Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 16 June 2017
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 9 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00182

Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms Impact Clinical Competence in Alzheimer’s Disease

  • 1Department of Psychology, Pontifícia Universidade Católica – Rio (PUC-Rio), Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
  • 2Department of Medical Psychology, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 3Diabetes Center/Department of Internal Medicine, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 4Institute of Psychiatry—Center for Alzheimer’s Disease, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ), Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
  • 5Post Graduation Program on Translational Biomedicine, Universidade do Grande Rio (Unigranrio), Caxias, Brazil
  • 6Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom

Decision-making is considered a fundamental aspect of personal autonomy and can be affected in psychiatric and neurologic diseases. It has been shown that cognitive deficits in dementia impact negatively on decision-making. Moreover, studies highlighted impaired clinical competence in neuropsychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. In this context, the current study explored the relationship between behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) and clinical competence, especially the capacity to consent to treatment, in Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Seventy-one patients with mild to moderate AD participated, completing assessments for capacity to consent to treatment, general cognition and neuropsychiatric disturbances. For each neuropsychiatric symptom, patients with and without the particular disturbance were compared on the different subscales of the MacArthur Competence Tool for Treatment (MacCAT-T; Understanding, Appreciation, Reasoning and Expression). The results showed that patients presenting delusions, as well as apathetic patients, had a lower ability to express a clear treatment choice compared to patients without these symptoms. By contrast, patients with dysphoria/depression had higher scores on this variable. Additionally, AD patients with euphoria had more difficulties discussing consequences of treatment alternatives compared to patients without this disturbance. None of the differences were confounded by global cognition. There were no between-group differences in clinical decision-making for patients with hallucinations, agitation/aggression, anxiety, irritability, disinhibition and aberrant motor behavior. These findings highlight the importance of taking BPSD into account when assessing decision-making capacity, especially clinical competence, in AD. Furthermore, reducing BPSD may lead to better clinical competence in patients with AD, as well as to improvements in patients and caregivers’ quality of life.

Introduction

In the context of contemporary clinical practice, in which the patients’ active participation in medical decisions is valued, the question of decision-making capacity, especially the competence to consent to treatment has become central. Moreover, treatment consent capacity, which refers to the ability to accept an offered treatment, refuse it, or select among alternatives, has important clinical, legal and ethical implications (President’s Commission for the Study of Ethical Problems in Medicine and Biomedical and Behavioral Research, 1982; Tepper and Elwork, 1984). Indeed, it is considered a fundamental aspect of personal autonomy and its careful assessment is essential to find the balance between autonomy for patients who are able to make their own decisions and protection for those with diminished decisional capacity (Berg et al., 2001; Berghmans and Widdershoven, 2003). In the case of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), these concerns seem even more relevant, since this is a condition affecting cognitive abilities critical to healthcare decision-making capacity (Marson, 2001). It has been suggested that offering the possibility to people with dementia (PwD) to participate in healthcare-related decisions may improve their well-being, quality of life and dignity compared to those excluded from taking part in decisions, who tend to be more depressed, frustrated and debilitated (Smebye et al., 2012).

Previous research exploring medical decision-making, also termed clinical competence, has shown that, compared with healthy control individuals, the treatment consent capacity of individuals with AD is reduced (Karlawish et al., 2005; Lui et al., 2012). These findings have been supported by longitudinal studies, which highlighted that clinical competence tends to decrease over time in mild cognitive impairment and AD (Moye et al., 2006; Okonkwo et al., 2008). The decrease of decisional capacity in AD has been related to cognitive decline in this population, with research focusing on cognitive predictors showing that problems with language, memory and executive function impact negatively on decisional capacity (Moye et al., 2006; Stormoen et al., 2014).

In addition to the characteristic cognitive deficits, behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) are an integral part of AD (Cerejeira et al., 2012; Zhao et al., 2016). BPSD are widespread non-cognitive symptoms, including apathy, aggression, delusions, psychosis, hallucinations, anxiety, irritability, depression and sleep disorders (van der Linde et al., 2014). The presence of BPSD has important negative consequences on AD patients and their caregivers. For instance, BPSD result in premature institutionalization, increased cost of care and diminished quality of life for both patients and caregivers (Black and Almeida, 2004; Scarmeas et al., 2007; Schaller et al., 2015; Feast et al., 2016). Mitoku and Shimanouchi (2014) explored the impact of BPSD on decision making capacity in older adults and highlighted that older adults with dementia presenting BPSD have decreased decisional capacity compared to patients without BPSD. Mograbi et al. (2015) showed in a large community-based study that BPSD correlated with unawareness of memory deficits in dementia, which may negatively affect clinical capacity.

