Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 24 May 2017
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 9 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00154

Gait Is Associated with Cognitive Flexibility: A Dual-Tasking Study in Healthy Older People

Markus A. Hobert1,2,3* Sinja I. Meyer1,2 Sandra E. Hasmann1,2 Florian G. Metzger4,5 Ulrike Suenkel1,2 Gerhard W. Eschweiler4,5 Daniela Berg1,2,3 Walter Maetzler1,2,3,5
  • 1Center for Neurology and Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Department of Neurodegeneration, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 2DZNE, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Tübingen, Germany
  • 3Department of Neurology, University of Kiel, Kiel, Germany
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 5Geriatric Center, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany

Objectives: To analyze which gait parameters are primarily influenced by cognitive flexibility, and whether such an effect depends on the walking condition used.

Design: Cross-sectional analysis.

Setting: Tübingen evaluation of Risk factors for Early detection of Neurodegenerative Disorders.

Participants: A total of 661 non-demented individuals (49–80 years).

Measurements: A gait assessment with four conditions was performed: a 20 m walk at convenient speed (C), at fast speed (F), at fast speed while checking boxes (FB), and while subtracting serial 7s (FS). Seven gait parameters from a wearable sensor-unit (McRoberts, Netherlands) were compared with delta Trail-Making-Test (dTMT) values, which is a measure of cognitive flexibility. Walking strategies of good and poor dTMT performers were compared by evaluating the patterns of gait parameters across conditions.

Results: Five parameters correlated significantly with the dTMT in the FS condition, two parameters in the F and FB condition, and none in the C condition. Overall correlations were relatively weak. Gait speed was the gait parameter that most strongly correlated with the dTMT (r2 = 7.4%). In good, but not poor, dTMT performers differences between FB and FS were significantly different in variability-associated gait parameters.

Conclusion: Older individuals need cognitive flexibility to perform difficult walking conditions. This association is best seen in gait speed. New and particularly relevant for recognition and training of deficits is that older individuals with poor cognitive flexibility have obviously fewer resources to adapt to challenging walking conditions. Our findings partially explain gait deficits in older adults with poor cognitive flexibility.

Introduction

Cognitive flexibility is part of the subdomain “shifting” of executive function (Miyake et al., 2000). It is controlled by the frontal lobe and associated areas (Miyake and Friedman, 2012), and influenced by aging (Wecker et al., 2005) and diseases, such as dementia (Stopford et al., 2012) and stroke (Rasquin et al., 2002). Cognitive flexibility is required for adapting behavior to external influences (Gilbert and Burgess, 2008; Klanker et al., 2013). This flexibility is necessary for the unrestricted performance of daily life during waking states, because it helps to make the right decisions in multitasking situations. Cognitive flexibility is often measured with the delta Trail-Making Test (dTMT) under experimental conditions (Ble et al., 2005; Bowie and Harvey, 2006; Coppin et al., 2006; Montero-Odasso et al., 2009; Hirota et al., 2010). Cognitive flexibility is likely associated with gait particularly during difficult walking situations, such as fast walking and walking when performing additional tasks, but this effect was not investigated in detail. This hypothesis arises from the following studies.

An investigation of 926 older community-dwelling persons with a mean age of 75 years using a single tasking (ST) 4 m walk at a convenient speed and a 7 m fast walk with obstacles (Ble et al., 2005) demonstrated an association between gait speed and cognitive flexibility, as measured with the dTMT in the fast walking with obstacles, but not in the convenient walking paradigm. This report was the first study to indicate an association of cognitive flexibility with gait speed in complex walking conditions in older adults (Ble et al., 2005; Coppin et al., 2006).

In another study of 493 Japanese individuals with a mean age of 74 years, the influence of cognitive flexibility on gait speed in particular under challenging walking conditions was also proposed (Hirota et al., 2010). The participants performed walking tasks of different levels of complexity, and the primary result revealed a stronger association of dTMT with the walking performance in more complex tasks compared to convenient walking.

Finally, the most convincing evidence for an interplay of cognitive flexibility and gait under dual tasking (DT) conditions comes from the Irish longitudinal Study on Aging (Killane et al., 2014). In this study, 4431 participants with a mean age of 62 years underwent walking tasks under ST and DT situations, and cognitive flexibility measures (i.e., the Color Trail Test) correlated significantly with gait speed only during DT, but not ST, situations.

