Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 01 December 2009
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 3 - 2009 | https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.052.2009

Better than expected or as bad as you thought? The neurocognitive development of probabilistic feedback processing

1
Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
2
Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden, The Netherlands
3
Department of Radiology, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, The Netherlands
Learning from feedback lies at the foundation of adaptive behavior. Two prior neuroimaging studies have suggested that there are qualitative differences in how children and adults use feedback by demonstrating that dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and parietal cortex were more active after negative feedback for adults, but after positive feedback for children. In the current study we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to test whether this difference is related to valence or informative value of the feedback by examining neural responses to negative and positive feedback while applying probabilistic rules. In total, 67 healthy volunteers between ages 8 and 22 participated in the study (8–11 years, n = 18; 13–16 years, n = 27; 18–22 years, n = 22). Behavioral comparisons showed that all participants were able to learn probabilistic rules equally well. DLPFC and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex were more active in younger children following positive feedback and in adults following negative feedback, but only when exploring alternative rules, not when applying the most advantageous rules. These findings suggest that developmental differences in neural responses to feedback are not related to valence per se, but that there is an age-related change in processing learning signals with different informative value.

Introduction

Learning to correctly adapt your behavior in a changing environment is an essential feature of human cognition and has been studied extensively over the past decades (for reviews, see Ridderinkhof and van den Wildenberg, 2005 ; Rushworth and Behrens, 2008 ). When adapting behavior, individuals often make use of feedback signals, which can be positive, encouraging the continuation of behavior, or negative, discouraging the continuation of behavior and signaling the need for adjustment. Prior studies have indicated that adaptive learning based on feedback signals undergoes pronounced developmental improvements between late childhood and early adulthood, as is evident from tasks in which participants need to switch between multiple rules (Crone and van der Molen, 2004 ; Somsen, 2007 ) or in which they need to infer sorting rules based on positive and negative signals (van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2008 ).
Early developmental improvements in adaptive behavior are observed when feedback has a direct mapping to deterministic rules (Somsen, 2007 ), however, when the feedback is probabilistic, changes in adaptive learning are observed until late adolescence (Hooper et al., 2004 ). In these situations, individuals must learn the statistical regularities between actions and outcomes, and use that information to interpret current feedback signals (see also Rangel et al., 2008 ). Feedback which is not directly mapped to behavior is often more complex because it requires individuals to attend to long term consequences and override the tendency to respond directly to local environmental change.
Neuroimaging studies have shown that regions previously associated with cognitive control and response selection (Miller and Cohen, 2001 ; Toni et al., 2002 ) are also active when adults receive negative performance feedback, including the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (Klein et al., 2007 ; Taylor et al., 2007 ). The dACC is thought to monitor action outcome regularities and is important for signaling adjustment (Botvinick et al., 2001 ; Yeung et al., 2004 ). In addition, the dACC may exercise behavioral control via the engagement of the DLPFC (Kerns et al., 2004 ; Zanolie et al., 2008 ), which in turn is important for trial-to-trial adjustments of behavior (Dosenbach et al., 2008 ). Similar to the DLPFC, the parietal cortex is also involved in feedback processing, in particular negative feedback (Crone et al., 2008 ; van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2008 ). Finally, these regions are thought to work in close concert with the basal ganglia, specifically the caudate nucleus, which is thought to be engaged when learning action- outcome regularities (for a review see Cools, 2008 ).
In two prior developmental studies we have identified the developmental time course of these regions during adaptive feedback processing. In the first study (Crone et al., 2008 ), participants were instructed to infer rules based on positive and negative feedback which could change without warning. Following Somsen (2007) , we were interested in the way children, adolescents, and adults processed negative feedback indicating a rule shift. As anticipated, adults engaged DLPFC, dACC, and the parietal cortex when processing negative feedback indicating a rule shift. A similar pattern was observed in 14- to 15-year-old adolescents, but 8- to 11-year-old children engaged these regions less following negative feedback in comparison to positive feedback or a low-level fixation baseline. In the second study (van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2008 ), participants were instructed to guess a correct rule. Because there were two possible rules, there was a 50% chances of receiving positive feedback, and therefore both feedback signals (negative and positive) were similarly salient and probable. Again, adults engaged DLPFC, dACC, and the parietal cortex following negative feedback, but in this study 8-year-old children engaged DLPFC and the parietal cortex more following positive feedback relative to negative feedback. The developmental trajectory of the dACC followed a different pattern, as it slowly emerged in response to negative feedback at the age of 12, but it was not more active following negative compared to positive feedback at a younger age (see also Velanova et al., 2008 ). Although the caudate nucleus was involved in these tasks, these studies revealed that there were no developmental differences in activation patterns.
Together, these findings indicate that the possible meaning of positive and negative feedback signals, and the role of the associated neural circuits, changes during development. However, prior studies could not dissociate between neural activation as a result of valence versus informative value, given that negative feedback always signaled response adjustment and therefore had different informative value than positive feedback. Thus, it remains to be determined how the involvement of DLPFC and the parietal cortex is dependent on valence versus informative value of the feedback.
Prior research suggests that differences in positive and negative feedback adjustment are the result of differences in attention regulation (Somsen, 2007 ). Following this hypothesis, it is argued that children are less able to update the relevant feedback information and therefore they are less flexible in selecting alternative actions. We therefore reasoned that the brain regions implicated in prior feedback studies may be sensitive to the informative value of feedback, and that activation in these brain regions is indicative of feedback attendance. Furthermore, we predicted that attention to feedback may also underlie the developmental differences in brain activation. We hypothesized that DLPFC and parietal cortex would be more active following positive feedback in children and following negative feedback in adults, but only when the feedback has informative value for learning and response adjustment. Thus, we sought to test how neural responses are sensitive to informative value for learning versus valence of feedback, and the developmental trajectory of feedback processing.
