Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 01 September 2015
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 7 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00156

The importance of neighborhood ecological assets in community dwelling old people aging outcomes: A study in Northern Portugal

Alice M. Bastos1* Carla G. Faria1 Emília Moreira1 Diana Morais1 José M. Melo-de-Carvalho1 M. Constança Paul2
  • 1Instituto Politécnico de Viana do Castelo – Escola Superior de Educação, Unidade de Investigação e Formação sobre Adultos e Idosos, Viana do Castelo, Portugal
  • 2Instituto de Ciências Biomédicas Abel Salazar, Unidade de Investigação e Formação sobre Adultos e Idosos, Universidade do Porto, Porto, Portugal

Human development is a bidirectional, person-context relational process, but scarce evidence is available about the relation between the individual variability across the life-span and the neighborhood ecological assets. Therefore, it is important that research focus not only on personal characteristics but on ecological assets as well. This way this study aims to analyze the association between neighborhood ecological assets categorized into four dimensions: human, physical or institutional, social or collective activity, accessibility, and the individual functioning. A 3% sample of residents aged 65 years and older in two downtown and three uptown parishes stratified by age and sex was interviewed at home using a protocol that included the Portuguese version of the Barthel Index in basic activities of daily living (BADL), the Lawton Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale (IADL), the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), and the Geriatric Depression Scale-15 items (GDS) for evaluating functionality, cognitive performance, and depression. The 162 participants were aged on average 75 years (sd = 7.0), 54% were women and 90% had less than 7 years of education. The majority of participants were independent in BADL (M = 90; sd = 17.7) and moderately dependent in IADL (M = 13, sd = 6.0), 20% showed cognitive impairment and a mean score of 8 (sd = 2.1) in GDS-15. After controlling for the effect of socio-demographic characteristics, functionality, and cognitive performance decreases in persons with worst outdoor mobility. On the other hand depressive symptoms are less common as the number of recreation opportunities, namely associative groups (cultural, educative, professional), increases. These results suggest that aging policies and practices must be ecologically embedded.

Introduction

Currently, human aging is seen as one of the highest challenges of the XXI century. In the western world, we won about 33 years in average life expectancy, which rose from about 46 years, in 1900, to about 79 years in the end of the century (Walker, 2010; Hooyman and Kiyak, 2011). However, longevity advances are not being accompanied by a similar transformation in societal terms, leading Baltes and Smith (2003) to question our capacity to conciliate longevity and dignity, especially in fourth age. Furthermore, considering the theoretical and methodological knowledge advance, in the Social and Behavioral Sciences, in terms of the life course perspective, aging is a lifelong process, situated in an historical space and time (Elder, 1974; Elder and Shanahan, 1998). Also, according to the life-span developmental psychology (Baltes, 1987, 1997; Baltes et al., 2006), development is not completed at adulthood (maturity); rather ontogenesis extends across life span. Thus, aging is a dynamic process of gains and losses, which follows from the biological and cultural architecture.

