Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 02 February 2018
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 10 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00023

Efficacy of Cognitive Training in Older Adults with and without Subjective Cognitive Decline Is Associated with Inhibition Efficiency and Working Memory Span, Not with Cognitive Reserve

  • 1Department of Cognitive Processes, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
  • 2Department of Methods of Research and Diagnostic in Education, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia, Madrid, Spain
  • 3Laboratory of Cognitive and Computational Neuroscience, Madrid, Spain
  • 4Department of Differential and Occupational Psychology, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
  • 5Center for the Prevention of Cognitive Impairment, Madrid-Salud, Madrid, Spain

The present study explores the role of cognitive reserve, executive functions, and working memory (WM) span, as factors that might explain training outcomes in cognitive status. Eighty-one older adults voluntarily participated in the study, classified either as older adults with subjective cognitive decline or cognitively intact. Each participant underwent a neuropsychological assessment that was conducted both at baseline (entailing cognitive reserve, executive functions, WM span and depressive symptomatology measures, as well as the Mini-Mental State Exam regarding initial cognitive status), and then 6 months later, once each participant had completed the training program (Mini-Mental State Exam at the endpoint). With respect to cognitive status the training program was most beneficial for subjective cognitive decline participants with low efficiency in inhibition at baseline (explaining a 33% of Mini-Mental State Exam total variance), whereas for cognitively intact participants training gains were observed for those who presented lower WM span.

Introduction

Cognitive training (CT) may contribute to delay or to prevent cognitive decline in older adults (Gates et al., 2011), although this finding remains controversial. A recent systematic review and meta-analysis conducted by Smart et al. (2017) through a standardized search in main databases (CINAHL Complete, Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, and PsycARTICLES) includes adults aged 55 or more with subjective cognitive decline (SCD) defined using published criteria, who receive non-pharmacological intervention or any control condition, with cognitive, behavioral, or psychological outcomes in controlled trails. They reported a small effect size of non-pharmacologic intervention in older adults with SCD, which was greater than obtained by other intervention types; results also revealed that CT had benefits on objectively measured cognitive functioning in this group.

Cognitive training is based on the idea that the brain function is modifiable even in old age. Factors such as education, occupation attainment, expertise, lifestyle, or fitness have been found to influence the trajectory of cognition throughout life (Kramer et al., 2004). These factors are associated with the concept of cognitive reserve; that is, the ability to optimize an individual’s performance in different tasks through the use of alternative neural circuits (Stern, 2012). Barulli and Stern (2013) suggested that cognitive reserve might be related to a more efficient and flexible brain networks recruitment. In recent years, there has been a considerable interest in understanding the relationship between cognitive reserve and cognition in late adulthood. Many studies have provided evidence that changes in cognition and the underlying brain function that take place in the aging process, are susceptible to modification and/or compensation (Robertson, 2013). Furthermore, other studies have pointed out the relevance of cognitive reserve as a factor which might modulate the efficacy of CT (Robertson, 2013; Mondini et al., 2016).

Besides, the initial level of functioning at baseline (Ranganath et al., 2011) should be considered as it can modulate CT outcomes. In a recent study conducted by Borella et al. (2017) the authors observed a compensation effect, that is, participants with lower baseline vocabulary scores, at an older age, and a weaker working memory (WM) performance benefited more from the training program. However, the role of WM performance varied depending on the transfer tasks considered (a visuo-spatial WM task, a short-term memory tasks, a measure of fluid intelligence, a measure of processing speed and two inhibitory measures). It is also well-known that changes in attention and executive functions related to the aging process have consequences such as information processing slowing, difficulties distinguishing relevant and irrelevant information, or in the simultaneous processing of different types of information (Stine-Morrow et al., 2006). Thus, it is expected that executive functioning at the baseline also modulates cognitive training outcomes. In this sense, some authors have argued that high-functioning individuals might show greater training gains than lower-functioning individuals as a result of their higher level of plasticity (see for example, Bissig and Lustig, 2007). Contrarily, an alternative hypothesis is that lower-functioning individuals will have greater training benefits than high-functioning individuals, because they have a higher learning potential (Olazarán et al., 2004).