We reasoned that feedback valence versus informative value could be disentangled after participants learned probabilistic feedback rules. In the probabilistic learning paradigm, participants need to learn from positive and negative feedback under different levels of probability, and therefore not all positive feedback signals response continuation and not all negative feedback signals response adjustment. The probabilistic learning (i.e., trial-and-error) task employed in this study was based on a prior study by Frank et al. (2004) , but was simplified for use with children. In our version of the probabilistic learning task, two different stimulus pairs (AB or CD) were presented in random order, and participants had to learn over trials that one stimulus was more likely to result in positive feedback (70–80%) (see Figure 1 ). Over the course of the experiment participants had to learn the statistical regularities and thus had to learn to choose the stimuli with a high probability of positive feedback (A and C) more often than those with a low probability of positive feedback (B and D).
Figure 1. (A) At the beginning of each trial a centrally located cue was presented with a jittered interval between 500 and 6000 ms, followed by a combined presentation of a stimulus pair and a response window of max. 2500 ms, after which feedback was presented for 1000 ms. After the feedback a short filler was presented, in the form of a blank screen, in order to compensate for different reaction times between trials and between participants (filler duration = 2500 ms – reaction time). (B) Average accuracy on AB and CD trials per age group.
When participants have gained knowledge of the statistical regularities, they were expected to more often apply the correct rule. Notably, in probabilistic learning tasks individuals generally do not consistently apply the correct rule but show matching behavior; i.e., they choose the correct stimulus with a frequency that is proportional to the probability of positive feedback associated with that stimulus (Estes, 1961 ; Herrnstein, 1961 ; Shanks et al., 2002 ; Frank and Kong, 2008 ). Thus, we anticipated that participants would apply the correct rule (in this study, choosing the high probability stimuli A and C) more often, but we also anticipated that they would remain exploring the alternative rule (choosing the low probability stimuli B and D). Therefore, this paradigm allowed us to investigate the processing of positive and negative feedback that carries different informative value. In particular, receiving negative feedback when choosing the correct rule should not be interpreted as a signal to switch to the alternative rule because the probability of positive feedback remains higher than for the alternative rule. In contrast, receiving negative feedback when choosing the alternative rule should lead to a switch to the correct rule. To be able to address the question how neural responses are sensitive to feedback signals in the context of learned rules, we only analyzed neural responses after participants had reached a learning plateau.
Based on prior studies, we expected that DLPFC and the parietal cortex would be sensitive to whether feedback signals required greater attention, and would contain greater informative value for performance adjustment on subsequent trials. Therefore, we expected that these regions would be engaged mostly after choosing the alternative rule (B or D), because this feedback contained learning signals for performance adjustment, independent of valence. We also examined the role of the dACC and the caudate as these regions have previously been implicated in feedback processing (Schultz, 2007 ; Cools, 2008 ; Rushworth and Behrens, 2008 ). We expected that the dACC would be most sensitive to negative feedback signals, particularly when indicating the need for behavioral adjustment (Kerns et al., 2004 ), whereas we expected that the caudate would be most sensitive to positive feedback which signals response continuation (Cools, 2008 ).
The second question concerned developmental differences in performance and neural activation. In prior research, developmental differences were observed between childhood and mid-adolescence, but differences between adolescence and adulthood remain unclear (Crone et al., 2008 ; van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2008 ). For this purpose, we compared behavioral and neural responses of three age groups; children (8–11 years), adolescents (13–16 years), and adults (18–22 years). Behaviorally, we predicted that differences in adaptive learning would be largest between childhood and adolescence, with refinement of learning between adolescence and adulthood (Luna and Sweeney, 2001 ; Crone and van der Molen, 2004 ; Somsen, 2007 ). In addition, we expected to find that these behavioral changes would be paralleled by changes in the areas involved in adaptive control (dACC, DLPFC, parietal cortex and caudate nucleus). For the fMRI analyses, we had three specific age-related hypotheses based on prior studies. First, we expected an increase in differentiation in the dACC for positive and negative feedback processing with increasing age (van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2008 ; Velanova et al., 2008 ). Second, we expected an attention-based shift in recruitment of DLPFC and the parietal cortex from positive to negative performance feedback with age. Third, we expected age differences in how learned probabilities would be associated with neural changes in feedback processing; in particular we predicted that feedback after exploring the alternative rule would be associated with developmental differences. Because of the children’s putative focus on positive feedback, we expected that with increasing age there would be a decrease in activity related to processing positive feedback and an increase in activity related to processing negative feedback following selection of the alternative rule.
Finally, our paradigm allowed us to investigate age differences in adaptive behavior, that is, whether participants stay or shift on subsequent trials based on the received feedback. Besides behavioral analyses of sequential effects, we also employed exploratory sequential condition analyses to further understand the relation between neural activation and subsequent adjustment of behavior (see also Kerns et al., 2004 ).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixty-seven healthy right-handed paid volunteers (35 female, 32 male; ages 8–22 participated in the fMRI experiment. Age groups were based on adolescent development stage, resulting in three age groups: children (8- to 11-year-olds, n = 18; 9 female), mid-adolescents (13- to 16-year-olds, n = 27; 13 female) and young adults (18- to 22-year-olds, n = 22; 13 female). A chi square analysis indicated that the gender distribution was similar across age groups, χ2(2) = 0.79, p = 0.67. All participants reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision and participants or their caregivers indicated an absence of neurological or psychiatric impairments. Participants and their caregivers (for minors) gave informed consent for the study and all procedures were approved by the medical ethical committee of the Leiden University Medical Center. In accordance with Leiden University Medical Center policy, all anatomical scans were reviewed and cleared by the radiology department following each scan. No anomalous findings were reported.

