Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Ecol. Evol., 25 January 2016
Sec. Conservation and Restoration Ecology
Volume 3 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2015.00148

Prey Preferences of the Jaguar Panthera onca Reflect the Post-Pleistocene Demise of Large Prey

  • 1School of Biological Sciences, School of Environment, Natural Resources and Geography, College of Natural Sciences, Bangor University, Bangor, UK
  • 2Centre for African Conservation Ecology, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University, Port Elizabeth, South Africa
  • 3Centre for Wildlife Management, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa
  • 4Wildlife Conservation Research Unit, Department of Zoology, Recanati-Kaplan Centre, University of Oxford, Abingdon, UK
  • 5Panthera, New York, NY, USA
  • 6Department of Fisheries and Wildlife, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA
  • 7Conservation Biogeography Lab, Ecology Department, Federal University of Goiás, Goiânia, Brazil
  • 8Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA

Documenting the impacts of the Pleistocene megafaunal extinctions on predator-prey interactions is a challenge because of the incomplete fossil record and depauperate extant community structure. We used a comparative ecological approach to investigate whether the existing prey preference patterns of jaguars Panthera onca were potentially affected by the Pleistocene extinctions in the Americas compared with large felids in Africa and Asia. We reviewed the literature and found 25 studies reporting 3214 jaguar kills recorded throughout the species' distribution. We found that jaguars significantly preferred capybara Hydrochaeris hydrochaeris and giant anteater Myrmecophaga tridactyla, and avoided agoutis, carnivorans, primates, black-eared opossum Didelphis marsupialis and tapirs. Generalized linear models showed that jaguars select prey primarily based on socio-ecological and behavioral traits (abundance and herd size), rather than morphological characteristics (body size). Nonetheless, their accessible prey weight range was 6–60 kg, preferred prey weight range was 45–85 kg, and mean mass of significantly preferred prey was 32 ± 13 kg leading to a predator to prey body mass ratio of 1:0.53, which is much less than that of other solitary felids (although 1:0.84 may be the relationship with the smallest jaguars). Compared with other large, solitary felids, jaguars have an unusual predator to prey body mass ratio, show limited effect of prey morphology as a driver of prey selection, lack evidence of optimal foraging beyond their preferred prey, and a lack of preferential hunting on Cetartiodactyla herbivores. These features, coupled with the reduction in jaguar body mass since the Pleistocene, suggest that the loss of larger potential prey items within the preferred and accessible weight ranges at the end-Pleistocene still affects jaguar predatory behavior. It may be that jaguars survived this mass extinction event by preferentially preying on relatively small species.

Introduction

Understanding how foraging individuals decide upon what to feed is essential for predicting links and feedbacks among individuals at the population level and amongst trophic levels (Railsback and Harvey, 2013). Morphological and behavioral characteristics of prey have previously been shown to be drivers of large predator prey selection (Hayward and Kerley, 2005). However, these elements may not contribute uniformly across all predators particularly given the different evolutionary histories of the New and Old World. Consequently, we studied the trait-mediated interactions between jaguars and their prey in the Americas. The jaguar is the largest felid in the Americas (Nowell and Jackson, 1996) and is currently listed on the IUCN Red List as Near Threatened based on a high likelihood of persistence over most of its wide (8.75 million km2) distribution (Caso et al., 2008), although they will soon qualify for Vulnerable status given current rates of habitat loss, reductions of their prey base, and human persecution (Caso et al., 2008; H. Quigley pers. comm.).

The jaguar evolved in the Old World ~2.5–3 million years before present and migrated into North America about 2 million years ago (Kurtén and Anderson, 1980). During much of the Pleistocene, its range extended much further north than at present (reaching Nebraska and Oregon in the U.S.A. (ibid) and southern England and the Netherlands in Europe also; Hemmer et al., 2001; Mol et al., 2011). Historically, the jaguar was found from the south-western U.S. to southern Argentina (Seymour, 1989), but its current distribution is considerably smaller (Figure 1). This dramatic range reduction over the last million or so years has led some to describe the jaguar today as a relictual population (Kurtén and Anderson, 1980). The decline in range since the mid-Pleistocene was accompanied by a 15–20% reduction in body mass and a change in limb proportions, such that extant jaguars have shorter metapodials, perhaps as a response to hunting in more closed habitats (Kurten, 1973).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Distribution map of the jaguar and location of study sites. Current jaguar distribution is shaded and these data were obtained from the IUCN Red List. Sites where dietary studies of jaguars have been conducted are shown as triangles, and the shaded triangles represent the sites of studies that had data used in the analyses. Site numbers refer to those in Table 1. Note there are alternative maps on jaguar distribution from panthera.org and the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service that differ from that of the IUCN.

The morphology and behavior of other large predators have evolved to enable them to optimally kill a limited number of prey species within a specific prey weight range (Hayward et al., 2006b, 2014). This weight range also corresponds to the age classes of prey killed (Gervasi et al., 2015). Jaguars are solitary hunters (Sunquist and Sunquist, 2002) and so their prey preferences could be expected to be similar to those of leopards Panthera pardus (Hayward et al., 2006a), given that the two species have been considered to be ecological analogs in the New and Old Worlds, respectively (Sunquist and Sunquist, 2002). However, jaguars differ markedly from leopards in being about twice the mass with shorter, more robust limb bones, and relatively wider forepaws that are comparable in relative dimensions to those of lions Panthera leo (Gonyea, 1976; Meachen-Samuels and Van Valkenburgh, 2009b). Moreover, jaguars are more similar to lions than leopards in having both absolutely and relatively larger upper canines and lateral incisors (Meachen-Samuels and Van Valkenburgh, 2009a). In fact, jaguars appear to have the most robust canine teeth of any living felid for their body size (Meachen-Samuels and Van Valkenburgh, 2009a). These morphological characteristics indicate a specialization on strength relative to mass suggesting that jaguars should be killing larger prey than leopards, on average.

Across their range, jaguars exhibit up to 100% variation in body mass (Sunquist and Sunquist, 2002) and this is likely to impact their hunting decisions, with the smallest forms being more similar to leopards in the size of their prey. For example, Hoogesteijn and Mondolfi (1996) found that floodplain jaguars were significantly larger [body mass: Llanos = 104.5 kg (males), 66.9 kg (females) and Pantanal = 99.5 kg (males), 76.7 kg (females)] than forest jaguars [body mass: Amazon = 83.6 kg (males), no data for females and Central American = 56.1 kg (males), 41.4 kg (females)]. This variability was also reflected in the diet of the populations, with the forest jaguars having a significantly lower mean weight of vertebrate prey at 5.8 kg, compared to 89 kg (including livestock) for floodplain jaguars (Hoogesteijn and Mondolfi, 1996).

Prey is fundamental to the existence of large predators (Fuller and Sievert, 2001; Karanth et al., 2004; Hayward et al., 2007b), but in the case of the jaguar, it is not clear from existing reviews what the key prey base is or even whether they are reliant on large prey species (López Gonzalez and Miller, 2002). The currently prevailing view is that jaguars are opportunistic predators whose diet simply reflects the available prey community at a site (Mondolfi and Hoogesteijn, 1986; Rabinowitz and Nottingham, 1986; Emmons, 1987; Sunquist and Sunquist, 2002; Harmsen et al., 2010). Not surprisingly given their wide distribution and habitat tolerance, the jaguar's diet is diverse with at least 85 species listed as prey (Weckel et al., 2006b) ranging from cattle weighing more than 200 kg to small rodents (Harmsen et al., 2010). Their prey is unusually diverse and includes species such as arboreal primates (Peetz et al., 1992), ocelots (Gonzalez-Maya et al., 2010), marine and river turtles (Carrillo et al., 2009; Salera et al., 2009; Veríssimo et al., 2012), tortoises and fish (Emmons, 1989), crocodilians and their eggs (da Silveira et al., 2010) and dolphins (Castañeda et al., 2013). The diversity of aquatic prey reflects the jaguar's habitat use and ability to swim (Guggisberg, 1975). The majority of these prey are killed, but jaguars readily scavenge from carcasses for up to 17 days after death (López González and Lorenzana Piña, 2002). Despite this diversity of prey and their apparent strength, jaguars tend to kill prey slightly smaller than themselves (predator:prey ratio = 1:0.6–0.9; de Oliveira, 2002; if domestic livestock are excluded).

