Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychiatry, 13 May 2011
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Translational research models and methods for mother-infant interactions and developmental studies View all 32 articles

When Mothers Go Wrong: Likely Neural Undercurrents Related to Poor Parenting


      Craig Howard Kinsley* Craig Howard Kinsley*Haddis Tujuba Haddis TujubaElizabeth E. A. Meyer
   Elizabeth E. A. Meyer
  • Department of Psychology, Center of Neuroscience, University of Richmond, Richmond, VA, USA

The mammalian model of survival begins with puzzling-out a simple but stark truth: Life must learn to care for life. We have described the experiences involved in changing from nulliparous female to mother, from unresponsive to committed. The transition taking place in the nervous system that underpins the shift from largely self-centered organism to other-focused caregiver is accompanied by an assortment of effects ranging from basic gene expression changes, to modifications of neuronal complexity and activity, to wholesale shifts in the size of specific brain structures. In total, the female changes in ways both subtle and striking, and for one simple reason: reducing the cost:benefit ratio required for successful rearing of young. The system, however, is imperfect. Here we attempt to reconcile the ultimate goals of life and evolution, the manifest reshaping of the female mammal’s brain in service to her young, with its incomplete or faulty development. That is, things go wrong; mothers can be apathetic, abusive, or worse. How might the inherent adaptation that, on average, directs the formation of the maternal brain, and which in turn, governs the set of behaviors required for successful reproduction and gene passage, fail to adequately express itself?

Susan Smith drives her car and two young boys into a lake in South Carolina, killing the children. Andrea Yates drowns five children, one after the other, in her family’s bathtub. These cold and heinous acts are doubly shocking because of the perpetrators’ relationship to the victims: the children’s mothers. How, we ask, can that most hard-wired of mammalian behaviors – in fact, the very word “mammal” derives from the act of caring – go awry? Why and in what ways do mothers vary in their degree of maternal motivation/care?

The simple answer is, nothing is perfect. Things go wrong, sometimes terribly so. From the standpoint of the brain’s regulation of maternal behavior, however, which of the factors that normally order maternal responsiveness might malfunction? The fosB gene is a good place to start.

Brown et al. (1996) established a regulatory role for the fosB gene in the display of mouse maternal behavior. A knockout of fosB gene activity, including preoptic area (POA) expression, led to a significant reduction in what the authors refer to as “nurturing,” wherein a lack of maternal responsiveness affected offspring survival, in the absence of observed deficits in basic hypothalamic activity, pregnancy, cognition, or olfaction. Reduced medial preoptic area (mPOA) neural activity is associated with poor maternal behavior (Numan and Insel, 2003), much as reductions in frontal lobe volume recently reported in human mothers may be related to fewer positive thoughts toward young (Kim et al., 2010). Both may depend, in part, on responding appropriately to offspring-related sensory stimuli. It appears, then, that some more intimate aspect of maternal–offspring interaction may be lacking, a defect which resembles an apparent inability to accurately attend to the offspring’s sensory load. Cues that normally elicit maternal behavior from the mother failed to do so in the fosB knockouts and, perhaps, failed to cascade onto the otherwise receptive brain. The possibility exists, then, that deficiencies in other maternally related genes (Contino et al., 2007; Ferguson et al., 2008; Kinsley et al., 2008; Mann and Lee, 2010), as well as fosB and its human analog – which plays a role in stress responsiveness (Vialou et al., 2010) and which could be related to reactions to young – may be associated with a diminution in maternal responsiveness or interest. Two major research questions arise: Do such genetic effects mean a greater likelihood of neglect or abuse? And is the system similar in humans?

A spate of recent papers describes some of the alterations that define the normal maternal brain. Kim et al. (2010), using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), reported significant structural changes in several major brain regions of human mothers over the first few postpartum months, during which the intimate relationship between mother and infant develops and deepens. The images of mothers’ brains at 2–4 weeks postpartum, and 3–4 months postpartum, showed increased gray matter volumes in prefrontal cortex, parietal lobe, and midbrain areas. Further, increased gray matter volume in the hypothalamus, substantia nigra, and amygdala was correlated with maternal positive perception of the baby (more positive, more gray matter). These results suggest that the first months of motherhood in humans are accompanied by structural changes in brain regions implicated in maternal motivation and behaviors (Kinsley and Meyer, 2010).

Other work is suggestive, too, as it parallels human experiences. Lippmann et al. (2007) reported that chronic maternal separation during the postnatal period induces long-term behavioral and neural modifications in the adult. Such individuals exhibited significant reductions in the level of the protein, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and marked maternal behavioral deficiencies. Francis et al. (1999) and others have shown that rat pups that experience their mothers licking and grooming, are likely to act the same way toward their own offspring, non-genomically passing-on behaviors to subsequent generations. Korosi and Baram (2009) suggest that early childhood deprivation of “maternal love” may lead to variable neuroendocrine stress responses and differential coping. It is of interest, therefore, to hear accounts of childhood stress and abuse in the sad cases of Yates and Smith above.