Behavioral Assessment

Parents filled out the Child Behavior Check List (CBCL, Achenbach, 1991 ) for participants younger than 18 years, in order to screen for psychiatric conditions. All participants scored below clinical levels on all subscales of the CBCL, and had scores within 1 SD of the mean of a normative standardized sample.
Participants completed two subscales (similarities and block design) of either the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) or the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) in order to obtain an estimate of their intelligence quotient (Wechsler, 1991 , 1997 ). There were no significant differences in estimated IQ scores between the different age groups, F(2, 66) = 1.63, p = 0.20 (see Table 1 ).
yes

Task Procedure

Probabilistic learning task

The procedure for the probabilistic learning task (Frank et al., 2004 ) was as follows: The task consisted of two stimulus pairs (called AB and CD). The stimulus pairs consisted of pictures of everyday objects (e.g., a chair and a clock). Each trial started with the display of one of the two stimulus pairs and subsequently the participant had to choose one of the two stimuli (e.g., A or B), which were presented on the left or the right side of the screen. The stimulus pairs were presented in random order. Participants were instructed to choose either the left or the right stimulus by pressing a button with the index or middle finger of the right hand within a 2500 ms window, which was followed by a 1000 ms feedback display. The feedback display consisted of a green V-signal for positive feedback and a red cross for negative feedback. If no response was given within 2500 ms, the text “too slow” was presented on the screen. This occurred on less than 2% of the trials.
The feedback displayed was probabilistic. Choosing stimulus A led to positive feedback on 80% of AB trials, whereas choosing stimulus B led to positive feedback on 20% of these trials. The CD pair procedure was similar, but probability for positive feedback was lower; choosing stimulus C led to positive feedback on 70% of CD trials, whereas choosing stimulus D led to positive feedback on 30% in these trials. Thus, the correct choice in order to obtain most positive feedback was A or C, whereas the incorrect choice was B or D.
Participants were instructed to earn as many points as possible (as indicated by receiving a positive feedback signal), but were also informed that it would not be possible to receive positive feedback on every trial. Further, participants were informed that although stimuli sometimes appeared on the right side and sometimes on the left side, that laterality was an irrelevant dimension. After the instructions and right before the scanning session, the participants played 40 practice rounds on a computer in a quiet laboratory to ensure proficiency on the task.
In total, the task in the scanner consisted of two blocks of 100 trials each: 50 AB trials and 50 CD trials per block. To ensure that participants had to learn a new mapping in both task blocks, the first and the second block consisted of different sets of pictures. The duration of each block was approximately 8.5 min. The stimuli were presented in pseudo-random order with a jittered interstimulus interval (min = 1000 ms, max = 6000 ms) optimized with OptSeq2 (surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/optseq/, Dale, 1999 ). During inter trial intervals, a central fixation cross was shown.

Data Acquisition

Participants were familiarized with the scanner environment on the day of the fMRI session through the use of a mock scanner, which simulated the sounds and environment of a real MRI scanner. Data were acquired using a 3.0T Philips Achieva scanner at the Leiden University Medical Center. Stimuli were projected onto a screen located at the head of the scanner bore and viewed by participants by means of a mirror mounted to the head coil assembly. First, a localizer scan was obtained for each participant. Subsequently, T2*-weighted Echo-Planar Images (EPI) (TR = 2.2 s, TE = 30 ms, 80 × 80 matrix, FOV = 220, 35 2.75 mm transverse slices with 0.28 mm gap) were obtained during two functional runs of 232 volumes each. The first two scans were discarded to allow for equilibration of T1 saturation effects. A high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical scan and a high-resolution T2-weighted matched-bandwidth high-resolution anatomical scan, with the same slice prescription as the EPIs, were obtained from each participant after the functional runs. Stimulus presentation and the timing of all stimuli and response events were acquired using E-Prime software. Head motion was restricted by using pillow and foam inserts that surrounded the head.

fMRI Data Analysis

Data were preprocessed using SPM5 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London). The functional time series were realigned to compensate for small head movements. Translational movement parameters never exceeded 1 voxel (<3 mm) in any direction for any subject or scan. There were no significant differences in movement parameters between age groups F(2, 65) = 0.152, p = 0.85, (see Table 1 ). Functional volumes were spatially smoothed using a 6 mm full-width half-maximum Gaussian kernel. Functional volumes were spatially normalized to EPI templates. The normalization algorithm used a 12 parameter affine transformation together with a nonlinear transformation involving cosine basis functions and resampled the volumes to 3 mm cubic voxels. The MNI305 template was used for visualization and all results are reported in the MNI305 stereotaxic space (Cosoco et al., 1997 ), an approximation of Talairach space (Talairach and Tourneaux, 1988 ).
Statistical analyses were performed on individual participants’ data using the general linear model in SPM5. The fMRI time series data were modeled by a series of events convolved with a canonical haemodynamic response function (HRF). The presentation of the feedback screen was modeled as 0-duration events. The stimuli and responses were not modeled separately as these occurred in one prior or overlapping EPI images as feedback presentation.
In the model, feedback was further subdivided into correct vs. alternative rule and positive vs. negative feedback. These trial functions were used as covariates in a general linear model, along with a basic set of cosine functions that high-pass filtered the data, and a covariate for run effects. The least-squares parameter estimates of height of the best-fitting canonical HRF for each condition were used in pair-wise contrasts. The resulting contrast images, computed on a participant-by-participant basis, were submitted to group analyses. At the group level, contrasts between conditions were computed by performing one-tailed t-tests on these images, treating participants as a random effect. We further performed voxelwise ANOVAs to identify regions that showed age-related differences in relation to feedback processing. We tested for linear increases (−1 0 1) and decreases (1 0 −1) in the contrasts specified below.
We applied AlphaSim (Ward, 2000 ) to calculate the appropriate threshold significance level and cluster size for the whole-brain analyses. A significance threshold of p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons was calculated by performing 10.000 Monte Carlo simulations in AlphaSim resulting in an uncorrected threshold of p < 0.001, requiring a minimum of 24 voxels in a cluster. This threshold was used for all whole-brain analyses.

Region of Interest Analyses

We used the Marsbar toolbox for use with SPM5 (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net , Brett et al., 2002 ) to perform Region of Interest (ROI) analyses to further characterize patterns of activation. We created ROIs of the regions that were identified in the functional mask of whole-brain analyses. The masks used to generate functional ROIs was based on the general (positive vs. negative feedback) contrasts (p < 0.001, > 24 voxels) across all participants, which was unbiased for effects of probability rule or age. Because this statistical image spanned several distinct functional brain regions in the striatum, we used Marsbar anatomical masks for the caudate nucleus to further specify our ROIs.
For all ROI analyses, effects were considered significant at an α of 0.0125, based on Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, p = 0.05/4 ROIs (caudate, DLPFC, parietal cortex and dACC), unless reported otherwise.