Hoogesteijn and Mondolfi (1996) describe the hunting technique frequently used by jaguars to kill cattle, whereby they jump on the back or side of the cattle, pull the animal's head around, unbalancing it and causing it to fall to the ground—often causing the animal to break its neck. The name jaguar is linked to their hunting technique: it comes from one of the Tupi-Guarani languages, deriving from the word “yaguara,” meaning “wild beast that overcomes its prey in a bound” (Seymour, 1989). Other hunting techniques used by jaguars include using the forepaws to hit smaller prey species (such as capybaras) to the ground and using a single bite to the nape of the neck to puncture the skull (Sunquist and Sunquist, 2002). This behavioral evidence suggests the jaguar has mechanisms by which to successfully kill large prey.

Reviews have indicated that medium and large prey are critical prey resources for jaguars (López Gonzalez and Miller, 2002), and that these vary depending on biome, competitor levels and prey availability (Sunquist, 2002). This study aimed to determine what the critical prey resources of jaguars are by determining the preferred prey of the species, and to investigate the impact of the Pleistocene extinctions on jaguar predatory ecology. There has been extensive research on the human-wildlife conflict arising from jaguar depredation on livestock (Rabinowitz, 1986; Quigley and Crawshaw, 1992; Hoogesteijn et al., 1993; Dalponte, 2002; Polisar et al., 2003; Michalski et al., 2006; Palmeira and Barrella, 2007; Hoogesteijn and Hoogesteijn, 2008; Rosas-Rosas et al., 2008; Soto-Shoender and Giuliano, 2011; Jêdrzejewski et al., 2014), so we focus predominately on wild prey in this study.

Methods

We searched Google Scholar and Web of Science for literature on the diet of jaguars, as well as gray literature (dissertations, theses) and the reference lists of any publications found. Keyword searches were for “jaguar” OR “Panthera onca” OR “onça” OR “onça-pintada” OR “onza” OR “yaguar” OR “yaguareté” OR “tigre Americano” AND “diet” OR “fezes” OR “presas” OR “heces” OR “disponibilidad” OR “alimentación” OR “dieta” OR “hábitos alimentarios” OR “depredación” OR “ecología alimentar.” Study sites that were surveyed over different years or in different treatments (hunting vs. non-hunting sections) were treated as separate data (Table 1). We included unpublished data if the raw data were obtained using standard, widely used analysis methods (scat analysis), as was done previously (Hayward et al., 2014; Lyngdoh et al., 2014).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Details on studies found and used in this meta-analysis including location, sample sizes, diet analysis method, and any assumptions made.

Due to the cryptic nature of jaguars (Harmsen et al., 2010), scat analysis is the primary method researchers have used to determine diet (Table 1). Scat analysis may under-represent turtles in jaguar diet as only the meat is eaten while the carapace remains intact yielding no evidence of the prey source within the scat (Carrillo et al., 2009), but over-represent reptiles, rodents and mesocarnivores (Martínez-Gutiérrez et al., 2015). However, scats were invariably collected from throughout the study sites, so problems associated with patchily distributed local prey are minimized (Steenweg et al., 2015). Distinguishing jaguar scat from sympatric puma Puma concolor scat based on size and other physical characteristics can lead to mis-diagnosis of the depositor (Farrell et al., 2000; Martínez-Gutiérrez et al., 2015), which will reduce the chances of obtaining significant preferences/avoidance in our analyses by broadening the dietary niche to include puma prey (hence our results are conservative). In the jaguar's case, DNA confirmation of the diet indicated that larger prey species are more likely to be present in scats than small prey species (Farrell et al., 2000), counteracting the traditional bias toward detecting smaller prey species from scat analysis (Mills, 1992). Prey availability was obtained from each study site from information presented in the literature (Table 1).

We used Jacobs' (1974) index to determine wild prey selectivity of jaguars following previous prey preference studies (Hayward et al., 2012, 2014; Lyngdoh et al., 2014). Where domestic livestock were killed alongside wild prey, we included both, but otherwise excluded studies solely focused on domestic livestock. Positive values of Jacobs' index indicate preference and negative values indicate avoidance (Jacobs, 1974). We calculated a Jacobs' index value for each prey species at each study site and then tested these using t-tests against a mean of zero, where data were normally distributed or a sign test where it was not, to determine significant preferences or avoidance (Hayward and Kerley, 2005). We also plotted Jacobs' index values with error bars to illustrate where a larger sample size was likely to lead to significant preference or avoidance, assuming the existing trend continued. We conducted these analyses for individual species and groups of related species (e.g. primates or Agouti spp.).

We developed a set of testable covariates that could potentially influence jaguar prey preferences from our literature review. A generalized linear model with a Gaussian distribution and an identity link function was conducted on these non-correlated variables. The independent variables used were socio-ecological/behavioral (prey relative abundance, habitat type and herd size at a site, and morphological (prey body mass and potential threat to a hunting jaguar; Table 2). Prior to model fitting, we standardized all covariates. We evaluated all possible combinations of models derived from the covariates. Model selection occurred using Akaike's information criterion (AIC) in a maximum likelihood framework (Akaike, 1973, 1974). We used model averaging to derive parameter estimates (Burnham and Anderson, 1998). Strongly supported relationships among individual variables were plotted using linear or loess best fit models.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Preference status (P/A; where –, denotes significantly avoided; +, significantly preferred; and ~, killed in accordance with relative abundance), mean Jacobs's index value of each jaguar prey species, number of studies recording it as potential prey (np) and actual prey (na), mean percentage abundance and kills of each prey species, body mass (three-quarters of adult female), and categories of herd size, main habitat and potential threat to a jaguar, based on Nowak (1999).

We estimated prey species body size as three-quarters of adult female body mass to account for juveniles and sub-adult prey killed (Jooste et al., 2013). Body mass, herd size, habitat use, and potential threat data were taken from Nowak (1999). Herd size is assumed to be an indicator of how well prey could detect predators (Hamilton, 1971; Fitzgibbon and Lazarus, 1995) and how the attraction effect of congregating prey affects predator detection of prey (Hebblewhite and Pletscher, 2002), and was a categorical variable with 1 relating to solitary individuals, two to species existing in pairs, three to small family groups, 4 to small herds (10–19) and 5 to large herds (>20; Table 2). Habitat type may influence predation as the density of vegetation can affect detectability and catchability, and predator and prey must overlap in habitat to encounter one another (Hayward and Kerley, 2005). A categorical variable of habitat use was used with 1 relating to open environments, 2 to woodlands, and 3 to dense forests (Table 2). Threat was a categorical variable where 0 represents no threat, 1 a minor threat, and two a major threat based on body size and the possession of weaponry, such as horns or teeth (Table 2).

We identified the accessible prey weight range following the break point analysis of Clements et al. (2014). The accessible prey weight range is most likely to encompass the preferred weight range of earlier prey preference studies, which we estimated from loess smoothed plots of mean species Jacobs' index scores against body mass (Hayward et al., 2014). We calculated the ideal mass as the mean body mass of those species that were significantly preferred. We estimated the body mass of jaguars as 60 kg, which is the lower range of adult female body mass (Nowak, 1999) and used this to determine the predator to prey mass ratio by dividing the ideal mass of prey by 60. We also include 38 kg as this is the lowest reported body mass of adult jaguars in Nowak (1999). We recognize jaguar body mass varies substantially throughout their range and these ratios could be affected by site-specific variability, however, we used this as representing the body mass over the entire distribution of the species.

We tested whether jaguar preferences for individual species varied with prey abundance, rainfall or size of prey available at a site (following Kiltie, 1984) using linear regression. We could not run generalized models on these variables because of insufficient sample size for individual species (cf the global models we ran with data from all species). Climate data were obtained from details presented in individual studies or the US National Climatic Data Center (http://www.ncdc.noaa.gov/). Note that the regression of Jacobs' index on prey abundance tests how jaguars respond to changes in the relative abundance of individual species, whereas the inclusion of prey abundance in the generalized linear model is looking at how a species' overall abundance affects its likelihood of being selected by jaguars.

Finally, we tested aspects of optimal foraging theory using linear regression. We hypothesized that (1) jaguars would show greater preferences at sites with higher species richness because there would be sufficient prey for them to specialize on particular species; and (2) jaguars would show greater preferences at sites with higher prey biomass for the same reason. We used total biomass, largest biomass of a single species, and body mass of the most preferred prey at a site as measures for this second hypothesis.

All analyses were conducted in R (R Core Development Team, 2008; Barton, 2013). Mean (±1 S.E.) values are presented throughout.

