Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 24 September 2013
Sec. Neuroimaging

Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Consolidation of Fear Memory


      Manish Asthana* Manish Asthana1*
      Katharina Nueckel Katharina Nueckel1
      Andreas Mühlberger Andreas Mühlberger2
      Dorothea Neueder Dorothea Neueder1
      Thomas Polak Thomas Polak1
      Katharina Domschke Katharina Domschke1
      Jürgen Deckert Jürgen Deckert1Martin J. Herrmann
   Martin J. Herrmann1
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
  • 2Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany

It has been shown that applying transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) influences declarative memory processes. This study investigates the efficacy of tDCS on emotional memory consolidation, especially experimental fear conditioning. We applied an auditory fear-conditioning paradigm, in which two differently colored squares (blue and yellow) were presented as conditioned stimuli (CS) and an auditory stimulus as unconditioned stimulus (UCS). Sixty-nine participants were randomly assigned into three groups: anodal, cathodal, and sham stimulation. The participants of the two active groups (i.e., anodal and cathodal) received tDCS over the left DLPFC for 12 min after fear conditioning. The effect of fear conditioning and consolidation (24 h later) was measured by assessing the skin conductance response (SCR) to the CS. The results provide evidence that cathodal stimulation of the left DLPFC leads to an inhibitory effect on fear memory consolidation compared to anodal and sham stimulation, as indicated by decreased SCRs to CS+ presentation during extinction training at day 2. In conclusion, current work suggests that cathodal stimulation interferes with processes of fear memory consolidation.

Introduction

Exaggerated fear is the root cause of the fear memory persistence, which further leads to the development of anxiety disorder (1, 2). A preponderance of neurobiological data from the past decades demonstrates that consolidation processes is responsible for making fear memories robust. Studies investigating the neurobiological nature of memory consolidation showed that memory consolidation is dependent on the interaction of neurons and their synapses (35). Hence, the process is commonly known as “synaptic consolidation” or “cellular consolidation”. Synaptic consolidation of memory highlights that synaptic plasticity plays an important role in learning and memory processes (4).

Research on learning and memory shows that the mechanism of synaptic plasticity depends upon several factors such as (i) initially activated neural circuit, (ii) release of second messengers, i.e., cyclic AMP (cAMP) and protein-kinase A (PKA), (iii) pre- and post-generated proteins, and (iv) the regulation of genes (4, 5).

In an Aplysia model, researchers have shown the changes occur during consolidation and formation process from short- and long-term (5). The incoming sensory inputs enhance the release of serotonin, which is followed by an increase of second messenger cAMP and PKA activity within the neuron. Furthermore, increase of PKA activity phosphorylates neurotransmitter channels, vesicles, and other proteins, which results in strengthening of the synaptic connections (5, 6). Schafe et al. (6) affirmed that protein synthesis is required during LTM storage, but not during STM storage. Protein synthesis during long-term storage strengthens the synaptic connections.

Furthermore, post-learning interferences of newly formed memory with electroconvulsive shock or protein synthesis blockers also modulate or disrupt LTM, but leave STM intact (7). It has been confirmed that the administration of propranolol, a beta-adrenergic receptor antagonist, during consolidation can disrupt this process (79).

Recent studies have shown that procedural and declarative memory consolidation can also be modulated by transcranial brain stimulation (10, 11). Elmer et al. (10) showed that cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) leads to decrease performance in short-term verbal learning compared with the placebo stimulation (sham) condition. Penolazzi et al. (11) demonstrated that tDCS could also modulate emotional memory. In detail, they reported that a combination of right anodal and left cathodal stimulation of the DLPFC facilitates the recall of pleasant compared to unpleasant and neutral images, whereas left anodal/right cathodal stimulation shows contrasting results. This double dissociation effect of tDCS adds evidence to the hypothesis of hemispheric specialization with regard to emotional processes.

Moreover, recent studies show that tDCS modulates attention (12), working memory processes (10, 1315), and behavioral inhibition (i.e., difficulty to inhibit response) (16). Jacobson et al. (16) found that anodal tDCS to the right inferior parietal gyrus improves behavioral inhibition suggesting that tDCS modulates cognitive control in healthy individuals. Balconi and Vitaloni (17) demonstrated that cathodal tDCS over the left DLPFC and anodal tDCS over the right supraorbital area attenuates the cognitive load in the incongruent processing task and limits the incongruence effect generated by semantic anomaly.