Results

Behavior

Performance

To investigate the age differences in learning performance for the different stimulus pairs we calculated the percentage of correct choices (choosing the high probability stimulus) per block of 20 trials for each participant, resulting in five blocks in total. Because the two runs in the scanner consisted of new stimulus pairs, the two runs were collapsed.
As expected, the age (8–11 years, 13–16 years, 18–22 years) × probability (AB, CD) × task block (5) ANOVA showed that participants learned to make more correct choices over time, as indicated by a main effect of task block, F(4, 260) = 40.44, p < 0.001, yes (See Figure 1 B). There was a significant difference in accuracy between the two probabilities; participants were more accurate on the AB (80%–20%) trials than the CD (70%–30%) trials, F(1, 65) = 11.58, p < 0.001, yes Contrary to predictions, there were no age differences in learning (age × task block interaction, F(8, 260) = 1.38, p = 0.11, yes yes), no age differences in accuracy on the two pairs (age × probability interaction, F(2, 65) = 0.941, p = 0.393, yes), and no age × probability × task block interaction (p > 0.10). A similar ANOVA for reaction times revealed no differences for age, probability, or task block (all p’s > 0.10) (see Table 1 ).
The task block factor allowed us to obtain the point in learning where participants reached a plateau. By selecting the task phase in which there were no longer differences in learning, we could examine how feedback was processed in the context of applying the correct (choosing the stimuli with a high probability of positive feedback) or alternative rule (choosing the stimuli with a low probability of positive feedback). Follow up comparisons showed that the last 60 trials were appropriate for this purpose, as performance stabilized and participants showed probability matching behavior (Shanks et al., 2002 ). That is, both the AB and the CD pairs showed no effects of block (learning) on accuracy in the last three blocks, F(2, 130) = 3.47, p = 0.08 and F(2, 130) = 1.81, p = 0.52, respectively. When we reanalyzed these last 60 trials, we still found a significant effect of stimulus pair, F(1, 65) = 16.51, p < 0.001, yes, and again no significant interactions with age (all p’s > 0.3).
To summarize, the behavioral results showed that all participants learned to perform more accurately over time and they learned faster on the easier AB trials than the more difficult CD trials. Performance stabilized in the last 60 trials, at which point participants showed probability matching behavior (Shanks et al., 2002 ).
The fMRI analyses focused on the last 60 trials. In order to have enough trial numbers in each condition, we collapsed across probabilities in the analyses below. Thus, we differentiated between over-learned high probabilities (A and C collapsed) and alternative low probabilities (B and D trials collapsed). These will be referred to as the correct and alternative rules. Each of these rules could result in positive and negative feedback.

fMRI Results Positive Versus Negative Feedback

Whole-brain comparisons across age groups

First, we identified the neural correlates of feedback processing by comparing the (positive feedback vs. negative feedback) contrast across all participants. This analysis revealed increased BOLD responses for positive feedback > negative feedback in several regions including the left and right caudate, left DLPFC and left parietal cortex (see Figure 2 A). The opposite contrast (negative > positive feedback) resulted in increased activation in the dACC. The coordinates for these comparisons (positive feedback vs. negative feedback) are reported in Table 2 .
Figure 2. (A) Regions from the (positive vs. negative feedback) contrasts across all participants (B) Parameter estimates and standard errors for positive and negative feedback that followed either the correct or the alternative rule displayed for each age group in left DLPFC, left parietal cortex, dACC and left caudate. Significant differences between brain activity in two conditions are indicated with an asterisk (*Bonferroni corrected).
yes

fMRI Region of Interest Results for Feedback × Rule × Age Group Interactions

Next, we tested for age differences and rule sensitivity in these regions by performing region of interest (ROI) analyses. The ROI analyses were restricted to the four a priori defined regions which emerged in the (positive vs. negative) contrast across participants: bilateral caudate, left DLPFC, left parietal cortex and dACC. In order to investigate whether there were age differences in how the statistical regularities learned by the participants had an effect on how feedback was processed we performed 3 × 2 × 2 ANOVAs testing for the interaction between valence (positive vs. negative) and rule (correct vs. alternative) as within-subjects factors and age (children, adolescents, adults) as the between-subjects factor for each ROI (see Figure 2 B).

Left DLPFC

The (age group × valence × rule) ANOVA for left DLPFC resulted in an interaction between valence and rule, F(2, 64) = 6.32, p < 0.01, showing that left DLPFC was more active for both negative and positive feedback after choosing the alternative rule compared to the correct rule, but this difference was larger for positive than negative feedback. In addition, there was an interaction between rule (AC vs BD) and age group, F(2, 64) = 3.87, p = 0.02, and a three-way interaction between rule, valence, and age group, F(2, 64) = 6.77, p < 0.01.
As can be seen in Figure 2 B, children and adolescents showed more activity for positive feedback after choosing the alternative rule compared to the correct rule (t(17) = 2.64, p < 0.01 and t(26) = 3.18, p < 0.004, respectively), whereas this difference was not present in adults. In addition, adults and adolescents showed more activity for negative feedback after choosing the alternative rule compared to the correct rule, (t(21) = −2.49, p = 0.02 and t(23) = −2.81, p < 0.01 respectively), but this difference was not present in children.

Left parietal cortex

The (age group × valence × rule) ANOVA for the left parietal cortex revealed a similar three-way interaction which approached significance, F(2, 64) = 3.16, p = 0.05 (see Figure 2 B). Although the pattern of activation for the different conditions in the left parietal cortex appears similar to the pattern for left DLFPC, it did not survive Bonferroni correction and none of the post hoc comparisons resulted in significant effects.

dACC

The (age group × valence × rule) ANOVA for the dACC resulted in a rule × valence interaction, F(2, 64) = 14.14, p < 0.001, an age × valence interaction, F(2, 64) = 4.11, p < 0.01, and an age × rule interaction, F(2, 64) = 4.81, p = 0.03, but the three-way interaction failed to reach significance F(2, 64) = 0.28, p = 0.75.
As can be seen in Figure 2 B, adults showed more activation in dACC after negative feedback than after positive feedback, F(1, 21) = 8.25, p < 0.01, but this was not found for the younger age groups. Children and adolescence, in contrast, showed more dACC activation after positive feedback for the alternative rule relative to the correct rule (t(17) = 2.51, p < 0.01 and t(26) = 3.44, p < 0.01 respectively). In addition, adults and adolescents showed more activity for negative feedback after choosing the alternative rule compared to the correct rule, (t(21) = −2.89, p < 0.01 and t(26) = −3.32, p < 0.003 respectively), but this difference was not present in children.