Such experiences likely change the brain in manifest ways, possibly influencing other cognitive and emotional processes. Keyser-Marcus et al. (2001) have reported that neurons in the mPOA demonstrate a significant increase in volume the closer an animal gets to parturition, as if the region were readying itself for the requisite maternal responsiveness to follow. Numan and Insel (2003) have discussed at length the role of the mPOA in maternal behavior, and the Kim et al. (2010) data implicate anterior hypothalamic structures such as the mPOA in their “good thoughts-good mother” data mentioned above. A model for likely neural changes that do not occur in so-called bad mothers would present a valuable research tool to the translational field.

In summary, the data suggest that adequate maternal motivation, far from an intrinsic or instinctual state, is, rather, a consequence of active processes “building” a responsive neural substrate. Therefore, if improperly assembled, an incomplete, or defective maternal brain may fail in its task of caring adequately for young – or worse – making the faulty maternal brain a valuable object for additional study.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the National Institutes of Health (NIMH: 1-R15-HD37578-01), and the NSF (NSF: BCS-0619544) for their support; and the University of Richmond for its generous and long-time support of student and faculty scholarship.

References

Brown, J. R., Ye, H., Bronson, R. T., Dikkes, P., and Greenberg, M. E. (1996). A defect in nurturing in mice lacking the immediate early gene, fosB. Cell 86, 297–309.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Contino, R., Friedenberg, J., Christon Norkunas, T., Worthington, D., Jablow, L., Baranova, A., Felicio, L. F., Lambert, K. G., and Kinsley, C. H. (2007). The expression of the maternal brain: specific genes involved in the neuroplasticity of motherhood. Abstr. - Soc. Neurosci. 33, Program#/Poster#: 309.5/DDD13.

Ferguson, T. R., Ferguson, T., Victoria, L., Norkunas, T., Worthington-Stoneman, D., Jones, C., French, K., Skurka, D., Chan, C., Baranova, A., Felicio, L. F., Lambert, K. G., and Kinsley, C. H. (2008). Diminution of deposits of aging-related neurodegenerative substances in the brains of maternal rats. Abstr. - Soc. Neurosci. 34, Program#/Poster#: 794.4/TT86.

Francis, D., Diorio, J., Kiu, D., and Meaney, M. J. (1999). Nongenomic transmission across generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science 286, 1155–1158.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keyser-Marcus, L., Stafisso-Sandoz, G., Gerecke, K., Jasnow, A., Nightingale, L., Lambert, K. G., Gatewood, J., and Kinsley, C. H. (2001). Alterations of medial preoptic area neurons following pregnancy and pregnancy-like steroidal treatment in the rat. Brain Res. Bull. 55, 737–745.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kim, P., Leckman, J. F., Mayes, L. C., Feldman, R., Wang, X., and Swain, J. E. (2010). The plasticity of human maternal brain: longitudinal changes in brain anatomy during the early postpartum period. Behav. Neurosci. 124, 695–700.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kinsley, C. H., Bardi, M., Karelina, K., Rima, B., Christon, L., Friedenberg, J., and Griffin, G. (2008). Motherhood induces, and maintains behavioral, and neural plasticity across the lifespan in the rat. Arch. Sex Behav. 37, 43–56.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kinsley, C. H., and Meyer, E. A. (2010). The construction of the maternal brain. Behav. Neurosci. 124, 710–714.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Korosi, A., and Baram, T. Z. (2009). The pathways from mother’s love to baby’s future. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 3:27. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.027.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lippmann, M., Bress, A., Nemeroff, C. B., Plotsky, P. M., and Monteggia, L. M. (2007). Long-term behavioural and molecular alterations associated with maternal separation in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 3091–3098.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mann, P. E., and Lee, J. O. (2010). Gene expression profiling in the brain of pregnant and lactating rats. Abstr. - Soc. Neurosci. 36, Program#/Poster#: 187.4/YY4.

Numan, M., and Insel, T. R. (2003). The Neurobiology of Parental Behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Vialou, V., Robison, A. J., Laplant, Q. C., Covington, H. E., Dietz, D. M., Ohnishi, Y. N., Mouzon, E., Rush, A. J., Watts, E. L., Wallace, D. L., Iñiguez, S. D., Ohnishi, Y. H., Steiner, M. A., Warren, B. L., Krishnan, V., Bolaños, C. A., Neve, R. L., Ghose, S., Berton, O., Tamminga, C. A., and Nestler, E. J. (2010). DeltaFosB in brain reward circuits mediates resilience to stress and antidepressant responses. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 745–752.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Citation: Kinsley CH, Tujuba H and Meyer EEA (2011) When mothers go wrong: likely neural undercurrents related to poor parenting. Front. Psychiatry 2:26. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00026

Received: 01 March 2011; Accepted: 28 April 2011;
Published online: 13 May 2011.

Copyright: © 2011 Kinsley, Tujuba and Meyer. This is an open-access article subject to a non-exclusive license between the authors and Frontiers Media SA, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and other Frontiers conditions are complied with.

*Correspondence: ckinsley@richmond.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.