Common in all of these studies is that they found associations of cognitive flexibility with gait. People with poor cognitive flexibility seem to have deficits in gait (control), especially in more difficult gait conditions. This might indicate that people with poor cognitive flexibility have a lower capacity to adapt to the demands of the more difficult walking condition. However, none of the above-mentioned studies reported other quantitative gait parameters beyond gait speed. As gait speed seems to be a sensitive but unspecific parameter to assess health in older age (Studenski et al., 2011), we were interested in whether cognitive flexibility is also associated with quantitative gait parameters “beyond” gait speed and with the “strategies” that are used in walking conditions with different levels of difficulty. This assessment can contribute to a mechanistic model of the interplay between cognitive flexibility and walking behavior.

Materials and Methods

Study Participants and Clinical Assessment

Data of 715 healthy, non-demented individuals who participated in the baseline assessment of the TREND study1 (Tübingen evaluation of Risk factors for Early detection of Neurodegenerative Disorders) (Hobert et al., 2011) were considered for this analysis.

The study protocol has been reported elsewhere (Gaenslen et al., 2014). In brief, the TREND study aims at the early detection of neurodegenerative diseases and includes healthy community-dwelling people with or without risk factors for such diseases, i.e., REM sleep behavior disorder, depression, or hyposmia. Study participants were recruited via newspaper advertisements, information events and flyers. All underwent a telephone screening and were considered if they denied psychiatric disorders (other than depression), epilepsy, multiple sclerosis, stroke, dementia, encephalitis malignancies and the need of walking aids. The participants were investigated prospectively in 2009 and 2010.

Out of the 715 participants who performed the measurements, a total of 54 participants were excluded because of technical issues with the sensor system (32), negative or missing dTMT data (13), Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) <25 (4) (Folstein et al., 1975) or a diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease (PD) (5) (Hughes et al., 1992). Therefore, 661 subjects were included in the analysis. Excluded participants did not significantly differ from the included cohort in age, sex, or education level. All participants included were between 49 and 80 years of age and able to walk independently without ambulatory aids or assistance. Table 1 lists the demographic characteristics.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Demographic data.

The ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of the University of Tübingen, Germany approved the study (Nr. 90/2009BO2). All subjects gave informed written consent.

Gait Assessment

Participants were instructed to walk along a 20 m long, obstacle-free path in an at least 1.5-m wide corridor under the following four conditions: (i) ST walking at a convenient speed; (ii) ST walking at a fast speed; (iii) DT walking at a fast speed and checking boxes at a fast speed; (iv) DT walking at fast speed and subtracting serial 7s at a fast speed. No prioritization of any task was given for the DT tasks. The order of the tasks was (i–iv) for all subjects. In the checking boxes task, study participants were asked to carry a clipboard with a sheet of paper on it. They had to mark the boxes of a table drawn on the paper with a cross as fast as possible. In the subtracting serial 7s task, participants had to subtract 7s from a random three-digit number continuously as fast as possible. The instruction were “Please walk with convenient gait speed and do not risk falling!” for task (i), “Please walk as fast as you can, do not run, do not risk falling!” for task (ii), “Please walk as fast as you can, do not run, do not risk falling, and mark each of the boxes on the sheet of paper with a cross as fast as you can!” for task (iii) and “Please walk as fast as you can, do not run, do not risk falling, and subtract serial 7s as fast as you can from the number I will shortly tell you!” for task (iv).

All subjects wore a small sensor unit (Dynaport Hybrid, McRoberts B.V., The Hague, The Netherlands) that was fixed at the lower back with a belt during the gait tasks. The sensor unit included a 3-axis accelerometer and 3-axis gyroscope with a sampling rate of 100 Hz. Only the middle 70% of steps of the recorded gait information were analyzed to avoid artifacts during gait acceleration and deceleration phases (Lindemann et al., 2008). Overall, number of steps that were included in the analyses ranged from 14 to 29. Quantitative gait parameters were calculated with established algorithms using acceleration in the anterior-posterior direction (Zijlstra and Hof, 2003; Brandes et al., 2006; Dijkstra et al., 2008; Houdijk et al., 2008) through the McRoberts web platform2. Raw data were filtered by a bandpass filter between 0.05 and 7 Hz and a tilt correction was used. The included parameters contribute basically to the following gait domains (Verghese et al., 2008): pace (gait speed, number of steps), rhythm (stride duration, double support time) and variability of gait (stride duration variability (calculated using the coefficient of variation (CV) of stride duration (Montero-Odasso et al., 2011)), phase coordination index (PCI, describing the regularity between right and left step phases) (Plotnik et al., 2008), and gait asymmetry (describing the relationship between the average swing times of right and left steps) (Yogev et al., 2007; Plotnik et al., 2009).