Results

We found 53 studies documenting the diet of jaguars collected over 176 study years and based on 5977 kill records (Table 1). These studies were conducted throughout the distribution of the jaguar (Figure 1). Twenty-five (25; 47%) of these studies, collected over 75 survey years with 3214 kill records (Table 1), had sufficient data for use in this meta-analysis. The remaining 28 studies were excluded due to inadequate prey availability data or a focus on livestock predation (Table 1). Brazil, Mexico and Costa Rica had the largest number of jaguar diet studies, while Brazil (9 useable of 20 studies), Guatemala (4 of 4) and Mexico (4 of 9) had most of the studies that could be included in this meta-analysis (Table 1). Studies that were used came from the suite of habitats used by jaguars including tropical forest, the Llanos, Cerrado, Pantanal, and Chacos. No studies from Argentina, Panama, or Paraguay could be used in this analysis (Figure 1).

Within our meta-analysis there were 111 wild species in the diet of jaguars, ranging in size from 1 kg rabbits to 130 kg lowland tapirs (these results and scientific names of species mentioned in the text are presented in Table 2). The species that appeared most frequently in their diet (16–21%) included capybara, wild pig, caiman, collared peccary, nine-banded armadillo, giant anteater and white-nosed coati. In addition, there were species that were always preyed upon if they were present, suggesting a possible preference (capybara, white-tailed deer, white-nosed coati, wild pigs, common caiman, marsh deer, crab-eating fox, crab-eating raccoon, and northern tamandua). In addition, collared peccary, red brocket deer, spotted paca, giant anteater, white-lipped peccary and South American coati were also killed by jaguars more than 85% of the time they were present in the prey community (Table 2). Conversely, Baird's tapir was never killed by jaguars in the sampled studies, and primates were killed at only four of 11 sites where they were recorded as present (Table 2).

Common caiman, white-nosed coati, Azara's agouti and black-eared opossum constituted over 20% of the available prey community where they were recorded (Table 2). Capybara, wild pigs, common caiman, nine-banded armadillo, collared peccary, white-nosed coati, giant anteater, and white-lipped peccary constituted more than 10% of the kills of jaguar where they occurred (Table 2).

When the relative abundance of prey species was accounted for, jaguars significantly preferred to prey on only two species: giant anteater [t(8) = 7.24, p < 0.001] and capybara [t(7) = 16.96, p < 0.001; Table 2; Figure 2A). Southern tamandua and marsh deer may become preferred with a larger sample size (Table 2; Figure 2A), all but two of the remaining 109 prey species were killed in accordance with their relative abundance within the prey community (Figure 2A). Both black-eared opossum [t(4) = −31.42, p < 0.001] and lowland tapir [t(6) = −2.66, p = 0.038] were significantly avoided, along with Baird's tapir if sample sizes were larger (Table 2; Figure 2A).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Plot of the mean Jacobs' index values (±1 S.E.) of preference for (A) prey species and (B) taxonomic groups that were reported as potential prey of jaguars in at least two study sites. Black bars represent significantly preferred species, white bars significantly avoided species, and gray bars species killed in accordance with their availability.

There was no indication that jaguars prefer a particular group of related prey, such as peccaries or armadillos, but they do significantly avoid carnivorans [t(27) = −2.86, p = 0.008], the agoutis [t(18) = −4.09, p < 0.001] and primates (binomial test 0/11, p < 0.001; Table 2; Figure 2B). All other broad taxonomic groups were killed in accordance with their abundance (Figure 2B).

Jaguars reduced their preference for white-nosed coatis as they became more abundant in the prey community (r2 = 0.92, n = 7, p < 0.001; Figure 3). No other prey species exhibited a significant relationship between their Jacobs' index value and their relative abundance (Figure 3). There was no relationship between the degree to which jaguars preferred or avoided a prey species and the mean annual rainfall at each study site where it occurred (Figure 4).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Relationship between site-specific preference (Jacobs' index value) and relative abundance for each prey species of jaguars with four or more Jacobs' index value estimates within the prey community. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals are shown unless they extend to far beyond the y-axis limits.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Plots of the relationship between prey preference of jaguar prey species or taxonomic groups with more than six site records and the mean annual rainfall at each site.

A generalized linear model of the drivers of jaguar prey preferences revealed that prey abundance was the most supported predictor variable (sum of Akaike's weights wi = 0.93), with prey herd size also strongly supported (wi = 0.74; Table 3). Model averaged parameter estimates from the top-ranking model showed that jaguar prey preferences were negatively associated with prey abundance (β = −1.39) and positively associated with herd size (β = 0.08; Table 3; Figure 5). Prey body mass was the next most important variable (Table 3), and a regression of Jacobs' index against prey body mass indicated that jaguars significantly preferred larger prey up until 100 kg (Figure 6). In our sample, tapirs are the only wild prey that exceed 100 kg and jaguars avoided both species.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Model selection statistics for the 10 most supported models of drivers of jaguar prey selection, including model averaged parameter estimates and summed Akaike's weights for each variable.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Plots of the relationship between the most important drivers of jaguar prey selection based on the generalized linear model (Table 3): mean prey abundance (linear), prey herd category (1–solitary, 2–pairs, 3–small groups, 4–groups of 5–20, and 5 large groups; linear) and prey body mass (three-quarters of adult female body mass; loess smooth).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Relationship between the Jacobs' index of each prey species with three or more records and prey body mass (both log transformed).

Segmented modeling showed strong support for two and four breakpoints in jaguar prey preference and prey body mass relationships (Table 4). We use the most conservative of these and discuss the results with two breakpoints hereafter. The segmented modeling showed the accessible prey weight range was from 6 to 60 kg (Figure 7). The preferred prey weight range of jaguars is 45 to 85 kg (Figure 5). Note that the upper limit of the preferred weight range is larger than that of the accessible weight range due to the loess smoothing function. The body mass of significantly preferred prey (ideal prey; capybara and giant anteater) is 32 ± 14 kg. The predator: ideal prey ratio for jaguars is therefore 1: 0.53 based on adult female jaguar body mass of 60 kg and 1:0.84 for adult body mass of 38 kg.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Model selection statistics for the segmented model to identify the preferred and accessible prey of jaguars.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Segmented model of the relationship between the mass rank of each prey species and jaguar preference. Preference is based on cumulative Jacobs' index values (+1 for standardization) following Clements et al. (2014). Actual prey masses corresponding to mass ranks are shown above the x-axis. Representative species sitting at breakpoints are shown (coati, paca, marsh deer).

Individual prey species within the preferred and accessible weight ranges were invariably increasingly avoided as they became more abundant within the prey community (Figure 3). Significantly preferred prey species (mean b = −0.1±2.2), species within the preferred weight range (b = −7.7±8.3) and significantly avoided prey (b = −1.8±1.9) had a mean slope of this relationship with standard error bars crossing 0 (Figure 3). Conversely, accessible prey (b = −3.9±2.7) and prey outside the accessible prey weight range (b = −9.4±3.8) had a consistently negative relationship between preference and abundance (Figure 3).

There was no support for our hypotheses relating to optimal foraging theory of jaguars (Figure 8). Jaguars did not show greater preferences where there was greater prey species richness or where there was a greater biomass of potential prey.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Regression plots testing aspects of optimal foraging theory where the degree of prey preference exhibited at a site is related to (A) the number of species there, (B) the biomass of all prey there, (C) the largest biomass of a species there, and (D) the body mass of the most preferred prey species there.

Discussion

Jaguars exhibit trait-mediated foraging interactions with their prey species (Railsback and Harvey, 2013) by significantly preferring capybara and giant anteater, and avoiding species outside their preferred and accessible weight ranges (Figure 2). In contrast to other large predators, the traits that mediate jaguar foraging decisions are ecological (prey abundance) and behavioral (herd size) more than morphological (body mass; Table 3). Thus, jaguars are not generalist predators as once thought, but prey specifically on particular species and according to certain prey characteristics.

