Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 12 December 2017
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 9 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00413

Epigenome-Wide Association Study of Cognitive Functioning in Middle-Aged Monozygotic Twins

Anna Starnawska1,2,3* Qihua Tan4,5 Matt McGue4,6 Ole Mors1,3,7 Anders D. Børglum1,2,3 Kaare Christensen4,5,8,9 Mette Nyegaard1,2,3 Lene Christiansen4,8
  • 1The Lundbeck Foundation Initiative for Integrative Psychiatric Research, iPSYCH, Aarhus, Denmark
  • 2Department of Biomedicine, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
  • 3Center for Integrative Sequencing, iSEQ, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
  • 4The Danish Twin Registry, Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
  • 5Department of Clinical Genetics, Odense University Hospital, Odense, Denmark
  • 6Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
  • 7Psychosis Research Unit, Aarhus University Hospital, Risskov, Denmark
  • 8The Danish Aging Research Center, Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
  • 9Department of Clinical Biochemistry and Pharmacology, Odense University Hospital, Odense, Denmark

As the world's population ages, the age-related cognitive decline presents a great challenge to world's healthcare systems. One of the molecular mechanisms implicated in cognitive ageing is DNA methylation, an epigenetic modification known to be a key player in memory formation, maintenance, and synaptic plasticity. Using the twin design we performed an epigenome-wide association study (EWAS) in a population of 486 middle-aged monozygotic twins (mean age at follow-up 65.9, SD = 6.1) and correlated their blood DNA methylation to their level (cross-sectional analysis) and change in cognitive abilities over 10 years (longitudinal analysis). We identified several CpG sites where cross-sectional cognitive functioning was associated with DNA methylation levels. The top identified loci were located in ZBTB46 (p = 5.84 × 10−7), and TAF12 (p = 4.91 × 10−7). KEGG's enrichment analyses of the most associated findings identified “Neuroactive ligand-receptor interaction” as the most enriched pathway (p = 0.0098). Change in cognitive functioning over 10 years was associated with DNA methylation levels in AGBL4 (p = 9.01 × 10−7) and SORBS1 (p = 5.28 × 10−6), with the first gene playing an important role in neuronal survival and the latter gene implicated before in Alzheimer's disease and ischemic stroke. Our findings point to an association between changes in DNA methylation of genes related to neuronal survival and change of cognitive functioning in aging individuals.

Introduction

The age-related cognitive decline is suggested to be caused by a combination of subtle and selective, region-specific changes resulting from alterations in dendritic morphology, cellular connectivity, Ca2+ homeostasis, gene expression and other factors altogether affecting neuronal plasticity (Burke and Barnes, 2006; Tripathi, 2012; Morrison and Baxter, 2014). According to monozygotic and dizygotic twin studies cognitive levels and cognitive decline are also influenced by individual's genetic background, with heritability estimates for these traits varying depending on the cognitive test applied (Swan, 1992; McGue and Christensen, 2002, 2013). However, the correlation of cognitive abilities between twins was reported to decline with age what points to non-shared environmental and stochastic effects accumulated across lifespan to influence levels of cognitive functioning in aging individuals (McCartney et al., 1990; McGue and Christensen, 2013).

Among various age-related changes taking place in the brain and multiple factors influencing them there is a growing body of evidence for a crucial role of DNA methylation in memory formation, maintenance, synaptic plasticity, and also cognitive functioning (Miller et al., 2008, 2010; Day and Sweatt, 2010; Yu et al., 2011; Roth et al., 2015; Halder et al., 2016; Maag et al., 2017). DNA methylation is an epigenetic modification known to be a powerful regulator of gene expression across tissues and lifespan (Feinberg, 2007; Jones, 2012; Lister et al., 2013; Medvedeva et al., 2014). Along the human life age-related changes in DNA methylation seem to be driven by two mechanisms: epigenetic clock and epigenetic drift, where the first phenomena describes functional age-related epigenetic changes at specific sites of the genome common across individuals, while the latter one relates to the global decrease in stability and precision of DNA methylation with age (Jones et al., 2015). These genome-wide changes in DNA methylation levels are complex and tend to increase with age in regions known for their low methylation, e.g., promoter-associated CpG islands, but decrease in regions with high DNA methylation, e.g., intergenic non-island CpG sites, thus leading to a global loss of methylation in the genome (Illingworth and Bird, 2009; Heyn et al., 2012). Maintenance of DNA methylation in the human genome across life depends mainly on two groups of enzymes: DNA methyltransferases (DNMTs), responsible for the addition of methyl group to the 5th position of cytosine, and ten-eleven translocation (TET) family enzymes that catalyze oxidation of 5-methylcytosine to forms further removed by base excision repair and substituted back with an unmodified cytosine (Chen and Riggs, 2011; Kohli and Zhang, 2013). There is a growing body of evidence linking these DNA methylation maintenance enzymes with cognitive functioning. Dnmt1 and Dnmt3a deficiency in adult mouse central neurons system was reported to cause defects in synaptic plasticity, learning and memory (Feng et al., 2010). In the mouse hippocampus cognitive decline was associated with decreased expression of Dnmt3a2, while rescuing its transcription levels was shown to restore cognitive functions in both young and aged mice (Oliveira et al., 2012, 2015). Tet1-deficient mice were reported to have normal brain development, but impaired spacial learning and memory. Moreover, decrease in Tet1 expression in neural progenitor cells was associated with aberrant promoter hypermethylation of genes associated with regulation of adult neurogenesis (Zhang et al., 2013). All these studies point to the important role of DNA methylation in cognitive functioning and age-related cognitive decline.