Cognitive Assessment: Trail-Making Test

The time needed to perform the Trail-Making Test (TMT) part B minus A was used to measure cognitive flexibility (dTMT = TMT part B – TMT part A). Details are described elsewhere (Crowe, 1998; Ble et al., 2005; Bowie and Harvey, 2006; Coppin et al., 2006). Briefly, numbers in part A must be connected on a sheet of paper in ascending order as fast as possible. This task primarily tests upper motor performance and visual scanning (Crowe, 1998). Numbers and letters in part B were connected in an alternating manner. This task tests motor performance, visual scanning, and additionally set shifting, i.e., cognitive flexibility (Crowe, 1998).

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using JMP software (version 11.1.1, SAS). Demographic and clinical parameters of the entire cohort and the subcohorts (see below) are presented as mean and standard deviation or frequency. Comparisons were performed using the Student’s t-test and the Chi squared test. The level of significance (two-sided) was set at 0.05 because of the exploratory nature of the study.

Regression analyses with quantitative gait parameters and the dTMT score were performed to analyze the influence of cognitive flexibility on gait parameters. Age, sex, education period, MMSE, and Becks Depression Inventory (BDI) (Hautzinger, 1991) were considered relevant covariates for gait tasks as shown before in this cohort (Hobert et al., 2011) and therefore included in the model.

We also defined the highest and lowest tertile of dTMT performers based on the individual dTMT score, according to Ble et al. (Ble et al., 2005), whether cognitive flexibility influences patterns of significant parameter changes across different walking conditions (Ble et al., 2005; Hobert et al., 2011). We therefore performed intra-group comparisons of every gait parameter between the walking conditions performed at a fast speed (ST fast walk, DT fast walk with checking boxes and DT fast walk with subtracting serial 7s) within good and poor dTMT performers separately using the Wilcoxon test for paired samples. We then compared these patterns of significant parameter changes across different walking conditions between good and poor dTMT performers, i.e., whether we can find significant differences between walking conditions within a dTMT performers group that does not occur in the other group.

Results

Correlations between Gait Parameters and Delta TMT Values

No parameter in the convenient ST walking condition was significantly correlated with the dTMT. Two parameters (gait speed (p = 0.03) and number of steps (p = 0.04)) in the fast ST walking condition were significantly correlated with the dTMT. Two parameters [gait speed (p = 0.009) and stride duration (p = 0.047)] in the fast DT walking condition with checking boxes were significantly correlated with the dTMT. Five parameters [gait speed (p < 0.0001), number of steps (p = 0.01)], stride duration (p = 0.0006), gait asymmetry (p = 0.02), and PCI (p = 0.01) in the fast DT walking condition with subtracting serial 7s correlated significantly with the dTMT. Note that this result is not relevantly affected by applying a Bonferroni-corrected p-value (0.05/28 = 0.0018). Table 2 provides the details.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Correlation values between quantitative gait parameters and the delta Trail-Making Test.

There is the trend that the highest r2 values, which indicated the strongest association with the dTMT, of all gait parameters included in the analyses, were found in gait speed across all four walking conditions. The highest r2 (7.4%) was observed in the walking at fast speed with simultaneously subtracting serial 7s condition.

Comparison of Gait Parameters between Good and Poor dTMT Performers

No parameter in the convenient ST walking condition was significantly different between the good and the poor dTMT performers. In the fast ST walking condition, two parameters [gait speed (p = 0.003) and number of steps (p = 0.02)] were different between groups. The following five parameters were significantly different between groups in the fast DT walking condition with checking boxes: gait speed (p < 0.0001), number of steps (p = 0.02), stride duration (p = 0.002), double support time (p = 0.02), and stride duration CV (p = 0.01). In the fast DT walking condition with subtracting serial 7s, the following six gait parameters were significantly different between the two dTMT groups: gait speed (p < 0.0001), number of steps (p = 0.03), stride duration (p < 0.0001), double support time (p = 0.004), gait asymmetry (p = 0.01), and PCI (p = 0.04). Details are provided in Supplementary Table 1.