Left and right caudate

Finally, we performed an (age group × valence × rule) ANOVA for the left caudate nucleus. This analyses did not reveal any age effects, but a main effect for feedback, F(1, 64) = 33.17, p < 0.001, and a feedback × rule interaction F(2, 64) = 17.21, p < 0.01. All age groups showed more activity for the alternative (low probability) compared to the correct rule (high probability) positive feedback (all p’s < 0.001), but there were no additional main or interaction effects (Figure 2 B). Similar analyses for right caudate yielded the same results; a main effect of feedback, F(1, 64) = 28.16, p < 0.005, and a feedback × rule interaction F(2, 64) = 19.33, p < 0.01.

Win Stay – Lose Shift Strategies: Behavior and Brain Analyses

Finally, to further investigate differences in feedback processing we explored developmental changes in decision-making strategies on the behavioral and neural level. In order to investigate the strategy used on the task we examined how often participants chose either the same stimulus after positive feedback (win-stay) or the other stimulus after negative feedback (lose-shift). For this set of analyses we further broke down the trials based on the subsequent choice when presented with the same stimulus pair; win-stay, win-shift, lose-stay and lose-shift. The factor ‘win-stay’ was computed by calculating the proportion of choice repetitions following positive feedback as a function of the total number of positive feedback events. Likewise, the factor ‘lose-shift’ was computed by calculating the proportion of choice shifts following negative feedback as a function of the total number of negative feedback events. Because previous analyses revealed that positive and negative feedback were processed differently dependent on rule type we analyzed the sequential effects for the correct and alternative rule separately.

Behavior

For correct rules, the univariate ANOVAs with age group as the between-subjects factor revealed a significant age difference in lose-shift strategies, F(2, 64) = 4.04, p < 0.02 as well as in win-stay strategies, F(2, 64) = 4.51, p < 0.02 (see Figure 3 A). These results illustrate that adults showed more optimizing behavior than adolescents and children; they stayed more often with the correct rule after positive feedback and shifted less often after negative feedback.
Figure 3. (A) Percentages of win-stay and lose-shift choices perage group and rule type, error bars represent standard error. (B) Parameter estimates and standard errors for positive and negative feedback that followed by either staying or shifting, displayed for each age group and rule type separately. Significant differences between brain activity in two conditions are indicated with an asterisk (*Bonferroni corrected).
For the alternative rules, the univariate ANOVAs revealed no age differences for win-stay strategies, F(2, 64) = 0.85, p = 0.43, but there was a significant age difference in lose-shift strategies, F(2, 64) = 3.91, p < 0.03. In the latter case, children showed less optimal behavior compared to the adolescents and adults; surprisingly, they stayed more often with the alternative (incorrect) rule after negative feedback.

ROI analyses

In order to explore the relation between brain activity and behavior on the subsequent trial, we compared brain activity after positive and negative feedback that resulted in staying or shifting for the two rule types separately. We explored the same ROIs as reported above. These analyses revealed significant shift and age effects only in the dACC and left DLPFC, but not in the caudate or the parietal cortex. In general, the ANOVAs showed that in adults, dACC and DLPFC were more active when participants shifted on the next trial. There were some differences in significance levels, but overall this effect seemed generally independent of feedback valence or rule. The analyses are described in more detail below.
The dACC showed the strongest relation between brain activity and subsequent behavioral change. When applying the correct rule, the shift × age group ANOVA for positive feedback revealed a main effect of shifting, F(1, 65) = 6.27, p < 0.01 but no interaction with age, F(2, 64) = 2.29, p = 0.11 (see Figure 3 B). There was more dACC activity when shifting after positive feedback. The same ANOVA for negative feedback revealed an age × shift interaction, F(2, 64) = 3.62, p = 0.03. Post hoc comparisons revealed that there was more dACC activity when shifting compared to staying after negative feedback for adults (t(21) = −2.76, p < 0.01) but not for the adolescents and children (both p’s > 0.1).
When applying the alternative rule, the shift × age group ANOVA for positive feedback revealed no significant effects of age or shifting. However, the same ANOVA for negative feedback revealed an age × shift interaction (F(2, 63) = 5.31, p < 0.01). Post hoc comparisons revealed that there was more dACC activity when shifting after negative feedback for adults (t(21) = −3.01, p < 0.01) but not for adolescents and children (both p’s > 0.2).
Finally, the pattern of activation in the left DLPFC appeared similar to that of the dACC (Figure S2 in Supplementary Material). The shift × age ANOVAs for the correct rule resulted in significant shift × age interactions for both positive and negative feedback (F(2, 63) = 4.46, p = 0.03 and F(2, 64) = 4.91, p = 0.02, respectively). Post hoc test revealed that there was more left DLPFC activity when shifting on the next trial after positive and negative feedback, but this was only significant for the adults (t(21) = −2.54, p < 0.01 and t(21) = −2.32, p = 0.03, respectively). There were no significant effects for the alternative rule (all p’s > 0.2).

Discussion

The goal of this study was to examine the neural developmental changes when processing positive and negative feedback signals in a probabilistic decision-making task. As predicted, all participants learned to choose the correct rules (high probability stimuli A and C) more often than the alternative rules (low probability stimuli B and D) (Frank et al., 2004 ; Klein et al., 2007 ). After approximately 40 trials, participants adapted a performance pattern consistent with ‘probability matching behavior’, and this behavioral phase was the focus of our further analyses.
Behavioral analyses showed two important patterns: (1) probability matching behavior occurred in all age groups, but there were no age differences in overall learning rate, and (2) task adaptive win-stay, lose-shift strategies were observed, but age differences in adaptive behavior indicated more task-adaptive optimizing behavior in adults. These task and age differences in decision-making strategy were paralleled by changes in functional brain activity; (1) neural responses in DLPFC, dACC, and caudate were sensitive to rule × feedback interactions and an age-related difference was observed in DLPFC and dACC, and (2) activity in DLPFC and dACC predicted behavioral change on subsequent trials more strongly in adults than in adolescents and children. These behavioral data and their neural correlates provide important new insights in feedback processing in general and across development. The discussion will be organized according to these themes.