Identification of these DNA methylation changes in an accessible tissue could allow for an early detection of decrease in cognitive functioning before more severe cognitive impairment appears, but also point us to genes involved in cognitive ageing.

In this study we aimed at investigating if blood DNA methylation signatures associate with cross-sectional level of cognitive functioning, as well as longitudinal change in cognition over a preceding 10-year period. As levels of both cross-sectional and longitudinal cognitive functioning are known to be influenced by genetic factors one of the powerful study designs to delineate epigenetic signatures associated with a phenotype of interest is using monozygotic twins (McGue and Christensen, 2013; Davies et al., 2015). Several studies reported differences in DNA methylation levels between discordant monozygotic twins to be associated with diagnosis of common disorders, such as cancer, diabetes, autoimmune diseases, as well as mental disorders (Javierre et al., 2010; Dempster et al., 2011, 2014; Heyn et al., 2013; Stefan et al., 2014; Castellani et al., 2015). We therefore performed an epigenome-wide association study (EWAS) between cognitive abilities and DNA methylation patterns in blood samples obtained from 243 pairs of monozygotic middle-aged twins, which allowed us to correct for possible genetic and shared environmental effects.

Materials and Methods

Study Population and Cognitive Functioning Assessment

The study was performed on 486 monozygotic twins (111 female and 132 male pairs) recruited by the Danish Twin Registry as a part of The Middle Aged Danish Twin Study (MADT). The cohort is a Danish nation-wide investigation of twins randomly selected among eligible twin pairs from birth cohorts 1931–1952 (Skytthe et al., 2013). MADT was initiated in 1998, and a follow-up study was conducted in 2008-11. Whole blood samples were collected from all individuals during the follow-up visit. Zygosity for MADT cohort was assessed as described before by Christiansen and co-authors (Christiansen et al., 2003). Informed consent was obtained for all participants. Written informed consents obtained from all participants, and the surveys, including collection of blood and use of survey information, were approved by the Regional Committees on Health Research Ethics for Southern Denmark (S-VF-19980072). The present study included all monozygotic twin pairs from the MADT cohort that participated in both surveys (at the admission and at follow-up). Mean age of investigated twins at follow-up was 65.9 (SD = 6.1, min = 55, max = 79).

General cognitive functioning was assessed at both occasions for all twins (at the admission and at follow-up) by computing a cognitive composite score. The score was based on six brief cognitive tests (verbal fluency, immediate word recall, delayed word recall, processing speed, attention, and working memory) as previously described (McGue and Christensen, 2002). There are two reasons for using the cognitive composite score rather than the individual cognitive component scores in our analyses. First, the cognitive component scores manifest the typical positive inter-correlation that is seen among specific cognitive measures and this is taken as evidence for the existence of a general cognitive factor that underlies all the individual abilities, which is what our cognitive composite seeks to assess (McGue and Christensen, 2001). Second, it was shown that the cognitive composite score used in this study is a substantially more reliable measure of cognitive functioning over time than the individual component scores (McGue and Christensen, 2001).

Scores obtained from each cognitive test were first standardized to a mean of 0 and SD of 1 and then the 6 scores were summed to obtain the composite score. The same standardization parameters were used with both the intake and follow-up assessments to ensure the two composite scores were commeasurable. No more than one missing component was allowed and in case of individuals where two or more component scores were missing the cognitive functioning score was coded as not available. In the case where only one component measure was missing the computed cognitive functioning score was generated by prorating the available scores (i.e., by multiplying the provisional score by 6/5), as described before for this cognitive measure (Petersen et al., 2016).

DNA Methylation Analysis

Whole blood samples were collected for all individuals during the follow-up assessment. Genomic DNA was extracted from buffy-coats with the use of the semi-automated salt precipitation protocol with Autopure System (Qiagen, Valencia, CA, USA). Genomic DNA (500 ng/sample) was bisulfite converted with EZ Methylation Gold Kit (Zymo Research, Irvine, CA, USA) and analyzed using the Infinium HumanMethylation450 BeadChips (Illumina, San Diego, CA, USA) array according to manufacturer's protocol. Obtained DNA methylation data was subjected to quality control with two different pipelines, combination of MethylAid (van Iterson et al., 2014) and minfi tools (Aryee et al., 2014). Probes with low bead count (<3 beads), high detection p-value (>0.01), zero signal, missing in >5% of samples and cross-reactive ones as reported before (Chen et al., 2013) were removed from the dataset, leaving 453,014 probes for further analysis. Data was normalized with the use of functional normalization (Fortin et al., 2014) and obtained beta values were further logit transformed.

Blood Cell Composition

Blood cell counts were available for 477 individuals, where blood leukocyte subtypes (monocytes, lymphocytes, basophils, neutrophils, eosinophiles) were counted using a Coulter LH 750 Hematology Analyzer (Beckman Coulter, Woerden, The Netherlands). For the remaining nine individuals, where blood cell composition was not available, data was imputed by partial least squares regression. A regression model was first fitted based on the samples for which measurements were available, and afterwards the model was applied for prediction of missing cell counts. The model used log(cell count+1) as response and included all beta values, that were available for all samples, as covariates. Detailed procedure for cell imputation is provided on GitHub (https://github.com/mvaniterson/wbccPredictor). Further covariates included were gender, age at follow-up visit, and sentrix position (modeled by two categorical variables indicating the position in each of the two directions on the chip: Left or Right and respectively 1–6 for the other direction). All calculations were performed in R (R Core Team, 2015).