Differences in Gait Adaptation Strategies between Good and Poor dTMT Performers

Both dTMT groups showed significant differences for gait speed, step time and double support time between the three walking conditions, and significant differences for number of steps between ST walking with fast speed and both DT walking conditions. In other words, the above-mentioned patterns of significant changes from one to another walking condition were identical between the good and poor dTMT performers. On the contrary, good, but not poor, TMT performers showed significant differences for stride time CV, gait asymmetry, and PCI between the DT walking condition with checking boxes and the DT walking condition with subtracting serial 7s. Poor, but not good, dTMT performers showed a significant difference for PCI between the ST walking condition with fast speed and the DT walking condition with checking boxes. In other words, the above-mentioned patterns of significant changes from one to another walking condition were different between the good and poor dTMT performers. Figure 1 provides an overview of these significant changes, with square brackets that are different between cohorts marked in bold.”

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Overview of patterns of significant parameter changes across different walking conditions in good and poor delta Trail-Making Test (dTMT) performers in different gait parameters (A–G). Data are shown with box plots. Horizontal lines mark the mean, boxes the first and third quartiles, and whiskers the outermost data point within 1.5-fold the interquartile range above the third quartile or below the first quartile. Square brackets indicate significant differences between walking conditions within a group. Bold square brackets indicate significant differences between distinct walking conditions that occurred in one group but not in the other group. Lower standard deviations in particular in variability-associated parameters under DT than under ST conditions may indicate some rhythmicity, cueing and “magnet” effects induced by the secondary task. These effects have been described previously (Ebersbach et al., 1995; Beauchet et al., 2010). Note that differences of absolute parameter values between groups are not provided in this figure. They are available in Supplementary Table 1. Detailed p-values of the square brackets are presented in Supplementary Table 2.

Note that use of a Bonferroni-corrected p-value of 0.0012 did not relevantly affect these results. Only double support time, a parameter relatively closely associated with the gait variability domain (Verghese et al., 2009; Callisaya et al., 2011), then also showed a different pattern of significant parameter changes across the walking conditions between good and poor dTMT performers. For sake of completeness, the correlations between gait parameters are provided in Supplementary Table 3.

Discussion

This study evaluated quantitative gait parameters of four walking paradigms in a large cohort of healthy older adults wearing a small sensor unit at their lower back, and investigated the association of gait parameters and walking paradigms with cognitive flexibility. Recognizing and addressing such specific deficits in therapy and training settings may lead to improved gait performance in older adults with poor cognitive flexibility.

Our results partially confirm the results of previous studies (Ble et al., 2005; Coppin et al., 2006; Montero-Odasso et al., 2009; Hirota et al., 2010; Berryman et al., 2013; Killane et al., 2014): More challenging walking paradigms require more cognitive flexibility than simple paradigms. In our study, it is reflected by an increasing number of significantly different quantitative gait parameters with increasing difficulty of the walking condition. Accordingly, our observations suggest that the best walking task for the assessment of cognitive flexibility is the DT fast walk with subtracting serial 7s. This task exhibited the highest number of quantitative gait parameters that were significantly associated with the dTMT, and it also included the gait parameter that correlated strongest with the dTMT (gait speed). This observation may be simply explained by the increased influence of supraspinal control mechanisms on gait under more challenging walking conditions (Maetzler et al., 2013; Hobert et al., 2014). However, the obviously higher influence of cognitive flexibility on walking during subtracting serial 7s compared to walking when checking boxes requires further reflection. The control of a DT situation per se is a cognitive task, which means that a cognitive process is involved in sharing of resources or shifting of attention between the different tasks, and controls the prioritization of the tasks (Hobert et al., 2011). Based on this, we assumed that the following individual situations were present in our experimental setting. A study participant performed two tasks with mainly motor components (walking and checking boxes) during the checking boxes DT and one cognitive task (the above-mentioned cognitive control of the DT situation). A person during the serial subtraction task performed one task with a primarily motor component (walking) and two tasks with primarily cognitive components (serial subtraction and cognitive control of the DT situation). The bottleneck hypothesis proposes that the processing of two tasks using the same (or similar) network(s) create(s) a bottleneck (Ruthruff et al., 2001; Yogev-Seligmann et al., 2008). Therefore, a gait paradigm with two cognitive tasks performed in a (relatively) healthy cohort should exhibit “more” correlation with the dTMT than a gait paradigm with one cognitive task. This presumption was observed in our study.