Moreover, it has been shown that decisional capacity is impaired in various neuropsychiatric disorders (for review see Rahman et al., 2001; Candia and Barba, 2011). Looking especially at clinical competence, previous research highlighted that patients with schizophrenia, bipolar mood disorder and major depression have significant impairments in decisional abilities (Grisso and Appelbaum, 1995; Howe et al., 2005; Appelbaum and Redlich, 2006). Additionally, studies demonstrated that for patients with acute psychosis, treatment consent capacity was related to the extent and severity of symptoms, especially positive symptoms such as delusions and hallucinations (Howe et al., 2005; Rutledge et al., 2008). However, for patients with chronic psychosis, Palmer et al. (2004) showed a relationship between cognitive and negative symptoms (unusual thought content, conceptual disorganization) and impaired medical decision-making capacity.

Considering the scarcity of studies in this field, the aim of the present study was to explore the relationship between behavioral disturbance and decision-making in AD by comparing patients with or without BPSD on their capacity to consent to treatment.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A consecutive series of 71 patients and caregivers dyads were recruited from an AD outpatient unit. We included participants diagnosed with possible or probable AD according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV-TR). The clinical diagnosis of AD was made by a psychiatrist, based on clinical interviews with the patients and their caregivers, cognitive screening tests, laboratory tests and imaging studies. Inclusion criteria were: mild Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR = 1; n = 50) and moderate (CDR = 2; n = 21) dementia according to the CDR and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores of 11–26. We excluded people with head trauma, aphasia, history of alcoholism, psychotic symptoms, epilepsy and uncontrolled medical problems such as hypertension, depression and diabetes.

The primary caregiver was defined as the main person responsible for the care of the patient. All of the caregivers had been previously informed of the AD diagnosis by the psychiatrist. The patients completed assessments about cognition and competence to consent to treatment. The caregivers provided information about the patients’ demographics, the ability to perform activities daily living (ADL), neuropsychiatric symptoms, functionality, dementia severity and had depression and burden of care assessments.

Instruments

Competence to Consent to Treatment

The MacArthur Competence Tool for Treatment (MacCAT-T; adapted to Brazilian Portuguese; Santos et al., 2017) was used to assess competence to consent to treatment (Grisso et al., 1997). This scale permits to explore four different abilities: understanding, appreciation, reasoning and expression. The Understanding section assesses the capacity to paraphrase what has been disclosed. The Appreciation section assesses whether the individual acknowledges that the disclosed information applies to him or her and whether he or she recognizes the treatment possible benefits. Reasoning explores whether the person mentions any consequence of the treatment alternatives, comparison among alternatives and any consequences that were not mentioned in the disclosure. Finally, in the Expression section, the individual is supposed to offer a clear expression of a treatment choice and to explain how this choice was made. Patient responses were rated using the following scoring: 2 points for adequate; 1 point for partially sufficient; and 0 points for insufficient. Total scores in the MacCAT-T subscales ranged as follows: Understanding, 0–6; Appreciation, 0–4; Reasoning, 0–8; Expression, 0–2.

Cognition

For a general measure of cognitive level, the MMSE was used (Folstein et al., 1975; Bertolucci et al., 1994). The total score ranges from 0 to 30, with lower scores indicating impaired cognition. The Alzheimer Disease Assessment Scale—Cognitive Subscale (ADAS-Cog; Schultz et al., 2001), which assesses the intensity of cognitive changes, was also applied.

The ADAS-cog is 11-item scale used to assess the severity of selected areas of cognitive impairment (memory, language, orientation, reason and praxis). The maximum score is 70 and higher scores indicate poorer performance. Finally, attention and working memory were assessed with the Wechsler Digit Span Test, Forward and Backward (Wechsler, 1997; Nascimento, 2000). Scores of Digit Span Forward vary from 0 to 16, and scores of Digit Span Backward vary from 0 to 14, with higher scores indicating better performance.