Statistical Analyses

The analyses were performed with two approaches: paired and unpaired. In the paired approach we first calculated the intra-pair difference in DNA methylation level, cross-sectional cognition (measured at follow-up) and 10-year longitudinal change of cognition, as well as difference in blood cell counts for each twin pair. Longitudinal change of cognition was calculated by subtracting the cognitive score obtained at the beginning of the study from the cognitive score at the follow-up. The null hypothesis that there was no change in cognition over 10-year period was tested with linear mixed model specifying twin pairing as random effect.

Epigenome-wide association study (EWAS) analyses were performed in four different combinations to test cross-sectional and longitudinal cognitive functioning in both within-pair (paired) and individual (unpaired) approaches. First, in the paired analyses we used linear regression models to test for an association between intra-pair DNA methylation difference and the cross-sectional cognition difference, as well as difference in longitudinal change in cognition. The models were adjusted for sex and age at the follow-up visit of investigated twin pairs, as well as for their blood cell composition difference. As a subtype of the paired EWAS analysis for cross-sectional cognition we repeated the analysis including only the 50% most discordant twins, expecting that removing the twin pairs with almost no intra-pair difference in cognition can strengthen the signal of the most associated loci with cognitive functioning.

Secondly, we performed the unpaired analyses where all twins were treated as separate individuals but controlled for dependency of observations within twin pairs in order to avoid deflated p-values. Association of DNA methylation with cognition at follow-up, as well as with change of cognition over time were investigated with linear mixed models in order to correct for relatedness in the sample. The unpaired analyses were also adjusted for sex, age at the follow-up visit and cell composition of the investigated individuals. This study used two types of analyses (paired and unpaired) as they involve different statistical models that differently estimate and handle confounding factors. The paired analysis works on intra-pair differences and corrects for the effects from pair-specific confounding factors (age and sex) on intra-pair differences. This aspect of the analysis is important because the intra-pair differences can increase with increasing age of the pair. Moreover, intra-pair difference can be different between male pairs and female pairs. The unpaired analysis also corrects for age and sex, but it does that for the effects on individual level of DNA methylation. The age and sex effects on intra-pair difference are ignored in this case. Additionally, as the unpaired analysis works on the individual level data it is capable of using incomplete twin pairs what increases sample size.

In this study we report all findings with unadjusted p < 10−5 to provide a better overview of the top associated findings, while the suggested genome-wide significance threshold for EWAS analyses performed on data generated with 450K DNA methylation array is p < 10−6 (Tsai and Bell, 2015).

Pathway enrichment analysis was performed using Kyoto Encyclopedia of Genes and Genomes (KEGG) and Pathway Commons enrichment with WebGestalt (Wang et al., 2013) against the genes included in the 450K DNA methylation array. P-values from enrichment analyses were corrected for multiple testing with Benjamini-Hochberg correction method (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).

Results

DNA methylation data for all investigated individuals (n = 486) passed all quality control steps performed with minfi and MethylAid tools. Cognition data was available for 485 individuals at admission (mean = −0.53, SD = 3.71, min = −9.41, max = 10.27) and 480 individuals at follow-up (mean = −2.17, SD = 3.76, min = −16.75, max = 12.53). Cognitive functioning of the investigated individuals was found to decrease over the 10-year period (p < 2 × 10−16, Supplementary Figure 1). Additional data on sample demographics split by sex of investigated monozygotic twin pairs is presented in Supplementary Table 1.

For the paired analysis regressing intra-pair difference in DNA methylation on intra-pair difference in cognition at follow-up resulted in five probes with p < 10−5 with cg05867245, mapping to the Zinc Finger And BTB Domain Containing 46 gene (ZBTB46), being the most associated finding with p = 5.84 × 10−7 (Table 1). The probe was also found to be associated in the 50% most discordant twins analysis, however with a higher p = 2.45 × 10−5. Apart from cg05867245, three more probes (p < 10−4) were identified for cognition at follow-up in both the analysis of all twins and the 50% most discordant twins (Supplementary Table 2). Regressing intra-pair methylation differences on longitudinal change in cognition over time resulted in 11 findings with p < 10−5, with the most associated probe cg03599618 mapping to ATP/GTP Binding Protein-Like 4 gene (AGBL4) with p = 9.01 × 10−7.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Overview of the results (p < 10−5) from the EWAS of cognitive functioning in the paired and unpaired analyses.

In the unpaired analyses 5 CpG sites were identified with p < 10−5, with the most associated probe identified as cg07898057, annotated to TATA Box Binding Protein (TBP)-Associated Factor gene (TAF12) (p = 4.91 × 10−7). Unpaired analysis of change of cognition over time resulted in only one probe with p < 10−5 (cg25421530) mapping to the Sorbin And SH3 Domain Containing 1 gene (SORBS1) (p = 5.28 × 10−6). All probes with p < 10−5 in paired or unpaired analyses were annotated with their most proximal gene, genomic position and CpG island context and are presented together with their regression p-values in Table 1. Manhattan plots depicting results from all four main analyses are presented as Figures 1A–D.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Manhattan plots from EWAS for cross-sectional (paired A and unpaired B) and longitudinal cognitive functioning (paired C and unpaired D) analyses. Probes with p < 10−6 are labeled with the probe name and annotation to the most proximal gene (A–C). Due to no findings with p < 10−6 in the unpaired analysis of longitudinal cognitive functioning the most associated probe was marked on (D).