In Spain a well-known CT program, called UMAM by its name in Spanish (programa de la Unidad de Memoria del Ayuntamiento de Madrid; English translation: Madrid City Council Memory Unit Program; Montejo et al., 2013) has shown benefits in memory in older adults without cognitive impairment, both in the post-training assessment and at 6-month follow-up (Montejo, 2015). However, the effects of this CT program on other cognitive domains have not been yet studied. The present study explores the role of cognitive reserve, executive functions (interference efficiency and cognitive flexibility) and individual WM capacity as factors modulating the efficacy of UMAM program in global cognitive status. Considering that people with SCD might be at an increased risk of developing dementia (Jessen et al., 2014; Mitchell et al., 2014), it is of most interest to investigate the benefits of a CT in this population. As main objective we investigate the role of cognitive reserve, executive functions (cognitive flexibility and inhibition efficacy), and WM span as factors modulating CT outcomes on global cognitive status in both groups. In line with previous studies (Olazarán et al., 2004; Mondini et al., 2016) we expect that cognitive reserve will have a greater relative weight than the other two factors as a predictor of CT outcomes on global cognitive status. Finally, we hypothesized that CT will have better outcomes in participants with lower cognitive reserve at the baseline, especially in SCD older adults.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Eighty-one older Spanish-speaking adults voluntarily participated in the present study. All of them were recruited from the Center for Cognitive Impairment Prevention (CCIP; Public Health Institute, Madrid City Council), where they were enrolled in the UMAM program. Details about this CT training are provided in Appendix. Figure 1 summarizes the main features of UMAM program.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Diagram describing the structure of a session in UMAM program, duration and number of training and maintaining sessions.

Exclusion criteria included: (1) Yesavage Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15; Sheikh and Yesavage, 1986) higher or equal to 5; (2) Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975; Spanish adaptation by Lobo et al., 1999) lower than 24 at baseline; (3) Low performance on the Logical Memory delayed recall subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale – Third Edition (Wechsler, 1997; lower than 10 units for people with 16 years of formal education or more; and lower than 6 units for people with 8–15 years of formal education). All participants had normal or corrected hearing and vision.

Forty-nine participants were identified as older adults with SCD (14 males and 35 females). According to Jessen et al.’s (2014) criteria, these participants: (a) presented self-perception of cognitive decline, mainly associated with memory loss; (b) had requested medical consultation regarding their memory complaints; (c) felt that their subjective decline affected their daily activities; (d) set the onset of their subjective decline within the last 5 years, and (e) concerns associated with their subjective decline were confirmed by a reliable informant. The remaining 32 participants (12 males and 20 females) did not meet criteria for SCD, and they formed a group of cognitively intact older adults (CI).

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for socio-demographic variables, depressive symptomatology, cognitive reserve, executive functions and WM span in both groups at baseline. SCD older adults as a group were less efficient in inhibition (interference) and also presented higher scores in GDS-15 than CI participants. However, it should be taken into account that all subjects had normal scores in this scale. Both groups were equivalent in MMSE scores at the baseline (cognitively intact participants: 28.94 ± 1.19; SCD older adults: 28.35 ± 1.63).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics of socio-demographic variables (Age, Years of formal education), cognitive reserve, depressive symptomatology, cognitive flexibility, inhibition efficiency, and working memory capacity (digit reordering) by group.

The present study complied with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the local Ethics Committees of the Participant Institutions.

Materials

Cognitive reserve was estimated using the Cognitive Reserve Questionnaire (Cuestionario de Reserva Cognitiva; Rami et al., 2011), a brief questionnaire suited for clinical context.

