Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neuroanat., 06 November 2015
Volume 9 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2015.00140

Forebrain neuroanatomy of the neonatal and juvenile dolphin (T. truncatus and S. coeruloalba)

  • 1Neuroscience Institute Cavalieri Ottolenghi, University of Turin, Orbassano, Italy
  • 2Department of Veterinary Sciences, University of Turin, Torino, Italy
  • 3Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padova, Legnaro, Italy

Knowledge of dolphin functional neuroanatomy mostly derives from post-mortem studies and non-invasive approaches (i.e., magnetic resonance imaging), due to limitations in experimentation on cetaceans. As a consequence the availability of well-preserved tissues for histology is scarce, and detailed histological analyses are referred mainly to adults. Here we studied the neonatal/juvenile brain in two species of dolphins, the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) and the striped dolphin (Stenella coeruleoalba), with special reference to forebrain regions. We analyzed cell density in subcortical nuclei, white/gray matter ratio, and myelination in selected regions at different anterior–posterior levels of the whole dolphin brain at different ages, to better define forebrain neuroanatomy and the developmental stage of the dolphin brain around birth. The analyses were extended to the periventricular germinal layer and the cerebellum, whose delayed genesis of the granule cell layer is a hallmark of postnatal development in the mammalian nervous system. Our results establish an atlas of the young dolphin forebrain and, on the basis of occurrence/absence of delayed neurogenic layers, confirm the stage of advanced brain maturation in these animals with respect to most terrestrial mammals.

Introduction

Experimentation on cetaceans is limited by obvious ethical concerns, which in several instances drastically reduce the availability of fresh tissue specimens. The peculiar anatomical conformation of whales and dolphins, and specifically the presence of a layer of blubber, greatly accelerate the post-mortem decay of nervous tissue upon stranding. Poor body conditions, and delay in samplings from stranded individuals, further contribute to the difficulty of obtaining quality samples for comparative neuroscience research, leaving several fine functional and neuroanatomical problems unsolved. Most of the studies on dolphin brain anatomy are restricted to adults (for general reference see Oelschläger and Oelschläger, 2009). The ubiquitous common bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus (T. truncatus, Tt), is the most extensively investigated cetacean species, yet there have been relatively few studies concerning its brain during fetal or early post-natal life. Studies on the development of the brain of dolphin species are scarce (Pirlot and Kamiya, 1975, 1982; Ridgway, 1986), and the majority of them were based on non-invasive approaches such as weight analysis at different ages (Marino, 1997), and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) directed at analysing either the whole head (Liste et al., 2006; Rauschmann et al., 2006; Montie et al., 2007, 2008) or the whole brain (Marino et al., 2001b, 2004a) (see Table 1). These investigations provided anatomical correlates indicating that at birth the dolphin brain is already at an advanced stage of development (Ridgway, 1990), as observed for terrestrial Cetartiodactyla and hoofed animals in general (Peruffo and Cozzi, 2014). However, studies based on brain weight at different ages indicate that the brain of the bottlenose dolphin continues growing until the animal reaches adulthood (Ridgway and Brownson, 1984). The issue of mammalian brain development around birth and its subsequent maturation during the juvenile stages is important to fully understand to what extent and how long neural plasticity can persist in different species. Unlike laboratory rodents (that possess small lissencephalic brains and have a lifespan of 1–3 years), cetaceans and humans share several features including large brain size, advanced gyrification, long lifespan (see Table 2) and advanced cognitive performances.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Literature available on the anatomical organization of the neonatal/juvenile dolphin brain.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Key biological data of selected dolphin species.

Only a few studies have directly addressed the architectural and histological/immunocytochemical aspects of the dolphin brain (Hof et al., 2005; Furutani, 2008; Kern et al., 2011; Butti et al., 2014; Mortensen et al., 2014). We took advantage of a stock of cetacean brain material collected by the Mediterranean marine mammal tissue bank (MMMTB) of the University of Padova to study the neonatal/juvenile dolphin brain neuroanatomy and developmental stage. We analyzed brain tissues obtained from 13 specimens of two different species, namely the bottlenose dolphin and the striped dolphin Stenella coeruleoalba (S. coeruleoalba, Sc), at neonatal, juvenile, and adult ages. Table 2 provides a synthetic comparison of the key biological parameters of the two species examined here (Tt and Sc), as well as four very similar species whose adult brains were been described by imaging methods (the short-beaked common dolphin Delphinus delphis, Dd; the [Eastern] spinner dolphin S. longirostris [orientalis], Sl; the pantropical spotted dolphin S. attenuata, Sa; and the Atlantic white-sided dolphin Lagenorhynchus acutus, La) or in a conventional atlas (Dd). We emphasize that all these species belong to the dolphin family and have closely related morphology. Furthermore, at least two of the dolphin species listed above (i.e., Sc and Dd) live in mixed schools (Bearzi et al., 2011) that may contain hybrids.