Thus, converging evidence points to tDCS as a successful neuromodulator tool (43). Earlier findings robustly suggest tDCS interference with memory consolidation processes (10, 1315). However, the neuromodulatory effect of tDCS on fear memory consolidation is still unknown, which is the focus of the current study. With respect to conditioned fear, we expect an enhancement of fear consolidation by anodal stimulation and attenuation by cathodal stimulation.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixty-nine students from the University of Würzburg were recruited through online advertisements to participate in the present study. The study was described in short, with the info that the study was conducted on 2 days, includes fear conditioning and a weak electrical stimulation of the brain. No psychology students were allowed. Participants received a financial gratification of 12 Euro in total on the second day. Participants were eligible for inclusion if they met the following criteria: (i) right handed; (ii) age 18–30 years; (iii) German native; (iv) females taking contraceptives during the study period. Participants were excluded from the trial if they met the following criteria: (i) any metal object or implant in brain, skull, scalp, or neck; (ii) implantable devices, including cardiac pacemakers and defibrillators; (iii) any neurological or psychiatric illnesses; (iv) pregnancy. Information about these criteria was obtained by questionnaires. Participants were randomly assigned into three groups: anodal, cathodal, and sham stimulation. The three stimulation groups do not differ with respect to the subjective ratings in anxiety sensitivity as measured by the ASI-3 questionnaire [anodal: 16.8 ± 9.1; cathodal: 16.2 ± 7.7; sham: 16.3 ± 9.8; F(2, 46) = 0.02; p = 0.98; ASI-3; (18)]. After data analysis (see below), three participants were excluded due to artifacts (i.e., origin of response to stimuli before a baseline in more than eight numbers of trials resulting in less than three artifact-free trials for each condition). Three subjects had to be excluded as they had only null responses; a null response was defined by amplitudes <0.01 μV. Seven participants were excluded because they do not show a conditioned response [conditioned stimuli (CS)+ > CS−during acquisition and also CS+ (acquisition)]. Two participants did not volunteer to participate on extinction training (24 h later: day 2). Three participants were rejected because tDCS stimulation stopped due to high-impedance of stimulation electrodes (>20 kΩ) (19). One participant was rejected as it was identified with depressive symptoms and one participant was recruited twice by mistake so only the first recorded data was considered in the current study. The demographical data of the remaining 49 participants is shown in Table 1. The study was in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki in their latest version from 2008 and has been approved by the ethics committee of the University of Würzburg. All participants gave written informed consent to participate in the study.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data of the participants.

Apparatus

Skin conductance responses (SCRs) were recorded by a constant voltage circuit with two 0.5 V across both electrodes and a 16-channel QuickAmp amplifier (Brain Products GmbH) with a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz and a notch filter of 50 Hz. Responses were recorded by using two Ag/AgCl electrodes (diameter = 13 mm) filled with non-hydrating gel. Both electrodes were attached to the volar surfaces on medial phalanges of the participants’ non-dominant palm (20). A digital display showed the skin conductance values, which were continuously recorded via Vision Recorder Version (1.0) software (Brain Products GmbH).

The unconditioned stimulus (UCS) consisted of 2000 ms of woman’s scream (code number 276) adapted from the International Affective Digital Sounds (IADS), delivered through an external sound card (Terratec, DMX 6 Fire USB) at an intensity of 102 db.

Colored squares (blue and yellow), serving as CSs, were presented at a 16° visual angle on central screen of 19″ monitor for 4 s with the inter-stimulus-interval (ISI) of 20–22 s and counterbalanced as CS+ and CS− to each participants, so that both squares were equally often selected as CS+ and CS−. The exposure time of the CSs and UCS was controlled using Presentation® Version 13.0 software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Albany, CA, USA).