In order to check if there is an overlap of results between all four EWAS analyses (with findings with p < 10−4) we prepared two Venn diagrams (Supplementary Figures 2A,B) that depict probe and gene overlap. Four probes (p < 10−4) were found to be common in the paired analyses between cross-sectional and longitudinal cognitive functioning. One probe was common in cognition at follow-up analyses between paired and unpaired analyses, and one probe was common in change of cognition analyses between paired and unpaired analyses (Table 2). Search for common genes across analyses identified the Ewing Tumor-Associated Antigen 1 (ETAA1), Annexin A6 (ANXA6), DIO3 Opposite Strand/Antisense RNA (DIO3OS), Transmembrane Protein 132D (TMEM132D), as well as Inositol Polyphosphate-5-Phosphatase A (INPP5A), and Transcriptional Adaptor 1 (TADA1) for the paired analyses (cross-sectional vs. longitudinal cognitive functioning). For cognition at follow-up, ZBTB46 and the Src Homology 2 Domain Containing F gene (SHF) were common between paired and unpaired analyses, while SORBS1 was a common gene for change of cognition. An overview of the overlapping probes with corresponding p-values and annotations is presented in Table 2, while additionally overlapping genes between analyses are presented in Supplementary Table 3.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overlap of associated probes (p < 10−4) identified from the paired and unpaired EWAS analyses.

Pathway analyses for KEGG and Pathway Commons were performed on all results with p < 10−4 obtained from both paired and unpaired analyses on cognition at follow-up and change of cognition (Table 3 and Supplementary Table 4). Pathway analysis for KEGG, based on top results from paired analysis of cognition at follow-up, identified “Neuroactive ligand-receptor interaction” (p = 0.0098 BH-adjusted p = 0.059) and “Gap junction” (p = 0.030, BH-adjusted p = 0.091) as the most enriched pathways. The most enriched pathways in Pathway Commons analysis, based on top results from paired cognition at follow-up, were “GABA receptor activation” (p = 0.0056, BH-adjusted p = 0.29) and “Neurotransmitter Receptor Binding And Downstream Transmission In The Postsynaptic Cell” (p = 0.032, BH adjusted p = 0.54). Enrichment signal in both pathways was driven by the same two genes: GABA(A) Receptor Subunit Gamma-3 (GABRG3) and Guanine Nucleotide Binding Protein (G Protein), Alpha Inhibiting Activity Polypeptide 3 (GNAI3). Overview of all results obtained from KEGG pathway analyses (pathways with BH-corrected p < 0.1) are presented in Table 3 and from Pathway Commons analyses (pathways with unadjusted p < 0.05) in Supplementary Table 4.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Enriched pathways in KEGG (BH-corrected p < 0.1) based on associated findings from EWAS analyses (p < 10−4).

Discussion

In this study we investigated if DNA methylation signatures in blood associate with cognitive functioning in middle-aged individuals in a cross-sectional and longitudinal manner. Use of a within-twin-pair design in this study of monozygotic twins allowed us to investigate epigenetic age-related changes in cognitive functioning regardless of the genetic background of the investigated individuals.

Investigation of cross-sectional cognitive functioning in 486 middle-aged twins identified DNA methylation of a CpG site positioned in an intronic CpG island of ZBTB46 as the most associated finding, which was supported in both the paired 50% most discordant and unpaired analyses, however with higher p-values. Higher p-value in the 50% most discordant twins analysis in comparison to the full MADT cohort is probably due to decreased number of individuals and thus decreased power of this model. ZBTB46 is a gene most highly expressed in cerebellum and cerebellar hemisphere, according to the GTEx (Lonsdale et al., 2013). Expression of Zbtb46 has been also shown to distinguish classical dendritic cells and their committed progenitors from other immune lineages (Satpathy et al., 2012). A genetic polymorphism in ZBTB46 (rs6062314) was associated with multiple sclerosis (Lill et al., 2013), with the identified SNP located 7298 bp from the ZBTB46 CpG site associated in our study. Search for the possible methylation Quantitative Trait Loci interactions between rs6062314 and methylation levels in fetal brain samples (Hannon et al., 2016) yielded no results.

Additionally, the unpaired analysis of cross-sectional cognitive functioning identified a probe positioned in a region 1,500 bp upstream from transcription start site of TAF12 as the most associated finding. One of the best-described functions of TAF12 is its interaction and recruitment of the growth arrest and DNA damage-inducible alpha protein (Gadd45a) (Schmitz et al., 2009), responsible for active DNA demethylation and known for its important role in neurodevelopment, neuronal injury, and ischemia, where Gadd45a mRNA levels are broadly increased throughout the ischemic cortex (Sultan and Sweatt, 2013) in mice. None of our top findings of p < 10−5 overlapped with the loci associated with non-pathological cognitive ageing in recent genome-wide association studies (Davies et al., 2014, 2015). Pathway analyses of the most associated findings associated with cross-sectional cognition measurement identified “Neuroactive ligand-receptor interaction” as the most enriched pathway suggesting methylation of neurobiology-related genes to be involved in cognitive functioning. A recent animal study, based on methylated DNA immunoprecipitation sequencing method, reported differential methylation in genes enriched in “main axon,” “dendritic spine head,” and “postsynaptic density” Gene Ontology pathways to be associated with associative memory maintenance (Halder et al., 2016). Interestingly, another animal study, based on whole-genome bisulfite sequencing, reported age-related impairment of cognitive flexibility to be associated with differential methylation of genes enriched in “postsynaptic density” Gene Ontology pathways (Ianov et al., 2017). These studies further support that epigenetic changes in genes in neurobiology-related pathways are associated with both cross-sectional, as well as longitudinal cognitive functioning.