During the late Pleistocene, there were more than 50 additional species of large (>40 kg) herbivores in the Americas (Kurtén and Anderson, 1980; Greenwood, 2009), and so jaguars evolved in ecosystems with a much higher diversity and availability of potential prey than found today. This is in contrast to Africa, where extant large carnivores prey upon herbivore communities that were similar in abundance, richness and diversity to those which occurred at the end of the Pleistocene (Lyons et al., 2004). This difference likely explains why the predator to prey body mass of jaguars is much smaller than other large solitary felids (Figure 9). For example, jaguars are often considered to be ecologically similar to leopards (Sunquist and Sunquist, 2002), yet jaguars preferentially prey on smaller species than leopards (Figure 9), despite jaguars having a larger body size. The lack of diverse large herbivores in the Holocene also likely explains why jaguars do not have prey selection driven by prey morphology (Figure 5), do not show any evidence of optimal foraging (Figure 8), are the only large felid not preferentially hunting Cetartiodactyla herbivores, and have reduced body mass compared to the Pleistocene (Seymour, 1989). The larger prey species that survived the Pleistocene extinctions in South America, such as tapirs, are currently above the upper limits of the accessible weight range of jaguars. Those Cetartiodactyla species that persist today are likely to have been kept at lower density than jaguars evolved with due to continual hunting pressure from humans since the Holocene. Thus, jaguars may be persisting on sub-optimal prey, as is the case for some puma (Yanez et al., 1986; Iriarte et al., 1990) and African wild dog Lycaon pictus populations (Woodroffe et al., 2007).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Plot of the predator to prey body mass ratio of solitary members of the genus Panthera and how this relates to the body mass of each species (Hayward et al., 2006a, 2012; Lyngdoh et al., 2014).

Additionally, even though the jaguar is regarded today as the largest and most dominant apex predator in the Americas, it has only occupied that role for the past 10,000 years. Near the end of the Pleistocene, the jaguar in the Americas was sympatric with a least ten carnivore species that were larger than itself (Cione et al., 2009). Thus, similar to that reported for the gray wolf (Canis lupus; Tedford et al., 2004), the jaguar was somewhat of a mesocarnivore rather than an apex carnivore for most of its evolutionary existence, so its predatory behavior should be viewed in that context. Indeed, the jaguar's ability to function as a mesopredator by preying on a wide range of different-sized species, particularly smaller species, is probably why this large felid species, together with the puma (P. concolor), survived the end-Pleistocene extinction in the Americas, whereas the other five sympatric large felid species went extinct (Van Valkenburgh and Hertel, 1998). The effects of the end-Pleistocene megafaunal extinctions, together with the “ghosts” of more dominant extinct carnivores (Connell, 1980), may explain why jaguars continue to preferentially prey upon species smaller than expected given their body size, especially in comparison to African large carnivores, which continuously had a high diversity of large herbivores on which to prey.

It is unclear if jaguars also preferentially preyed upon relatively small prey during the Pleistocene, or simply reduced their preferred prey size in the Holocene as a strategy to survive the end-Pleistocene extinctions. Given the high diversity of potential prey species during the Pleistocene in the Americas (higher diversity than contemporary African savannah ecosystems; Lyons et al., 2004), it seems unlikely that jaguars would have restricted themselves to such small prey. In fact, their larger body size in the Pleistocene would have allowed jaguars accessible prey between 30 and 150 kg, with preferred prey around 90 kg (based on the equations in Van Valkenburgh et al., 2015). Interestingly, other carnivore species, ranging from black-footed ferrets Mustela nigripes to coyotes Canis latrans, appear to have significantly different diets and ecological niches between the Pleistocene and Holocene (Owen et al., 2000; Meachen et al., 2014), probably as an adaptation to survive the end-Pleistocene extinctions, and the same is likely true for jaguars. For the coyotes, the switch to smaller prey species in Holocene coincided with a reduction in body size (Meachen et al., 2014). This suggests that jaguars also could have exhibited adaptive behavioral changes during the Holocene to preferentially prey upon smaller species. Similar to the coyote, this possible behavioral adaptation coincided with a decrease in body size of the jaguar during the Holocene. Furthermore, there is no evidence that canids in North America expanded their dietary niche following the Pleistocene extinctions, possibly because competition with humans outweighed the advantages of niche expansion (Pardi and Smith, 2015).

Adding further pressure to a predator with a limited or reduced abundance of suitably sized prey, previous research has highlighted the importance of areas with no human hunting as jaguar prey refugia (Harmsen et al., 2010). For example, important jaguar prey species, such as peccaries, spotted paca and nine-banded armadillo are heavily hunted by humans (Redford, 1992; Jorgenson and Redford, 1993). Given the importance of prey in determining predator densities (Fuller and Sievert, 2001; Karanth et al., 2004; Hayward et al., 2007b), reduced prey abundance has been, and probably still is, keeping jaguar populations below densities at which they evolved. Thus we reiterate that the “empty forests syndrome” (Redford, 1992; Wilkie et al., 2011) can have cascading impacts on all trophic levels, including apex predators (Steinmetz et al., 2013).

Both peccary species are within the accessible and preferred weight ranges of jaguars, however neither is preferred. Although frequently killed by jaguars, there is no evidence that the predator-prey relationship between jaguars and peccaries is a coevolutionary predator-prey “arms race” given that existing evidence suggests the species evolved at separate times and places (Mayer and Wetzel, 1987). This lack of preference may be because peccaries are formidable opponents that can seriously injure jaguars (Perry, 1970). Furthermore, severe population reductions of peccaries during the late-Pleistocene may have prevented jaguars from optimally foraging on them. In fact, several peccary species did not survive these extinctions (Kurtén and Anderson, 1980; Greenwood, 2009), and those that did likely suffered severe population bottlenecks, similar to that found for other Pleistocene ungulate survivors such as bison Bison bison and musk ox Ovibos moschatus (Cione et al., 2009) and their loss is likely to have led to a chain of extinction for large scavenging birds such as condors, vultures and teratorns (Van Valkenburgh and Hertel, 1998). Consequently, the surviving jaguars may have been those that preferentially preyed upon species smaller than peccaries, and thus the non-preference for peccaries by modern jaguars could be an artifact from the end-Pleistocene extinctions. Despite this, jaguars can have significant impacts on peccary populations, such as in Iguaçu National Park, Brazil, where half the peccary population was killed by jaguars annually (Crawshaw, 1995). Elsewhere, white-lipped peccaries were found in 89% of jaguar scats along beaches in Corcovado National Park (Carrillo et al., 2009) and collared peccaries were killed almost three-times more frequently than any other prey species in the Sierra Madre de Chiapas of Mexico (Cruz et al., 2007). Our results also support hypotheses that the expanding wild boar population in the Brazilian Atlantic Forest will enable jaguars to recolonize the region (Verdade et al., 2015), because wild boars are killed in accordance with their abundance (Table 2) and are expected to function as buffer prey facilitating this expansion.

Jaguars prefer capybara and giant anteaters throughout their range and we assume this is based on optimal foraging of high energy meat yield for minimal investment in handling and processing, and limited injury risk. They are unselective in their predation on capybara age classes, but can kill a substantial proportion of a population over short periods of time (30% of a population within 2 months; Schaller and Vasconcelos, 1978). Just as the heavy predation by leopards Panthera pardus on baboons Papio spp. does not indicate preferential predation (Hayward et al., 2006a), the high levels of predation on caiman (over 62% of all caiman mortalities; Azevedo and Verdade, 2012) do not mean jaguars preferentially prey on them (Table 2). Although previous studies have shown reptiles can constitute up to 54% of a jaguars diet (Ramalho, 2006), a broader review of 19 studies showed that this is closer to 21 ± 3% (although this drops to 0.9 ± 0.1% when means weighted by sample size are used; calculated from da Silveira et al., 2010).

Although we focus this study on wild prey, jaguars are well known predators of livestock (Hoogesteijn et al., 1993; Quigley et al., 2015). Data from Polisar et al. (2003) show that jaguars prefer to kill cattle less than 1 year of age weighing less than 120 kg (D = 0.60) and avoid adult cattle (D = −0.78). Garrote (2012) showed that jaguars preferred pigs (D = 0.91) over horses (D = 0.38) and avoided cattle (D = −0.98). These results serve to reinforce our prey preference results, but also suggest that the predator naiveté that livestock have evolved through the domestication process allows predators to kill larger individuals than is possible with wild prey.

Other large predators we have studied exhibited reduced preferences for preferred prey species as they became more abundant within the prey community (Hayward, 2011). Conversely, they show increasing preference for non-preferred prey within the preferred weight range as they became more abundant (Hayward, 2011). Jaguars show no such relationship, with non-preferred species also being less preferred as they become more abundant (Figure 3). We contend that this is further evidence that jaguar predatory ecology has been substantially altered since the late-Pleistocene extinctions. This is in addition to the unusually small predator to prey body mass ratio of jaguars compared to other solitary felids, the limited effect of prey morphology as a driver of jaguar prey selection, the absence of evidence of optimal foraging by jaguars, the reduction in jaguar body mass since the Pleistocene, and the absence of preferentially hunting on Cetartiodactyla herbivores.

