing an interfering working memory load reduces food craving (Kemps and Tiggemann, 2010, 2015).

With regard to dieting behavior, it has been found that current dieters display lower working memory performance than non-dieters and this impairment is partly related to preoccupying thoughts about food, weight, and shape (Kemps and Tiggemann, 2005; Kemps et al., 2005). These studies, however, assessed general working memory performance, but did not measure working memory performance related to relevant cues (i.e., food). Moreover, it was not differentiated if dieters were successful or unsuccessful. In a recent study, it could be shown that successful dieters’ reaction times were less affected by food cues in a working memory task, indicating that dieting success may indeed moderate food cue-related working memory performance (Higgs et al., 2015).

In the current study, food cue-related working memory performance was investigated as a function of both current dieting status and dieting success with a version of the n-back task. Based on a previous study (Meule et al., 2012d), it was expected that working memory performance would be reduced (i.e., longer reaction times, higher number of omission errors) in response to food cues as compared to neutral cues. Furthermore, it was expected that dieters would demonstrate reduced working memory performance compared to non-dieters (Kemps and Tiggemann, 2005; Kemps et al., 2005). This reduced performance was expected to be observed particularly in response to food cues in unsuccessful dieters, but not in successful dieters (who may even show better task performance in response to food cues, similar to what has been found in motor response inhibition tasks; Houben et al., 2012; Meule et al., 2014a). Finally, it was explored if lower working memory performance was associated with higher subsequent food craving and, therefore, if task performance mediated a possible association between dieting status and/or dieting success and food cue-induced craving.

Materials and Methods

Participants

This study adhered to the guidelines outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki as revised in 2008. Seventy female students (Mage = 22.0 years, SD = 3.28; MBMI = 21.5 kg/m2, SD = 2.82) participated in exchange for course credits. Except for gender, no inclusion or exclusion criteria were applied. Mean food deprivation (i.e., time since last meal) was M = 4.57 h (SD = 5.11). Twenty-four participants (34.3%) indicated that they were currently dieting.

Measures and Materials

Dieting Status

Dieting status (yes/no) was assessed with a single question (“Are you currently restricting your food intake to control your weight [e.g., by eating less or avoiding certain foods]?”; cf. Meule et al., 2012b).

Perceived Self-Regulatory Success in Dieting (PSRS)

Dieting success was assessed with the PSRS (Fishbach et al., 2003; Meule et al., 2012c). This three-item questionnaire asks participants how successful they are in watching their weight and losing extra weight, and how difficult it is for them to stay in shape. Responses are scored on a seven-point scale (1–7), anchored not successful/not difficult and very successful/very difficult. After reverse coding the third item, all items are summed up and, thus, scores can ranged between three and 21. Higher scores indicate higher perceived self-regulatory success. Internal consistency was α = 0.633 in the current study. Note that the term dieting success when referring to PSRS scores will be used throughout this manuscript for the sake of brevity, although other descriptions such as successful weight regulation would also be appropriate (Houben et al., 2012; Friese et al., 2015).

Restraint Scale (RS)

Restrained eating was assessed with the RS (Herman et al., 1978; Dinkel et al., 2005). This ten-item questionnaire asks participants about their general concern for dieting and weight fluctuations. Responses are scored on a four-point (0–3, five items) and five-point (0–4, five items) scale, with response categories ranging, for example, from never/not at all to always/extremely. All items are summed up and, thus, scores can range between zero and 35. Higher scores indicate stronger restrained eating tendencies. Internal consistency was α = 0.770 in the current study.