In the intra-pair analyses the longitudinal change in cognitive functioning (mainly decline) was most highly associated with a probe positioned in AGBL4, also known as CCP6. The gene is known for its brain-specific expression, with highest expression levels observed in frontal cortex (Lonsdale et al., 2013) and its function in catalyzing deglutamylation of tubulin. The accumulation of posttranslationally added glutamate side chains at microtubules is directly linked to neurodegeneration, thus highlighting the important role of AGBL4 for neuronal survival (Rogowski et al., 2010). It is possible that differential methylation of AGBL4 impacts its expression and further protein levels, what dysregulates the tubulin deglutamylation and leads to neurodegeneration and decrease of cognitive function, however this hypothesis needs to be further tested in translational models of cognitive aging.

Interestingly, the most associated finding of cognitive change in the unpaired analyses was a SORBS1 probe, also associated with the trait in the paired analysis but with a higher p-value. Genetic polymorphism in SORBS1 has been suggested to be associated with brain infarction related to ischemic stroke. However since the finding was reported in Japanese individuals it should be replicated in other populations to be directly related to our study (Hagiwara et al., 2008). Intriguingly, SORBS1 expression has been found to be upregulated in the hippocampus of individuals suffering from Alzheimer's disease in comparison to unaffected controls (Blalock et al., 2004), supporting its potential function in cognitive impairment.

A possible limitation of this study is the use of the more easily accessible blood samples to study DNA methylation signatures of a brain-related trait. However, recent studies that investigated the possibility of using blood as a proxy for brain-related phenotypes reported DNA methylation status in brain to be mirrored at many CpG sites in blood, thus supporting the use of more commonly available blood samples for studying mental health (Aberg et al., 2013). It should also be noted that the blood samples used in this study were collected from twins only at the follow-up visit, not on both occasions. Accordingly, we were only able to correlate longitudinal change of cognition with DNA methylation status at follow-up, while we cannot estimate a possible relation between cognition change and ageing-related changes in methylation. This study provides only information on the associations between cognitive functioning and DNA methylation levels in blood. Further research is warranted to answer the question if the associated differences in DNA methylation levels are causal of cognitive functioning or are a consequence of any disease's pathological process (Aberg et al., 2013; Mill and Heijmans, 2013).

In addition, perhaps of particular relevance for the longitudinal measure of cognitive aging identification of an appropriate population for such a study is a challenge, since part of investigated individuals may be too healthy to show any age-related cognitive decline or too ill to be able to perform the test (Minder et al., 2002) resulting in decreased variance in the sample of the studied trait. In our sample we observed a decrease in mean score on the cognition assessment test in comparison to the first visit of investigated twins, however, for some individuals improvement in their cognitive score was recorded during the follow-up assessment. The improvement in the cognition score at follow-up can be linked with possible changes in lifestyle of an individual, practice effect or illness at the first visit. Reduced risk of dementia and cognitive decline incident has been linked with higher levels of education, occupational complexity, cognitive leisure activities, while active cognitive lifestyle was even associated with cognitive recovery (Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2006; Marioni et al., 2012). Additionally, nutrition, level of physical and social activity are well-established lifestyle factors linked to preventing cognitive decline and proposed to be used in interventions to restore and maintain the cognitive level (Williams and Kemper, 2010).

In this study we did not have access to information on medication, which could have an influence on blood DNA methylation levels and cognitive functioning. A recent study showed the use of an approved anti-asthma medication to reduce neuroinflammation, elevate hippocampal neurogenesis and improve learning and memory in old rats, making it highly relevant to include drug information in future studies of DNA methylation and cognitive functioning (Marschallinger et al., 2015). Moreover, level of educational attainment is associated with cross-sectional cognitive functioning, but not with the rate of cognitive decline in aging individuals (Wilson et al., 2009; Zahodne et al., 2011). Data on educational attainment was not available for this study. Future studies adjusting for this variable will delineate to what extend our EWAS findings for cross-sectional measure of cognitive functioning are influenced by the level of education.

In conclusion, this study investigated possible blood DNA methylation signatures related to cross-sectional and longitudinal cognitive functioning in middle-aged individuals and identified associations with this epigenetic modification of neuronal survival and ischemia-related genes. In the future, in order to get a deeper insight into the mechanisms related to cognitive aging, replication and follow-up of these findings in an independent population is warranted, preferably using both blood and more biologically cognition-related tissue, like frontal cortex or hippocampus.