While this meta-analysis highlights the number of studies conducted on jaguar foraging ecology, it illustrates that research exhibits substantial spatial variation across the Americas, being dominated by work in Brazil and Mexico (Table 1; Figure 1) and data from these sites may be influencing our results. It also concurs with earlier reviews that concluded that research on this species has been limited by small sample sizes and a corresponding lack of information on prey abundance (Sunquist, 2002). In previous research on Africa's large predator guild, we were able to use trait-based aspects of their foraging to predict the number of large predators that could be sustained at a site, the home range sizes, and the diet using the results of prey preference studies similar to this one (Hayward et al., 2007a,b, 2009; Jooste et al., 2013). This was not possible here because jaguar prey abundance data is invariably presented as a relative measure rather than actual densities, which is satisfactory for determining prey preferences (Hayward et al., 2006a) but inadequate for these other purposes. The frequent use of camera traps to monitor prey communities in jaguar territories offers the opportunity to derive population density estimates via entropy modeling (Rowcliffe et al., 2008, 2011) and these would assist predictive ecology for these species.

This research highlights the important prey species and weight ranges of prey that are necessary for the conservation of jaguars and will be useful if plans for reintroduction come to fruition (Galetti et al., 2013). It also highlights areas for focus in future research programmes. Most importantly, however, this study illustrates the predatory response of an apex predator to the Pleistocene extinctions and more recent overhunting in which jaguars exhibit a diverse prey spectrum with minimal evidence of adhering to optimal foraging rules and, where they do, preferentially killing smaller than predicted prey (Figure 9).

Author Contributions

MH designed, analyzed and wrote up this study. JK collected data and wrote-up this study. RM wrote-up this study. AN collected data and wrote-up this study. SR collected data and wrote-up this study. LS collected data and wrote-up this study. BV wrote-up this study.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Howard Quigley, Adrian Schiavini and Carlos Alberto Lopez Gonzalez for their substantial assistance in improving this manuscript and the IUCN Red List for providing spatial distribution data for the jaguar for Figure 1.

References

Akaike, H. (1973). Information Theory and an Extension of the Maximum Likelihood Principle. Budapest: Akademiai Kiado.

Google Scholar

Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans. Automat. Contr. AC 19, 716–723. doi: 10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amin, M. (2004). Patrones de Alimentación y Disponibilidad de Presas del Jaguar (Panthera onca) y del Puma (Puma concolor) en la Reserva de la Biosfera Calakmul, Campeche, México. México: Universidad Nacional Autonoma de México.

Aranda, M. (1994). Importancia de los pacaries (Tayassu spp.) en la alimentacion del jaguar (Panthera onca). Acta Zool. Mex. 62, 11–22.

Google Scholar

Aranda, M., and Sanchez-Cordero, V. (1996). Prey spectra of jaguar (Panthera onca) and puma (Puma concolor) in tropical forests of Mexico. Stud. Neotrop. Environ. 31, 65–67. doi: 10.1076/snfe.31.2.65.13334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azevedo, F. C. C., and Verdade, L. M. (2012). Predator-prey interactions: jaguar predation on caiman in a floodplain forest. J. Zool. 286, 200–207. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2011.00867.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barton, K. (2013). Package ‘MuMIn’. R package version 1.5. Aberdeen.

Burnham, K. P., and Anderson, D. R. (1998). Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Carrillo, E., Fuller, T. K., and Saenz, J. C. (2009). Jaguar (Panthera onca) hunting activity: effects of prey distribution and availability. J. Trop. Ecol. 25, 563–567. doi: 10.1017/S0266467409990137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caso, A., Lopez-Gonzalez, C., Payan, E., Eizirik, E., and de Oliveira, T. (2008). Panthera onca. Gland: IUCN. Available online at: http://www.iucnredlist.org/details/15953/0 Accessed 03 June 2015

Castañeda, F., Herrera, L., and Pereira, S. (2013). Behaviour of two male jaguars scavenging on a marine dolphin in Honduras. Cat News 58, 3–12.

Cavalcanti, S. M. C., and Gese, E. M. (2010). Kill rates and predation patterns of jaguars (Panthera onca) in the southern Pantanal, Brazil. J. Mammal. 91, 722–736. doi: 10.1644/09-MAMM-A-171.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceballos, G., Chávez, C., Zarza, H., and Manterola, C. (2005). Ecología y conservación del jaguar en la región de Calakmul. Biodiversitas 62, 1–7.

Google Scholar

Chiarello, A. G. (1999). Effects of fragmentation of the atlantic forest on mammal communities in south-eastern Brazil. Biol. Conserv. 89, 71–82. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3207(98)00130-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiarello, A. G. (2000). Density and population size of mammals in remnants of brazilian atlantic forest. Conserv. Biol. 14, 1649–1657. doi: 10.1046/j.1523-1739.2000.99071.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chinchilla, F. A. (1997). La dieta del jaguar (Panthera onca), el puma (Felis concolor) y el manigordo (Felis pardalis)(Carnivora; Felidae) en el parque nacional corcovado, costa rica. Rev. Biol. Trop. 45, 1223–1230.

Google Scholar

Cione, A. L., Tonni, E. P., and Soibelzon, L. (2009). Did Humans Cause the Late Pleistocene-Early Holocene Mammalian Extinctions in South America in a Context of Shrinking Open Areas? Amsterdam: Springer.

Google Scholar

Clements, H. S., Tambling, C. J., Hayward, M. W., and Kerley, G. I. H. (2014). An objective approach to determining the weight ranges of prey preferred by and accessible to the five large African carnivores. PLoS ONE 9:e101054. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connell, J. H. (1980). Diversity and the coevolution of competitors, or the ghost of competition past. Oikos 35, 131–138. doi: 10.2307/3544421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawshaw, P. G. Jr. (1995). Comparative Ecology of Ocelot (Felis pardalis) and Jaguar (Panthera onca) in a Protected Subtropical Forest in Brazil and Argentina. Unpublished PhD thesis, Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

Crawshaw, P. G. Jr., and Quigley, H. B. (2002). Hábitos Alimentarios del Jaguar y el Puma en el Pantanal, Brasil, con Implicaciones Para su Manejo y Conservación. México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Wildlife Conservation Society y Fondo de Cultura Económica.

Google Scholar

Cruz, E., Palacios, G., and Guiris, M. (2007). Situación Actual del Jaguar en Chiapas. México: Conabio-Alianza WWF/Telcel-Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.

da Silveira, R., Ramalho, E. E., Thorbjarnarson, J. B., and Magnusson, W. E. (2010). Depredation by jaguars on caimans and importance of reptiles in the diet of jaguar. J. Herpetol. 44, 418–424. doi: 10.1670/08-340.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalponte, J. (2002). Dieta del Jaguar y Depredación de Ganado en el Norte del Pantanal, Brasil. México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Wildlife Conservation Society y Fondo de Cultura Económica.

de Azevedo, F. C. C. (2008). Food habits and livestock depredation of sympatric jaguars and pumas in the Iguaçu national park area, south brazil. Biotropica 40, 494–500. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.2008.00404.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Azevedo, F. C. C., and Murray, D. L. (2007). Spatial organization and food habits of jaguars (Panthera onca) in a floodplain forest. Biol. Conserv. 137, 391–402. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2007.02.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Oliveira, T. G. (2002). Ecologia Comparativa de la Alimentación del Jaguar y del Puma en el Neotrópico. México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Wildlife Conservation Society y Fondo de Cultura Económica.

Emmons, L. H. (1987). Comparative feeding ecology of felids in a neotropical rainforest. Behav. Ecol. Sociob. 20, 271–283. doi: 10.1007/BF00292180

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emmons, L. H. (1989). Jaguar predation on chelonians. J. Herpetol. 23, 311–314. doi: 10.2307/1564460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Facure, K. G., and Giaretta, A. A. (1996). Food habits of carnivores in a coastal Atlantic forest of southeastern Brazil. Mammalia 60, 499–502. doi: 10.1515/mamm-1996-0319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrell, L. E., Roman, J., and Sunquist, M. E. (2000). Dietary separation of sympatric carnivores identified by molecular analysis of scats. Mol. Ecol. 9, 1583–1590. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-294x.2000.01037.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzgibbon, C. D., and Lazarus, J. (1995). Antipredator Behaviour of Serengeti Ungulates: Individual Differences and Population Consequences. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Foster, R. J., Harmsen, B. J., Valdes, B., and Pomilla, C., and Doncaster, C. P. (2010). Food habits of sympatric jaguars and pumas across a gradient of human disturbance. J. Zool. 280, 309–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2009.00663.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuller, T. K., and Sievert, P. R. (2001). Carnivore Demography and the Consequences of Changes in Prey Availability. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press and the Zoological Society of London.