In the present article, we report data obtained at different ages, and compare the brain anatomy of very young (neonatal/juvenile) and adult dolphins, by combining histological studies, analysis of white/gray matter ratio (WM/GM), and of white matter myelination in selected regions. The data were also compared to those available in the literature for adult specimens (e.g., MRI descriptions) and to data obtained for mice (for cerebellum; see below). To better characterize the developmental stage reached by the central nervous system (CNS) of dolphins at birth, the analysis was extended to the cerebellum, to identify the existence/exhaustion of transient germinal layers in the cerebellar cortex, a hallmark for mammalian CNS development (Altman, 1969). In parallel, our study was aimed at obtaining a more detailed neuroanatomical definition of the neonatal/juvenile dolphin forebrain, which in terrestrial mammals is known to host the persistence of neurogenic processes in the form of stem cell niches lining the lateral ventricle wall (the so-called subventricular zone; see Bonfanti and Ponti, 2008; Bonfanti and Peretto, 2011).

Material And Methods

Tissue Samples

Dolphin Tissues

In this study we used samples from the brains of 13 dolphins, 10 bottlenose dolphins, and 3 striped dolphins, stored in the MMMTB of the University of Padova at Legnaro, Italy (see Table 3). The MMMTB is a CITES recognized (IT020) research center and tissue bank (Ballarin et al., 2005), sponsored by the Italian Ministry of the Environment and the University of Padova. The MMMTB includes tissues from (a) cetaceans stranded along the Italian coastline, and (b) captive dolphins sent to the Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science of the University of Padova for post-mortem examination. Tissue samples preserved in the MMMTB are distributed to qualified research centers worldwide, following a specific documented request, according to national and international regulations on protected species (CITES).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Detail of the sampled bottlenose and striped dolphins.

The bottlenose and the striped dolphin have very similar shape and anatomy; differences in size and weight (Tt is generally larger than Sc) are actually well evident in oceanic animals, but they are less marked in dolphins living in comparatively smaller basins (including the Mediterranean). Ages will be referred to as neonatal (shortly after birth; 7–20 days), juvenile (3–6 months), and adult (15–50 years). A synthesis of the key biological parameters of the two species is reported in Table 2.

Tissue samples used for the present investigation consisted of coronal brain slices (Figure 1), including the cerebellum (Figure 2C), approximately 1–1.5 cm thick, collected in the necropsy room of the Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science of the University of Padova at Legnaro, and fixed by immersion in 4% buffered formalin. Post-mortem delay before actual sampling varied between 18 and 40 h.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. (A) Brain tissue samples of Tt and Sc used in this study. Arrow, orientation of coronal cuts performed to obtain thick brain slices (an example is showed on the right). ID, identification numbers of all animals used (see Table 3); L, left hemisphere; R, right hemisphere. Color lines indicate the amount of tissue analyzed histologically for each animal and hemisphere (neonatal Tt, shades of blue; juvenile Sc, red; adult Tt, shades of green; adult Sc, shades of yellow), as a percentage of the whole brain extension (black backlot; not in scale). Arrowheads indicate the position of slices photographed to establish anterior–posterior levels; those outside the color lines were used for quantification of white/gray matter ratio (white squares on the right indicate animals used only for these analyses). (B) Photographs of sample slices, positioned at their exact level along the brain anterior–posterior axis (levels in scale with respect to the whole brain length; total brain length – indicated on the right, beside the brains – not in scale); all slices are represented with the lateral ventricle on the right, with the same orientation used in the drawings in the following figures. On the right, representative brains showing the position of all coronal cuts performed to obtain the slices (in each age group brains from different animals are considered; not in scale). The length values are referred to Tt-186 (blue line), Sc-320 (red line); the value in adults (9,92 ± 2,6 cm) is an average length obtained from an estimation of our material and data reported in literature, for both Tt and Sc (green and yellow lines).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. (A) To obtain a graphic representation of single brain slices the contours of each coronal section surface (external -pial- surface and white/gray matter limits) were drawn: GM, gray matter (gray areas); WM, white matter (white areas); LV lateral ventricle; H, histological analysis (Cresyl violet staining) was performed within some areas, with particular reference to the subcortical gray matter of forebrain regions (see also Figures 4 and 5). (B) Representative drawings showing how single, thick brain slices were cut into blocks (black lines) to be included and frozen for subsequent cryostat cutting; dashed areas indicate the blocks analyzed histologically in these representative slices; all blocks analyzed in this study at the different anterior–posterior levels are listed in Table 4. Hist, histological analysis; Gal, Gallyas method. (C) Comparison of drawings obtained from neonatal Tt and juvenile Sc samples with information available from literature, obtained with different approaches at different ages. a,a’, representative slices of neonatal Tt and juvenile Sc brain material analyzed in this study (see Figure 1); a, coronal brain slices; a’, drawing carried out at the same level; b, originally acquired coronal magnetic resonance images (MRIs) sections of the infant brain (less than 6 months; from Marino et al., 2004a). c, c’, c”, reference images of adult dolphin brains; originally acquired coronal sections stained for cell bodies with Nissl method (c) and for myelinated fibers (c’), from The Yakovlev-Haleem collection at the National Museum of Health and Medicine, Armed Forces Institute of Pathology; c” originally acquired coronal MRI sections of the adult brain (from Marino et al., 2001aTursiops truncatus; from Marino et al., 2004bStenella longirostris orientalis). (D) Cerebellar tissue samples used in this study. On the left, coronal cutting direction (arrow) to obtain thick slices. In the middle, two representative coronal slices from neonatal Tt and juvenile Sc; scale bars, 1 cm. On the right, cresyl violet staining of cerebellar lamellae; scale bar, 200 μm.