Indirect support for the above hypothesis comes from studies investigating individuals with motor network deficits, e.g., patients with mild-to-moderate PD (i.e., at a disease stage, where the motor deficits are generally more prominent than the cognitive deficit). These patients might experience the performance of two motor tasks and one cognitive task as more challenging than the performance of one motor task and two cognitive tasks. Our prospective longitudinal study of PD patients with and without falls during an observation period of 3.5 years demonstrated that only DT deficits in the checking boxes task, but not in the subtracting serial 7s task, predicted the first fall in the former group (Heinzel et al., 2016). A recent study did not find any additional value of a cognitive DT paradigm on falls in 263 mild-to-moderate PD patients (Smulders et al., 2012). Unfortunately, the authors did not include a secondary task with a primarily motor component in their study protocol (Smulders et al., 2012).

After the association of cognitive flexibility with walking conditions, we analyzed the association of cognitive flexibility with different gait parameters: The parameter that was most closely associated with the dTMT was gait speed, followed by stride duration and number of steps. This result indicates that pace-associated parameters are more closely correlated with cognitive flexibility than variability-associated parameters, which is basically consistent with the results of a recent study (Martin et al., 2013). However, the authors in the previous study focused on the comparison of gait and executive function in general, and not specifically on cognitive flexibility (Martin et al., 2013). The result is still surprising, because one may associate cognitive flexibility with adaptation, rather than velocity aspects of gait. Regardless of the mechanisms for the “dominance” of gait speed over other quantitative gait parameters for, e.g., detection of gait deficits per se (Lord et al., 2013), motor-cognitive interference deficits (Al-Yahya et al., 2011), and aspects of general health and survival (Studenski et al., 2011), gait speed is a non-specific parameter. This fact is also true for the prediction of cognitive flexibility using this parameter. Gait speed (only) explained 7.4% of the variance of dTMT in the most challenging walking task of our setting.

As a general comment, single quantitative gait parameters may not reach sufficiently high prediction values for any kind of pathology or alterations of human movement that eventually enable an individual diagnosis. The more promising approach to differentiate between specific pathologies / alterations and control states and detect progression and changes due to therapy may be the use of “gait parameter panels” or multivariate regression models. Such models can account for the complex interplay between (dys)function and the compensation mechanisms that are involved in complex movements, such as gait and balance performance in particular under challenging conditions (Maetzler and Hausdorff, 2012; Lord et al., 2013; Maetzler et al., 2013).

This study demonstrated also that cognitive flexibility influences the “type” of walking pattern a person uses in distinct challenging walking situations. Good dTMT performers exhibited significantly increased variability, asymmetry, and irregularity of gait (Hausdorff et al., 2006; Plotnik et al., 2007; Montero-Odasso et al., 2011) in the subtracting serial 7s DT compared to the (easier) checking boxes DT. Notably, poor dTMT performers did not adapt their parameters accordingly, which indicates an impairment of this adaptation strategy. We interpret this finding as follows: Individuals with poor cognitive flexibility reach the maximum adaption capability of their walking pattern earlier than individuals with good cognitive flexibility in a sequence of walking tasks with increasing levels of complexity. A recent study (Lowry et al., 2012) indirectly supports our hypothesis. This study compared the gait parameters of walking along a figure 8 (where changes between walking patterns are necessary) with straight walking (no changes necessary) in 106 old adults and found a significant association between dTMT and the number of steps only when walking on the figure 8 (Lowry et al., 2012).

The present study has some limitations. First, quantitative gait parameters were assessed with a wearable sensor at the lower back. This technique may not be as accurate as more complex gait evaluation systems in measuring at least some of the gait parameters, e.g., double support time. Second, we analyzed approximately 14 m of steady state walking. Although other studies used even shorter distances (Ble et al., 2005; Montero-Odasso et al., 2011; Killane et al., 2014), longer walking distances may deliver more valid gait parameters. It has been shown that the reliability of gait parameters improves with increasing number of steps. This is the case for gait variability parameters (Galna et al., 2013), whereas gait speed reaches already the steady state after 2.5 m (Lindemann et al., 2008). Third, the different tasks were not randomized and may result in a learning effect, but this effect should be comparable in all groups.