Food Cravings Questionnaire-State (FCQ-S)

Current food craving was assessed with the FCQ-S (Cepeda-Benito et al., 2000; Meule et al., 2012a). This 15-item questionnaire asks participants about the intensity of their momentary craving for specific foods. Responses are scored on a five-point scale (1–5), with response categories ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. All items are summed up and, thus, scores can range between 15 and 75. Higher scores indicate more intense current food craving. Internal consistencies ranged from α = 0.872 to α = 0.919 in the current study.

n-back Task

Thirty pictures of high-calorie foods (both savory and sweet foods) and 30 pictures of neutral stimuli (flowers, office supplies, household items) were selected from the food.pics database1 (Blechert et al., 2014).2 Food pictures did not contain meat or fish because vegetarians were not excluded from the study. Food and neutral pictures did not differ in jpg file size, visual complexity, and contrast [all ts(58) < 1.48, ps > 0.140]. All foods displayed on the food images were high caloric (M = 354.77 kcal/100g, SD = 148.01; M = 736.95 kcal/image, SD = 832.70). The n-back task was compiled with E-prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA) and displayed on an LCD TFT 22″ monitor. In this task, stimuli are presented one-by-one and subjects are instructed to press a button whenever a stimulus is presented that is the same as the one presented n trials previously (so-called targets; in this case, it was a 2-back task; Figure 1). Participants first performed a practice block with numbers, which consisted of 14 trials, and received feedback in case of a false response. The test phase consisted of a block with food pictures and a block with neutral pictures (order of blocks was counterbalanced across subjects). Each picture was presented four times, but only once as a target, resulting in 120 trials in each block including 30 targets. Order of trials was pseudo-randomized such that order of target trials was equal in both blocks. Each picture was presented for 1500 ms or until a response was made. Between trials, a blank screen was displayed for 1000 ms (Figure 1). Thus, each block had a duration of approximately 5 min.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Representative screen displays of trials in the food block of the n-back task. Participants were instructed to press a key in response to pictures that have been displayed two trials earlier. ITI = Inter-trial interval.

Procedure

Participants were instructed to refrain from eating at least 1 h before the experiment to ensure that they were not fully sated. Participants were tested individually in the laboratory. After arrival, they read and signed informed consent and completed the FCQ-S for the first time. Then, they performed the practice block and the first test block. Participants then completed the FCQ-S for the second time, before performing the second test block. After the task, they completed the FCQ-S for the third time. At the end of the experiment, participants completed the PSRS, RS, and other questionnaires. Finally, height and weight were measured.

Data Analyses

Trials with a reaction time of ≤150 ms were excluded from analyses (cf. Meule et al., 2012d). Measures of interest in the n-back task were reaction time (M = 584 ms, SD = 100) and number of omission errors (M = 9.67 errors, SD = 6.41). Commission errors (i.e., pressing the button in response to non-targets) were rare (M = 4.09 errors, SD = 2.89) and not further analyzed.

Associations between participant characteristics (age, BMI, food deprivation) and questionnaire measures (dieting status, dieting success, restrained eating, current food craving) were examined with t-tests and correlations (Table 1). Multilevel models were calculated with HLM version 7.01 (Raudenbush et al., 2011) to examine associations between task performance and questionnaire measures. Specifically, block type (food block = 1, neutral block = 2) was entered at level 1 and dieting status (1 = dieting, 2 = non-dieting), dieting success, and an interaction between dieting status × dieting success were entered at level 2 for predicting reaction time (Table 2) and omission errors (Table 3). Similar models were calculated using restrained eating instead of dieting and dieting success as predictor variable (Tables 4 and 5). As there was a large variation in food deprivation (i.e., time since last meal; see Participants section), it was included as a control variable at level 2 in all models. Significant interactions were followed up with t-tests and linear regression analyses, as described below.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics for food deprivation, age, body mass index (BMI), dieting success, and restrained eating in the full sample and as a function of dieting status and correlations between variables.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Coefficients with robust standard errors of the mixed model with dieting status and dieting success as predictors of reaction time.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Coefficients with robust standard errors of the mixed model with dieting status and dieting success as predictors of omission errors.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Coefficients with robust standard errors of the mixed model with restrained eating as predictor of reaction time.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Coefficients with robust standard errors of the mixed model with restrained eating as predictor of omission errors.