Procedure

The present study was conducted at two consecutive days maintaining the time difference between two sessions from 20 to 26 h. The study involved three stages: habituation, acquisition, and extinction. The first session (day 1) consisted of the habituation and acquisition stages, in which participants learned the association of conditioned (CS) and unconditioned stimuli (UCS). On day 1, the experiment had two phases: (i) the habituation phase, consisting of eight trials in which squares of each color were presented on the monitor to reach a stable response to the stimuli and (ii) the acquisition phase, during which 16 trials of squares of each color were presented. During acquisition phase, one of the colored squares was paired with the scream (75% reinforcement rate); after 10–20 min of fear acquisition, electrodes for electrical stimulation were applied. As previous studies showed that the consolidation process occurs during the time window from minutes to 6 h (4, 21). Hence, in the current study all participants were stimulated after 10–20 min. The variable time frame of 10–20 min depended on the fitting of the tDCS electrodes. During the extinction phase (day 2), all participants underwent extinction training (see Figure 1). During extinction training, 16 CS+ and CS− were repeatedly presented in absence of the UCS.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Post-treatment of fear learning with tDCS disrupts fear consolidation. (A) Schematic overview of experimental procedure. (B) Mean differential SCRs (CS+ minus CS−) during habituation, acquisition, and extinction (first five trials) for each experimental group (anodal, cathodal, and sham). The three groups showed normal fear acquisition. Fear return was observed in the group anodal and sham. In contrast there was no trace of fear observed in the cathodal group.

DC Stimulation

Transcranial direct current stimulation is a non-invasive method, in which direct current is applied over the scalp to modulate human brain excitability (2224). The exact mechanism of tDCS is unclear, but it is assumed that several minutes of anodal tDCS excites the neurons below the stimulated region and leads to the depolarization, while cathodal tDCS has an inhibitory effect, i.e., it reduces the neuronal firing resulting in hyper-polarization. This suggests that tDCS functions via modulating the cortical excitability of the stimulated regions (25).

tDCS was delivered by a battery-driven stimulator (DC-Stimulator-Plus, NeuroConn GmbH, Ilmenau, Germany) approved for use in humans. A pair of conductive-rubber electrodes (size 5 cm × 7 cm = 35 cm2) coated with Ten20 cream conductive paste (Waever and Company, CO, USA) was positioned over the left DLPFC (electrode position F3) and left mastoid (reference electrode) (14, 26, 27) according to the international 10–20 electroencephalographic-system (28). For active tDCS, a constant current of 1 mA was applied for 12 min (current density: 0.0286 mA/cm2) (29, 30). The current was ramped up or down over the first and last 10 s of stimulation, respectively. The stimulator, always ranging below 20 kΩ, controlled the impedance. During sham condition the constant current was ramped up over the first 10 s once the DC had reached a current flow of 1 mA the current ramped down over 10 s. Therefore, the sham stimulation led to the same sensation in the participants, but had no long lasting effects (31, 32).

Analysis

Participants were excluded from the analysis when not showing conditioning (CS+ < CS−during acquisition) and CS+ (acquisition) having more than 50% artifacts (i.e., ≥8 trials in CS+ or CS−) in the raw data of SCR. Participants were also excluded when the tDCS electrodes read impedances higher than 20 kΩ during the active and sham condition.

Offline analysis was performed in stepwise manner firstly recorded SCRs data was filtered at 1 Hz, and segmented into different phases (e.g., habituation, acquisition, and extinction) as well as single CS+ and CS− trials. Secondly, each segment was baseline corrected 1,000 ms prior to the onset of the stimuli and characterized by the peak response of the SCR signal between 1 and 4.5 s after stimulus onset. Finally, artifact rejection was conducted manually for all 72 trials per participants.

For the statistical analysis, the first CS+ and CS− trial were disregarded due to the orienting response at the beginning of the session in all phases (33). The remaining trials were used to produce average normalized SCR scores (CS+ and CS−) within subjects. Raw SCR scores were square root transformed to normalize distributions. To test the fear memory trace, after tDCS all trials of the acquisition phase (i.e., trials 2–16) were considered, however, to test the effect of tDCS on fear consolidation we looked at first five trials of the extinction phase (i.e., trials 2–6). It is worth mentioning that if consolidation is interfered, the analysis of early period of retention test hints the impairment of consolidation as suggested in literature of fear conditioning (33, 34).

The effect of stimulation on fear memory consolidation was analyzed by a three-factor ANOVA with stimuli (CS+ and CS−), time [acquisition (day 1) and extinction (day 2)], and group (anodal, cathodal, and sham) as main factors.