The Stroop test (Golden, 1978) and the Trail Making Test parts A and B (TMT-A and TMT-B; Reitan, 1994) were used to assess executive function processes, such as inhibition efficiency and flexibility, respectively. Stroop test provides a measure representing the ability to inhibit an automatized/habitual response (read the word) in favor of an alternative one (color naming). Poor inhibition efficiency has been considered an early sign of cognitive deterioration in the progression to Alzheimer disease. TMT is a common measure of cognitive flexibility and planning ability. Especially, part B is considered a sensitive measurement of cognitive flexibility in geriatric assessment, although it requires other cognitive abilities, such as psychomotor speed and visual scanning. B/A ratio score provides an indicator of executive control according to its correlation with task-switching ability. The assessment protocol also included a digit reordering task (MacDonald et al., 2001), which involves maintaining and manipulating information in WM in contrast with other measures which are only focused on maintenance.

Procedure

An extensive neuropsychological assessment of each participant was conducted at two different times, one immediately after recruitment (baseline), and then 6 months later (endpoint). Neuropsychological assessments were conducted by an experienced psychologist or psychiatrist at the Center for the Prevention of Cognitive Impairment of Madrid-Salud, a public institution depending on Madrid City Council. In the first session the participant completed the screening tests (MMSE, GDS-15), and the cognitive reserve questionnaire. At the beginning of this first session participants were informed about the main goals of the study and signed an informed consent document. All the remaining neuropsychological tests were applied in a different session. The order in which the tests were presented was randomized in each session for each participant. Neuropsychological tests were applied and scored following the standard instructions provided in the users’ manuals.

Statistical Analysis

Previous analyses were conducted in order to obtain (1) descriptive statistics by group across measures (pre and post) and across five categories of outcomes (percentages), (2) pre-training differences between groups in MMSE (univariate ANOVA), (3) the effect of UMAM program in the total sample (comparing MMSE scores between pre and post by means of a t-test for related measures), (4) a mixed ANOVA (2 groups × 2 measures) with predictors as covariates, and (5) a repeated measures ANOVA for each group in order to explore intragroup differences between pre and post-training measures. Effect size was estimated in the last analysis by means of partial eta-square (ηp2).

Fourteen models were developed for each group by predictor combination, following a procedure described in Mondini et al. (2016). The dependent measure employed in the analyses resulted from subtracting the baseline scores to the endpoint scores in MMSE (post-training).

Regarding predictor variables, we obtained a total score for each participant reflecting an estimation of her/his cognitive reserve. With respect to digit reordering, we considered the number of series correctly ordered by each participant as a measure of his/her WM span. In order to obtain a measure of cognitive flexibility we computed the ratio score part B/part A using data from participants’ TMT, in order to obtain a measure of cognitive flexibility (Lezak, 1995). Finally, regarding the Stroop test, we used the Interference index proposed by Chafetz and Matthews (2004).

All resulting models were compared taking into account a set of indexes providing goodness-of-fit measures: Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978), Aikake Information Criterion (AIC; Burnham and Anderson, 2002), Bayes factor (BF; Lavine and Schervish, 1999), and R2. Lower BIC and AIC indexes, as well as higher BF and R2, are associated with better model fit of the data. IBM SPSS statistical software v. 20.0 was used to obtain BIC, AIC and R2, but BF was computed with version 0.9.8 of the BayesFactor package developed by Perception and Cognition Lab. Department of Psychological Sciences at the University of Missouri1. Once a model has been chosen by means of its goodness-of-fit, we used the statistic t-test critical values in order to determine for each variable if it has a significant weight in the prediction or not.

Results

Table 2 shows means and standard deviation by group across MMSE measures (pre and post). Once differences in MMSE between post and pre-training were computed for all subjects it was possible to recode values in five categories of training outcomes: (a) a positive value greater or equal than 3 was considered as moderate improvement, (b) positive values equal to 1 and 2 were mild improvement, (c) a value equal to zero meant unchanged performance o null improvement, (d) negative values -1 and -2 formed a category of mild worsening, and (e) negative values greater or equal to -3 points were considered moderate worsening. Table 2 also summarizes percentages in these five categories by group. There were no statistical differences in percentages across categories between groups, χ2(4) = 2.552, p = 0.635.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Descriptive statistics (mean and standard deviation -between parenthesis-) of MMSE by group across measures (pre and post), significance of difference between pre and post in each group, effect size estimation, and percentages of moderate and mild improvement, null improvement, and mild and moderate worsening in MMSE outcomes after training by group.