Mediation analyses were conducted with PROCESS for SPSS (Hayes, 2013) to examine an indirect effect of dieting behavior on food craving after each block via task performance (reaction time, omission errors). Food craving before the n-back task was included as covariate. Indirect (i.e., mediation) effects were evaluated with bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals based on 10,000 bootstrap samples.

Results

Associations between Participant Characteristics and Questionnaire Measures

Current dieters did not differ from non-dieters in age, BMI, dieting success, and food deprivation, but had higher scores on restrained eating (Table 1). Dieting success was uncorrelated with age and food deprivation, but negatively correlated with restrained eating and BMI. Restrained eating was uncorrelated with age, but positively correlated with BMI and food deprivation (Table 1).

Food craving was higher after the food block (M = 39.3, SD = 11.5) than before the task [M = 34.9, SD = 9.40, t(68) = 5.92, p < 0.001] and compared to food craving after the neutral block [M = 34.5, SD = 10.3, t(67) = 7.13, p < 0.001]. Food craving before the task and after the neutral block did not differ from each other [t(68) = 0.37, p = 0.716]. Current dieters did not differ from non-dieters in current food craving before the task, after the food block, and after the neutral block (all ts < 0.77, ps > 0.448). Dieting success and restrained eating were uncorrelated with current food craving before the task, after the food block, and after the neutral block (all rs < 0.165, ps > 0.177).

Task Performance

Block type significantly predicted reaction time (Table 2). As the food block was coded with 1 and the neutral block was coded with 2, the negative coefficient indicates that reaction time was slower in the food block (M = 595 ms, SD = 103) than in the neutral block (M = 573 ms, SD = 112). Dieting status showed a cross-level interaction with block type when predicting reaction time (Table 2). To follow up the nature of this interaction, paired t-tests were calculated, comparing reaction time in the food and neutral block in dieters and non-dieters separately. In current dieters, reaction time in the food block (M = 590 ms, SD = 104) was slower than in the neutral block [M = 543 ms, SD = 122, t(23) = 2.39, p = 0.03]. In non-dieters, reaction time did not differ between block types (t(45) = 0.98, p = 0.332).

Dieting success significantly predicted omission errors on level 2 and in cross-level interaction with block type (Table 3). These effects, however, were further qualified by a dieting status × dieting success cross-level interaction with block type (Table 3). To follow up the nature of this interaction, a regression analysis was calculated with dieting status, dieting success (mean centered), and the interaction dieting status × dieting success predicting an omission errors difference score (i.e., omission errors in the food block minus omission errors in the neutral block). Dieting status (b = 0.36, SE = 0.96, p = 0.710) and dieting success (b = 0.03, SE = 0.15, p = 0.864) did not predict omission errors difference score. However, there was a significant interaction between dieting status × dieting success (b = 0.74, SE = 0.29, p = 0.013). Probing this interaction revealed that dieting success was negatively associated with the number of omission errors in current dieters, but not in non-dieters (Figure 2). Specifically, current dieters made more omission errors in the food relative to the neutral block with decreasing dieting success or, vice versa, current dieters made fewer omission errors in the food relative to the neutral block with increasing dieting success.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Simple slopes probing the interaction of dieting status and dieting success when predicting omission errors in the food block minus omission errors in the neutral block. There was a negative association between dieting success and omission errors in current dieters, but not in non-dieters. The asterisk indicates p < 0.05.

Restrained eating did not predict task performance (Tables 4 and 5). The only significant predictor of task performance in these models was block type when predicting reaction time (Table 4), similar to the model described above (Table 2).