In the present study, we focused on the following aging outcomes: functional dependency, cognitive functioning, and depression. These outcomes have mostly been analyzed under a socio-demographic characteristics perspective. Functional dependency has been tightly associated to age, gender, education, and marital status. This way, dependent old people are mainly women, widows, people living alone and with low education (Murtagh and Hubert, 2004; Formiga et al., 2007; Santos et al., 2007; Espigares et al., 2008; Torres et al., 2009; Espigares and Torres, 2010; Espelt et al., 2010; Nunes et al., 2010). Functional dependency prevalence takes different shapes, as it refers to basic activities of daily living (BADL) or to instrumental activities of daily living (IADL). The majority of old people is independent in BADL (Costa et al., 2006; Schneider et al., 2008; Nakatani et al., 2009; Espigares and Torres, 2010) but those who are 70 or older are more prone to be dependent in IADL, and if they are older than 80 years, they might be dependent on both IADL and BADL (Nunes et al., 2010). Research has shown that both the prevalence and the rate of dependency increase with age (Espigares et al., 2008; Espelt et al., 2010) and that dependency is associated with several factors as cognitive impairment, depression, neurological diseases, back, neck and shoulders pain, osteoarthritis, chronic back problems, osteoporosis, more medication, lower economic resources (Murtagh and Hubert, 2004; Formiga et al., 2007; Santos et al., 2007; Schneider et al., 2008; Nunes et al., 2010). On the other hand age is not the most important factor for cognitive impairment measured by the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE; Green et al., 2008; Karlamangla et al., 2009), but it has been found a global decline in cognitive functioning with age (Hooren et al., 2007; Bourne et al., 2010; Martínez-Vidal et al., 2011). Beyond age, other factors have been associated to cognitive decline in old people, as gender, marital status, education, social support network, socioeconomic status, and lifestyle (Hooren et al., 2007; Karlamangla et al., 2009). In general, a worse cognitive functioning is present in men, old people living alone or institutionalized, with a small social network confined to family, with little physical activity and having scarce socioeconomic and educational resources (Hooren et al., 2007; Green et al., 2008; Lindwall et al., 2008; Karlamangla et al., 2009; Oliveira et al., 2011). Although it is consensual that age brings a decrease in cognitive functioning, cognitive resources that are built in childhood and adulthood can prevent it or delay it (Yount, 2008). The other aging outcome considered in this study is depression, considered the most prevalent psychopathological disorder in old age (Seby et al., 2011). Depression has been related to several sociodemographic variables, such as age, gender, marital status, socioeconomic status, and education. Higher levels of depression occur mostly in old people aged 70 or older (Beekman et al., 2002; Chou and Chi, 2005; García-Peña et al., 2008), in old women (Copeland et al., 2004; McCusker et al., 2005; Tsai et al., 2005; García-Peña et al., 2008; Weele et al., 2009; Oliveira et al., 2011; Seby et al., 2011), old people living alone (Tsai et al., 2005; Oliveira et al., 2011; Seby et al., 2011), and old people with lower education and low socioeconomic resources (Tsai et al., 2005; Perrino et al., 2009; Akyol et al., 2010; Engmann, 2011). Besides the sociodemographic profile, other risk factors have been identified, such as previous history of depression, lack of social support perception, narrow social network, one or more physical diseases, adverse life events, poor quality of life perception, and lack of a regular spiritual practice (Copeland et al., 2004; Chou and Chi, 2005; Koizumi et al., 2005; McCusker et al., 2005; Tsai et al., 2005; Wilson et al., 2007; García-Peña et al., 2008; Akyol et al., 2010; Seby et al., 2011). The three analyzed dimensions in the present study – functionality, cognitive functioning, and depression – have a central role on aging, since they interact and influence each other, acting as roots for the successful or unsuccessful aging.

In a developmental systems perspective, Lerner (2002, 2012) has developed a series of studies about the positive development of young adults, highlighting the role of the “ecological assets,” wherein “assets can be conceived of within individuals, in the physical space, and emerging in the dynamic between the two” (Theokas and Lerner, 2006, p. 62). Regarding the Study of Positive Youth Development, these authors (2006) proposed four classes of observed ecological assets to organize the actual resources and opportunities in the environments – human, physical or institutional, collective activity, and accessibility. The concept definition, according to the above mentioned authors are as follows: (1) Human resources are defined as the strengths, skills, talents, and facilities of people and as instantiated by the roles they have. The characteristics, activities, and behaviors of individuals provide a manifestation of the social norms of a particular context; (2) Physical or institutional resources are intended to document opportunities for learning, recreation, and engagement with individuals and the physical world around oneself and, as well, for providing routines and structure for individuals. At the neighborhood level, the presence of libraries, community centers, and cultural experiences that are within walking distance, or available through public transportation may increase their use and thus their potential benefits; (3) Collective activity is intended to document mutual engagement between community members, parents, youth, schools personnel, and institutions of society. These organizations, groups, or mutual activities represent the combined efforts and actions of different sets of individuals, documenting ties and networks of a community’s associational life and the climate of the key context of development; (4) Accessibility intents to document the ability of residents to partake of human resources and resource opportunities in the context. Accessibility can be conceptualized and operationalized in multiple ways: (a) documents physical ease of access and can refer to the transportation capacity and hours of operation of local businesses, infrastructure, or cultural institutions in a local community; (b) can refer to the potential of youth to interact with the adults in the setting or what is the ratio of adults to children in a given neighborhood or how long has a family lived in a neighborhood; (c) can refer also to the safety of the physical environment and free of dangers.