Google Scholar

Galetti, M., Eizirik, E., Beisiegel, B., Ferraz, K., Cavalcanti, S., Srbek-Araujo, A. C., et al. (2013). Atlantic rainforest's jaguars in decline. Science 342:930. doi: 10.1126/science.342.6161.930-a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garla, R. C., Setz, E. Z. F., and Gobbi, N. (2001). Jaguar (Panthera onca) food habits in atlantic rain forest of southeastern brazil. Biotropica 33, 691–696. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.2001.tb00226.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrote, G. (2012). Depredación del jaguar (Panthera onca) sobre el ganado en los llanos orientales de Colombia. Mastozoología Neotrop. 19, 139–145.

Google Scholar

Gervasi, V., Nilsen, E. B., and Linnell, J. D. C. (2015). Body mass relationships affect the age structure of predation across carnivore–ungulate systems: a review and synthesis. Mammal. Rev. 45, 253–266. doi: 10.1111/mam.12047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gómez-Ortiz, Y., and Monroy-Vilchis, O. (2013). Feeding ecology of puma Puma concolor in Mexican montane forests with comments about jaguar Panthera onca. Wildl. Biol. 19, 179–187. doi: 10.2981/12-092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonyea, W. J. (1976). Adaptive differences in the body proportions of large felids. Acta Anat. 96, 81–96. doi: 10.1159/000144663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Maya, J. F., Navarro-Arquez, E., and Schipper, J. (2010). Ocelots as prey items of jaguars: a case from Talamanca, Costa Rica. Rev. Biol. Trop. 45, 1223–1229.

Google Scholar

Greenwood, A. D. (2009). “Ancient D. N. A., and the genetic consequences of late pleistocene extinctions,” in American Megafaunal Extinctions at the End of the Pleistocene (Amsterdam: Springer), 107–123.

Guadamuz, V. H. M. (2012). Cambios en la Abundancia, Actividad Temporal y Dieta de Jaguar (Panthera onca), Otros Felinos y Sus Presas en el Parque Nacional Santa Rosa, Área de Conservación Guanacaste, Costa Rica, Unpublished thesis, Heredia: Universidad Nacional de Costa Rica.

Guggisberg, C. A. W. (1975). Wild Cats of the World. New York, NY: Taplinger Publishing Co.

Google Scholar

Gutierrez, D. C., and Porras, J. C. (2008). Ecología Poblacional de Jaguar (Panthera onca) y Puma (Puma concolor) y Dieta de Jaguar, en el Sector Pacífico de la Cordillera de Talamanca. Costa Rica. San Jose, CA: Universidad Latina de Costa Rica.

Hamilton, W. D. (1971). Geometry of the selfish herd. J. Theor. Biol. 31, 295–311. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(71)90189-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmsen, B. J., Foster, R. J., Silver, S. C., and Ostro, L., Doncaster, C. P. (2010). “The ecology of jaguars in the cockscomb basin wildlife sanctuary, belize,” in The Biology and Conservation of Wild Felids, eds D. W. Macdonald and A. Loveridge (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 403–416.

Hayward, M. W. (2011). Scarcity in the prey community yields anti-predator benefits. Acta Oecol. 37, 314–320. doi: 10.1016/j.actao.2011.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, M. W., Hayward, G. J., Druce, D., and Kerley, G. I. H. (2009). Do fences constrain predator movements on an evolutionary scale? home range, food intake and movement patterns of large predators reintroduced to addo elephant national park, south africa. Biodiv. Conserv. 18, 887–899. doi: 10.1007/s10531-008-9452-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, M. W., Henschel, P., O'Brien, J., Hofmeyr, M., and Balme, G. A., and Kerley, G. I. H. (2006a). Prey preferences of the leopard (Panthera pardus). J. Zool. 270, 298–313. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00139.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, M. W., Hofmeyr, M., O'Brien, J., and Kerley, G. I. H. (2006b). Prey preferences of the cheetah Acinonyx jubatus: morphological limitations or the need to capture rapidly consumable prey before kleptoparasites arrive? J. Zool. 270, 615–627. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00184.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, M. W., Hofmeyr, M., O'Brien, J., and Kerley, G. I. H. (2007a). Testing predictions of the prey of the lion (Panthera leo) derived from modelled prey preferences. J. Wildl. Manage. 71, 1567–1575. doi: 10.2193/2006-264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, M. W., Jedrzejewski, W., and Jedrzejewska, B. (2012). Prey preferences of the tiger Panthera tigris. J. Zool. 286, 221–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2011.00871.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, M. W., and Kerley, G. I. H. (2005). Prey preferences of the lion (Panthera leo). J. Zool. 267, 309–322. doi: 10.1017/S0952836905007508

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, M. W., Lyngdoh, S., and Habib, B. (2014). Diet and prey preferences of dholes (Cuon alpinus): dietary competition within Asia's apex predator guild. J. Zool. 294, 255–266. doi: 10.1111/jzo.12171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, M. W., O'Brien, J., and Kerley, G. I. H. (2007b). Carrying capacity of large African predators: predictions and tests. Biol. Conserv. 139, 219–229. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2007.06.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebblewhite, M., and Pletscher, D. H. (2002). Effects of elk group size on predation by wolves. Can. J. Zool. 80, 800–809. doi: 10.1139/z02-059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hemmer, H., Kahlke, R.-D., and Vekua, A. K. (2001). The Jaguar-Panthera onca gombaszoegensis (Kretzoi, 1938)(Carnivora: Felidae) in the late lower pleistocene of Akhalkalaki (south Georgia; Transcaucasia) and its evolutionary and ecological significance. Geobios 34, 475–486. doi: 10.1016/S0016-6995(01)80011-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernandez, C. G. E. (2006). Dieta, uso de Hábitat y Patrones de Activdad del Puma (Puma concolor) y el Jaguar (Panthera onca) en La Selva. Guatemala: Universidad de San Carlos de Guatemala.

Google Scholar

Hernández-SaintMartín, A. D., Rosas-Rosas, O. C., Palacio-Núñez, J., Tarango-Arambula, L. A., Clemente-Sanchez, F., and Hoogesteijn, A. L. (2015). Food habits of jaguar and puma in a protected area and adjacent fragmented landscape of northeastern Mexico. Nat. Areas J. 35, 308–317. doi: 10.3375/043.035.0213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoogesteijn, A. L., and Mondolfi, E. (1996). Body mass and skull measurements in four jaguar populations and observations on their prey base. Bull. Fla. Mus. Nat. Hist. 39, 1952–1219.

Hoogesteijn, R., and Hoogesteijn, A. (2008). Conflicts between cattle ranching and large predators in Venezuela: could use of water buffalo facilitate felid conservation? Oryx 42, 132–138. doi: 10.1017/S0030605308001105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoogesteijn, R., Hoogesteijn, A., and Mondolfi, E. (1993). Jaguar predation and conservation: cattle mortality caused by felines on three ranches in the Venezuelan Llanos. Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 65, 391–407.

Google Scholar

Iriarte, J. A., Franklin, W. L., Johnson, W. E., and Redford, K. H. (1990). Biogeographic variation of food habits and body size of the american puma. Oecologia 85, 185–190. doi: 10.1007/BF00319400

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, J. (1974). Quantitative measurement of food selection - a modification of the forage ratio and Ivlev's electivity index. Oecologia 14, 413–417. doi: 10.1007/BF00384581

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jêdrzejewski, W., Cerda, H., Viloria, A., and Schmidt, K. (2014). Predatory behavior and kill rate of a female jaguar (Panthera onca) on cattle. Mammalia 78, 235–238. doi: 10.1515/mammalia-2012-0113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jooste, E., Hayward, M., Pitman, R., and Swanepoel, L. H. (2013). Effect of prey mass and selection on predator carrying capacity estimates. Eur. J. Wildl. Res. 59, 487–494. doi: 10.1007/s10344-013-0696-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorgenson, J. P., and Redford, K. H. (1993). Humans and big cats as predators in the Neotropics. Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 65, 367–390.

Karanth, K. U., Nichols, J. D., Kumar, N. S., and Link, W. A., and Hines, J. E. (2004). Tigers and their prey: predicting carnivore densities from prey abundance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 4854–4858. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0306210101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiltie, R. A. (1984). Size ratios among sympatric neotropical cats. Oecologia 61, 411–416. doi: 10.1007/BF00379644

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuriowa, A., and Ascorra, C. (2002). Dieta y Densidad de Posibles Presas de Jaguar en las Inmediaciones de la Zona de Reserva Tambopata-Candamo, Perú. México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Wildlife Conservation Society y Fondo de Cultura Económica.