Mediation Analyses

There was an indirect effect of dieting status on food craving after the food block via reaction time difference score (i.e., reaction time in the food block minus reaction time in the neutral block; Figure 3). That is, being a dieter was indirectly related to higher food craving after the food block compared to being a non-dieter. This effect, however, was not directly observable, but mediated by current dieters’ slower reactions in the food block relative to reaction time in the neutral block. A similar analysis with food craving after the neutral block as outcome variable did not indicate such an indirect effect (bootstrap estimate -0.41, 95%CI [-1.82, 0.15]). Furthermore, similar mediation models with omission errors difference score as mediator, which additionally included dieting success and the interaction of dieting status × dieting success as predictors (moderated mediation, model no. 7 in PROCESS) did not reveal any indirect effects.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Mediation model of an indirect effect of dieting status (independent variable) on food craving after the food block (outcome variable) via reaction time in the food block minus reaction time in the neutral block (mediator). Food craving before the task was entered as covariate. The dashed line indicates that there was an indirect effect of dieting status on food craving after the food block (bootstrap estimate -1.03, 95%CI [-3.19, -0.03]) in the absence of a total (c) or direct (c’) effect. Asterisks indicate p ≤ 0.05.

Discussion

In the current study, female students performed a working memory task with food and neutral stimuli. Food craving was higher after the food block compared to before and compared to food craving after the neutral block, indicating that the palatable food cues used in the n-back task induced craving. Reactions in response to food cues were slower than in response to neutral cues in general and in current dieters in particular. Mediation analyses showed that there was an indirect effect of dieting status on current food craving after the food block via reaction time. Specifically, current dieters reacted slower in response to food cues relative to neutral cues and these reactions were, in turn, predictive of higher subsequent food craving. In contrast to previous studies (Kemps and Tiggemann, 2005; Kemps et al., 2005), current dieters did not show impairments in working memory performance in general. This might be explained by methodological differences between studies as Kemps et al. (2005) employed working memory span tasks, which have been argued to require more subcomponents of executive functions (Diamond, 2013).

While the finding of slower reaction time to food cues replicates findings from a previous study with a similar n-back task (Meule et al., 2012d), these slowed reactions were predictive of more intense food craving, which was not found in the previous study. One reason for this discrepancy might be that current food craving was only assessed once after completion of the entire task in the previous study (Meule et al., 2012d), but was not measured promptly after each block, like it was done in the present study. Nevertheless, results are in line with studies in which induction of food craving resulted in a slowing of responses in subsequent reaction time tasks (e.g., Kemps et al., 2008) and with a study showing that slowed responses in response to food cues in a motor inhibition task predicted subsequent food craving (Meule et al., 2014b). Furthermore, the current study is the first to show that current dieters are more prone to this slowing of reactions to food cues compared to non-dieters, which mediates experiencing more intense food craving subsequently.

In contrast to a previous finding (Meule et al., 2012d), omission errors did not differ between the food and the neutral block, which may be related to changes in stimulus selection and design (e.g., no differentiation between sweet and savory foods). However, it was found that dieting status and dieting success interactively predicted the number of omission errors: higher dieting success was associated with fewer omission errors in response to food cues relative to neutral cues in current dieters. Of note, this effect was not associated with current food craving, that is, successful dieters experienced just as much food craving as unsuccessful dieters did. This is in line with other studies reporting that exposure to palatable food cues increases food craving to an equal extent in successful and unsuccessful dieters (Houben et al., 2012), at least when they are hungry (Meule et al., 2014a). Although successful and unsuccessful dieters appear to be equally tempted by palatable food, successful dieters seem to be better able to resist these temptations. This is also in line with the goal conflict model of eating behavior, which posits that “even though palatable food stimuli also prime the eating enjoyment goal [in successful dieters], the increased accessibility of the dieting goal helps them to inhibit eating enjoyment and to engage in healthy eating” (Stroebe et al., 2013, p. 125). One mechanism of this successful self-regulation appears to be exertion of inhibitory control over appetitive responses toward palatable food as has been demonstrated by better motor response inhibition in food-related behavioral tasks (Houben et al., 2012; Meule et al., 2014a). The current results extend these findings to another domain of executive functioning, namely working memory. Similar to results from behavioral inhibition tasks (Houben et al., 2012; Meule et al., 2014a), successful dieters did not show better task performance in general, but specifically in response to food cues. Thus, it appears that palatable food cues boost executive functioning in successful dieters, leading to better working memory performance and inhibitory control (or, vice versa, that food cues impair executive functioning in unsuccessful dieters).