Considering all participants as a group there was a significant improvement from pre to post-training MMSE measure, t(80) = -2.46, p < 0.02. Groups have very similar MMSE means at the baseline (pre-training measure), F(1,79) = 3.119, p = 0.81. Results in 2 × 2 mixed ANOVA with covariates pointed out that only the interaction Group × Measure × Stroop’s Interference reached statistical significance, F(1,45) = 7.847, p < 0.008, ηp2 = 0.148. The interaction Group × Measure × Digit reordering only approached statistical significance, F(1,45) = 3.487, p = 0.068. There was only a significant difference between MMSE pre- and post-training measures in the SCD group, F(1,46) = 5.380, p = 0.0025, with a small effect size (ηp2 = 0.105).

Predictive Models’ Goodness-of-Fit Regarding CT Outcomes in the General Cognitive Status (MMSE)

Table 3 shows all possible models involving selected predictors for each group. Models in bold have the best values across goodness-of-fit indexes, that is: lower BIC and AIC, as well as higher BF and R2, are associated with better model fit of the data. With respect to MMSE, an inspection of Table 3 underscored that in the cognitively intact group, the best model was the 4th, with digit reordering score as the only predictor (BIC = 102.57; AIC = 98.47; BF = 2.79; R2 = 0.17; p = 0.033). As the results evidenced, the lower the digit reordering score at the baseline was, the greater the benefit of training on cognitive status (MMSE), t = -2.41, p = 0.023, given that the sign of the statistical test is negative. However, a different picture emerged in the SCD group, since the best model was the 6th, with cognitive reserve and interference as significant predictors (BIC = 119.74; AIC = 113.88; BF = 13.59; R2 = 0.33; p = 0.002). The most influential variable in this case was interference (t = -3.74, p = 0.001; see Figure 2), given that cognitive reserve did not reached statistical significance (t = 1.65, p = 0.110). As before, the negative sign revealed that the lower Stroop’s Interference score at the baseline was, the better the benefit of training on MMSE.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Goodness-of-fit of all models predicting UMAM training outcomes in MMSE.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Stroop’s Interference scores and differences between Tl (post-training measure) and T0 (pre-training) measure in MMSE for the SCD group. Dotted lines delimit 95% confidence interval.

Discussion

UMAM program had a significant benefit on global cognitive status taking all participants as a group. Results based on percentages did not support any association between groups (CI, SCD) and categories of change in cognitive status after training. Given that the clinical significance of mild changes raises doubts, it is more adequate to highlight the difference in the percentage of moderate improvement between CI and SCD groups (3.2 and 8.5% respectively). There were no differences between groups in MMSE pre-training measure. However, further analysis revealed that the interaction between group (CI, SCD) and measure (pre, post) was modulated by Stroop’s Interference, a measure of controlled inhibition efficacy that was considered a relevant predictor of cognitive status outcomes in our study. It is important to pointed out that intragroup differences between pre and post measures revealed that cognitive training was more beneficial for SCD older adults than for CI participants, although effect size was small.

With respect to the main objective of the study, if we consider CT impact in participants’ MMSE (difference between the endpoint and the baseline score) there are different predictor variables that explain the results for each group. In the cognitively intact group a lower WM span at the baseline (score in digit reordering) predicts a positive outcome in global cognitive status (MMSE) after CT. This WM measure explains a 17% of the total variance associated to the measure of change in cognitive status. By contrast, in the SCD group CT outcomes were better for those subjects previously showing low efficiency in inhibition at the baseline. In this case, Stroop’s Interference explains a significant part (33%) of the total variance in MMSE change between the baseline and the endpoint. It must be taken into account that SCD participants showed less ability at the baseline than cognitively intact older adults to inhibit automatized responses (i.e., reading) when trying to name the color. This difference between cognitively intact and SCD older adults in executive performance has been also reported in some previous studies. For example, Kirova et al. (2015) concluded that very early stages of AD are associated with a deficit in executive functions (difficulties with tasks involving divided attention and inhibition of interfering stimuli). Seo et al. (2016) concluded that poor executive functioning (inhibition and goal-directed behaviors) could represent one of the first cognitive signs in the course of AD. They found that a pre-MCI group showed significantly lower scores for visual immediate recall, fluency tests, and Stroop color naming in the Color–Word incongruent condition than the control group. The pattern of results that we observed in SCD older adults seems to show that with respect to a global measure of cognition (the MMSE) the UMAM training program produces larger benefits for individuals with lower efficiency in inhibition at the baseline, and that the variability in dependent variable is explained in a great portion by inhibition efficiency.