Neuropsychiatric Symptoms

The Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) was used to assess 10 neuropsychiatric disturbances commonly observed in dementia (Cummings et al., 1994; Camozzato et al., 2008). The scale evaluates the presence of delusions, hallucinations, dysphoria, anxiety, agitation/aggression, euphoria, disinhibition, irritability/lability, apathy, aberrant motor activity, night-time behavior disturbances and appetite and eating abnormalities. The NPI is administrated to the patient’s caregiver, who rates each item in relation to their frequency (1 [absent] to 4 [frequent]) and to their severity (1 [mild] to 3 [severe]). The total score can range from 0 to 144 points, with higher scores indicating more severe psychopathology. For the purpose of this study, the subscales for each symptom were used, with patients being dichotomized according to the presence of the symptom (see “Statistical Analysis” Section below).

Dementia Severity

To determine dementia severity, the full protocol of the CDR was used (Morris, 1993; Maia et al., 2006), with severity ranging from 0 (no dementia) to 3 (severe dementia). Only patients with CDR 1 and 2 were included in the study.

Ethical Issues

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ)/Institute of Psychiatry Ethics Committee with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ)/Institute of Psychiatry Ethics Committee (Research Ethics Committee number 536.634).

Statistical Analysis

Demographic and clinical data are presented as means with standard deviation and range for the whole group. Dichotomous variables are presented as absolute numbers with percentages.

First, exploratory Pearsons correlations investigated the relationship between cognitive variables and clinical competence. Second, patients were dichotomized into two groups per subscale of the NPI. The first group included patients that did not report any symptoms on that particular subscale, whereas the second group comprised all the participants that had a score of 1 or more on that specific NPI subscale. Then, for each of the 10 NPI subscales, using Student t-tests for independent samples, we compared the mean scores on each scale of the MacCAT-T between the two groups. In case of a significant difference between the groups, the test was repeated now correcting for the ADAS-Cog score, to rule out the effect of global cognition differences. Additionally, to ensure that the significant differences between groups were not due to differences in sex, level of education, age and dementia severity, we tested differences according to these variables for each MacCAT-T section. For this purpose, independent samples t-tests were used, with patients being dichotomized according to sex, CDR (1: mild vs. 2: moderate dementia), educational level (1–9 years vs. 10+) and age (61–74 years old vs. 75+).

All tests were performed in SPSS20 (IBM-SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). A p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Participant Characteristics

A total of 71 patients with AD participated in this study. Sample characteristics and scores in the MacCAT-T can be found in Table 1. MacCAT-T results are consistent with previous data from patients with dementia, showing lower scores for Understanding and Reasoning when compared to a group of healthy controls (Moye et al., 2004).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics and scores on the neuropsychiatric inventory (NPI) and MacArthur Competence Tool for Treatment (MacCAT-T).

The frequency of patients with neuropsychiatric symptoms can be seen in Table 2. In addition, no significant differences in the MacCAT-T subscales were found for sex, level of education, age and disease severity.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Frequency of NPI symptoms.

Correlation Analyses

Correlations between cognitive variables and clinical competence can be seen in Table 3. Understanding was significantly correlated MMSE (r = 0.28, p < 0.05), ADAS-Cog (r = −0.30, p < 0.05) and Digit-backward (r = 0.24, p < 0.05). There was a significant correlation between Reasoning and ADAS-Cog (r = −0.28, p < 0.05), as well as between Reasoning and Digit-backward (r = 0.25, p < 0.05). There were no significant associations between Appreciation score and the cognitive variables, neither between Expression and these variables.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations between cognitive variables and clinical competence.