Mouse Tissues

All experimental procedures conducted according to the European Communities Council Directive and the Italian law for the care and use of experimental animals. Mouse tissues were obtained from CD-1 mice (Charles River, Italy) used in a previous work (Ponti et al., 2006b; refer to this publication for experimental procedures) and stored in our lab at the Neuroscience Institute Cavalieri Ottolenghi (N.I.C.O., Orbassano, Turin, Italy). Cerebellum tissue samples were from neonatal (postnatal day 1, P1), postnatal (P10), and puberal (P21) female mice (Mus musculus; n = 1 each age considered). Fixed cerebella were cut in blocks, frozen at -80°C and cryostat sectioned (30 μm thick) coronally.

Gross Anatomy of Dolphin Tissue Slices

To obtain a graphic representation of individual brain levels, the front face of thick brain slices was photographed and imported in Neurolucida (Micro-Brightfield, Colchester,VT, USA), where the outlines of each coronal section, including those of the external (pial) surface and those at the white matter/gray matter limits, were drawn (Figure 2A). The contours were then imported in Photoshop to obtain images of each brain level (see Figure 3). This procedure was followed both for specimens that underwent histological analysis and for those that were simply photographed (see Table 3). These drawings were used for two types of analyses: (i) comparison of the neonatal/juvenile dolphin brain with previous literature data, such as MRI of similar age and/or atlases obtained from adult animals (Figure 2C); (ii) quantification of the WM/GM (see below).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Anterior–posterior levels obtained from drawings of coronal slices (as indicated in Figure 2) cut from brains of both species at different ages: neonatal Tt, levels Tt1–Tt12; juvenile Sc, levels Sc1–Sc6; adult Tt and Sc, levels L1–L5. Squares of different colors indicate the animal specimens whose slices have been used for drawing single levels (the same as indicated in Figure 1). Note that more numerous, although less complete brains are available for neonatal Tt, whereas only one specimen, yet with a more complete brain, is available for juvenile Sc. All the young brains were used for establishing a forebrain atlas (see Figure 7). Adult brains were used for age comparisons and white/gray matter ratio quantification. The position of each level from left to right corresponds to its exact relative location with respect to the whole brain length. Dotted lines show the best matched coronal levels from adjacent slices; L1–L5, ideal levels shared by different animals, species and ages; asterisks indicate the levels closest to ideal levels. All drawings are in scale.

Tissue Processing for Histology, Gallyas, and Immunocytochemistry

Smaller blocks were cut from the thick formalin-fixed tissue slices (about 1,5 cm × 2,5 cm; see Figure 2B), washed in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB), pH 7.4, for 24 h, then cryoprotected sucrose solutions at gradually increasing concentrations, up to 30% in 0.1 M PB and subsequently frozen by immersion in liquid nitrogen-chilled isopentane at -80°C. Cryostat sections (40 μm thick) were cut on glass slides treated with 3-Aminopropyltriethoxysilane (Sigma–Aldrich, 741442) and processed for histological, silver staining, and immunocytochemical analyses. Some cryostat sections were processed for standard Gallyas stain of myelin using silver nitrate (Gallyas, 1979). Thick slices and blocks thereof used for histological analyses at different anterior–posterior levels and different ages are summarized in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Summary of all thick slices and relative blocks (see also Figure 2B) used for histologic analyses in the neonatal, juvenile, and adult dolphin brains, at different anterior–posterior levels (see Figure 3).

For immunocytochemical analyses, the sections were incubated in 1% H2O2 – phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) for 20 min, rinsed in PBS and then pre-incubated for 1 h at room temperature in blocking buffer [3% horse serum (HS), 2% bovine serum albumin (BSA), 1% Triton X-100 in 0.01 M PBS, pH 7.4] to reduce non-specific staining. The sections were then incubated for 24–48 h at 4°C in a solution of 0.01 M PBS, pH 7.4, containing 0.5% Triton X-100, 2% HS, 1% BSA and a primary antibody directed against Ki67 (NCL-Ki67p, made in rabbit, Novocastra, 1:1000), a nuclear protein expressed in all phases of the cell cycle except the resting phase, commonly used as a marker for cell proliferation; Glial Fibrillary Acidic Protein (GFAP, made in rabbit, Dako, 1:2000), an astrocytic marker. Immunohistochemical reactions were performed by the avidin–biotin–peroxidase method (Vectastain ABC Elite kit; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA) and revealed using 3,3′-diaminobenzidine (3% in Tris-HCl) as chromogen. The sections were counterstained with Cresyl violet, according to standard procedures currently employed in our lab at the N.I.C.O. (see Ponti et al., 2006a,b), mounted with DPX Mountant (Sigma–Aldrich, 06522) and examined using an E-800 Nikon microscope (Nikon, Melville, NY, USA) connected to a color CCD Camera.