Given these results, what happens in the studies about aging? Aging is a process that implies a series of alterations in the biological, psychological, and social domains. The multiple profiles that can result from the several combinations occurring among these alterations make aging a multifaceted process, shaped by previous development and, in which the individual has a proactive role, in that, in the interaction with the environment, he can become his own aging’s author. This way, different life styles in different environments result in different aging processes, which approach or distance themselves from active or successful aging. Researches on Environmental Gerontology (Wahl et al., 2012) made a first theoretical approach to the works of Lawton and the lifespan developmental perspective in a conceptual article that aims to guide future research agenda and propose theoretical propositions to an integrative model of Aging Well. Nonetheless, these theoretical propositions were only discussed in theoretical terms and not empirically tested. According to Dannefer (2003, 2009), over the course of life, people accumulate advantages or disadvantages that may optimize or limit the aging process.

In this context, we may ask: can the life-span developmental perspective using the methodology adopted by Theokas and Lerner (2006) contribute to a better understanding of aging in place? The aim of this study is to demonstrate that ecological assets are associated with aging outcomes, namely functionality, cognitive functioning, and depression, in an urban environment.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sample size was estimated to be 3% of the resident population aged 65 years and older living in the five parishes/inner city of Viana do Castelo based on estimates of inter-census population for 2006, stratified by age and sex. The IPVC scientific committee approved the study and all persons willing to participate gave informed consent. Persons were interviewed at home by a trained researcher and a stakeholder (usually the priest or the president of the parish committee) in each parish indicated the first person to be interviewed. This person in turn pointed × persons, usually ranging from 4 to 10, and from them we randomly selected 50% to initiate the process. This can be viewed as cluster sampling with clusters with variable size.

Data Collection

The collected information included socio-demographic characteristics, person’s daily functioning and household and neighborhood relationships. The Lawton instrumental activities of daily living scale (IADL-Lawton) was used to assess functionality, the MMSE for cognitive performance and the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) for depression. Ecological assets by parish included physical facilities (Elder and health facilities, nursing homes and culture/sports/recreation facilities among others) and social resources (Sports/cultural, health support associations, educative associations, and local/professional groups) based on a social diagnostic report of the City Council/Interparish committee (Relatório Social, 2008). Information focusing the resources and social necessities of each selected parish was analyzed. Accessibility indicators were based on the inter-census population estimates: dependency index (persons aged 65 years or more/persons aged 15–64 years), longevity index (proportion of persons 85 years or more among those aged at least 65 years), and characteristics of the public transport network based.

Data Analysis

After data description, involving personal, micro, and macro-environmental characteristics, downtown and uptown residents were compared using non-parametric tests, the chi-square for categorical variables and the Kruskal–Wallis for ordinal/discrete variables. Hierarchical regression models were used to analyze predictors of functionality (IADL-Lawton), cognition (MMSE), and depression (GDS). The first block included socio-demographic characteristics; a second block included household and neighborhood assets that were entered in the model using a stepwise procedure; in the same way a third and fourth blocks included ecological assets, namely physical and social community resources and accessibility. Since the distribution of the MMSE scores did not follow the normal distribution we used a transformed variable – the square root of the number of errors [√(30 – score in MMSE = number of errors; Jacquin-Gada et al., 1997)].

Results

A total of 162 persons were interviewed in the five parishes (n = 85 in downtown and n = 77 in uptown) and half of them are aged between 65 and 74 years old (Table 1). Most of them are women (54.3%). About 9.3% are illiterate, most are married (46.9%) or widower (40.1%). Overall 56.8% had blue collar occupations during their professional life, being this proportion higher among the uptown residents comparing to downtown (p = 0.018). Participants lived in the same parish for 63 years (sd = 31.5) and the majority considered to have good household conditions and a good relationship with neighbors (80.9%), being this proportion even higher among the downtown residents (p = 0.035). Overall 28.4% of participants report difficulties in outdoor mobility.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Human resources and microenvironment accessibility resources.

The MMSE scores were associated with age (p < 0.001) and there were significant differences between the youngest and oldest groups; the IADL-Lawton score increased with age and the eldest had a higher score than the remainder. The GDS score were associated with age (p < 0.05) and there were significant differences between the two younger groups (65–74 vs. 75–84).

Table 2 shows the physical, social, and accessibility resources by each parish in downtown and uptown. Downtown parishes have more physical resources for childhood/ adolescence, for education and more culture/sports and recreation facilities, as well as more sports and cultural groups and local/professional groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Macroenvironmental physical, social, and accessibility resources by parish.