NPI Subscales

Patients with delusions had a lower score on the expression scale of the MacCAT-T (without: 1.9 ± 0.5 vs. with: 1.4 ± 0.9, t(69) = 2.91, p = 0.005, Figure 1B). This remained statistically significant after correction for the ADAS-Cog score (p = 0.006). Expression was also affected in patients with dysphoria/depression, who showed higher scores (without: 1.6 ± 0.8 vs. with: 1.9 ± 0.3, t(69) = −2.10, p = 0.039, Figure 1D), which remained significant after correction for the ADAS-Cog score (p = 0.038). This variable was also affected in apathetic patients, who had a lower score on expression as compared with the patients scoring 0 on this NPI subscale (without 1.9 ± 0.3 vs. with: 1.6 ± 0.7, t(69) = 2.35, p = 0.021, Figure 1I). After correcting for the ADAS-Cog score, this difference remained statistically significant (p = 0.023).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Differences on MacArthur Competence Tool for Treatment (MacCAT-T) subscales according to the presence of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD; A–J) bar graph of the mean with standard deviation of the MacCAT-T subscales per subscale of the neuropsychiatric inventory (NPI). White bars represent the patients with a score of 0 on that domain, black bars those patients with a score of 1 or higher on that domain; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01; significant differences can be seen for delusions, dysphoria and apathy (expression of choice) and for euphoria (reasoning).

AD patients with euphoria had a lower score on the reasoning scale of the MacCAT-T (without: 3.4 ± 1.5 vs. with: 1.7 ± 0.5, t(69) = 2.12, p = 0.038, Figure 1H). However, this effect is not statistically significant after correction for the ADAS-Cog scores (p = 0.082).

There were no between-group differences in MacCAT-T scores for patients with hallucinations (Figure 1A), agitation/aggression (Figure 1C), anxiety (Figure 1E), irritability (Figure 1F), disinhibition (Figure 1G) and aberrant motor behavior (Figure 1J; in all cases, p > 0.05).

Discussion

The aim of this exploratory study was to investigate the relationship between behavioral symptoms and capacity of clinical decision-making in AD. The results indicate that patients with symptoms of delusions and apathy exhibit impaired expression of choice in comparison to patients without these symptoms. By contrast, patients with symptoms of dysphoria and depression have higher scores on this particular subscale relative to patients without symptoms. None of the differences were confounded by global cognition as measured by the ADAS-Cog. Additionally, patients with euphoria score lower on the subscale of reasoning compared to patients without these symptoms, however, when correcting for global cognition using the ADAS-Cog, this result is no longer significant.

Worse performances on the expression section of the MacCAT-T were seen for patients with both delusion and apathy. The result for delusions may be explained by an error in reality monitoring, which may impair the ability of patients to clearly express and explain treatment choices. For apathy, increasing space has been given to the role of emotional processes in decision making (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979; Damasio, 1994). It is possible that apathetic patients cannot rely on affective information to express their treatment choices. Apathy is also characterized by lack of motivated behavior, including the difficulty to engage in a cognitively demanding task (Marin, 1990) and this may be a potential reason for the lower scores of apathetic patients when asked to express a treatment choice. Indeed, the expression section of the MacCAT-T appears as a highly demanding task in term of motivational resources. Additionally, the impaired capacity to clearly express a treatment choice may affect decision making. For example, patients impaired in their ability to express their decisions may be excluded from the decision-making process by caregivers, contributing to malignant social psychology, which is defined by caregivers’ behaviors that undermine the personhood and wellbeing of PwD (Kitwood, 1997).

In our study, better expression of treatment choice was shown for individuals presenting depression. This result is in line with the literature highlighting that depressed people may have preserved decision-making capacities (Appelbaum and Grisso, 1995; for review see Hindmarch et al., 2013) and awareness of condition (Mograbi and Morris, 2014; Bertrand et al., 2016). An explanation can be found in the depressive realism theory, which argues that depression contributes to a more realistic judgment, as opposed to the normal positive or optimistic biases that are associated with an euthymic mood state (Dobson and Franche, 1989; Taylor, 1989).

Results showing that the presence of mania decreased the capacity on the reasoning section are consistent with findings in the literature showing executive function deficits in bipolar disorder, especially during mania (Dixon et al., 2004; Mur et al., 2007). Preserved executive functions, including abilities such as cognitive flexibility, problem-solving, planning and inhibition, appear essential in the reasoning section of the MacCAT-T, in which patients have to consider treatment alternatives and compare the consequences of these treatments. Previous studies exploring the relationship between specific neuropsychological abilities and medical decision-making showed that tests evaluating executive functioning correlated with the reasoning component of treatment consent capacity in clinical and healthy populations (for review, see Palmer and Savla, 2007). Additionally, mania has been associated with reduced awareness about the condition (de Assis da Silva et al., 2015a,b; 2016), including in patients with AD (Migliorelli et al., 1995). Lack of awareness about the condition may impact negatively reasoning about the treatment.