Image Processing and Data Analyses

All the images were analyzed using Adobe Photoshop CS4 (Adobe Systems, San Jose, CA, USA). Only general adjustments to color, contrast, and brightness were made.

Quantitative evaluations (cell density, cell size, WM/GM rate) were performed by means of the Neurolucida software (MicroBrightfield, Colchester, VT, USA). Measurements were obtained from: 20 sections in total from four animals (two animals for each ages – neonatal/juvenile and adult) to determine cell density (expressed as number of cells/area); 200 cells in total from four animals (two animals for each age), to determine cells size (area); 116 photographed slices from 13 animals (n = 5 neonatal, n = 1 juvenile, hereafter merged and referred to as neo/juvenile; n = 7 for adult) for WM/GM measurements (see Table 5). To compare different animals, regions of interest with matching positions along the anterior–posterior axis were chosen (Figure 3). All the graphs were obtained using Graph Pad Prism (San Diego California, USA). Statistical analyses were performed by Graph Pad Prism software and included unpaired (two-tailed) Student’s t-test (comparing only two groups). p < 0.05 was considered as statistically significant. Data are expressed as mean ± SD.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Material employed for analysis of white/gray matter ratio (W/G) in the dolphin brain at different ages. Each level corresponds to a photographed brain slice.

Identification of Main White and Gray Matter Profiles in Brain Tissue Slices from Neonatal Bottlenose Dolphins, and Adult Bottlenose and Striped Dolphins: Definition of Brain Levels

Anterior–posterior levels were obtained by considering the drawings derived from all slices examined in young animals (see Figure 1 and Table 3): a total of 12 (T1–T12, for neonatal Tt) and 6 (S1–S6, for juvenile Sc) (Figure 3). The higher number of levels in Tt was due to greater availability of animal specimens with respect to Sc (see also Figure 1). Five ideal levels of the dolphin brain were identified by adding and comparing all brain levels of Tt and Sc, so that each of the actual levels (and slices) analyzed in our study could be related to one of them (Figure 3). For instance, analyses were carried out on all the 12 levels to better exploit the Tt material, then the conclusions were centered on those levels which appeared more similar to the six Sc levels and to the five ideal levels (correspondences are indicated in Figure 3). Slices from adult animals (both species) were drawn in the same way. These drawings were intended mainly for comparative assessment of WM/GM; they were also used to assemble the atlas, in addition to literature data (The Yakovlev-Haleem collection at the National Museum of Health and Medicine; Pilleri et al., 1980).

Results

Cytoarchitecture in Major Subcortical Structures of Neonatal Bottlenose Dolphins and Juvenile Striped Dolphins: an Atlas of the Neonatal/Juvenile Dolphin Forebrain