Feedback processing in adults

Our analysis of positive and negative feedback processing in a probabilistic environment demonstrated that feedback-related activity in the DLPFC, dACC and caudate was dependent on valence and information value. We started out with a general whole-brain comparison for positive versus negative feedback and used ROI analyses to explore the areas identified in this contrast. This analysis revealed that especially left DLPFC, dACC and bilateral caudate were sensitive to feedback × rule context interactions. Before interpreting age differences in these activation patterns, we start out with the interpretation of feedback sensitivity observed in adults, which will set the stage for interpreting the developmental effects.
When exploring the data for adults separately, the results showed increased recruitment of DLPFC after receiving negative feedback following the alternative compared to the correct rule. Given that negative feedback after choosing the alternative, but not the correct, rule indicates the need for a switch in behavior, the adult findings are consistent with previous studies demonstrating negative feedback-related sensitivity in DLPFC for feedback that is important for subsequent behavioral adjustment (Kerns, 2006 ; van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2008 ; Zanolie et al., 2008 ) and not for negative feedback per se.
Besides DLPFC, the parietal cortex has previously been implicated in feedback processing (Crone et al., 2008 , van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2008 ) and implementing cognitive control as part of the fronto-parietal network (Brass et al., 2005 ; Bunge et al., 2002 ; Dosenbach et al., 2008 ). In support of this hypothesis our whole-brain analyses revealed that the left superior parietal cortex was involved in feedback processing. However, in contrast with previous studies (van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2008 ), our subsequent post hoc analyses could not confirm a strong contribution of the superior parietal cortex. Possibly, the parietal cortex was more engaged in prior studies because these involved trial-to-trial learning, whereas in the current study we investigated feedback processing when rules were already learned. Future research is necessary to elucidate the role of the superior parietal cortex in feedback processing in relation to learning.
The analyses of dACC revealed a very similar activation pattern as DLPFC, however the dACC activation pattern in adults was more supportive of a general increase in activity after negative feedback regardless of rule type. Possibly, this finding indicates that, at least in adults, the dACC has a more general role in processing negative feedback; both in terms of detecting general conflict (Brown and Braver, 2005 ) and signaling the need for behavior change (Holroyd and Coles, 2008 ; Rushworth, 2008 ).
Finally, the caudate nucleus also showed sensitivity to feedback and rule type, but this region was more active after positive compared to negative feedback when participants chose the alternative rule. Given that this effect was specific for positive feedback, and that the probability for positive feedback for the alternative rule was low, the signal in the caudate could reflect a positive prediction error; i.e., signaling that the outcome is better than predicted (for review see Schultz, 2007 ).
Together, analysis of the adult activation pattern confirms prior findings showing that DLPFC and dACC are sensitive to negative feedback and the caudate is sensitive to positive feedback, but the findings further elucidate that these neural responses are dependent on the extent to which these feedback signals provide a learning signal of future performance. That is, DLPFC and caudate responses were more pronounced after selecting the incorrect rule which had a low probability of resulting in positive feedback, but which may have been important to explore. In contrast, when applying over-learned high probability rules, DLPFC and caudate were less involved, possibly because the informative value was smaller.

Feedback Processing: Developmental Comparisons

The neural activation patterns described above were differentially sensitive to age modulations. The first notable finding is that of differential activation patterns in the DLPFC. All participants, regardless of age, showed increased recruitment of DLPFC when choosing the alternative rule compared to the correct rule. However, children, but not adults, showed more activation in DLPFC after positive feedback when choosing the alternative rule. In contrast, adults, but not children, showed more activation in DLPFC after negative feedback when choosing the alternative rule. Adolescents seemed to be in a transition phase, because their neural response to positive feedback was similar to that observed in children, but their neural response to negative feedback was similar to that observed in adults. Thus, consistent with prior studies, these developmental differences indicate a shift from focus on positive to a focus on negative feedback with age (Somsen, 2007 ; Crone et al., 2008 ; van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2008 ), which appears to continue across adolescence. In addition, the current results extend previous findings by showing that developmental differences in neural responses to feedback are not related to valence per se, but suggest an age-related change in processing learning signals with different informative value.
In contrast, for all age groups the caudate nucleus was more active for positive compared to negative feedback, in particular when participants chose the alternative rule. This finding indicates that part of the feedback processing network, which is implicated in processing statistical regularities of reward (Schultz, 2007 ) matures already at an early age, whereas the part of the network that is involved in processing negative feedback and the subsequent control of behavior has a more protracted developmental time course. These findings are consistent with prior reports using cognitive tasks, as these studies have also reported early maturation of subcortical regions and protracted development of cortical brain areas (Casey et al., 2004 ; van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2008 ; Velanova et al., 2008 ). It should be noted that other developmental studies have reported increased sensitivity of the striatum in early adolescence, however, these studies have employed paradigms with a more affective content, such as gambling tasks with real monetary rewards or emotion recognition (Ernst et al., 2005 ; Galvan et al., 2006 ; McClure-Tone et al., 2008 ; van Leijenhorst et al., 2009 ). In future studies, it will be of interest to examine whether the caudate activation can be modulated by the use of affective task modulations when learning rules or processing performance feedback.