Some potential limitations of the current study must be considered. First, the absence of a control group may limit the interpretation of the results. Nevertheless, the lack of a control group is a feature of the current study, considering that we are exploring neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia and their impact on decision making. Additionally, data from previous studies were reported, providing comparison points. Second, additional data from neuropsychological testing would have been useful to support some of the potential cognitive explanations proposed above. Third, because of statistical analysis limitation due to the sample size, the severity and the frequency of the neuropsychiatric symptoms were not taken into account. Fourth, we used DSM-IV-TR diagnosis criteria, which have been criticized by their insufficient diagnostic specificity. Nonetheless, DSM-IV-TR criteria have been used thoroughly in dementia research and this is a limitation that our study shares with a very large number of publications in the field of dementia studies. Finally, the present study did not consider the possible interaction of multiple neuropsychiatric symptoms in relation to decision-making capacity. Specifically in the case of apathy and depression, which have been shown to be both related to expression of a choice in the present study, although there is some overlap between the conditions, recent research has emphasized how these are separate entities (Aalten et al., 2008; Robert et al., 2009; Spalletta et al., 2010; Selbæk and Engedal, 2012). Future studies should explore the impact of multiple neuropsychiatric symptoms on the capacity to consent treatment.

Most studies in the literature focused on the cognitive predictors of decision-making capacity in dementia. However, our study stresses also a relationship between BPSD and treatment consent capacity in AD patients. These findings may have important clinical implications. Indeed, in addition to improving patients and caregivers’ quality of life, strategies aiming to reduce behavioral disturbances may lead to better decisional abilities for patient with dementia, which consequently will increase patient’s autonomy and well-being. To the best of your knowledge, this is the first study exploring directly the relationship between BPSD and consent to treatment capacity in AD patients. Therefore, future research is needed, especially to distinguish the impact of both cognitive and behavioral deficits on decision-making capacity in AD. Studies using a longitudinal design may be useful in order to understand the direction of the relationship BPSD/decision-making capacity.

Author Contributions

MCND, DCM, JL and JL-F conceived the study design. RLS and EB were responsible for data collection. ED and DCM performed the data analysis. EB, DCM and ED drafted the manuscript. All authors revised and approved the final manuscript and are accountable for all aspects of the work.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer SB and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation, and the handling Editor states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by CNPq—Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico—Braziland by FAPERJ-Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio de Janeiro-Brazil. The authors also acknowledge financial support from the Capes Foundation, Ministry of Education of Brazil.