Results

Means and standard deviations for SCRs in response to the conditioned stimulus are shown in Table 2. Two-way ANOVA was designed to assess differential fear response during habituation and acquisition among groups, with main factors of group (anodal, cathodal, and sham) and time (habituation and acquisition). The differential fear response was assessed by subtracting responses to the CS− from responses to the CS+ in corresponding trials. The differential scores were averaged across subjects. Statistic showed significant effect of time [F(1, 46) = 101.67; p < 0.01] but no effect of group or interaction [F(2, 46) = 2.57; p > 0.05]. This affirms that differential fear responses, i.e., SCR values did not differed significantly during habituation and acquisition among groups. Follow-up one-way ANOVA revealed non-significant differences during acquisition among groups [F(2, 46) = 1.42; p = 0.25].

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean and SD of skin conductance response (μS) during fear-conditioning phases separately for the three tDCS groups.

To evaluate the effect of stimulation on fear memory consolidation, a three-factor ANOVA was calculated showing a significant main effect of stimuli [F(1, 46) = 106.32; p < 0.01], time [F(1, 46) = 32.73; p < 0.01], and interaction effects stimuli × time [F(1, 46) = 109.97; p < 0.01] and stimuli × time × group [F(2, 46) = 5.03; p = 0.01] (see Figure 2). A follow-up t-test was used to compare the anodal and cathodal group with regard to the amount of attenuation between acquisition and extinction phase for the differential values between CS+ and CS− (see Figure 2), which differed, significantly [t(1, 32) = 2.34; p < 0.05] with lower values for the cathodal group. In a similar way, the cathodal group also displayed diminished differential values compared to the sham group [t(1, 31) = −2.88; p < 0.01]. This might indicate that cathodal stimulation has an inhibitory effect on fear consolidation, while anodal stimulation and sham did not differ significantly from each other [t(1, 29) = −0.32; p > 0.05] (see Figure 2). Chi-square test revealed non-significant differences in the gender distribution among the three groups (anodal, cathodal, and sham) (χ2 = 1.94, df = 2, p = 0.38).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Plot shows post-to-pre mean differential SCRs (Δ extinction minus Δ acquisition) for all experimental groups [i.e., anodal, cathodal, and sham]. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01 (between acquisition and extinction); error bars represent standard errors.

Discussion

To investigate the effects of tDCS on the fear consolidation process, we applied left DLPFC stimulation to healthy participants for duration of 12 min after fear conditioning. The results show that cathodal, but not anodal stimulation disrupts fear memory consolidation.

The DLPFC has an important role in memory and learning processes (10, 13). Studies using a verbal-memory task (10) or N-back task (13), respectively, showed improvement in memory after stimulating the left DLPFC, indicating the efficiency of tDCS in modulating DLPFC activity (14, 19, 35). Moreover, improvement in memory was due to anodal stimulation of the DLPFC. However, cathodal stimulation showed a reciprocal effect (35). In an EEG study, Zaehle and colleagues observed reduced accuracy in a working memory task along with tDCS of the DLPFC. The main result of the present study was an impaired fear memory consolidation after left DLPFC stimulation, which further supports the role of DLPFC stimulation in memory consolidation (10, 11, 1315, 36). In synopsis of evidence from the existing literature and the present results show a crucial impact of tDCS over the left DLPFC on fear memory consolidation processes.

Baudweig et al. (37) demonstrated in a neuroimaging study, that 5-min of cathodal tDCS leads to 38% reduction in cortical excitability, while 15–20 min of stimulation leads to a reduction of 28%. In contrast, they failed to observe any changes in cortical excitability after anodal tDCS. Nevertheless, their findings demonstrated the efficacy of tDCS in the modulation of cortical excitability in humans. Moreover, recent neuroimaging study by Keeser et al. (38) showed the modulation of resting state functional connectivity via prefrontal tDCS. They well demonstrated in their study that the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) has enhanced neural activations between frontal brains regions and tDCS applied brain regions.

Moreover, studies investigating sleep and memory processes showed that the spindle activity generated during slow-wave-sleep (SWS) is important in sending information to the hippocampus, which acts as a buffer and stores the relevant information. The generated spindle activity during SWS activates the newly encoded information in the hippocampus resulting in consolidation. This reactivation of hippocampal memory via spindle activity synchronizes with the neo-cortex and sends the newly stored information back to the neo-cortex which further triggers LTP processes (39). The spindle activity assists the strengthening of neuronal connections and potentiates consolidation. The induction of DC stimulation on the frontal area (F3) of the brain might facilitate the slow oscillatory activity, which has been suggested to aid in the potentiation of consolidation processes (14).