Adaptive Behavior and Brain Activation Across Development

One of the challenging questions for future studies is how the neural activation is associated with trial-to-trial learning. For example, we did not observe age differences in general learning performance, despite differences in neural activation. This was unexpected, and again demonstrates that differences in neural activation can be present without differences in observable behavior (Ladouceur et al., 2004 ). However, consistent with prior studies, the sequential analyses revealed that with age, participants became better at using the negative feedback signals to adjust their behavior on subsequent trials (Crone and van der Molen, 2004 ). As expected, when receiving positive feedback after having applied the correct rule, participants were more likely to stay and select the same stimulus on the subsequent trial. Likewise, when receiving negative feedback after having applied the incorrect alternative rule, participants were more likely to shift and select the correct stimulus on the subsequent trial. Overall, adults appeared better at optimizing than adolescents, and adolescents performed better than children. Based on these findings, in combination with the developmental differences in neural activation, the data are supportive of a linear increase across adolescence. Although these findings differ from earlier reports which have showed larger differences in early adolescence than in later adolescence (e.g. Ladouceur et al. 2004 ) the findings are consistent with prior fMRI results showing late changes in brain activation and behavior (e.g. Scherf et al., 2006 ; van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2008 ).
Intriguingly, even though children were more likely than adults to shift after receiving negative feedback when applying the correct rule, they were also more likely to stay after receiving negative feedback when applying the incorrect alternative rule. The reason for this behavioral pattern is still unclear, but it is possible that children waited with shifting when applying the incorrect alternative rule until they received positive feedback (20%). Future research should use task manipulations that allow for further investigation of this hypothesis.
We performed exploratory analyses to investigate the relation between brain activity and win-stay, lose-shift behavior, although it should be noted that these analyses are preliminary as our study design was not optimized to test for these differences. The analyses on the ROIs identified in the main analyses revealed that, consistent with prior research, dACC and left DLPFC activity predicted behavioral adjustment on the subsequent trial in adults (Kerns et al., 2004 ; Jocham et al., 2009 ). However, this pattern was observed for both rule types and appeared independent of feedback valence. Possibly, the dACC and left DLPFC were important for trial-by-trial adjustment (Kerns et al., 2004 ). We found a similar pattern in adolescents, but only when applying the correct rule. We failed to find similar relations in children, which may indicate that the neural mechanisms that facilitate future behavioral adjustment are still immature or that they employed different strategies to perform the task. These interpretations are consistent with an ERP study showing increased error related negativity across adolescence (Ladouceur et al., 2007 ). Furthermore, the same study showed that only in adults the ERN amplitude was related to task performance.
The current study is limited by the relatively small number of trials for some of the contrasts examining the neural correlates of shifting behavior. Future studies should make use of tasks that are optimized for studying these developmental differences in more detail.
In addition, a challenging direction for future research will be to investigate the developmental differences in the learning phase. The combined use of computational reinforcement learning models (Klein et al., 2007 ) with imaging techniques could be a promising endeavor to parse out the developmental changes in different phases of learning (e.g. learning rate) and their neural correlates. These methods could be combined with trial-to-trial data categorization to understand how the observed developmental change in sensitivity from positive to negative feedback hinders or facilitates learning locally versus oriented towards future goals.

Conclusion

Taken together, the current findings confirm that DLPFC, dACC and caudate are important for probabilistic feedback processing, and show that they have dissociable roles as reflected in differential sensitivity to feedback valence and rule types. The DLPFC and dACC were sensitive to information value in response to negative feedback, but the caudate was sensitive to information value in response to positive feedback. These findings are consistent with previously suggested computational models of feedback learning (Cohen, 2008 ; Frank and Kong, 2008 ).
The results of this study replicate the previously reported developmental shift in sensitivity from positive to negative feedback as reflected in neural activation in the DLPFC, with a transition phase in adolescence. Using probabilistic feedback stimuli, we could dissociate between two competing hypotheses with respect to this developmental change. The results confirm the hypothesis that this shift is associated with different attention focus on learning signals and disconfirm the hypothesis that this shift reflects a simple valence effect. Further understanding of the age related changes in strategy differences, and how to influence decision-making strategies by guiding attention regulation, promise to be useful sources to improve learning behavior of children and adolescents.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at http://www.frontiersin.org/humanneuroscience/paper/10.3389/neuro.09/052.2009/