Thus, the results obtained contradict our initial predictions, but they are in line with the alternative approach considering that a relevant factor modulating CT benefits is the participant’s level of functioning at the baseline (Ranganath et al., 2011). Accordingly, lower-functioning individuals would benefit more from CT because they have more room for improvement. Our results are also in tune with a recent study conducted by Bamidis et al. (2015) in a community-dwelling sample of cognitively healthy and impaired older adults. The authors used global cognition as a primary measure. This composite score was derived from three different cognitive scores (episodic memory, WM, and executive function), by averaging the z-standardized scores. In summary, Bamidis colleagues study showed a robust modulation effect of executive functions’ baseline performance on training (combined physical and cognitive) benefits, that is: the lower the baseline performance, the more benefits were found in global cognition. However, our results differ from others obtained in previous studies providing the relevance of cognitive reserve as a factor that modulates the outcome of a cognitive training program on participant’s cognitive status [Franzmeier et al., 2016 with a sample of amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI) patients; Mondini et al., 2016 with a sample of mild to moderate patients with dementia; Olazarán et al., 2004 with MCI and mild to moderate AD patients]. It must be highlighted that SCD older adults had lower cognitive reserve than their cognitively intact peers at the baseline, although such differences only approached statistical significance. This contrast with the results obtained by Mondini colleagues showing that cognitive training has more positive outcome in lower cognitive reserve patients than in higher cognitive reserve patients. Our results also contrast with previous ones showing that educational attainment (proxy of cognitive reserve) modulates the effectivity of training in cognitively normal older adults, being participants with low educational level the group in which outcomes after training are better (Clark et al., 2016).

In line with Lövdén et al. (2010) proposal regarding training efficacy of a mismatch between what they termed the “supply” (one’s capacity for plasticity) and “demand” (the demands required by the environment and one’s capacity for flexibility), a suggestive interpretation of our results might be raised. In this sense, individuals with lower executive functioning and WM span at the baseline have shown larger gains from UMAM training than those with higher level or capacity, perhaps because there was more of a mismatch between the demands of CT and their own capacity for plasticity (supply). Participants with higher previous executive functioning and WM capacity were performing at an optimal level prior to training that is why they may not have profited as much from the type of training employed in our study.

From a practical point of view, these results are remarkably useful since they indicate that a multifactorial CT program (UMAM) is more effective in the SCD group. It might also contribute to the development of alternative programs for others who possibly need different intervention strategies. A follow-up study is currently underway to investigate if the effects of UMAM training are maintained at 12 months.

Finally, as potential limitations of this study, it should be noted that in both groups percentage of females are greater than the corresponding of males, and also sample size could be extended in future studies to strengthen conclusions. Additionally, we have considered small changes as improvement or worsening, therefore we must be cautious when interpreting the results, since these changes could be caused by measurement errors, regression to the mean or practice effects. A relevant future direction would be to explore if the pattern of results obtained could change with long-term interventions.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Ethical Commitee of the San Carlos Clinical Hospital (Madrid), which is one of the main Medical Institution that participates in the research project (PSI2015-68793-C3-1,2,3-R), with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Ethical Commitee of the San Carlos Clinical Hospital.