Restrained eating was unrelated to current food craving and task performance in the current study. Similarly, other researchers have argued that previous experiences with successful weight control (i.e., PSRS scores) are a more important predictor of disinhibition to food cues (or disinhibited food intake; Houben et al., 2012). The current results extend these observations by highlighting the importance of further differentiating between current dieting and non-dieting in addition to successful and unsuccessful weight regulation. This may be necessary as it appears that there is a subgroup of individuals with high PSRS scores who are not concerned with regulating eating and weight (Nguyen and Polivy, 2014). Although restrained eating (as measured with the RS in particular) has been consistently found to be associated with overeating and higher BMI (Stroebe, 2008), it appears that taking current dieting status and dieting success into account may be better suited for identifying mechanisms that predict successful or unsuccessful eating- and weight regulation (Stroebe et al., 2013).

Interpretation of results is limited to young women and, thus, future studies are necessary that extend findings to other samples such as men or samples with a higher range in age and BMI. Furthermore, laboratory food intake was not measured in the current study and it would be worthwhile to examine if increased food craving or a higher number of omission errors in response to food cues does actually translate to increased food consumption after the task. Finally, while dieting status, dieting success, and food craving were measured with self-report questionnaires in the current study, future studies may investigate if task performance in such an n-back task will be able to predict actual dieting behavior and food intake in daily life, for example, as assessed with ecological momentary assessment.

Given the findings obtained in the current study, future research may examine if food-related working memory performance can be improved by repeated training and if this translates into higher dieting success. Although working memory trainings have been found to increase working memory performance, it appears that these effects do not generalize to measures of “real-world” cognitive skills (Schwaighofer et al., 2015; Melby-Lervåg et al., 2016). Recent studies suggest, however, that general working memory trainings may indeed modify eating-related outcomes. In a study with obese children, for example, a combined inhibition and working memory training attenuated weight regain after an inpatient weight-loss treatment, albeit this effect was no longer present after 12 weeks (Verbeken et al., 2013). Most recently, a working memory training has been found to reduce eating concerns and emotional eating in obese adults (Houben et al., 2016). Similar to studies, in which food-related motor response inhibition trainings are used in an effort to modify eating behavior (Stice et al., 2016), future studies may develop and examine the effectiveness of food-related working memory trainings. Possibly, such trainings may facilitate dieting success by strengthening executive functions while at the same time reducing food cue reactivity through food cue exposure (Jansen et al., 2015).

Author Contributions

AM conceived the study, analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript.

Funding

AM is supported by the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (ERC-StG-2014 639445 NewEat, granted to Prof. Dr. Jens Blechert). Publication of this work was supported by the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Salzburg.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank J. Grabow, T. Nill, M. Kretzschmar, M. Mayerhofer, E. Miczka, O. Rufman, and S. Thum for their help with data collection.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.food-pics.sbg.ac.at
  2. ^ Picture number in the food.pics database: food items – 6, 9, 10, 22, 27, 38, 41, 42, 56, 71, 82, 83, 89, 106, 111, 113, 115, 116, 120, 133, 137, 142, 143, 145, 159, 165, 166, 167, 172, 173; neutral items – 1000, 1001, 1002, 1003, 1004, 1005, 1007, 1009, 1011, 1012, 1013, 1014, 1015, 1016, 1019, 1020, 1021, 1023, 1024, 1025, 1026, 1028, 1029, 1030, 1031, 1032, 1033, 1034, 1035, 1036.