Conclusion

This study demonstrates that cognitive flexibility is associated with walking, in particular under challenging walking conditions, in a cohort of older adults without relevant motor and cognitive deficits. We also demonstrated that older individuals with poor cognitive flexibility use a pattern in variability-related gait parameters across walking conditions that differs from individuals with good cognitive flexibility. This difference might indicate a lower capability of the former population to adapt to challenging walking situations with different demands. Our findings add relevant information to our understanding of gait and balance deficits in older adults with poor cognitive flexibility and may give a basis for interventional studies.

Author Contributions

MH, DB, and WM made substantial contributions to the acquisition, analysis and interpretation of data for the work. MH, SM, SH, FM, US, and GE made substantial contributions to the acquisition of the data. MH and WM drafted the paper, all remaining authors revised the draft critically for important intellectual content. All authors gave their final approval of the version to be published, and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Funding

WM reports grants from EU project Moving beyond, SENSE-PARK; Neuroalliance, MF and personal fees from Licher, Roelke Pharma, UCB and GSK. DB is member of an Advisory Board of UCB pharma GmbH and receives honoraria from UCB pharma GmbH. She reports grants from Michael J. Fox Foundation, Janssen Pharmaceutica N.V., German Parkinson’s Disease Association (dPV), BMWi, BMBF, Parkinson Fonds Deutschland gGmbH, UCB Pharma GmbH, TEVA Pharma GmbH, EU, Novartis Pharma GmbH, Boehringer Ingelheim Pharma GmbH, Lundbeck, MH received travel grants by Abbvie and Merz.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all participants in this study. We are grateful to Rob van Lummel, Erik Ainsworth, and Joris Meijer, McRoberts B.V., The Hague, The Netherlands for technical support. We acknowledge support by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and Open Access Publishing Fund of University of Tüingen.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00154/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.trend-studie.de
  2. ^ www.mcroberts.nl/analysis

References

Al-Yahya, E., Dawes, H., Smith, L., Dennis, A., Howells, K., and Cockburn, J. (2011). Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 715–728. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauchet, O., Allali, G., Poujol, L., Barthelemy, J. C., Roche, F., and Annweiler, C. (2010). Decrease in gait variability while counting backward: a marker of ‘magnet effect’? J. Neural Transm. 117, 1171–1176. doi: 10.1007/s00702-010-0463-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berryman, N., Bherer, L., Nadeau, S., Lauzière, S., Lehr, L., Bobeuf, F., et al. (2013). Executive functions, physical fitness and mobility in well-functioning older adults. Exp. Gerontol. 48, 1402–1409. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2013.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ble, A., Volpato, S., Zuliani, G., Guralnik, J. M., Bandinelli, S., Lauretani, F., et al. (2005). Executive function correlates with walking speed in older persons: the InCHIANTI study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 410–415. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53157.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowie, C. R., and Harvey, P. D. (2006). Administration and interpretation of the trail making test. Nat. Protoc. 1, 2277–2281. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandes, M., Zijlstra, W., Heikens, S., van Lummel, R., and Rosenbaum, D. (2006). Accelerometry based assessment of gait parameters in children. Gait Posture 24, 482–486. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callisaya, M. L., Blizzard, L., Schmidt, M. D., Martin, K. L., McGinley, J. L., Sanders, L. M., et al. (2011). Gait, gait variability and the risk of multiple incident falls in older people: a population-based study. Age Ageing 40, 481–487. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afr055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coppin, A. K., Shumway-Cook, A., Saczynski, J. S., Patel, K. V., Ble, A., Ferrucci, L., et al. (2006). Association of executive function and performance of dual-task physical tests among older adults: analyses from the InChianti study. Age Ageing 35, 619–624. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowe, S. F. (1998). The differential contribution of mental tracking, cognitive flexibility, visual search, and motor speed to performance on parts A and B of the trail making test. J. Clin. Psychol. 54, 585–591.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dijkstra, B., Zijlstra, W., Scherder, E., and Kamsma, Y. (2008). Detection of walking periods and number of steps in older adults and patients with Parkinson’s disease: accuracy of a pedometer and an accelerometry-based method. Age Ageing 37, 436–441. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afn097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebersbach, G., Dimitrijevic, M. R., and Poewe, W. (1995). Influence of concurrent tasks on gait: a dual-task approach. Percept. Mot. Skills 81, 107–113.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). ‘Mini-mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198.