Author Contributions

RL-H, MTM-A, and SR-V: study concept and design, analysis, and interpretation. MLD-L, PM, and MM: preparation of paper and critical review. JMP, JdF-L, and DL-S: interpretation, critical review, and data coding. All authors contributed in writing the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness and FEDER funds (PSI2015-68793-C3-3-R), by a predoctoral fellowship from the Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness to DL-S (PSI2012-38375-C03-01), and a predoctoral fellowship by la Caixa Foundation to JdF-L.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00023/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^http://pcl.missouri.edu/bf-reg

References

Bamidis, P. D., Fissler, P., Papageorgiou, S. G., Zilidou, V., Konstantinidis, E. I., Billis, A. S., et al. (2015). Gains in cognition through combined cognitive and physical training: the role of training dosage and severity of neurocognitive disorder. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:152. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barulli, D., and Stern, Y. (2013). Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 502–509. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bissig, D., and Lustig, C. (2007). Who benefits from memory training? Psychol. Sci. 18, 720–726. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01966.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borella, E., Carbone, E., Pastore, M., De Beni, R., and Carretti, B. (2017). Working memory training for healthy older adults: the role of individual characteristics in explaining short- and long-term gains. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:99. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burnham, K. P., and Anderson, D. R. (2002). Model Selection and Multimodal Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach. New York NY: Springer-Verlag.

Google Scholar

Chafetz, M. D., and Matthews, L. H. (2004). A new interference score for the stroop test. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 19, 555–567. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2003.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, D. O., Xu, H., Unverzagt, F. W., and Hendrie, H. (2016). Does targeted cognitive training reduce educational disparities in cognitive function among cognitively normal older adults? Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 31, 809–817. doi: 10.1002/gps.4395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franzmeier, N., Unterauer, E., Ewers, M., Düring, M., Mueller, C., Ruiescu, D., et al. (2016). Effects of age, APOE ε4, cognitive reserve and hippocampal volume on cognitive intervention outcome in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. Parkins. 6:246. doi: 10.4172/2161-0460.1000246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gates, N. J., Sachdev, P. S., Fiatarone Singh, M. A., and Valenzuela, M. (2011). Cognitive and memory training in adults at risk of dementia: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 11:55. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-11-55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test: Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. Chicago, IL: Stoelting.

Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., van Boxtel, M., Bretelerc, M., Ceccaldi, M., Chételat, G., et al. (2014). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 10, 844–852. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirova, A. M., Bays, R. B., and Lagalwar, S. (2015). Working memory and executive function decline across normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. Biomed Res. Int. 2015:748212. doi: 10.1155/2015/748212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., Bherer, L., Colcombe, S. J., Dong, W., and Greenough, W. T. (2004). Environmental influences on cognitive and brain plasticity during aging. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 59, M940–957. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.9.M940

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavine, M., and Schervish, M. J. (1999). Bayes factors: what they are and what they are not. Am. Stat. 53, 119–122.

Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lobo, A., Saz, P., Marcos, G., Día, J. L., De la Cámara, C., and Ventura Aznar, S. (1999). Revalidación y normalización del Mini-Examen Cognoscitivo (primera versión en castellano del Mini-Mental Status Examination) en la población general geriátrica [Re-validation of the Mini-Examen Cognoscitivo (first Spanish version of the mini-mental status examination) in elderly people]. Med. Clín. 112, 767–774.

Google Scholar

Lövdén, M., Bäckman, L., Lindenberger, U., Schäfer, S., and Schmiedek, F. (2010). A theoretical framework for the study of adult cognitive plasticity. Psychol. Bull. 136, 659–676. doi: 10.1037/a0020080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, M. C., Almor, A., Henderson, V. W., Kempler, D., and Andersen, E. S. (2001). Assessing working memory and language comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Lang. 78, 17–42. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, A. J., Beaumont, H., Ferguson, D., Yadegarfar, M., and Stubbs, B. (2014). Risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older people with subjective memory complaints: meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 130, 439–451. doi: 10.1111/acps.12336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mondini, S., Madella, I., Zangrossi, A., Bigolin, A., Tomasi, C., Michieletto, M., et al. (2016). Cognitive reserve in dementia: implications for cognitive training. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:84. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montejo, P. (2015). Estudio de los Resultados de un Programa de Entrenamiento de Memoria y Estimulación Cognitiva Para Mayores de 65 Años sin Deterioro Cognitivo. Doctoral, thesis, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid.