Histological analyses based on Cresyl violet staining carried out at different anterior–posterior levels of forebrain regions (Table 4; Figures 4 and 5) were aimed at studying the cytoarchitectonics and neuronal morphology of the subcortical gray matter of the forebrain. Four major nuclei were identified both in neonatal T. truncatus and juvenile S. coeruloalba (indicated with A–D in capital Times New Roman font in Figures 46). These nuclei frequently had ill-defined limits, since they were separated by a combination of white and gray matter, well recognizable with respect to homogeneous white matter tracts (e.g., the corpus callosum and the internal capsule). However their extension and neuroanatomical location were very similar in neonatal and juvenile dolphins of both species. A five level (L1–L5) atlas was drawn by comparing the gross anatomy of the brain slices and forebrain histology/cytoarchitecture obtained from our series of neonatal T. truncatus and juvenile S. coeruloalba with previous MRI data obtained in young animals, and histological atlas of the adult dolphin brain (The Yakovlev-Haleem collection at the National Museum of Health and Medicine; The Welker collection at the University of Wisconsin Madison). For S. coeruloalba, in the absence of species-specific reference work, we used a MRI study (Marino et al., 2004b) performed on Sl, that belongs to the same genre. Special attention was given to levels L2 and L4, that host most of the forebrain nuclei (Figure 7). Given the aim of the present study, we focused on forebrain areas directly surrounding the lateral ventricle and possibly hosting remnants of the germinal layer of the embryonic subventricular zone. The amygdaloid complex was not considered in detail. Comparative analyses of these nuclei in young and adult dolphins showed striking similarities in area and topography, in line with the advanced development of the dolphin brain in the perinatal phase. To verify such conclusion, qualitative and quantitative analysis were performed at different ages (Figure 6). All neurons, at all ages examined, showed a prevalent multipolar morphology and constant size of the cell body (Figure 6C). The lateral part of nucleus C was characterized by small, densely packed cells (C′), subsequently identified as glial cells by immunocytochemical staining for the astrocytic marker GFAP (showed only in adults in Figure 6). In addition, neuronal cell density and size were evaluated in three forebrain nuclei (A, B, C) at different ages (Figure 6C). The average cell density was higher in some nuclei of the neonate (A and B) with respect to adults (A, p = 0,0340; B, ∗∗∗p < 0,0001). On the whole, histological, morphological and quantitative data clearly identified the caudate nucleus (A), thalamus (B), putamen (C), and claustrum (D) (see Table in Figure 6C, and atlas in Figure 7), and confirmed substantial similarities between neonatal and adult brains.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Histological identification of cytoarchitectonics in the forebrain of the neonatal Tt. Four main nuclei can be identified in the forebrain subcortical gray matter (A–D), indicated with Times New Roman font and circled on photographs at representative coronal levels of the brain (Tt4, Tt5, Tt7; selected among those indicated in Figure 3 and indicated top left). anteriorly and posteriorly to levels Tt2 and Tt11, respectively, no subcortical gray matter is present (top right). Dark gray, gray matter; light gray, white matter (WM); wgm, subcortical white matter intermixed with gray matter (checked pattern); Cx, cerebral cortex; CC, corpus callosum; EC, external capsule; IC, internal capsule; LV, lateral ventricle (bent arrow in enlarged drawings on the left); e, ependyma. Note that nucleus B and ependyma of the lateral ventricle wall are separated by a mixture of white and gray matter in the absence of any thick, tightly packed cell layer which is typical of germinal layers in neonatal terrestrial mammals (arrowheads; see text). Scale bars, 100 μm.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Histological identification of cytoarchitectonics in the forebrain of the juvenile Sc. No subcortical gray matter is present anteriorly and posteriorly to levels Sc1 and Sc5, respectively. The same forebrain nuclei showed in Figure 4 for neonatal Tt (A–D) Times New Roman font and circled on photographs are detectable in juvenile Sc at representative levels of the brain (Sc2–4). The enlarged drawing depicting Sc4 has been rotated with respect to the square for reasons of space. For abbreviations, see Figure 4. Scale bars, 100 μm.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. (A) Cytoarchitectonics and morphology of neurons in different forebrain nuclei of neonatal Tt and juvenile Sc. Gray matter nuclei are indicated in Times New Roman font (A–D), as in Figures 4 and 5. CC, corpus callosum; IC, internal capsule; LV, lateral ventricle; wgm, subcortical white matter intermixed with gray matter; Cx, cerebral cortex. Scale bar, 10 μm. (B) Histological analyses carried out on the adult (Tt and Sc) dolphin forebrain. The same forebrain nuclei (A–C) described in neonatal and juvenile dolphin brain can easily be identified in the adult forebrain of both species; abbreviations as in (A). Scale bars: lower magnifications and GFAP staining, 100 μm; higher magnifications, 10 μm. (C) Table summarizing the main histological, morphological and quantitative measure parameters analyzed in the neonatal, juvenile, adult dolphin forebrain, leading to nuclei identification (also based on data reported in Figures 4 and 5); right, quantitative analyses of cell density and neuronal size in three forebrain nuclei at different ages.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 7. Atlas in five levels of the ideal dolphin brain at perinatal stages, with particular reference to forebrain, subcortical periventricular structures (levels 2 and 4), based on the analyses of neonatal Tt and juvenile Sc reported in Figures 46; top left, blue and red lines indicate levels analyzed in neonatal Tt and juvenile Sc, respectively, then merged in five ideal levels (L1–L5, corresponding to those indicated in Figure 3). CN, caudatus; P, putamen; T, thalamus; Cl, claustrum; LV, lateral ventricle; CC, corpus callosum; IC, internal capsule; EC, external capsule; LC, limbic cleft; SLC, superior lateral cleft; PC, paralimbic cleft; Ip, insular pocket. The amygdaloid complex was not considered.

Interestingly enough, either the caudate nucleus (A) and thalamus (B), or, more frequently, an intermixed white/gray matter stripe, were lying directly beneath the ependyma (Figures 4 and 5), all along the lateral wall of the lateral ventricle (extending from levels Tt4 to Tt10 and Sc2 to Sc5). Therefore the thick germinal layer typically occurring in the periventricular region of all neonatal terrestrial mammals studied so far was clearly absent (see Discussion for references). Further immunocytochemical analysis of Ki67 antigen expression in the lateral wall of the lateral ventricle did not detect any proliferative activity in periventricular, sub-ependymal position (not shown), thus confirming the absence of periventricular germinal layers in dolphins around birth.