References

Aalten, P., Verhey, F. R. J., Boziki, M., Brugnolo, A., Bullock, R., Byrne, E. J., et al. (2008). Consistency of neuropsychiatric syndromes across dementias: results from the European Alzheimer Disease Consortium. Part II. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 25, 1–8. doi: 10.1159/000111082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appelbaum, P. S., and Grisso, T. (1995). The macarthur treatment competence study. I: mental illness and competence to consent to treatment. Law Hum. Behav. 19, 105–126. doi: 10.1007/bf01499321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appelbaum, P. S., and Redlich, A. (2006). Impact of decisional capacity on the use of leverage to encourage treatment adherence. Community Ment. Health J. 42, 121–130. doi: 10.1007/s10597-005-9015-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berg, J. W., Appelbaum, P. S., , Lidz, C. W., Parker, L. S. (2001). Informed Consent: Legal Theory and Clinical Practice. Fair Lawn, NJ: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Berghmans, R. L., and Widdershoven, G. A. (2003). Ethical perspectives on decision-making capacity and consent for treatment and research. Med. Law 22, 391–400.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bertolucci, P. H., Brucki, S. M., Campacci, S. R., and Juliano, Y. (1994). The mini-mental state Examination in a general population: impact of educational status. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 52, 1–7. doi: 10.1590/S0004-282X1994000100001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertrand, E., Dourado, M. C. N., Laks, J., Morris, R. G., Landeira-Fernandez, J., and Mograbi, D. C. (2016). Mood-congruent recollection and anosognosia in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 84, 55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Black, W., and Almeida, O. P. (2004). A systematic review of the association between the Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia and burden of care. Int. Psychogeriatr. 16, 295–315. doi: 10.1017/s1041610204000468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camozzato, A. L., Kochhann, R., Simeoni, C., Konrath, C. A., Pedro Franz, A., Carvalho, A., et al. (2008). Reliability of the Brazilian Portuguese version of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) for patients with Alzheimer’s disease and their caregivers. Int. Psychogeriatr. 20, 383–393. doi: 10.1017/S1041610207006254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Candia, P. C., and Barba, A. C. (2011). Mental capacity and consent to treatment in psychiatric patients: the state of the research. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 24, 442–446. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328349bba5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cerejeira, J., Lagarto, L., and Mukaetova-Ladinska, E. B. (2012). Behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. Front. Neurol. 3:73. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummings, J. L., Mega, M., Gray, K., Rosenberg-Thompson, S., Carusi, D. A., and Gornbein, J. (1994). The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 44, 2308–2314. doi: 10.1212/WNL.44.12.2308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and The Human Brain. New York, NY: Putnam and Sons.

Google Scholar

de Assis da Silva, R., Mograbi, D. C., Bifano, J., Santana, C. M. T., and Cheniaux, E. (2016). Insight in bipolar mania: evaluation of its heterogeneity and correlation with clinical symptoms. J. Affect. Disord. 199, 95–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Assis da Silva, R., Mograbi, D. C., Camelo, E. V. M., Bifano, J., Wainstok, M., Silveira, L. A. S., et al. (2015a). Insight in bipolar disorder: a comparison between mania, depression and euthymia using the Insight Scale for Affective Disorders. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 37, 152–156. doi: 10.1590/2237-6089-2015-0014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Assis da Silva, R., Mograbi, D. C., Silveira, L. A. S., Nunes, A. L. S., Novis, F. D., Landeira-Fernandez, J., et al. (2015b). Insight across the different mood states of bipolar disorder. Psychiatr. Q. 86, 395–405. doi: 10.1007/s11126-015-9340-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, T., Kravariti, E., Frith, C., Murray, R. M., and McGuire, P. K. (2004). Effect of symptoms on executive function in bipolar illness. Psychol. Med. 34, 811–821. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703001570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobson, K., and Franche, R.-L. (1989). A conceptual and empirical review of the depressive realism hypothesis. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 21, 419–433. doi: 10.1037/h0079839

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feast, A., Moniz-Cook, E., Stoner, C., Charlesworth, G., and Orrell, M. (2016). A systematic review of the relationship between behavioral and psychological symptoms (BPSD) and caregiver well-being. Int. Psychogeriatr. 28, 1761–1774. doi: 10.1017/s1041610216000922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Grisso, T., and Appelbaum, P. S. (1995). The macarthur treatment competence study. III: abilities of patients to consent to psychiatric and medical treatments. Law Hum. Behav. 19, 149–174. doi: 10.1007/bf01499323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grisso, T., Appelbaum, P. S., and Hill-Fotouhi, C. (1997). The MacCAT-T: a clinical tool to assess patients’ capacities to make treatment decisions. Psychiatr. Serv. 48, 1415–1419. doi: 10.1176/ps.48.11.1415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hindmarch, T., Hotopf, M., and Owen, G. S. (2013). Depression and decision-making capacity for treatment or research: a systematic review. BMC Med. Ethics 14:54. doi: 10.1186/1472-6939-14-54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howe, V., Foister, K., Jenkins, K., Skene, L., Copolov, D., and Keks, N. (2005). Competence to give informed consent in acute psychosis is associated with symptoms rather than diagnosis. Schizophr. Res. 77, 211–214. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47, 263–292. doi: 10.2307/1914185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karlawish, J. H. T., Casarett, D. J., James, B. D., Xie, S. X., and Kim, S. Y. H. (2005). The ability of persons with Alzheimer disease (AD) to make a decision about taking an AD treatment. Neurology 64, 1514–1519. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000160000.01742.9d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitwood, T. M. (1997). Dementia Reconsidered: The Person Comes First. Buckingham: Open University Press.