Kurten, B. (1973). Pleistocene jaguars of North America. Comment. Biol. Soc. Sri. Fennica 62, 1–23.

Kurtén, B., and Anderson, E. (1980). Pleistocene Mammals of North America. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.

Leite, M. R. P., and Galvão, F. (2002). El Jaguar, el Puma y el Hombre en Tres Áreas Protegidas del Bosque Atlántico Costero de Paraná, Brasil. México: Fondo de Cultura Económica, Universidad Autónoma de México, Wildlife Conservation Society.

Google Scholar

López González, C. A., and Lorenzana Piña, G. (2002). Carrion use by jaguars (Panthera onca) in Sonora, Mexico. Mammalia 66, 603–605.

Google Scholar

López Gonzalez, C. A., and Miller, B. J. (2002). Do jaguars (Panthera onca) depend on large prey? West. North Am. Nat. 62, 218–222.

Google Scholar

Lyngdoh, S., Shrotriya, S., Goyal, S. P., Clements, H., Hayward, M. W., and Habib, B. (2014). Prey preferences of the snow leopard (Panthera uncia): regional diet specificity holds global significance for conservation. PLoS ONE 9:e88349. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, S. K., Smith, F. A., and Brown, J. H. (2004). Of mice, mastodons and men: human-mediated extinctions on four continents. Evol. Ecol. Res. 6, 339–358.

Google Scholar

Martínez-Gutiérrez, P. G., Palomares, F., and Fernández, N. (2015). Predator identification methods in diet studies: uncertain assignment produces biased results? Ecography 38, 922–929. doi: 10.1111/ecog.01040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Márquez, I. J. (2009). Disponibilidad, Uso de Habitat y Estado de Salud del Jaguar (Panthera onca) en Los Parques Nacionales Laguna del Tigre y Sierra de Lacandón. Consejo Nocioal de Ciencia y Tecnologia, Guatemala.

Google Scholar

Mayer, J. J., and Wetzel, R. M. (1987). Tayassu pecari. Mamm. Species 12, 1–7. doi: 10.2307/3503865

CrossRef Full Text

McBride, R., Giordano, A., and Ballard, W. B. (2010). Note on the winter diet of jaguars Panthera onca in the paraguyan transitional chaco. Bellbird 4, 1–4.

Meachen-Samuels, J., and Van Valkenburgh, B. (2009a). Craniodental indicators of prey size preference in the Felidae. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 96, 784–799. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2008.01169.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meachen-Samuels, J., and Van Valkenburgh, B. (2009b). Forelimb indicators of prey-size preference in the Felidae. J. Morphol. 270, 729–744. doi: 10.1002/jmor.10712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meachen, J. A., Janowicz, A. C., Avery, J. E., and Sadlier, R. W. (2014). Ecological changes in coyotes (canis latrans) in response to the Ice Age megafaunal extinctions. PLoS ONE 9:e116041. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0116041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michalski, F., Boulhosa, R. L. P., and Peres, C. A. (2006). Human-wildlife conflicts in a fragmented Amazonian forest landscape: determinants of large felid depredation on livestock. Anim. Conserv. 9, 179–190. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-1795.2006.00025.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mills, M. G. L. (1992). A Comparison of Methods Used to Study Food Habits of Large African Carnivores. London: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Mol, D., Logchem, W. V., and Vos, J. D. (2011). New record of the european jaguar, Panthera onca gombaszoegensis (Kretzoi, 1938), from the Plio-Pleistocene of Langenboom (The Netherlands). Cainozoic Res. 8, 35–40.

Google Scholar

Mondolfi, E., and Hoogesteijn, R. (1986). Notes on the Biology and Status of the Jaguar in Venezuela. Washington, DC.: National Wildlife Federation.

Moreno, R. S., Kays, R. W., and Samudio, R. (2006). Competitive release in diets of ocelot (Leopardus pardalis) and puma (Puma concolor) after jaguar (Panthera onca) decline. J. Mammal. 87, 808–816. doi: 10.1644/05-MAMM-A-360R2.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno Ruiz, R. S. (2006). Parámetros Poblacionales y Aspectos Ecológicos de los Felinos y sus Presas en Cana, Parque Nacional Darien, Panamá. Panamá: Universidad Nacional de Panamá.

Novack, A. J., Main, M. B., Sunquist, M. E., and Labisky, R. F. (2005). Foraging ecology of jaguar (Panthera onca) and puma (Puma concolor) in hunted and non-hunted sites within the maya biosphere reserve, Guatemala. J. Zool. 267, 167–178. doi: 10.1017/S0952836905007338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowak, R. M. (1999). Walker's Mammals of the World. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

Nowell, K., and Jackson, P. (1996). Wild Cats: Status, Survey and Conservation Action Plan. Gland: IUCN/Species Survival Commission Cat Specialist Group.

Google Scholar

Núñez, R., Miller, B., and Lindzey, F. (2000). Food habits of jaguars and pumas in Jalisco, Mexico. J. Zool. 252, 373–379. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2000.tb00632.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olmos, F. (1993). Notes on the food habits of Brazilian “Caatinga” carnivores. Mammalia 57, 126–130.

Google Scholar

Owen, P. R., Bell, C. J., and Mead, E. M. (2000). Fossils, diet, and conservation of black-footed ferrets (Mustela nigripes). J. Mammal. 81, 422–433. doi: 10.1093/jmammal/81.2.422

CrossRef Full Text

Palmeira, F. B. L., and Barrella, W. (2007). Conflitos causados pela predação de rebanhos domésticos por grandes felinos em comunidades quilombolas na Mata Atlântica. Biota Neotrop. 7, 119–128. doi: 10.1590/S1676-06032007000100017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmeira, F. B. L., Crawshaw, P. G., Haddad, C. M., Ferraz, K. M. P. M. B., and Verdade, L. M. (2008). Cattle depredation by puma (Puma concolor) and jaguar (Panthera onca) in central-western Brazil. Biol. Conserv. 141, 118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2007.09.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pardi, M. I., and Smith, F. A. (2015). Biotic responses of canids to the terminal Pleistocene megafauna extinction. Ecography. doi: 10.1111/ecog.01596. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peetz, A., Norconk, M. A., and Kinzey, W. G. (1992). Predation by jaguar on howler monkeys (Alouatta seniculus) in Venezuela. Am. J. Primat. 28, 223–228. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350280307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perili, M. L. L. (2009). Ecologia Alimentar da Onça-pintada (Panthera onca) na Região sul do Pantanal Utilizando Análise de Fezes – uma Comparação com o Método Direto de Rádio-telemetria GPS. Campo Grande: Federal University of Mato Grosso do Sul.

Perovic, P. G. (2002). Conservación del Jaguar en el Noroeste de Argentina. Conservación del jaguar en el noroeste de Argentina. México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Wildlife Conservation Society y Fondo de Cultura Económica.

Perry, R. (1970). The World of the Jaguar. New York, NY: Taplinger Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Polisar, J., Maxit, I., Scognamillo, D., Farrell, L., Sunquist, M. E., and Eisenberg, J. F. (2003). Jaguars, pumas, their prey base, and cattle ranching: ecological interpretations of a management problem. Biol. Conserv. 109, 297–310. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3207(02)00157-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porfirio, G. E. O. (2009). Ecologia Alimentar da Onça-Pintada (Panthera onca) na Sub-Regiao do Pantanal de Miranda, M. S. Brazil. Matto Grosso.

Google Scholar

Quigley, H., Hoogesteijn, R., Hoogesteijn, A., Foster, R., Payan, E., Corrales, D., et al. (2015). Observations and preliminary testing of jaguar depredation reduction techniques in and between core jaguar populations. Parks 21, 1–10. doi: 10.2305/IUCN.CH.2014.PARKS-21-1HQ.en

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quigley, H. B., and Crawshaw, P. G. (1992). A conservation plan for the jaguar Panthera onca in the Pantanal region of Brazil. Biol. Conserv. 61, 149–157. doi: 10.1016/0006-3207(92)91111-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Development Team (2008). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Rabinowitz, A. (1986). Jaguar predation on domestic livestock in Belize. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 14, 170–174.