Lin et al. (40) showed that fear acquisition and fear extinction share some common and uncommon mechanism. They suggested that both processes require protein synthesis (cAMP response element-binding protein), kinase (phosphatidylinositol 3-kinase), and NMDA receptors in the amygdala for the consolidation mechanism. Furthermore, they demonstrated long-term behavioral changes after fear and its extinction. It was argued that learning about fear as well as extinction triggers a Ca2+ ion influx in amygdalar neurons. The increase in Ca2+ ion concentration promotes long-term synaptic depression (LTD) (14, 40). This suggests that extinction processes suppress Ca2+ ion concentrations and lead to synaptic changes, which may lead to extinction of fear memory.

It is important to address here that the tDCS parameters used in the current work ensure better control to investigate its efficacy in conditioning fear. Nitsche and Paulus (29, 30) affirmed earlier that an electrical stimulation lasting 10–13 min modulates cortical excitability and shows long-term effects up to 90 min.

Since slight modification via induction of low potential negative or positive potential might be sufficient to modulate mental processing effectively, neural connectivity and excitability holds a sensitive mechanism (44). In addition, the higher the amplification of induced current the more effective it would be, but the exploration of tDCS effects in psychiatry disorder has not yet been completed. With our findings we propose that the amplification used in the current work would be sufficient and effective in similar study and paradigm.

One point we have to discuss is the number of drop outs in our sample. Ten out of 69 were rejected by the fact that the conditioning procedure was not working. The number of 19 dropouts out of 69 participants is similar to other studies using a fear-conditioning paradigm [for example (41), with a 23% drop out rate, compared to 27% dropout rate in our study]. However the study by Glenn and colleagues also found that using a scream as UCS like in our study is efficient, but an electric shock is more aversive leading to a better conditioning effect, which might be the reason for our high dropout rate. A second point which should be discussed is the fact, that our participants were not less sensitive for anxiety symptoms (mean value 16.3) as assessed with the ASI-3 questionnaire, compared to other healthy populations [for example (42): 13.8 in college students but 25.2 and above in different anxiety patient samples]. It might be that this low ASI-3 values also contribute to high dropout rate to missing conditioning. In summary, we do not think that this dropout rate was higher compared to other studies, especially as we had some additional factors leading to addition drop out, like measuring on two consecutive days, and using tDCS, which also lead independently to drop outs.

Although, the current study demonstrated the efficacy of tDCS over left DLPFC on fear memory consolidation, some limitations of this finding must be taken into account. Firstly, methodological limitation of tDCS protocol and the modulation of left DLPFC response to certain stimulation reflect a narrow perspective of functional connectivity and cortical excitability. Secondly, due to limited sample size, the results might be biased with the number of random events. Finally, we also make no claim, whether tDCS influenced the fear association or fear recall.

In conclusion, our findings highlight that the application of cathodal tDCS over left DLPFC disrupts fear memory consolidation. However, anodal stimulation has no enhancing effect on conditioned fear. Moreover, the presently suggested role of the DLPFC in extinction and in turn the negative effects of cathodal tDCS on fear memory may contribute to novel therapeutic approaches in the prevention of the development of pathological memories, i.e., PTSD.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Inge Gröbner for technical assistance. We thank Dr. Priyanka Srivastava for her valuable input in drafting this paper. This study was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG-GRK 1253/2), SFB TRR 58-C4. Also by the STIBET and the University of Wuerzburg in the funding program Open Access Publishing. The funding sources had no role in study design, data collection and analyses, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

1. Indovina I, Robbins TW, Nunez-Elizalde AO, Dunn BD, Bishop SJ. Fear-conditioning mechanisms associated with trait vulnerability to anxiety in humans. Neuron (2011) 69(3):563–71. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.12.034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

2. Parsons RG, Ressler KJ. Implications of memory modulation for post-traumatic stress and fear disorders. Nat Neurosci (2013) 16(2):146–53. doi:10.1038/nn.3296

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

3. Benfenati F. Synaptic plasticity and the neurobiology of learning and memory. Acta Biomed (2007) 78(Suppl 1):58–66.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

4. Dudai Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu Rev Psychol (2004) 55:51–86. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142050

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

5. Pradeep FS, Pradeep BV, Palaniswamy M. Biology of learning and memory genes. J Neurol Neurophysiol (2011) 2(4):1–13.

6. Schafe GE, Nadel NV, Sullivan GM, Harris A, LeDoux JE. Memory consolidation for contextual and auditory fear conditioning is dependent on protein synthesis, PKA, and MAP kinase. Learn Mem (1999) 6(2):97–110.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

7. Cahill L, McGaugh JL. Modulation of memory storage. Curr Opin Neurobiol (1996) 6(2):237–42.

8. McGaugh J, Roozendaal B. Drug enhancement of memory consolidation: historical perspective and neurobiological implications. Psychopharmacology (2009) 202(1-3):3–14. doi:10.1007/s00213-008-1285-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

9. Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL. Memory modulation. Behav Neurosci (2011) 125(6):797–824.