Concerning accessibility, downtown parishes have a higher number of Bus Transportation in their territory. Minimum frequency of bus lines is lower in a downtown and in an uptown parish. Both dependency and longevity indexes are higher in downtown parishes.

The hierarchical regression models of functionality (IADL-Lawton), cognitive performance (MMSE) and depression (GDS) including a first set of personal characteristics and a second set of ecological assets (stepwise inclusion) indicated that 37% of the variability in functionality on the IADL-Lawton is explained by socio-demographic characteristics (age and marital state), household/neighborhood characteristics (outdoor mobility) and accessibility (dependency index and longevity index; Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Multiple regression models* for functionality, cognitive performance, and depression including micro and macro environmental resources.

Having difficulties in outdoor mobility (p < 0.001) and living in a parish with a lower dependency index (p < 0.05) and a higher longevity index (p < 0.01) is related to a higher score in IADL-Lawton (indicating worse levels in functionality), after controlling for the effect of socio-demographic characteristics.

Overall 32% of the variance in the MMSE (number of errors) is explained by the inclusion of socio-demographic characteristics (age and education), household and neighborhood characteristics (outdoor mobility) and accessibility (dependency index). Having difficulties in outdoor mobility (p < 0.05) and living in a parish with a lower dependency index (p < 0.05) is related to a higher number or errors in MMSE, after controlling for the effect of socio-demographic characteristics.

A total of 18% of the variance in the GDS is explained by inclusion of socio-demographic characteristics (marital state) and social resources. After controlling for demographic characteristics, depression decreases with the number of social resources (p < 0.05).

Discussion

Considering the aim of this study we may conclude that cognition is more determined by sociodemographic characteristics rather than ecological assets, while variability in functionality is more linked to ecological assets, namely outdoor mobility and longevity index. As far as depression is concerned, the availability of social resources seems to be the most important ecological asset for ameliorating depression.

With respect to sociodemographic characteristics, there are more women in all age groups, and this number increases as the age group also increases. Illiteracy and lower education are common, mainly, among older elderly, while medium and high education (4–10 years) are typical among younger elderly. The number of widows increases with age, while the number of married elderly decreases. In relation to occupations, most elderly were retired including all elderly aged 85 or older. This way, the sociodemographic profile observed in this sample meets the profile usually described in gerontological literature, a predominance of women, married or widowed persons, retired with no occupation and with low education (Araújo et al., 2007; Espigares et al., 2008; Torres et al., 2010).

The capacity to perform BADL, assessed by Barthel Index, was significantly different among age groups, wherein functional decline was more marked after 85 years of age. This result supports literature that presents a significant increase in dependency for BADL from the fourth age. Santos et al. (2007) found that the incapacity index, measured by Barthel Index, increased from 23,6%, between 60 and 69 years, to 35% between 70 and 79 years, and to 47,7% from 80 years. This steepest functional decline from 85 years old on can be explained, partially, by the fact that most elderly participants of this study are inactive. Furthermore, the literature in the field has shown that the increase of dependency in BADL with age stems from physiological changes and pathologies typical of the aging process (Murtagh and Hubert, 2004; Torres et al., 2010).

In our study, we found significant differences in functionality to IADL, measured by Lawton Index, among age groups. Specifically, there was a more marked decline from 80 years, being more expressive after 85 years old. This data matches those presented in literature which associates the increase in dependency to the increase in age and which shows that the functional decline in IADL is previous to the functional decline in BADL. So, relatively to IADL measured by Lawton Index, Torres et al. (2010) found statistically significant differences according to age group, wherein the elderly with more than 77 years old were more dependent than those with less than 70 years. In the same direction, Costa et al. (2006) and Nakatani et al. (2009) found that, although most elderly participants in their study were independent in BADL, most of them also presented some dependence in IADL. Likewise Nunes et al. (2010) observed that while the functional decline in BADL occurred, mainly, in elderly aged from 80, the functional decline in IADL occurred in elderly aged from 70.