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist 4-18/and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT, University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108, 624–652.
Brass, M., Derrfuss, J., Forstmann, B., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 314–316.
Brett, M. C., Anton, J. -L., Valabregue, R., and Poline, J. -B. (2002). Region of interest analysis using an spm toolbox. Neuroimage, 16, 497.
Brown J. W., and Braver T. S. (2005). Learned predictions of error likelihood in the anterior cingulate cortex. Science 307, 1118–1121.
Bunge, S. A., Hazeltine, E., Scanlon, M. D., Rosen, A. C., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Dissociable contributions of prefrontal and parietal cortices to response selection. Neuroimage 17, 1562–1571.
Casey, B. J., Davidson, M. C., Hara, Y., and Thomas, K. M. (2004). Early development of subcortical regions involved in non-cued attention switching. Dev. Sci. 7, 534–542.
Cohen, M. X. (2008). Neurocomputational mechanisms of reinforcement-guided learning in humans: A review. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 113–125.
Cools, R. (2008). Role of dopamine in the motivational and cognitive control of behavior. Neuroscientist 14, 381–395.
Cosoco, C. A., Kollokian, V., Kwan, R. K. S., and Evans, A. C. (1997). Brainweb: online interface of a 3-d mri simulated brain database. Neuroimage 5, s425.
Crone, E. A., and van der Molen, M. W. (2004). Developmental changes in real life decision making: Performance on a gambling task previously shown to depend on the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Dev. Neuropsychol. 25, 251–279.
Crone, E. A., Zanolie, K., van Leijenhorst, L., Westenberg, P. M., and Rombouts, S. A. (2008). Neural mechanisms supporting flexible performance adjustment during development. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 165–177.
Dale, A. M. (1999). Optimal experimental design for event-related fmri. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 109–114.
Dosenbach, N. U., Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2008). A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 12, 99–105.
Ernst M, Nelson E. E, Jazbec S, McClure E. B, Monk C. S, Leibenluft E, Blair, J., and Pine, D. S. (2005). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. Neuroimage 25, 1279–1291.
Estes, W. K. (1961). A descriptive approach to the dynamics of choice behavior. Behav. Sci. 6, 177–184.
Frank, M. J., and Kong, L. (2008). Learning to avoid in older age. Psychol. Aging 23, 392–398.
Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L. C., and O’Reilly, R. C. (2004). By carrot or by stick: Cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science. 306, 1940–1943.
Galvan, A., Hare, T. A., Parra, C. E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G., and casey, B. J. (2006). Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J. Neurosci. 26, 6885–6892.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1961). Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 4, 267–272.
Holroyd, C. B., and Coles, M. G. (2008). Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior. 44, 548–559.
Hooper, C. J., Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., and Yarger, R. S. (2004). Adolescents’ performance on the iowa gambling task: Implications for the development of decision making and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Dev. Psychol. 40, 1148–1158.
Jocham, G., Neumann, J., Klein, T. A., Danielmeier, C., and Ullsperger, M. (2009). Adaptive coding of action values in the human rostral cingulate zone. J. Neurosci. 29, 7489–7496.
Kerns, J. G. (2006). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex activity in an fmri study of trial-to-trial adjustments on the simon task. Neuroimage 33, 399–405.
Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., and Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303, 1023–1026.
Klein, T. A., Neumann, J., Reuter, M., Hennig, J., von Cramon, D. Y., and Ullsperger, M. (2007). Genetically determined differences in learning from errors. Science 318, 1642–1645.
Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H., and Westerberg, H. (2002). Increased brain activity in frontal and parietal cortex underlies the development of visuospatial working memory capacity during childhood. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 1–10.
Ladouceur, C. D., Dahl, R. E., and Carter, C. S. (2004). ERP correlates of action monitoring in adolescence. In Adolescent Brain Development: Vulnerabilities and Opportunities, (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 1021), R. E. Dahl and L. P. Spear, eds (New York, New York Academy of Sciences), pp. 329–336.
Ladouceur, C. D., Dahl, R. E., and Carter, C. S. (2007). Development of action monitoring through adolescence into adulthood: ERP, and source localization. Dev. Sci. 10, 874–891.
Luna, B., and Sweeney, J. A. (2001). Studies of brain and cognitive maturation through childhood and adolescence: A strategy for testing neurodevelopmental hypotheses. Schizophr. Bull. 27, 443–455.
McClure-Tone, Fromm, S., Blair, R. J., Pine, D. S., and Ernst, M. (2008). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens activation to emotional facial expressions in children and adolescents at risk for major depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 266.
Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). Ani ntegrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202.
Rangel A., Camerer C., and Montague, R. (2008). A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision-making, Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 9, 545–556.
Ridderinkhof, K. R., and van den Wildenberg, W. P. (2005). Neuroscience. Adaptive coding. Science 307, 1059–1060.
Rushworth, M. F. (2008). Intention, choice and the medial frontal cortex. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 181–207
Rushworth, M. F., and Behrens, T. E. (2008). Choice, uncertainty and value in prefrontal and cingulate cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 389–397.
Scherf, K. S., Sweeney, J. A., and Luna, B. (2006). Brain basis of developmental change: The case of visuospatial working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1045–1058.
Schultz, W. (2007). Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends Neurosci. 30, 204–210.
Shanks, D. R., Tunney, R. J., and McCarthy, J. D. (2002). A re-examination of probability matching and rational choice. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 15, 233–250.
Somsen, R. J. (2007). The development of attention regulation in the wisconsin card sorting task. Dev. Sci. 10, 664–680.
Talairach, J., and Tourneaux, P. (1988). Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. Stuttgart, Thieme Verlag.
Taylor, S. F., Stern, E. R., and Gehring, W. J. (2007). Neural systems for error monitoring: Recent findings and theoretical perspectives. Neuroscientist 13, 160–172.
Toni, I., Rowe, J., Stephan, K. E., and Passingham, R. E. (2002). Changes of cortico-striatal effective connectivity during visuomotor learning. Cereb. Cortex 12, 1040–1047
van Duijvenvoorde, A. C., Zanolie, K., Rombouts, S. A., Raijmakers, M. E., and Crone, E. A. (2008). Evaluating the negative or valuing the positive? Neural mechanisms supporting feedback-based learning across development. J. Neurosci. 28, 9495–9503.
van Leijenhorst, L., Zanolie, K., Van Meel, C. S., Westenberg, P. M., Rombouts, S. A., and Crone, E. A. (2009). What motivates the adolescent? Brain regions mediating reward sensitivity across adolescence. Cereb. Cortex. bhp078.
Velanova, K., Wheeler, M. E., and Luna, B. (2008). Maturational changes in anterior cingulate and frontoparietal recruitment support the development of error processing and inhibitory control. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2505–2522.
Ward, B. D. (2000). Simultaneous inference for fmri data. Available at: http://afni.Nimh.Nih.Gov/afni/docpdf/alphasim.Pdf , (last accessed 5 january 2009).
Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition. Manual. San Antonio, The Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale�Third Edition. Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, The Psychological Corporation.
Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychol. Rev. 111, 931–959.
Zanolie, K., Van Leijenhorst, L., Rombouts, S. A., and Crone, E. A. (2008). Separable neural mechanisms contribute to feedback processing in a rule-learning task. Neuropsychologia 46, 117–126.
Keywords:
development, fMRI, feedback, decision-making, learning, cognitive control
Citation:
van den Bos W, Güroğlu B, van den Bulk BG, Rombouts SARB and Crone EA (2009). Better than expected or as bad as you thought? The neurocognitive development of probabilistic feedback processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 3:52. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.052.2009
Received:
21 August 2009;
 Paper pending published:
12 September 2009;
Accepted:
03 November 2009;
 Published online:
01 December 2009.

Edited by:

Elizabeth D. O'Hare, University of California at Berkeley, USA

Reviewed by:

Adriana Galván, University of California, USA
Monique Ernst, National Institute of Mental Health, USA
Copyright:
© 2009 van den Bos, Güroğlu, van den Bulk, Rombouts and Crone. This is an open-access article subject to an exclusive license agreement between the authors and the Frontiers Research Foundation, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original authors and source are credited.
*Correspondence:
Wouter van den Bos, Department of Psychology, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, 2300 RB, Leiden, The Netherlands. e-mail: WBos@fsw.leidenuniv.nl
Download