Google Scholar

Gaenslen, A., Wurster, I., Brockmann, K., Huber, H., Godau, J., Faust, B., et al. (2014). Prodromal features for Parkinson’s disease–baseline data from the TREND study. Eur. J. Neurol. 21, 766–772. doi: 10.1111/ene.12382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galna, B., Lord, S., and Rochester, L. (2013). Is gait variability reliable in older adults and Parkinson’s disease? Towards an optimal testing protocol. Gait Posture 37, 580–585. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.09.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, S. J., and Burgess, P. W. (2008). Executive function. Curr. Biol. 18, R110–R114. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M., Doniger, G. M., Springer, S., Yogev, G., Simon, E. S., and Giladi, N. (2006). A common cognitive profile in elderly fallers and in patients with Parkinson’s disease: the prominence of impaired executive function and attention. Exp. Aging Res. 32, 411–429. doi: 10.1080/03610730600875817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hautzinger, M. (1991). The beck depression inventory in clinical practice. Nervenarzt 62, 689–696.

Heinzel, S., Maechtel, M., Hasmann, S. E., Hobert, M. A., Heger, T., Berg, D., et al. (2016). Motor dual-tasking deficits predict falls in Parkinson’s disease: a prospective study. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 26, 73–77. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirota, C., Watanabe, M., Sun, W., Tanimoto, Y., Kono, R., Takasaki, K., et al. (2010). Association between the trail making test and physical performance in elderly Japanese. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 10, 40–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2009.00557.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobert, M. A., Maetzler, W., Aminian, K., and Chiari, L. (2014). Technical and clinical view on ambulatory assessment in Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neurol. Scand. 130, 139–147. doi: 10.1111/ane.12248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobert, M. A., Niebler, R., Meyer, S. I., Brockmann, K., Becker, C., Huber, H., et al. (2011). Poor trail making test performance is directly associated with altered dual task prioritization in the elderly–baseline results from the TREND study. PLoS ONE 6:e27831. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houdijk, H., Appelman, F. M., Van Velzen, J. M., Van der Woude, L. H. V., and Van Bennekom, C. A. M. (2008). Validity of DynaPort GaitMonitor for assessment of spatiotemporal parameters in amputee gait. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 45, 1335–1342.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hughes, A. J., Ben-Shlomo, Y., Daniel, S. E., and Lees, A. J. (1992). What features improve the accuracy of clinical diagnosis in Parkinson’s disease: a clinicopathologic study. Neurology 42, 1142–1146.

Google Scholar

Killane, I., Donoghue, O. A., Savva, G. M., Cronin, H., Kenny, R. A., and Reilly, R. B. (2014). Relative association of processing speed, short-term memory and sustained attention with task on gait speed: a study of community-dwelling people 50 years and older. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 69, 1407–1414. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glu140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klanker, M., Feenstra, M., and Denys, D. (2013). Dopaminergic control of cognitive flexibility in humans and animals. Front. Neurosci. 7:201. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindemann, U., Najafi, B., Zijlstra, W., Hauer, K., Muche, R., Becker, C., et al. (2008). Distance to achieve steady state walking speed in frail elderly persons. Gait Posture 27, 91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, S., Galna, B., and Rochester, L. (2013). Moving forward on gait measurement: toward a more refined approach. Mov. Disord. 28, 1534–1543. doi: 10.1002/mds.25545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowry, K. A., Brach, J. S., Nebes, R. D., Studenski, S. A., and VanSwearingen, J. M. (2012). Contributions of cognitive function to straight- and curved-path walking in older adults. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 93, 802–807. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maetzler, W., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2012). Motor signs in the prodromal phase of Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 27, 627–633. doi: 10.1002/mds.24973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maetzler, W., Nieuwhof, F., Hasmann, S. E., and Bloem, B. R. (2013). Emerging therapies for gait disability and balance impairment: promises and pitfalls. Mov. Disord. 28, 1576–1586. doi: 10.1002/mds.25682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, K. L., Blizzard, L., Wood, A. G., Srikanth, V., Thomson, R., Sanders, L. M., et al. (2013). Cognitive function, gait, and gait variability in older people: a population-based study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 68, 726–732. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., and Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M., Bergman, H., Phillips, N. A., Wong, C. H., Sourial, N., and Chertkow, H. (2009). Dual-tasking and gait in people with mild cognitive impairment. The effect of working memory. BMC Geriatr. 9:41. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-9-41