White Matter Quantification in the Neonatal, Juvenile, and Adult Dolphin Brain

A quantification of the areas was performed to measure the WM/GM ratio by using the drawings of white and gray matter limits in each coronal slice cut at different levels (approximated to standard brain levels, L1–L5) in 13 animals of both species and different ages (Table 5 and Figure 8). On the whole, the WM/GM ratio was lower in neonatal (0,39601 ± 0,13137) and juvenile (0,48419 ± 0,088024) individuals (Figure 8A, top left), whereas it was higher (0,73267 ± 0,19124) in adults (Figure 8A, top right). Statistical analysis was performed by adding all brain levels available in all animals, since in some animals it was not possible to have all levels. Comparison of neonatal Tt (0,3960 ± 0,1314) vs. juvenile Sc (0,4842 ± 0,0880), and juvenile (0,4842 ± 0,0880) vs. adult (0,7401 ± 0,1860), showed no significant differences (p = 0,2477 and p = 0,0239, respectively; unpaired t-test – two tailed). However, a small but significant difference (p = 0,015; unpaired t-test – two tailed) was detected between the neonatal/juvenile (0,4401 ± 0,1042) and adult brains (0.7469 ± 0,1964) (Figure 8A, bottom). Standard deviations were higher in some adult specimens, particularly for levels L1, L2, L5, namely, the most rostral or posterior parts, where the white matter is progressively replaced by cortical gray matter.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 8. White matter development in dolphins of different ages. (A) Analysis of white/gray matter ratio (W/G) in the neonatal, juvenile, and adult dolphin (neonatal Tt, juvenile Sc, adult Tt and Sc). (B) Myelinization in axons of the corpus callosum detected with Gallyas stain technique. Scale bar, 100 μm.

The extent of myelination was assessed by Gallyas stain. Such analysis revealed the presence of myelin sheaths in the corpus callosum of dolphins already around birth, the pattern of stain being only slightly increased in intensity at juvenile and adult ages (Figure 8B).

Extension/Exhaustion of Transient Germinal Layers in the Cerebellar Cortex of The Neonatal T. truncatus and Juvenile S. coeruloalba (and Comparison with Mouse)

Histological analysis (Cresyl violet staining) and Ki67 antigen immunocytochemical detection were used to investigate the existence and/or exhaustion of a proliferative external granule layer (EGL; a hallmark of cerebellar development in mammals; Figure 9A, top right) in the cerebellar cortex of neonatal Tt and juvenile Sc. For comparison we also analyzed neonatal, postnatal, and adult mouse cerebella (Figure 9A). Laboratory rodents have a thick EGL at birth, whereas a clear-cut molecular layer (ML) and a well formed inner granular layer (IGL) are lacking (Figure 9A, top). The entire granule cell population of IGL completes its formation only later, at pre-pubertal stages, after progressive thickening of both ML and IGL during postnatal stages (Figure 9A, bottom).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 9. Histological study and cell proliferation analysis in the cerebellar cortex of mice and dolphins, at different postnatal ages. (A) Mouse cerebellum at neonatal, postnatal and puberal stages; the panel on the right summarizes the morphogenesis of the inner granule layer (IGL) in mammals, which occurs mostly postnatally through migration of granule cell precursors from a subpial, proliferative external granule layer (EGL, subsequently exhausted). Micrographs: note the absence of well formed IGL and molecular layer (ML) in the mouse cerebellar cortex at birth, and the persistence of a proliferative EGL until the pre-puberal ages; at puberal stages the IGL is fully formed and the EGL is exhausted. (B) Dolphin cerebellar cortex at neonatal (top) and juvenile (bottom) stages. Note the thick, well formed IGL and ML in T. truncatus at birth; a thin, proliferative EGL (Ki67+ cells on the right) is still present on the cerebellar surface. In juvenile S. coeruloalba (bottom) the EGL has disappeared and the ML is directly in contact with the cerebellar surface (pia mater). Scale bars: 100 μm; mouse low magnifications (top), 500 μm; mouse P0 and Ki67, 50 μm.

The cerebellum of neonatal Tt showed a relatively thin EGL in subpial position (recognizable as a layer of tightly packed, small, darkly stained cells with numerous actively proliferating cells) on the cerebellar surface, and thick, well formed IGL and ML within the cerebellar cortex (Figure 9B, top; Table 6). Immunodetection of Ki67 antigen clearly confirmed the occurrence of cell proliferation in the EGL of neonatal Tt (Figure 9B, top right). By contrast, no histological evidence for EGL was detectable in the juvenile Sc cerebellar cortex (Figure 9B, bottom), whose subpial surface, as expected, was also devoid of proliferating cells, thus showing that exhaustion of proliferative EGL has already occurred in dolphins 3–6 months after birth. However, the extension of IGL in juvenile Sc was quite similar to that observed in neonatal Tt, supporting the evidence for an advanced stage of development in the cerebellar cortex of the dolphin around birth (see Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 6. Existence/exhaustion of transient germinal layers and formation of mature neuronal layers in the cerebellar cortex of neonatal Tt, juvenile Sc, and neonatal, postnatal, pubertal mouse.