Even in relation to functionality in IADL, the regression analysis showed the 15% of variability is explained by sociodemographic characteristics. Other studies also found that advanced age and lower education are independent factors related to functional dependency (Formiga et al., 2007; Espigares et al., 2008). However, in our study, when the ecological assets were included in the regression analysis, we found that along with sociodemographic characteristics, outdoor mobility explains 32% of the functionality in IADL variability, and when the longevity index is included, this model increases the explanation to 37%. These results suggest that ecological assets should be considered both in the assessment and intervention programs that aim to prevent functional decline.

In what concerns cognition, measured the MMSE, we found statistically significant differences according to age. Specifically, cognitive decline is higher among those participants who are 80 years or older, being more expressive after 85 years. This result fits with several studies that show that there is a general cognitive decline with age, even though age is not the referred as the main factor (Hooren et al., 2007; Bourne et al., 2010; Martínez-Vidal et al., 2011; Oliveira et al., 2011). This cognitive decline can be explained, to a certain point, by the high rate of illiteracy and of lower education in the elderly aged 85 or more, and also by the high number of retired and inactive elderly within this age group. Therefore, the literature has shown, the higher the education level the better the cognitive functioning. This way, Hooren et al. (2007) found that people with higher education had a better cognitive performance, suggesting that this may be due to having greater reserve capacity. Also Bosma et al. (2003) showed that lower education and little cognitively stimulating jobs contributed to a higher cognitive decline. Beyond education and activity, other variables have been associated with cognitive functioning. Specifically, elderly receiving more social support (Bourne et al., 2010), with larger social networks (Holtzman et al., 2004; Green et al., 2008) and married (van Gelder et al., 2006; Karlamangla et al., 2009) had lower cognitive decline. In this study, beyond the 26% of variability in cognitive performance explained by age and education, household and neighborhood indicators, namely outdoor mobility increases the explained variability to 30%, and accessibility indicators provide a further increase to 32%. Again, ecological assets seem to play an important role in cognitive functioning and so they should be included in planned proactive actions for community dwellers.

Concerning depression, there were no significant differences according to age, which is congruent with the literature in the field (Copeland et al., 2004). However, there are higher depression rates from 80 years of age on, which are congruent with the literature showing an increasing trend in depression with age (Beekman et al., 2002; Oliveira et al., 2011). This increase may be related, in this study, to the functional and cognitive decline and to a lesser availability of social resources, diminishing the opportunity for social interaction. Moreover, in the studies of Lenze et al. (2005) and Nunes et al. (2010), depression was a risk factor for incapacity and dependency. Other studies have reported an association with other variables, such as being female (Copeland et al., 2004; McCusker et al., 2005; Tsai et al., 2005; Weele et al., 2009; Oliveira et al., 2011; Seby et al., 2011), low education and socio-economic resources (Tsai et al., 2005; Akyol et al., 2010; Engmann, 2011), living alone (Tsai et al., 2005; Oliveira et al., 2011; Seby et al., 2011), low emotional support (Koizumi et al., 2005), and suffering physical diseases (Oliveira et al., 2011). In the present study, the sociodemographic variables, namely marital status, explain 14% of the variability in depression. However, when the ecological assets are included in the regression analysis, namely social resources the variability explained increases to 18%, suggesting that as the number of leisure opportunities increases depression decreases. These results are relevant because they show that ecological assets, specifically the leisure opportunities, should be integrated in proactive interventions in this field.

Alongside with works of Lerner (Theokas and Lerner, 2006; Urban et al., 2009) regarding the extent of a youth positive development associated with ecological assets, this is the first study showing that besides the acknowledged contribution of sociodemographic variables for aging outcomes, neighborhood ecological assets may have important contributions for ameliorating outcomes such as IADL, cognitive performance and depression in community dwelling old people. Considering the aim of this study we may conclude that cognition has a stronger association with sociodemographic characteristics rather than with ecological assets, while variability in functionality is more linked to ecological assets, namely outdoor mobility and longevity index. As far as depression is concerned, the availability of social resources seems to be the most important ecological asset for ameliorating depression. Aging policies and practices should account for these aspects and be ecologically embedded.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The Associate Editor Lia Fernandes declares that, despite of having collaborated with the author M. Constança Paul, the review process was handled objectively. The other authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

The authors thank Professor M. Carolina Silva for the contribution to the design planning and statistical analysis of this study.