Google Scholar

Lui, V. W.-C., Lam, L. C.-W., Chau, R. C.-M., Fung, A. W.-T., Wong, B. M.-L., Leung, G. T.-Y., et al. (2012). Capacity to make decisions on medication management in Chinese older persons with mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Int. Psychogeriatri. 24, 1103–1111. doi: 10.1017/s1041610212000129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maia, A. L. G., Godinho, C., Ferreira, E. D., Almeida, V., Schuh, A., Kaye, J., et al. (2006). Application of the Brazilian version of the CDR scale in samples of dementia patients. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 64, 485–489. doi: 10.1590/S0004-282X2006000300025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marin, R. S. (1990). Differential diagnosis and classification of apathy. Am. J. Psychiatry 147, 22–30. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.1.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marson, D. C. (2001). Loss of competency in Alzheimer’s disease: conceptual and psychometric approaches. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 24, 267–283. doi: 10.1016/s0160-2527(01)00064-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Migliorelli, R., Tesón, A., Sabe, L., Petracca, G., Petracchi, M., Leiguarda, R., et al. (1995). Anosognosia in Alzheimer’s disease: a study of associated factors. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 7, 338–344. doi: 10.1176/jnp.7.3.338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitoku, K., and Shimanouchi, S. (2014). The decision-making and communication capacities of older adults with dementia: a population-based study. Open Nurs. J. 8, 17–24. doi: 10.2174/1874434620140512001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mograbi, D. C., Ferri, C. P., Stewart, R., Sosa, A. L., Brown, R. G., Laks, J., et al. (2015). Neuropsychological and behavioral disturbance correlates of unawareness of memory impairment in dementia: a population-based study. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 28, 3–11. doi: 10.1177/0891988714541868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mograbi, D. C., and Morris, R. G. (2014). On the relation among mood, apathy, and anosognosia in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 20, 2–7. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713001276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. C. (1993). The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43, 2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/WNL.43.11.2412-a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moye, J., Karel, M. J., Azar, A. R., and Gurrera, R. J. (2004). Capacity to consent to treatment: empirical comparison of three instruments in older adults with and without dementia. Gerontologist 44, 166–175. doi: 10.1093/geront/44.2.166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moye, J., Karel, M. J., Gurrera, R. J., and Azar, A. R. (2006). Neuropsychological predictors of decision-making capacity over 9 months in mild-to-moderate dementia. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 21, 78–83. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2005.00288.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mur, M., Portella, M. J., Martínez-Arán, A., Pifarré, J., and Vieta, E. (2007). Persistent neuropsychological deficit in euthymic bipolar patients: executive function as a core deficit. J. Clin. Psychiatry 68, 1078–1086. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v68n0715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nascimento, E. (2000). Adaptation and Validation of Test Wais-III for a Brazilian Context. Doctoral thesis. Brasilia: Universidade de Brasília. Available online at: http://bases.bireme.br/cgi-bin/wxislind.exe/iah/online/?IsisScript=iah/iah.xis&src=google&base=LILACS&lang=p&nextAction=lnk&exprSearch=317037&indexSearch=ID

Okonkwo, O. C., Griffith, H. R., Copeland, J. N., Belue, K., Lanza, S., Zamrini, E. Y., et al. (2008). Medical decision-making capacity in mild cognitive impairment: a 3-year longitudinal study. Neurology 71, 1474–1480. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000334301.32358.48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmer, B. W., Dunn, L. B., Appelbaum, P. S., and Jeste, D. V. (2004). Correlates of treatment-related decision-making capacity among middle-agedand older patients with schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 61, 230–236. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.3.230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmer, B. W., and Savla, G. N. (2007). The association of specific neuropsychological deficits with capacity to consent to research or treatment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 13, 1047–1059. doi: 10.1017/s1355617707071299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

President’s Commission for the Study of Ethical Problems in Medicine and Biomedical and Behavioral Research. (1982). Making Health Care Decisions: A Report on the Ethical and Legal Implications of Informed Consent in the Patient-Practitioner Relationship. (Vol. 1) Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.