10. Elmer S, Burkard M, Renz B, Meyer M, Jancke L. Direct current induced short-term modulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex while learning auditory presented nouns. Behav Brain Funct (2009) 5:29. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-5-29

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

11. Penolazzi B, Di Domenico A, Marzoli D, Mammarella N, Fairfield B, Franciotti R, et al. Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on episodic memory related to emotional visual stimuli. PLoS One (2010) 5(5): :e10623. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010623

CrossRef Full Text

12. Stone DB, Tesche CD. Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention. Neuroreport (2009) 20(12):1115–9. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832e9aa2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

13. Fregni F, Boggio PS, Nitsche M, Bermpohl F, Antal A, Feredoes E, et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Exp Brain Res (2005) 166(1):23–30. doi:10.1007/s00221-005-2334-6

CrossRef Full Text

14. Marshall L, Molle M, Hallschmid M, Born J. Transcranial direct current stimulation during sleep improves declarative memory. J Neurosci (2004) 24(44):9985–92. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2725-04.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

15. Marshall L, Molle M, Siebner HR, Born J. Bifrontal transcranial direct current stimulation slows reaction time in a working memory task. BMC Neurosci (2005) 6:23. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-6-23

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

16. Jacobson L, Ezra A, Berger U, Lavidor M. Modulating oscillatory brain activity correlates of behavioral inhibition using transcranial direct current stimulation. Neurophysiol Clin (2012) 123(5):979–84. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.016

CrossRef Full Text

17. Balconi M, Vitaloni S. The tDCS effect on alpha brain oscillation for correct vs. incorrect object use. The contribution of the left DLPFC. Neurosci Lett (2012) 517(1):25–9. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2012.04.010

CrossRef Full Text

18. Taylor S, Zvolensky MJ, Cox BJ, Deacon B, Heimberg RG, Ledley DR, et al. Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initial validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychol Assess (2007) 19(2):176–88. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.19.2.176

CrossRef Full Text

19. Bellaiche L, Asthana M, Ehlis A-C, Polak T, Herrmann MJ. The modulation of error processing in the medial frontal cortex by transcranial direct current stimulation. Neurosci J (2013) 2013:10. doi:10.1155/2013/187692

CrossRef Full Text

20. Dawson ME, Schell AM, Filion DL, Berntson GG. The Electrodermal System Handbook of Psychophysiology. New York: Cambridge University Press (2007).

21. McGaugh JL. Memory – a century of consolidation. Science (2000) 287(5451):248–51. doi:10.1126/science.287.5451.248

CrossRef Full Text

22. Fregni F, Pascual-Leone A. Technology insight: noninvasive brain stimulation in neurology-perspectives on the therapeutic potential of rTMS and tDCS. Nat Clin Pract Neurol (2007) 3(7):383–93.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

23. George MS, Aston-Jones G. Noninvasive techniques for probing neurocircuitry and treating illness: vagus nerve stimulation (VNS), transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuropsychopharmacology (2010) 35(1):301–16. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.87

CrossRef Full Text

24. Priori A, Mameli F, Cogiamanian F, Marceglia S, Tiriticco M, Mrakic-Sposta S, et al. Lie-specific involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in deception. Cereb Cortex (2008) 18(2):451–5. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm088

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

25. Arul-Anandam AP, Loo C. Transcranial direct current stimulation: a new tool for the treatment of depression? J Affect Disord (2009) 117(3):137–45. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

26. Veltman DJ, Rombouts SARB, Dolan RJ. Maintenance versus manipulation in verbal working memory revisited: an fMRI study. Neuroimage (2003) 18(2):247–56. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00049-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

27. Zhang JX, Leung H-C, Johnson MK. Frontal activations associated with accessing and evaluating information in working memory: an fMRI study. Neuroimage (2003) 20(3):1531–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