References

Akyol, Y., Durmus, D., Dogan, C., Bek, Y., and Canturk, F. (2010). Quality of life and level of depressive symptoms in the geriatric population. Turk. J. Rheumatol. 25, 165–173. doi: 10.5152/tjr.2010.23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Araújo, F., Ribeiro, J. L. P., Oliveira, A., and Pinto, C. (2007). Validação do índice de Barthel numa amostra de idosos não institucionalizados. Revis. Portug. Saúde Públ. 25, 59–66.

Google Scholar

Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life span developmental psychology: on the dynamics between growth and decline. Dev. Psychol. 23, 611–626. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny: selection, optimization, and compensation as foundation of developmental theory. Am. Psychol. 52, 366–380. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.4.366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltes, P., Lindenberger, U., and Staudinger, U. (2006). “Lifespan theory in developmental psychology,” in Theoretical Models of Human Development, ed. R. Lerner (New York: Wiley), 569–664.

Google Scholar

Baltes, P. B., and Smith, J. (2003). New frontiers in the future of aging: from successful aging of the young old to the dilemmas of the fourth age. Gerontology 49, 123–135. doi: 10.1159/000067946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beekman, A., Geerlings, S., Deeg, D., Smit, J., Schoever, R., Beurs, E., et al. (2002). The natural history of late-life depression. A 6-year prospective study in the community. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59, 605–611.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bosma, H., Boxtel, M., Ponds, R., Houx, P., and Jolles, J. (2003). Education and age-related cognitive decline: the contribution of mental workload. Educ. Gerontol. 29, 165–173. doi: 10.1080/10715769800300191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourne, P., Charles, C., and Warren, S. (2010). Cognitive functionality of older men in St. Catherine, Jamaica. N. Am. J. Med. Sci. 2, 150–157. doi: 10.4297/najms.2010.3150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou, K., and Chi, I. (2005). Prevalence and correlates of depression in Chinese oldest-old. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 20, 41–50. doi: 10.1002/gps.1246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Copeland, J., Beekman, A., Braam, A., Dewey, M., Delespaul, P., Fuhrer, R., et al. (2004). Depression among older people in Europe: the EURODEP studies. World Psychiatry 3, 45–49.

Google Scholar

Costa, E., Nakatani, A., and Bachion, M. (2006). Capacidade de idosos da comunidade para desenvolver atividades de vida diária e atividades instrumentais de vida diária. Acta Paul. Enferm. 19, 43–48. doi: 10.1590/S0103-21002006000100007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dannefer, D. (2003). Cumulative advantage/disadvantage and the life course: cross-fertilizing age and social science theory. J. Gerontol. 58B, S327–S337. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.6.S327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dannefer, D. (2009). Stability, homogeneity, agency: cumulative dis/advantage and problems of theory. Swiss J. Gerontol. 35, 193–210.

Google Scholar

Elder, G. H. Jr. (1974). Children of the Great Depression: Social Change in Life Experiences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Elder, G., and Shanahan, M. (1998). “The life course and human development,” in Theories of Human Development: Contemporary Perspectives, ed. R. Lerner (New York: Wiley).

Google Scholar

Engmann, B. (2011). Mild cognitive impairment in the elderly. A review of the influence of depression, possible other core symptoms, and diagnostic findings. GeroPsych 24, 71–76.

Google Scholar

Espelt, A., Font-Ribera, L., Rodriguez-Sanz, M., Artazcoz, L., Ferrando, J., Plaza, A., et al. (2010). Disability among older people in a Southern European City in 2006: trends in gender and socioeconomic inequalities. J. Women Health 19, 927–933. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2009.1608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espigares, J., and Torres, E. (2010). Ageing-related services as a key element for social cohesion. Serv. Indus. J. 30, 1263–1277. doi: 10.1080/02642060802337349

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espigares, J., Torres, E., and Muñoz, T. (2008). Population ageing, dependency and healthcare services utilisation. Estúd. Econ. Aplic. 26, 1–30.