Discussion

In the present report we exploited material belonging to 10 bottlenose dolphins and three striped dolphins conserved at the MMMTB in Padova, with the aim of gaining a deeper insight into the neuroanatomy of the neonatal/juvenile dolphin brain. We focused on developmental stages during the perinatal period, with particular reference to forebrain and cerebellar regions that are known to host neurogenic layers in terrestrial mammals. The development of dolphins has been studied and described in detail in the accurate work of Štěrba et al. (2000). In their comprehensive article, Štěrba et al. (2000) analyzed several specimens of Sl, Sa, Dd, and Phocoena (this latter non-delphinid species is not relevant here and will not be discussed further). The specimens were painstakingly compared to the reference stages detailed by O’Rahilly (1972), who used embryonic and fetal material from the Carnegie reference collection. Since marine mammals (including dolphins) lack external hair (a key temporal landmark for developmental stages in terrestrial mammals), Štěrba et al. (2000) established a new set of stages adapted from that of O’Rahilly. Data relative to staging and development basically demonstrated that (not-surprisingly) the specimens belonging to the genus Stenella and Delphinus are very similar. The body measures of all the species fall within the same range, with minimal variations due to the single specimens and their relative sample numerosity in the different stages. Another study on the same subject (Thewissen and Heyning, 2007) adopted a different staging for fetal development in dolphins, but came to conclusions very similar to those of Štěrba et al. (2000).

The rationale for comparing the species that we mainly describe in our study, i.e., the bottlenose dolphin, with specimens of the striped dolphin, is based on their common origin in the family Delphinidae and on the similarity of their morphology (see also Table 2). Furthermore, their developmental stages are comparable, as showed in a recent study (Cozzi et al., 2015) in which embryos and fetuses of Sc, Dd, and Tt were analyzed for progressive very early deposition of calcium salts in the tympanoperiotic complex (a rather distinctive trait of marine Cetartiodactyla). The bottlenose dolphin Tt is approx. 1/10 larger than Sc at birth in the Mediterranean area (Cagnolaro et al., 2015), but the stages of fetal development are very similar and follow the same pattern.

Here we present an atlas of the neonatal/juvenile forebrain regions (Figure 7). This allowed comparing the neuroanatomical features observed in the very young dolphin with data gathered in our adult specimens, and with data currently available in the literature for different species of adult dolphins (The Yakovlev-Haleem collection at the National Museum of Health and Medicine; The Welker collection at the University of Wisconsin Madison; Jacobs et al., 1979; Pilleri et al., 1980; Oelschläger et al., 2008; see Figure 2C). The four main characteristics of brain morphology investigated in our study (i–iv in the following text; summarized in Figure 10) indicate an advanced developmental stage of the dolphin brain at birth, and were derived from separate but convergent neuroanatomical observations: (i) forebrain neuroanatomy; (ii) perinatal myelination; (iii) postnatal genesis of granules in the cerebellar cortex; (iv) absence of a well defined sub-ventricular germinal layer. Though comparisons of two different species, even those that are morphologically similar, carries the potential for misinterpretations of results, our data shows that the brains of the bottlenose and striped dolphin are easily comparable. Although their absolute weights differ, the sizes of the single components (selected nuclei of the forebrain, cortical areas, etc.) remained proportionally consistent throughout the experimental series.

(i) Forebrain Neuroanatomy

Topographical location, extension, and cytoarchitectonics of the main forebrain nuclei are very similar at different ages from birth to adulthood (Figures 46). The aspect of corresponding coronal slices (e.g., Figure 10A’), and the neuronal morphology and size in the main forebrain nuclei appear quite similar at all ages (Figure 6). A slightly increased neuronal density in the caudate nucleus and thalamus around birth could simply depend on the growth in size of these nuclei (a similar trend was previously reported for cortical areas in the brain of the same species; Garey and Leuba, 1986). These observations suggest early maturation of the basal forebrain circuits, including the striate and its connections, which are fundamental for motor coordination and learning abilities (Ridgway, 1990). In humans, the brain grows from just under 25% of its adult weight at birth to full development with complete wiring by approximately 17–18 years. Such prolonged dependence on parents increases the period during which learning can occur (Bonner, 1980; Ridgway, 1990). Dolphins are born after a long gestational period (about 12 months, see Table 2) with brains at an advanced stage of development, which enables them to swim and perform activities (including decodification of acoustic signals essential for orientation and familial bonding) requiring a relative maturity. It has been estimated that the weight of the dolphin brain at birth is about 42% of the average weight of the adult brain, and exceeds 80% at weaning (18 months), which is higher than the value obtained for the human brain at 3–4 years of age. Dolphins are considered to reach full brain development when they reach sexual maturity (see Table 2), i.e., in about half the time required for the development of the human brain (see also Figure 10C).