Montejo, P., Montenegro, M., Reinoso, A. I., De Andrés, M. E., and Claver, M. D. (2013). El método UMAM de Entrenamiento de Memoria [The UMAM Memory Training Method]. Available at: Retrieved from http://www.madridsalud.es/centros/monograficos/deterioro/UMAM.pdf

Olazarán, J., Muñiz, R., Reisberg, B., Peña-Casanova, J., del Ser, T., Cruz-Jentoft, A. J., et al. (2004). Benefits of cognitive-motor intervention in MCI and mild to moderate Alzheimer disease. Neurology 63, 2348–2353. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000147478.03911.28

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rami, L., Valls-Pedret, C., Bartrés-Faz, D., Caprile, C., Solé-Padullés, C., Castellvi, M., et al. (2011). Cuestionario de reserva cognitiva. Valores obtenidos en población anciana sana y con enfermedad de Alzheimer [Cognitive reserve questionnaire. Scores obtained in a healthy elderly population and in one with Alzheimer’s disease]. Rev. Neurol. 52, 195–201.

Google Scholar

Ranganath, C., Flegal, K. E., and Kelly, L. L. (2011). Can cognitive training improve episodic memory? Neuron 72, 688–691. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M. (1994). Ward Halstead’s contributions to neuropsychology and the halstead-reitan neuropsychological test battery. J. Clin. Psychol. 50, 47–70. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199401)50:1<47::AID-JCLP2270500106>3.0.CO;2-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, I. H. (2013). A noradrenergic theory of cognitive reserve: implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 34, 298–308. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 6, 461–464. doi: 10.1214/aos/1176344136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seo, E. H., Kim, H., Lee, K. H., and Choo, I. L. H. (2016). Altered executive function in pre-mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 54, 933–940. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheikh, J. I., and Yesavage, J. A. (1986). “Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS). Recent evidence and development of a shorter version,” in Clinical Gerontology: A Guide to Assessment and Intervention, ed. T. L. Brink (New York, NY: The Haworth Press), 165–173.

Google Scholar

Smart, C. M., Karr, J. E., Areshenkoff, C. N., Rabin, L. A., Hudon, C., Gates, N., et al. (2017). Non-pharmacologic interventions for older adults with subjective cognitive decline: systematic review, meta-analysis, and preliminary recommendations. Neuropsychol. Rev. 27, 245–257. doi: 10.1007/s11065-017-9342-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 11, 1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Shake, M. C., Miles, J. R., and Noh, S. R. (2006). Adult age differences in the effects of goals on self-regulated sentence processing. Psychol. Aging 21, 790–803. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1997). WMS-III. Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive reserve, executive functions, working memory, cognitive training, efficacy, cognitive status

Citation: López-Higes R, Martín-Aragoneses MT, Rubio-Valdehita S, Delgado-Losada ML, Montejo P, Montenegro M, Prados JM, de Frutos-Lucas J and López-Sanz D (2018) Efficacy of Cognitive Training in Older Adults with and without Subjective Cognitive Decline Is Associated with Inhibition Efficiency and Working Memory Span, Not with Cognitive Reserve. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:23. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00023

Received: 14 November 2017; Accepted: 18 January 2018;
Published: 02 February 2018.

Edited by:

Ashok Kumar, University of Florida, United States

Reviewed by:

Paul J. Colombo, Tulane University, United States
Laura Lorenzo-López, University of A Coruña, Spain

Copyright © 2018 López-Higes, Martín-Aragoneses, Rubio-Valdehita, Delgado-Losada, Montejo, Montenegro, Prados, de Frutos-Lucas and López-Sanz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ramón López-Higes, rlopezsa@psi.ucm.es

Download