References

Blechert, J., Meule, A., Busch, N. A., and Ohla, K. (2014). Food-pics: an image database for experimental research on eating and appetite. Front. Psychol. 5: 617. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cepeda-Benito, A., Gleaves, D. H., Williams, T. L., and Erath, S. A. (2000). The development and validation of the state and trait food-cravings questionnaires. Behav. Ther. 31, 151–173. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(00)80009-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinkel, A., Berth, H., Exner, C., Rief, W., and Balck, F. (2005). German version of the restraint scale for the assessment of restrained eating. Diagnostica 51, 67–74. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.51.2.67

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fedoroff, I. C., Polivy, J., and Herman, C. P. (2003). The specificity of restrained versus unrestrained eaters’ responses to food cues: general desire to eat, or craving for the cued food? Appetite 41, 7–13. doi: 10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S., and Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not unto temptation: momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 296–309. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friese, M., Engeler, M., and Florack, A. (2015). Self-perceived successful weight regulators are less affected by self-regulatory depletion in the domain of eating behavior. Eat. Behav. 16, 5–8. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, P. A. (2016). Executive-control processes in high-calorie food consumption. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 25, 91–98. doi: 10.1177/0963721415625049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Herman, C. P., Polivy, J., Pliner, P., Threlkeld, J., and Munic, D. (1978). Distractibility in dieters and nondieters: an alternative view of “externality”. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 36, 536–548. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.36.5.536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgs, S., Dolmans, D., Humphreys, G. W., and Rutters, F. (2015). Dietary self-control influences top–down guidance of attention to food cues. Front. Psychol. 6:427. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Friese, M., Schmeichel, B. J., and Baddeley, A. D. (2011). “Working memory and self-regulation,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation, 2nd Edn, eds K. D. Vohs and R. F. Baumeister (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 204–225.

Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., and Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houben, K., Dassen, F. C., and Jansen, A. (2016). Taking control: working memory training in overweight individuals increases self-regulation of food intake. Appetite 105, 567–574. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.06.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houben, K., Nederkoorn, C., and Jansen, A. (2012). Too tempting to resist? Past success at weight control rather than dietary restraint determines exposure-induced disinhibited eating. Appetite 59, 550–555. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jansen, A., Houben, K., and Roefs, A. (2015). A cognitive profile of obesity and its translation into new interventions. Front. Psychol. 6:1807. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemps, E., and Tiggemann, M. (2005). Working memory performance and preoccupying thoughts in female dieters: evidence for a selective central executive impairment. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 44, 357–366. doi: 10.1348/014466505X35272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemps, E., and Tiggemann, M. (2010). A cognitive experimental approach to understanding and reducing food cravings. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19, 86–90. doi: 10.1177/0963721410364494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemps, E., and Tiggemann, M. (2015). A role for mental imagery in the experience and reduction of food cravings. Front. Psychiatry 5:193. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemps, E., Tiggemann, M., and Grigg, M. (2008). Food cravings consume limited cognitive resources. J. Exp. Psychol. 14, 247–254. doi: 10.1037/a0012736

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemps, E., Tiggemann, M., and Marshall, K. (2005). Relationship between dieting to lose weight and the functioning of the central executive. Appetite 45, 287–294. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2005.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowe, M. R. (1995). Restrained eating and dieting: replication of their divergent effects on eating regulation. Appetite 25, 115–118. doi: 10.1006/appe.1995.0047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, J., Andrade, J., Kavanagh, D. J., and Hetherington, M. (2012). Elaborated intrusion theory: a cognitive-emotional theory of food craving. Curr. Obes. Rep. 1, 114–121. doi: 10.1007/s13679-012-0010-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melby-Lervåg, M., Redick, T. S., and Hulme, C. (2016). Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer” evidence from a meta-analytic review. Pers. Psychol. Sci. 11, 512–534. doi: 10.1177/1745691616635612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A., Lutz, A., Vögele, C., and Kübler, A. (2012a). Food cravings discriminate differentially between successful and unsuccessful dieters and non-dieters. Validation of the food craving questionnaires in German. Appetite 58, 88–97. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A., Lutz, A., Vögele, C., and Kübler, A. (2012b). Self-reported dieting success is associated with cardiac autonomic regulation in current dieters. Appetite 59, 494–498. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A., Papies, E. K., and Kübler, A. (2012c). Differentiating between successful and unsuccessful dieters: validity and reliability of the perceived self-regulatory success in dieting scale. Appetite 58, 822–826. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A., Skirde, A. K., Freund, R., Vögele, C., and Kübler, A. (2012d). High-calorie food-cues impair working memory performance in high and low food cravers. Appetite 59, 264–269. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A., Lutz, A. P. C., Krawietz, V., Stützer, J., Vögele, C., and Kübler, A. (2014a). Food-cue affected motor response inhibition and self-reported dieting success: a pictorial affective shifting task. Front. Psychol. 5:1–17. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A., Lutz, A. P. C., Vögele, C., and Kübler, A. (2014b). Impulsive reactions to food-cues predict subsequent food craving. Eat. Behav. 15, 99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A., Westenhöfer, J., and Kübler, A. (2011). Food cravings mediate the relationship between rigid, but not flexible control of eating behavior and dieting success. Appetite 57, 582–584. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, C., and Polivy, J. (2014). Eating behavior, restraint status, and BMI of individuals high and low in perceived self-regulatory success. Appetite 75, 49–53. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papies, E. K., Stroebe, W., and Aarts, H. (2008). Healthy cognition: Processes of self-regulatory success in restrained eating. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1290–1300. doi: 10.1177/0146167208320063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., Cheong, Y. F., Congdon, R. T., and du Toit, M. (2011). HLM 7 – Hierarchical Linear and Nonlinear Modeling. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc.