Author Contributions

Conception and design: AS, QT, LC, and MM; Analysis: AS, QT, and LC; Interpretation: AS, QT, MM, OM, AB, KC, MN, and LC; Drafting the manuscript: AS, QT, MN, and LC; Revising the article and final approval of the version to be published: AS, QT, MM, OM, AB, KC, MN, and LC.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Mette Sørensen and Birgit Debrabant for valuable technical discussions. Funding: this study was supported by The European Union's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2011) under grant agreement n°259679, The Danish National Program for Research Infrastructure 2007 [09-063256], The Danish Council for Independent Research (DFF—6110-00016), and the Lundbeck Foundation, Denmark (R155-2014-1724). The funding bodies had no involvement in the study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00413/full#supplementary-material

References

Aberg, K. A., Xie, L. Y., McClay, J. L., Nerella, S., Vunck, S., Snider, S., et al. (2013). Testing two models describing how methylome-wide studies in blood are informative for psychiatric conditions. Epigenomics 5, 367–377. doi: 10.2217/epi.13.36

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aryee, M. J., Jaffe, A. E., Corrada-Bravo, H., Ladd-Acosta, C., Feinberg, A. P., Hansen, K. D., et al. (2014). Minfi: a flexible and comprehensive Bioconductor package for the analysis of Infinium DNA methylation microarrays. Bioinformatics 30, 1363–1369. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate-a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Methodol. 57, 289–300.

Google Scholar

Blalock, E. M., Geddes, J. W., Chen, K. C., Porter, N. M., Markesbery, W. R., and Landfield, P. W. (2004). Incipient Alzheimer's disease: microarray correlation analyses reveal major transcriptional and tumor suppressor responses. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 2173–2178. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308512100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, S. N., and Barnes, C. A. (2006). Neural plasticity in the ageing brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 30–40. doi: 10.1038/nrn1809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellani, C. A., Laufer, B. I., Melka, M. G., Diehl, E. J., O'Reilly, R. L., and Singh, S. M. (2015). DNA methylation differences in monozygotic twin pairs discordant for schizophrenia identifies psychosis related genes and networks. BMC Med. Genomics 8:17. doi: 10.1186/s12920-015-0093-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Lemire, M., Choufani, S., Butcher, D. T., Grafodatskaya, D., Zanke, B. W., et al. (2013). Discovery of cross-reactive probes and polymorphic CpGs in the Illumina Infinium HumanMethylation450 microarray. Epigenetics 8, 203–209. doi: 10.4161/epi.23470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Z., and Riggs, A. D. (2011). DNA methylation and demethylation in mammals. J. Biol. Chem. 286, 18347–18353. doi: 10.1074/jbc.R110.205286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christiansen, L., Frederiksen, H., Schousboe, K., Skytthe, A., von Wurmb-Schwark, N., Christensen, K., et al. (2003). Age- and sex-differences in the validity of questionnaire-based zygosity in twins. Twin Res. 6, 275–278. doi: 10.1375/136905203322296610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, G., Armstrong, N., Bis, J. C., Bressler, J., Chouraki, V., Giddaluru, S., et al. (2015). Genetic contributions to variation in general cognitive function: a meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies in the CHARGE consortium (N = 53949). Mol. Psychiatry 20, 183–192. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, G., Harris, S. E., Reynolds, C. A., Payton, A., Knight, H. M., Liewald, D. C., et al. (2014). A genome-wide association study implicates the APOE locus in nonpathological cognitive ageing. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 76–87. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Day, J. J., and Sweatt, J. D. (2010). DNA methylation and memory formation. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1319–1323. doi: 10.1038/nn.2666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dempster, E. L., Pidsley, R., Schalkwyk, L. C., Owens, S., Georgiades, A., Kane, F., et al. (2011). Disease-associated epigenetic changes in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Hum. Mol. Genet. 20, 4786–4796. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddr416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dempster, E. L., Wong, C. C. Y., Lester, K. J., Burrage, J., Gregory, A. M., Mill, J., et al. (2014). Genome-wide methylomic analysis of monozygotic twins discordant for adolescent depression. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 977–983. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinberg, A. P. (2007). Phenotypic plasticity and the epigenetics of human disease. Nature 447, 433–440. doi: 10.1038/nature05919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, J., Zhou, Y., Campbell, S. L., Le, T., Li, E., Sweatt, J. D., et al. (2010). Dnmt1 and Dnmt3a maintain DNA methylation and regulate synaptic function in adult forebrain neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 423–430. doi: 10.1038/nn.2514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fortin, J.-P., Labbe, A., Lemire, M., Zanke, B. W., Hudson, T. J., Fertig, E. J., et al. (2014). Functional normalization of 450k methylation array data improves replication in large cancer studies. Genome Biol. 15, 503. doi: 10.1186/s13059-014-0503-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagiwara, N., Kitazono, T., Kamouchi, M., Kuroda, J., Ago, T., Hata, J., et al. (2008). Polymorphism in the sorbin and SH3-domain-containing-1 (SORBS1) gene and the risk of brain infarction in the Japanese population: the Fukuoka Stroke Registry and the Hisayama study. Eur. J. Neurol. 15, 481–486. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2008.02105.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halder, R., Hennion, M., Vidal, R. O., Shomroni, O., Rahman, R.-U., Rajput, A., et al. (2016). DNA methylation changes in plasticity genes accompany the formation and maintenance of memory. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 102–110. doi: 10.1038/nn.4194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hannon, E., Spiers, H., Viana, J., Pidsley, R., Burrage, J., Murphy, T. M., et al. (2016). Methylation QTLs in the developing brain and their enrichment in schizophrenia risk loci. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 48–54. doi: 10.1038/nn.4182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyn, H., Carmona, F. J., Gomez, A., Ferreira, H. J., Bell, J. T., Sayols, S., et al. (2013). DNA methylation profiling in breast cancer discordant identical twins identifies DOK7 as novel epigenetic biomarker. Carcinogenesis 34, 102–108. doi: 10.1093/carcin/bgs321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyn, H., Li, N., Ferreira, H. J., Moran, S., Pisano, D. G., Gomez, A., et al. (2012). Distinct DNA methylomes of newborns and centenarians. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 10522–10527. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1120658109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ianov, L., Riva, A., Kumar, A., and Foster, T. C. (2017). DNA methylation of synaptic genes in the prefrontal cortex is associated with aging and age-related cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:249. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Illingworth, R. S., and Bird, A. P. (2009). CpG islands–'A rough guide'. FEBS Lett. 583, 1713–1720. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2009.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Javierre, B. M., Fernandez, A. F., Richter, J., Al-Shahrour, F., Martin-Subero, J. I., Rodriguez-Ubreva, J., et al. (2010). Changes in the pattern of DNA methylation associate with twin discordance in systemic lupus erythematosus. Genome Res. 20, 170–179. doi: 10.1101/gr.100289.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, M. J., Goodman, S. J., and Kobor, M. S. (2015). DNA methylation and healthy human aging. Aging Cell 14, 924–932. doi: 10.1111/acel.12349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, P. A. (2012). Functions of DNA methylation: islands, start sites, gene bodies and beyond. Nat. Rev. Genet. 13, 484–492. doi: 10.1038/nrg3230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohli, R. M., and Zhang, Y. (2013). TET enzymes, TDG and the dynamics of DNA demethylation. Nature 502, 472–479. doi: 10.1038/nature12750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lill, C. M., Schjeide, B.-M. M., Graetz, C., Ban, M., Alcina, A., Ortiz, M. A., et al. (2013). MANBA, CXCR5, SOX8, RPS6KB1 and ZBTB46 are genetic risk loci for multiple sclerosis. Brain 136, 1778–1782. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lister, R., Mukamel, E. A., Nery, J. R., Urich, M., Puddifoot, C. A., Johnson, N. D., et al. (2013). Global epigenomic reconfiguration during mammalian brain development. Science 341:1237905. doi: 10.1126/science.1237905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lonsdale, J., Thomas, J., Salvatore, M., Phillips, R., Lo, E., Shad, S., et al. (2013). The Genotype-Tissue Expression (GTEx) project. Nat. Genet. 45, 580–585. doi: 10.1038/ng.2653