Google Scholar

Formiga, F., Ferrer, A., Castejon, J., Olmedo, C., and Puiol, R. (2007). Risk factors for functional decline in nonagenarians: a one-year follow-up. Gerontology 53, 211–217. doi: 10.1159/000100780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Peña, C., Wagner, F., Garcia, S., Cedillo, T., Bermúdez, C., Gonzalez, J., et al. (2008). Depressive symptoms among older adults in México city. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 23, 1973–1980. doi: 10.1007/s11606-008-0799-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, A., Rebok, G., and Lyketsos, C. (2008). Influence of social network characteristics on cognition and functional status with aging. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 23, 972–978. doi: 10.1002/gps.2023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holtzman, R., Rebok, G., Saczynski, J., Kouzis, A., Doyle, K., and Eaton, W. (2004). Social network characteristics and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. J. Gerontol. 59B, 278–284. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.6.P278

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hooren, S., Valentijn, A., Bosma, H., Ponds, R., Boxtel, M., and Jolles, J. (2007). Cognitive functioning in healthy older adults aged 64-81: a cohort study into the effects of age, sex, and education. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 14, 40–54. doi: 10.1080/138255890969483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hooyman, N. R., and Kiyak, H. A. (2011). Social Gerontology: A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Boston: Pearson Education Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.

Google Scholar

Jacquin-Gada, H., Fabrigoule, C., Commenges, D., and Dartigues, J. F. (1997). A five year longitudinal sutdy of the mini-mental state examination in normal aging. Am. J. Epidemiol. 145, 498–506. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karlamangla, A., Miller-Martinez, D., Aneshensel, C., Seeman, T., Wight, R., and Chodosh, J. (2009). Trajectories of cognitive function in late life in the United States: demographic and socioeconomic predictors. Am. J. Epidemiol. 170, 331–342. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koizumi, Y., Awata, S., Kuriyama, S., Ohmori, K., Hozawa, A., Seki, T., et al. (2005). Association between social support and depression status in the elderly: results of a 1-year community-based prospective cohort study in Japan. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 59, 563–569. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2005.01415.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenze, E., Schulz, R., Martire, L., Zdaniuk, B., Glass, T., Kop, W., et al. (2005). The course of functional decline in older people with persistently elevated depressive symptoms: longitudinal findings from the cardiovascular health study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 569–575. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53202.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lerner, R. M. (2002). Concepts and Theories of Human Development, 3rd Edn. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Lerner, R. M. (2012). Essay review: developmental science: past, present, and future. Int. J. Deve. Sci. 6, 29–36.

Google Scholar

Lindwall, M., Rennemark, M., and Berggren, T. (2008). Movement in mind: the relationship of exercise with cognitive status for older adults in the Swedish National Study on Aging and Care (SNAC). Aging Ment. Health 12, 212–220. doi: 10.1080/13607860701797232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Vidal, A., Martínez, A., Pereira, M., and Martínez-Patiño, M. (2011). Effect of a combined programo f physical activity and intellectual activity in the cognitive functioning of the elderly. J. Hum. Sport Exerc. 6, 462–473. doi: 10.4100/jhse.2011.62.27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCusker, J., Cole, M., Dufouil, C., Dendukuri, N., Latimer, E., Windholz, S., et al. (2005). The prevalence and correlates of Major Depression and Minor Depression in older medical inpatients. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 1344–1353. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53404.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murtagh, K., and Hubert, H. (2004). Gender differences in physical disability among an elderly cohort. Am. J. Public Health 94, 1406–1411. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.94.8.1406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakatani, A., Silva, L., Bachion, M., and Nunes, D. (2009). Capacidade funcional em idosos na comunidade e propostas de intervenções pela equipe de saúde. Revis. Electrón. Enferm. 11, 144–150.

Google Scholar

Nunes, D., Nakatani, A., Silveira, E., Bachion, M., and Souza, M. (2010). Capacidade funcional, condições socioeconómicas e de saúde de idosos atendidos por equipes de Saúde da Família de Goiânia. Ciência Saúde Coletiva 15, 2887–2898. doi: 10.1590/S1413-81232010000600026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliveira, J., Benabarre, S., Lorente, T., Rodriguez, M., Barros, A., Quintana, C., et al. (2011). Detecting psychogeriatric problems in primary care: factors related to psychiatric symptoms in older community patients. Ment. Health Fam. Med. 8, 11–19.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Perrino, T., Brown, S., Mason, C., and Szapocznik, J. (2009). Depressive symptoms amog urban Hispanic older adults in Miami: prevalence and sociodemographic correlates. Clin. Gerontol. 32, 26–43.

Google Scholar

Relatório Social. (2008). Relatório de Diagnóstico Social. Viana do Castelo: Câmara Municipal de Viana do Castelo.