Google Scholar

Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F., and Bühner, M. (2015). Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educ. Psychol. 50, 138–166. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2015.1036274

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Lawrence, N. S., Kemps, E., and Veling, H. (2016). Training motor responses to food: a novel treatment for obesity targeting implicit processes. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 49, 16–27. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroebe, W. (2008). “Restrained eating and the breakdown of self-regulation,” in Dieting, Overweight, and Obesity - Self-Regulation in a Food-Rich Environment, ed. W. Stroebe (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 115–139.

Stroebe, W., Mensink, W., Aarts, H., Schut, H., and Kruglanski, A. W. (2008). Why dieters fail: testing the goal conflict model of eating. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 26–36. doi: 10.1037/a0030849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroebe, W., Van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Papies, E. K., and Aarts, H. (2013). Why most dieters fail but some succeed: a goal conflict model of eating behavior. Psychol. Rev. 120, 110–138. doi: 10.1037/a0030849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiggemann, M., Kemps, E., and Parnell, J. (2010). The selective impact of chocolate craving on visuospatial working memory. Appetite 55, 44–48. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuschl, R. J. (1990). From dietary restraint to binge eating: some theoretical considerations. Appetite 14, 105–109. doi: 10.1016/0195-6663(90)90004-R

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Stroebe, W., and Aarts, H. (2011a). Through the eyes of dieters: biased size perception of food following tempting food primes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 293–299. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Stroebe, W., and Aarts, H. (2012). The rise and fall of self-control: temptation-elicited goal activation and effortful goal-directed behavior. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 4, 546–554. doi: 10.1177/1948550612471061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Stroebe, W., and Aarts, H. (2013). Successful restrained eating and trait impulsiveness. Appetite 60, 81–84. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Stroebe, W., Papies, E. K., and Aarts, H. (2011b). Implementation intentions as goal primes: boosting self-control in tempting environments. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 41, 551–557. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.799

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verbeken, S., Braet, C., Goossens, L., and van der Oord, S. (2013). Executive function training with game elements for obese children: a novel treatment to enhance self-regulatory abilities for weight-control. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 290–299. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: diet, dieting success, restrained eating, working memory, executive functioning, food cues

Citation: Meule A (2016) Dieting and Food Cue-Related Working Memory Performance. Front. Psychol. 7:1944. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01944

Received: 10 May 2016; Accepted: 28 November 2016;
Published: 14 December 2016.

Edited by:

Julia M. Hormes, University at Albany (SUNY), USA

Reviewed by:

Ulrich Schweiger, Lübeck University Medical School, Germany
Eva Kemps, Flinders University, Australia
Emily Feig, Drexel University, USA

Copyright © 2016 Meule. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Adrian Meule, adrian.meule@sbg.ac.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.