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maag, J. L. V., Kaczorowski, D. C., Panja, D., Peters, T. J., Bramham, C. R., Wibrand, K., et al. (2017). Widespread promoter methylation of synaptic plasticity genes in long-term potentiation in the adult brain in vivo. BMC Genomics 18:250. doi: 10.1186/s12864-017-3621-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marioni, R. E., van den Hout, A., Valenzuela, M. J., Brayne, C., and Matthews, F. E. (2012). Active cognitive lifestyle associates with cognitive recovery and a reduced risk of cognitive decline. J. Alzheimers Dis. 28, 223–230. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-110377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marschallinger, J., Schäffner, I., Klein, B., Gelfert, R., Rivera, F. J., Illes, S., et al. (2015). Structural and functional rejuvenation of the aged brain by an approved anti-asthmatic drug. Nat. Commun. 6:8466. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCartney, K., Harris, M. J., and Bernieri, F. (1990). Growing up and growing apart: a developmental meta-analysis of twin studies. Psychol. Bull. 107, 226–237. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGue, M., and Christensen, K. (2001). The heritability of cognitive functioning in very old adults: evidence from Danish twins aged 75 years and older. Psychol. Aging 16, 272–280. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGue, M., and Christensen, K. (2002). The heritability of level and rate-of-change in cognitive functioning in danish twins aged 70 years and older. Exp. Aging Res. 28, 435–451. doi: 10.1080/03610730290080416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGue, M., and Christensen, K. (2013). Growing old but not growing apart: twin similarity in the latter half of the lifespan. Behav. Genet. 43, 1–12. doi: 10.1007/s10519-012-9559-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medvedeva, Y. A., Khamis, A. M., Kulakovskiy, I. V., Ba-Alawi, W., Bhuyan, M. S. I., Kawaji, H., et al. (2014). Effects of cytosine methylation on transcription factor binding sites. BMC Genomics 15:119. doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-15-119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mill, J., and Heijmans, B. T. (2013). From promises to practical strategies in epigenetic epidemiology. Nat. Rev. Genet. 14, 585–594. doi: 10.1038/nrg3405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, C. A., Campbell, S. L., and Sweatt, J. D. (2008). DNA methylation and histone acetylation work in concert to regulate memory formation and synaptic plasticity. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 89, 599–603. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, C. A., Gavin, C. F., White, J. A., Parrish, R. R., Honasoge, A., Yancey, C. R., et al. (2010). Cortical DNA methylation maintains remote memory. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 664–666. doi: 10.1038/nn.2560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minder, C. E., Müller, T., Gillmann, G., Beck, J. C., and Stuck, A. E. (2002). Subgroups of refusers in a disability prevention trial in older adults: baseline and follow-up analysis. Am. J. Public Health 92, 445–450. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.92.3.445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, J. H., and Baxter, M. G. (2014). Synaptic health. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 835–837. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliveira, A. M. M., Hemstedt, T. J., and Bading, H. (2012). Rescue of aging-associated decline in Dnmt3a2 expression restores cognitive abilities. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1111–1113. doi: 10.1038/nn.3151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliveira, A. M. M., Hemstedt, T. J., Freitag, H. E., and Bading, H. (2015). Dnmt3a2: a hub for enhancing cognitive functions. Mol. Psychiatry 21, 1130–1136. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, I., McGue, M., Tan, Q., Christensen, K., and Christiansen, L. (2016). Change in depression symptomatology and cognitive function in twins: a 10-year follow-up study. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 19, 104–111. doi: 10.1017/thg.2016.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2015). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Available online at: http://www.gbif.org/resource/81287 (Accessed May 1, 2016).