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M., Muir, S. W., Hall, M., Doherty, T. J., Kloseck, M., Beauchet, O., et al. (2011). Gait variability is associated with frailty in community-dwelling older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 66, 568–576. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plotnik, M., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2007). A new measure for quantifying the bilateral coordination of human gait: effects of aging and Parkinson’s disease. Exp. Brain Res. 181, 561–570. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0955-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plotnik, M., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2008). Bilateral coordination of walking and freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 1999–2006. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06167.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plotnik, M., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2009). Bilateral coordination of gait and Parkinson’s disease: the effects of dual tasking. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 80, 347–350. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.157362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasquin, S. M., Verhey, F. R., Lousberg, R., Winkens, I., and Lodder, J. (2002). Vascular cognitive disorders: memory, mental speed and cognitive flexibility after stroke. J. Neurol. Sci. 203, 115–119. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(02)00264-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruthruff, E., Pashler, H. E., and Klaassen, A. (2001). Processing bottlenecks in dual-task performance: structural limitation or strategic postponement? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 8, 73–80.

Google Scholar

Smulders, K., Esselink, R. A., Weiss, A., Kessels, R. P., Geurts, A. C., and Bloem, B. R. (2012). Assessment of dual tasking has no clinical value for fall prediction in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. 259, 1840–1847. doi: 10.1007/s00415-012-6419-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stopford, C. L., Thompson, J. C., Neary, D., Richardson, A. M. T., and Snowden, J. S. (2012). Working memory, attention, and executive function in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Cortex 48, 429–446. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Studenski, S., Perera, S., Patel, K., Rosano, C., Faulkner, K., Inzitari, M., et al. (2011). Gait speed and survival in older adults. JAMA 305, 50–58. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.1923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verghese, J., Holtzer, R., Lipton, R. B., and Wang, C. (2009). Quantitative gait markers and incident fall risk in older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 64, 896–901. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glp033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verghese, J., Robbins, M., Holtzer, R., Zimmerman, M., Wang, C., Xue, X., et al. (2008). Gait dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment syndromes. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 56, 1244–1251. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01758.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wecker, N. S., Kramer, J. H., Hallam, B. J., and Delis, D. C. (2005). Mental flexibility: age effects on switching. Neuropsychology 19, 345–352. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.3.345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yogev, G., Plotnik, M., Peretz, C., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2007). Gait asymmetry in patients with Parkinson’s disease and elderly fallers: When does the bilateral coordination of gait require attention? Exp. Brain Res. 177, 336–346. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0676-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yogev-Seligmann, G., Hausdorff, J. M., and Giladi, N. (2008). The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov. Disord. 23, 329–342; quiz 472. doi: 10.1002/mds.21720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zijlstra, W., and Hof, A. L. (2003). Assessment of spatio-temporal gait parameters from trunk accelerations during human walking. Gait Posture 18, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(02)00190-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aging, dual tasking, executive function, gait, cognitive flexibility

Citation: Hobert MA, Meyer SI, Hasmann SE, Metzger FG, Suenkel U, Eschweiler GW, Berg D and Maetzler W (2017) Gait Is Associated with Cognitive Flexibility: A Dual-Tasking Study in Healthy Older People. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:154. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00154

Received: 21 July 2016; Accepted: 04 May 2017;
Published: 24 May 2017.

Edited by:

Jean Mariani, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, France

Reviewed by:

Francesca Morganti, University of Bergamo, Italy
Claudine Lamoth, University Medical Center Groningen, Netherlands

Copyright © 2017 Hobert, Meyer, Hasmann, Metzger, Suenkel, Eschweiler, Berg and Maetzler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Markus A. Hobert, markus.hobert@uksh.de

Download