Google Scholar

Santos, K., Koszuoski, R., Dias-da-Costa, J., and Pattussi, M. (2007). Fatores associados com a incapacidade funcional em idosos do Município de Guatambu, Santa Catarina, Brasil. Cadernos Saúde Públ. 23, 2781–2788. doi: 10.1590/S0102-311X2007001100025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, R., Marcolin, D., and Dalacorte, R. (2008). Avaliação funcional de idosos. Sci. Med. (Porto Alegre) 18, 4–9.

Google Scholar

Seby, K., Chaudhury, S., and Chakraborty, R. (2011). Prevalence of psychiatric and physical morbidity in an urban geriatric population. Indian J. Psychiatry 53, 121–127. doi: 10.4103/0019-5545.82535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theokas, C., and Lerner, R. (2006). Observed ecological assets in families, schools and neighborhoods: conceptualization, measurement, and relations with positive and negative developmental outcomes. Appl. Dev. Sci. 10, 61–74. doi: 10.1207/s1532480xads1002_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres, G., Reis, L., and Reis, L. (2010). Assessment of functional capacity in elderly residents of an outlying área in the hinterland of Bahia/Northeast Brazil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 68, 39–43. doi: 10.1590/S0004-282X2010000100009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres, G., Reis, L., Reis, L., Fernandes, M., and Alves, G. (2009). Avaliação da capacidade de realização das atividades cotidianas em idosos residentes em domicílio. Revis. Baiana 33, 453–465.

Google Scholar

Tsai, Y., Yeh, S., and Tsai, H. (2005). Prevalence and risk factors for depressive symptoms among community-dwelling elders in Taiwan. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 20, 1097–1102. doi: 10.1002/gps.1281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urban, J., Bizan, S., and Lerner, R. (2009). The role of neighborhood ecological assets and activity involvement in youth developmental outcomes: differential impacts of asset poor and asset rich neighborhoods. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 30, 601–614. doi: 10.1007/s10964-010-9549-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Gelder, B., Tijhuis, M., Kalmijn, S., Giampaoli, S., Nissinen, A., and Kromhout, D. (2006). Marital status and living situation during a 5-year period are associated with a subsequent 10-year cognitive decline in older men: the FINE study. J. Gerontol. 61B, 213–219. doi: 10.1093/geronb/61.4.P213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wahl, H.-W., Iwarsson, S., and Oswald, F. (2012). Aging well and the environment: toward an integrative model and research agenda for the future. Gerontologist 52, 306–316. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnr154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, A. (2010). “Ageing and quality of life in Europe,” in The SAGE Handbook of Social Gerontology, eds D. Dannefer and C. Phillipson (Los Angeles: SAGE), 573–586.

Google Scholar

Weele, G., Gussekloo, J., Waal, M., Craen, A., and Mast, R. (2009). Co-occurrence of depression and anxiety in elderly subjects aged 90 years and its relationship with functional status, quality of life and mortality. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 24, 595–601. doi: 10.1002/gps.2162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, K., Mottram, P., and Sixsmith, A. (2007). Depressive symptoms in the very old living alone; prevalence, incidence and risk factors. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 22, 361–366. doi: 10.1002/gps.1682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yount, K. (2008). Gender, resources across the life course, and cognitive functioning in Egypt. Demography 45, 907–926. doi: 10.1353/dem.0.0034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: ecological assets, activities of daily living, cognitive functioning, depression, aging outcomes

Citation: Bastos AM, Faria CG, Moreira E, Morais D, Melo-de-Carvalho JM and Paul MC (2015) The importance of neighborhood ecological assets in community dwelling old people aging outcomes: A study in Northern Portugal. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:156. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00156

Received: 30 May 2015; Accepted: 28 July 2015;
Published: 01 September 2015.

Edited by:

Lia Fernandes, University of Porto, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Wolfgang Michael Schlicht, University of Stuttgart, Germany
Koteswara Rao Valasani, The University of Kansas, USA

Copyright © 2015 Bastos, Faria, Moreira, Morais, Melo-de-Carvalho and Paul. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alice M. Bastos, Instituto Politécnico de Viana do Castelo – Escola Superior de Educação, Unidade de Investigação e Formação sobre Adultos e Idosos, Avenue Capitão Gaspar de Castro, 4900 Viana do Castelo, Portugal, abastos@ese.ipvc.pt

Download