Rogowski, K., van Dijk, J., Magiera, M. M., Bosc, C., Deloulme, J.-C., Bosson, A., et al. (2010). A family of protein-deglutamylating enzymes associated with neurodegeneration. Cell 143, 564–578. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2010.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, E. D., Roth, T. L., Money, K. M., SenGupta, S., Eason, D. E., and Sweatt, J. D. (2015). DNA methylation regulates neurophysiological spatial representation in memory formation. Neuroepigenetics 2, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.nepig.2015.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satpathy, A. T., Wumesh, K. C., Albring, J. C., Edelson, B. T., Kretzer, N. M., Bhattacharya, D., et al. (2012). Zbtb46 expression distinguishes classical dendritic cells and their committed progenitors from other immune lineages. J. Exp. Med. 209, 1135–1152. doi: 10.1084/jem.20120030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitz, K.-M., Schmitt, N., Hoffmann-Rohrer, U., Schäfer, A., Grummt, I., and Mayer, C. (2009). TAF12 recruits Gadd45a and the nucleotide excision repair complex to the promoter of rRNA genes leading to active DNA demethylation. Mol. Cell 33, 344–353. doi: 10.1016/j.molcel.2009.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skytthe, A., Christiansen, L., Kyvik, K. O., Bødker, F. L., Hvidberg, L., Petersen, I., et al. (2013). The danish twin registry: linking surveys, national registers, and biological information. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 16, 104–111. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.77

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefan, M., Zhang, W., Concepcion, E., Yi, Z., and Tomer, Y. (2014). DNA methylation profiles in type 1 diabetes twins point to strong epigenetic effects on etiology. J. Autoimmun. 50, 33–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jaut.2013.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sultan, F. A., and Sweatt, J. D. (2013). The role of the Gadd45 family in the nervous system: a focus on neurodevelopment, neuronal injury, and cognitive neuroepigenetics. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 793, 81–119. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-8289-5_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swan, G. E. (1992). Decline in Cognitive Performance in Aging Twins. Arch. Neurol. 49, 476. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530290058012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tripathi, A. (2012). New cellular and molecular approaches to ageing brain. Ann. Neurosci. 19, 177–182. doi: 10.5214/ans.0972.7531.190410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, P. C., and Bell, J. T. (2015). Power and sample size estimation for epigenome-wide association scans to detect differential DNA methylation. Int. J. Epidemiol. 44, 1429–1441. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyv041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenzuela, M. J., and Sachdev, P. (2006). Brain reserve and dementia: a systematic review. Psychol. Med. 36, 441–454. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Iterson, M., Tobi, E. W., Slieker, R. C., den Hollander, W., Luijk, R., Slagboom, P. E., et al. (2014). MethylAid: visual and interactive quality control of large Illumina 450k datasets. Bioinformatics 30, 3435–3437. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Duncan, D., Shi, Z., and Zhang, B. (2013). WEB-based GEne SeT analysis toolkit (WebGestalt): update 2013. Nucleic Acids Res. 41, W77–W83. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkt439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, K. N., and Kemper, S. (2010). Interventions to reduce cognitive decline in aging. J. Psychosoc. Nurs. Ment. Health Serv. 48, 42–51. doi: 10.3928/02793695-20100331-03

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, R. S., Hebert, L. E., Scherr, P. A., Barnes, L. L., Mendes de Leon, C. F., and Evans, D. A. (2009). Educational attainment and cognitive decline in old age. Neurology 72, 460–465. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000341782.71418.6c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, N.-K., Baek, S. H., and Kaang, B.-K. (2011). DNA methylation-mediated control of learning and memory. Mol. Brain 4:5. doi: 10.1186/1756-6606-4-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zahodne, L. B., Glymour, M. M., Sparks, C., Bontempo, D., Dixon, R. A., MacDonald, S. W. S., et al. (2011). Education does not slow cognitive decline with aging: 12-year evidence from the victoria longitudinal study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 1039–1046. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711001044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, R.-R., Cui, Q.-Y., Murai, K., Lim, Y. C., Smith, Z. D., Jin, S., et al. (2013). Tet1 regulates adult hippocampal neurogenesis and cognition. Cell Stem Cell 13, 237–245. doi: 10.1016/j.stem.2013.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: epigenome-wide association study, DNA methylation, epigenetic epidemiology, cognition, cognitive aging, whole blood, monozygotic twins

Citation: Starnawska A, Tan Q, McGue M, Mors O, Børglum AD, Christensen K, Nyegaard M and Christiansen L (2017) Epigenome-Wide Association Study of Cognitive Functioning in Middle-Aged Monozygotic Twins. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:413. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00413

Received: 31 August 2017; Accepted: 28 November 2017;
Published: 12 December 2017.

Edited by:

Ashok Kumar, University of Florida, United States

Reviewed by:

Lara Ianov, University of Alabama at Birmingham, United States
Betty M. Tijms, VU University Medical Center, Netherlands

Copyright © 2017 Starnawska, Tan, McGue, Mors, Børglum, Christensen, Nyegaard and Christiansen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anna Starnawska, as@biomed.au.dk

Download