Rahman, S., Sahakian, B. J., Cardinal, R. N., Rogers, R. D., and Robbins, T. W. (2001). Decision making and neuropsychiatry. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 271–277. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01650-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robert, P., Onyike, C. U., Leentjens, A. F. G., Dujardin, K., Aalten, P., Starkstein, S., et al. (2009). Proposed diagnostic criteria for apathy in Alzheimer’s disease and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Eur. Psychiatry 24, 98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutledge, E., Kennedy, M., O’Neill, H., and Kennedy, H. G. (2008). Functional mental capacity is not independent of the severity of psychosis. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 31, 9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2007.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santos, R. L., de Sousa, M. F. B., Simões Neto, J. P., Bertrand, E., Mograbi, D. C., Landeira-Fernandez, J., et al. (2017). MacArthur competence assessment tool for treatment in Alzheimer disease: cross-cultural adaptation. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 75, 36–43. doi: 10.1590/0004-282x20160181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarmeas, N., Brandt, J., Blacker, D., Albert, M., Hadjigeorgiou, G., Dubois, B., et al. (2007). Disruptive behavior as a predictor in Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 64, 1755–1761. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.12.1755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaller, S., Mauskopf, J., Kriza, C., Wahlster, P., and Kolominsky-Rabas, P. L. (2015). The main cost drivers in dementia: a systematic review. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 30, 111–129. doi: 10.1002/gps.4198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, R. R., Siviero, M. O., and Bertolucci, P. H. F. (2001). The cognitive subscale of the andquot “Alzheimer’s disease assessment Scaleandquot” in a brazilian sample. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 34, 1295–1302. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x2001001000009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selbæk, G., and Engedal, K. (2012). Stability of the factor structure of the neuropsychiatric inventory in a 31-month follow-up study of a large sample of nursing-home patients with dementia. Int. Psychogeriatr. 24, 62–73. doi: 10.1017/S104161021100086X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smebye, K. L., Kirkevold, M., and Engedal, K. (2012). How do persons with dementia participate in decision making related to health and daily care? A multi-case study. BMC Health Serv. Res. 12:241. doi: 10.1186/1472-6963-12-241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spalletta, G., Musicco, M., Padovani, A., Perri, R., Fadda, L., Canonico, V., et al. (2010). Neuropsychiatric symptoms and syndromes in a large cohort of newly diagnosed, untreated patients with alzheimer disease. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18, 1026–1035. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181d6b68d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stormoen, S., Almkvist, O., Eriksdotter, M., Sundström, E., and Tallberg, I.-M. (2014). Cognitive predictors of medical decision-making capacity in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 29, 1304–1311. doi: 10.1002/gps.4114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E. (1989). Positive Illusions: Creative Self-Deception and The Healthy Mind. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Tepper, A. M., and Elwork, A. (1984). Competence to consent to treatment as a psycholegal construct. Law Hum. Behav. 8, 205–223. doi: 10.1007/bf01044693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Linde, R. M., Dening, T., Matthews, F. E., and Brayne, C. (2014). Grouping of behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 29, 562–568. doi: 10.1002/gps.4037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale®-3rd Edn. (WAIS®-III). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

Zhao, Q.-F., Tan, L., Wang, H.-F., Jiang, T., Tan, M.-S., Tan, L., et al. (2016). The prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 190, 264–271. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: BPSD, clinical capacity, decision making, consent to treatment, dementia, Alzheimer’s disease

Citation: Bertrand E, van Duinkerken E, Landeira-Fernandez J, Dourado MCN, Santos RL, Laks J and Mograbi DC (2017) Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms Impact Clinical Competence in Alzheimer’s Disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:182. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00182

Received: 26 November 2016; Accepted: 24 May 2017;
Published: 16 June 2017.

Edited by:

Agustin Ibanez, Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), Argentina

Reviewed by:

Olivier Piguet, University of Sydney, Australia
Sandra Baez, Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), Argentina

Copyright © 2017 Bertrand, van Duinkerken, Landeira-Fernandez, Dourado, Santos, Laks and Mograbi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elodie Bertrand, elodie.bertrand1@gmail.com

Download