28. Jasper H. Report of committee on methods of clinical exam in EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol (1958) 10:370–5. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(58)90053-1

CrossRef Full Text

29. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol (2000) 527(Pt 3):633–9. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

30. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology (2001) 57(10):1899–901. doi:10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

31. Iyer MB, Mattu U, Grafman J, Lomarev M, Sato S, Wassermann EM. Safety and cognitive effect of frontal DC brain polarization in healthy individuals. Neurology (2005) 64(5):872–5. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000152986.07469.E9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

32. Liebetanz D, Koch R, Mayenfels S, Konig F, Paulus W, Nitsche MA. Safety limits of cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation in rats. Clin Neurophysiol (2009) 120(6):1161–7. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2009.01.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

33. Schiller D, Monfils MH, Raio CM, Johnson DC, Ledoux JE, Phelps EA. Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature (2010) 463(7277):49–53. doi:10.1038/nature08637

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

34. Kindt M, Soeter M. Reconsolidation in a human fear conditioning study: a test of extinction as updating mechanism. Biol Psychol (2013) 92(1):43–50. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.09.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

35. Zaehle T, Sandmann P, Thorne J, Jancke L, Herrmann C. Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory performance: combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. BMC Neurosci (2011) 12(1):2. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-12-2

CrossRef Full Text

36. Marshall L, Helgadottir H, Molle M, Born J. Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature (2006) 444(7119):610–3. doi:10.1038/nature05278

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

37. Baudewig J, Siebner HR, Bestmann S, Tergau F, Tings T, Paulus W, et al. Functional MRI of cortical activations induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Neuroreport (2001) 12(16):3543–8. doi:10.1097/00001756-200111160-00034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

38. Keeser D, Meindl T, Bor J, Palm U, Pogarell O, Mulert C, et al. Prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation changes connectivity of resting-state networks during fMRI. J Neurosci (2011) 31(43):15284–93. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0542-11.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

39. Born J, Rasch B, Gais S. Sleep to remember. Neuroscientist (2006) 12(5):410–24. doi:10.1177/1073858406292647

CrossRef Full Text

40. Lin CH, Yeh SH, Lu HY, Gean PW. The similarities and diversities of signal pathways leading to consolidation of conditioning and consolidation of extinction of fear memory. J Neurosci (2003) 23(23):8310–7.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

41. Glenn CR, Lieberman L, Hajcak G. Comparing electric shock and a fearful screaming face as unconditioned stimuli for fear learning. Int J Psychophysiol (2012) 86(3):214–9. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.09.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

42. Wheaton MG, Deacon BJ, McGrath PB, Berman NC, Abramowitz JS. Dimensions of anxiety sensitivity in the anxiety disorders: evaluation of the ASI-3. J Anxiety Disord (2012) 26(3):401–8. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.01.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

43. Lang N, Siebner HR, Ward NS, Lee L, Nitsche MA, Paulus W, et al. How does transcranial DC stimulation of the primary motor cortex alter regional neuronal activity in the human brain? Eur J Neurosci (2005) 22(2):495–504. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04233.x

CrossRef Full Text

44. Utz KS, Dimova V, Oppenländer K, Kerkhoff G. Electrified minds: transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) and Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation (GVS) as methods of non-invasive brain stimulation in neuropsychology – a review of current data and future implications. Neuropsychologia (2010) 48(10):2789–810. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.002

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: transcranial direct current stimulation, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, fear conditioning, fear memory consolidation

Citation: Asthana M, Nueckel K, Mühlberger A, Neueder D, Polak T, Domschke K, Deckert J and Herrmann MJ (2013) Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on consolidation of fear memory. Front. Psychiatry 4:107. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00107

Received: 24 June 2013; Accepted: 31 August 2013;
Published online: 24 September 2013.

Edited by:

Stefan Borgwardt, University of Basel, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Tarek Rajji, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Canada
Wolnei Caumo, Universidade Federal Do Rio Grande Do Sul, Brazil

Copyright: © 2013 Asthana, Nueckel, Mühlberger, Neueder, Polak, Domschke, Deckert and Herrmann. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Manish Asthana, Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Würzburg, Füchsleinstrasse 15, DE 97080 Würzburg, Germany e-mail: E_Asthana_M@mail.klinik.uni-wuerzburg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.