Google Scholar

Rabinowitz, A., and Nottingham, B. G. (1986). Ecology and behaviour of the jaguar (Panthera onca) in Belize, Central America. J. Zool. 210, 149–159. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1986.tb03627.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Railsback, S. F., and Harvey, B. C. (2013). Trait-mediated trophic interactions: is foraging theory keeping up? TREE 28, 119–125. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.08.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramalho, E. (2006). Uso do Habitat e Dieta da Onça-Pintada (Panthera onca) em Uma Area de Várzea, Reserva de Desenvolvimento Sustentável Mamirauá, Amazônia Central, Brasil. Manaus: Instituto Nacional de Pesquisa da Amazônia.

Redford, K. H. (1992). The empty forest. Bioscience 42, 412–422. doi: 10.2307/1311860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosas-Rosas, O. C., Bender, L. C., and Valdez, R. (2008). Jaguar and puma predation on cattle calves in northeastern Sonora, Mexico. Rangeland Ecol. Manag. 61, 554–560. doi: 10.2111/08-038.1

CrossRef Full Text

Rowcliffe, J. M., Carbone, C., Jansen, P. A., Kays, R., and Kranstauber, B. (2011). Quantifying the sensitivity of camera traps: an adapted distance sampling approach. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2, 464–476. doi: 10.1111/j.2041-210x.2011.00094.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowcliffe, J. M., Field, J., Turvey, S. T., and Carbone, C. (2008). Estimating animal density using camera traps without the need for individual recognition. J. Appl. Ecol. 45, 1228–1236. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2664.2008.01473.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rueda, P., Mendoza, G., Martínez, D., and Rosas-Rosas, O. C. (2013). Determination of the jaguar (Panthera onca) and puma (Puma concolor) diet in a tropical forest in San Luis Potosi, Mexico. J. Appl. Anim. Res. 41, 484–489. doi: 10.1080/09712119.2013.787362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salera, G., Portelinha, T. C. G., and Malvasio, A. (2009). Predation on Adult Females of Podocnemis Expansa Schweigger (Testudines, Podocnemididae) by Panthera onca Linnaeus (Carnivora, Felidae), in Tocantins State. Biota Neotropica 9. Available online at: http://www.biotaneotropica.org.br/v9n3/en/abstract?short-communication+bn00709032009

Schaller, G. B. (1983). Mammals and their biomass on a Brazilian ranch. Arq. Zool. 31, 1–36. doi: 10.11606/issn.2176-7793.v31i1p1-36

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaller, G. B., and Vasconcelos, J. (1978). Jaguar predation on capybara. Z. Saugetierkd. 43, 296–301.

Google Scholar

Scognamillo, D., Maxit, I., Sunquist, M. E., and Polisar, J. (2003). Coexistence of jaguar (Panthera onca) and puma (Puma concolor) in a mosaic landscape in the Venezuelan llanos. J. Zool. 259, 269–279. doi: 10.1017/S0952836902003230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seymour, K. L. (1989). Panthera onca. Mamm. Species 340, 1–9. doi: 10.2307/3504096

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silveira, L. (2004). Ecologia Comparada e Conservação da Onça-Pintada (Panthera onca) e Onça-parda (Puma concolor), no Cerrado e Pantanal. Unpublished PhD thesis, Universidade de Brasilia.

Sollmann, R. (2011). Ecology and Conservation of the Jaguar (Panthera onca) in the Cerrado Grasslands of Central Brazil. Unpublished PhD thesis, Berlin, Freie Universität Berlin.

Sollmann, R., Betsch, J., Furtado, M., Hofer, H., Jacomo, A. T. A., Palomares, F., et al. (2013). Note on the diet of the jaguar in central Brazil. Eur. J. Wildl. Res. 59, 445–448. doi: 10.1007/s10344-013-0708-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soto-Shoender, J. R., and Giuliano, W. M. (2011). Predation on livestock by large carnivores in the tropical lowlands of Guatemala. Oryx 45, 561–568. doi: 10.1017/S0030605310001845

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenweg, R., Gillingham, M., Parker, K., and Heard, D. C. (2015). Considering sampling approaches when determining carnivore diets: the importance of where, how, and when scats are collected. Mamm. Res. 60, 207–216. doi: 10.1007/s13364-015-0222-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinmetz, R., Seuaturien, N., and Chutipong, W. (2013). Tigers, leopards, and dholes in a half-empty forest: assessing species interactions in a guild of threatened carnivores. Biol. Conserv. 163, 68–78. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2012.12.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sunquist, M. (2002). Historia de la Investigación Sobre el Jaguar en el Continente Americano. México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Wildlife Conservation Society y Fondo de Cultura Económica.

Sunquist, M., and Sunquist, F. (2002). Wild Cats of the World. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Taber, A. B., Novaro, A. J., Neris, N., and Colman, F. H. (1997). The food habits of sympatric jaguar and puma in the Paraguayan Chaco. Biotropica 29, 204–213. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.1997.tb00025.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tedford, R. H., Wang, X., Van Valkenburgh, B., and Wayne, R. K. (2004). “Evolutarionary history, molecular systematics and evolutionary ecology of wild canids,” in Biology and Conservation of Wild Canids, eds D. W. Macdonald and C. Sillero-Zubiri (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press).

Trovati, R. G., Campos, C., and Brito, B. (2008). Nota sobre convergência e divergência alimentar de canídeos e felídeos (Mamalia: Carnivora) simpátricos no Cerrado brasileiro. Neotrop. Biol. Conserv. 3, 95–100.

Google Scholar

Van Valkenburgh, B., Hayward, M. W., Ripple, W. J., Meloro, C., and Roth, V. L. (2015). The impact of large terrestrial carnivores on Pleistocene ecosystems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1502554112. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Valkenburgh, B., and Hertel, F. (1998). The decline of North American Predators during the Late Pleistocene. The decline of North American Predators during the Late Pleistocene. Springfield, IL: Illinois State Museum Scientific Papers.

Verdade, L. M., Palomares, F., do Couto, H. T. Z., and Polizel, J. L. (2015). Recent land-use changes and the expansion of an exotic potential prey: a possible redemption for Atlantic forest jaguars? Anim. Conserv. doi: 10.1111/acv.12221. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veríssimo, D., Jones, D., Chaverri, R., and Meyer, S. R. (2012). Jaguar Panthera onca predation of marine turtles: conflict between flagship species in Tortuguero, Costa Rica. Oryx 46, 340–347. doi: 10.1017/S0030605311001487

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weckel, M., Giuliano, W. M., and Silver, S. C. (2006a). Cockscomb revisited: jaguar diet in the Cockscomb Basin Wildlife Sanctuary, Belize. Biotropica 38, 687–690. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.2006.00190.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weckel, M., Giuliano, W. M., and Silver, S. C. (2006b). Jaguar (Panthera onca) feeding ecology: distribution of predator and prey through time and space. J. Zool. 270, 25–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00106.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkie, D. S., Bennett, E. L., Peres, C. A., and Cunningham, A. A. (2011). The empty forest revisited. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1223, 120–128. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05908.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodroffe, R., Lindsey, P. A., Romanach, S. S., and ole Ranah, S. M. K. (2007). African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) can subsist on small prey: implications for conservation. J. Mammal. 88, 181–193. doi: 10.1644/05-MAMM-A-405R1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yanez, J. L., Crdenas, J. C., Gezelle, P., and Jaksic, F. M. (1986). Food habits of the southermost mountain lions (Felis concolor) in South America: natural versus livestocked ranges. J. Mammal. 67, 604–606. doi: 10.2307/1381301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: predator-prey interactions, apex predator, optimal foraging theory, Pleistocene megafaunal extinction, capybara, giant anteater, accessible prey, preferred prey weight range

Citation: Hayward MW, Kamler JF, Montgomery RA, Newlove A, Rostro-García S, Sales LP and Van Valkenburgh B (2016) Prey Preferences of the Jaguar Panthera onca Reflect the Post-Pleistocene Demise of Large Prey. Front. Ecol. Evol. 3:148. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2015.00148

Received: 07 October 2015; Accepted: 11 December 2015;
Published: 25 January 2016.

Edited by:

Ricardo Baldi, Centro Nacional Patagónico (CENPAT - CONICET), Argentina

Reviewed by:

Adrian Schiavini, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas, Argentina
Carlos Alberto Lopez Gonzalez, Universidad Autonoma de Queretaro, Mexico

Copyright © 2016 Hayward, Kamler, Montgomery, Newlove, Rostro-García, Sales and Van Valkenburgh. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Matt W. Hayward, m.hayward@bangor.ac.uk

Download