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 10. Comparison between neonate and adult dolphins, and between dolphins and terrestrial mammals. Different features converge to confirm the advanced developmental stage of the dolphin nervous system at birth. (A) Neuroanatomical features: (A’), neonatal/juvenile dolphin brain anatomy is very similar to that described in adults; (A”) white matter growth and myelination in dolphins are already advanced at neonatal/juvenile stages; WM, white matter; GM, gray matter; in the graph on the right the means calculated at single brain levels of different animals are showed. (B) Existence/exhaustion of postnatal germinal layers; (B’), advanced development of the cerebellar cortex in neo/postnatal dolphins: the external germinal layer (EGL) is extremely thin at birth and disappears at a very juvenile stage; the granule cell layer (IGL) is almost formed at birth; (B”) no germinal layer is detectable along the wall of the lateral ventricle at birth in dolphins, whereas it is present in all mammals studied so far (see references in the text). (C) The neonatal brain: comparison between aquatic and terrestrial mammals.

(ii) White Matter Growth and Myelination Around Birth

In our experimental series, the growth of white matter is well advanced very close to birth and myelination has already occurred (Figure 10A”). As previously described in studies using non-invasive approaches in the Atlantic white-sided dolphin (a species member of the same dolphin family, see Table 2), white matter measurements in MRI represent myelinated or mature white matter volumes, whereas gray matter measurements are the sum of immature white matter, cortical gray matter, and subcortical gray matter volumes (Montie et al., 2008). Thus, white matter in neonates can be underestimated by MRI (at least in Atlantic white-sided dolphin, another species of the family Delphinidae), whereas in our specimens white and gray matter were identified directly on the slices. We only found a slight increase in the WM/GM from neonates to adults (Figures 3 and 10A”), thus confirming that white matter growth is already advanced around birth, which has also been suggested by previously reported biochemical studies (Lesch, 1969). The occurrence of myelination was also investigated through histological analysis with Gallyas staining, which confirmed its presence around birth. Myelination is nearly complete at birth in precocious animals; in most mammals it occurs relatively late in development in a defined temporal sequence. In mice and rats, it starts at birth in the spinal cord but the brain is involved only later in postnatal life, whereby most of the nerve fibers in the corpus callosum are non-myelinated in the neonate (Sternberger et al., 1978; Vincze et al., 2008). In humans, the peak of myelination occurs much later, during the 1st year of life, and coincides with the development of motor abilities and specific cognitive functions (Fields, 2008; Snaidero and Simons, 2014). On the other hand, newborn terrestrial Cetartiodactyla (including ruminants, swine, camels) must be immediately able to stand and move to survive in the wild. The advanced myelination at birth that we note here in dolphins (marine Cetartiodactyla) fulfills the immediate need of the newborn to possess all the swimming competences required for life, including the ability to reach the surface and breath.

(iii) Postnatal Genesis of Granules in the Cerebellar Cortex

Analysis of the developmental stage of delayed neurogenic processes also converged to indicate advanced brain maturation. Indeed, the postnatal genesis of the population of granules in the dolphin cerebellar cortex is quite advanced. The cerebellar cortex is a hallmark of postnatal neuronal development in mammals, characterized by a process of delayed neurogenesis (see Ponti et al., 2008; Bonfanti and Peretto, 2011) leading to formation of the largest CNS neuronal population, i.e., the granule cells, organized in the cell IGL. The IGL derives from radial, centripetal migration of granule cell precursors of a proliferating, transient external granule layer or external germinal layer (EGL; Altman,1969; Abraham et al., 2001; Figure 9A). EGL exhaustion on the cerebellar surface occurs at species-specific postnatal stages and is tightly linked to postnatal CNS maturation in mammals that differ in their degrees of motor skills at birth, age of puberty, and lifespan (Sanchez-Villagra and Sultan, 2002; Ponti et al., 2008, 2010). In altricial mammals (somatically immature at birth, such as the mouse and rat), the formation/maturation of the cerebellum can be protracted until puberty, since this organ is crucially involved in the acquisition of coordinated sensorimotor behavior. Yet, across mammals, the most mature cerebella at birth still have an EGL, indicating that the mossy fiber-granule cell connectivity is not yet fully developed and may depend on external experiences to fully mature (Sanchez-Villagra and Sultan, 2002). Our analysis of the dolphin cerebellar cortex shows signs of very advanced development, i.e., substantial IGL formation close to birth, and EGL disappearing at very early juvenile stages (3–6 months) with respect to the age of puberty (around 5–7 years, or even later) (Figure 10B”).

(iv) Absence of a Well Defined Sub-ventricular Germinal Layer

In our study, both histology and Ki67 antigen immunocytochemical detection did not reveal any proliferative activity in the sub-ependymal area of the lateral ventricle, suggesting the absence of periventricular germinal layers in dolphins at birth. Here we emphasize that the strong Ki67 antigen stain obtained in the EGL of the cerebellar cortex also represented an internal control (Figure 9B).

All the aspects examined here depend on several variables linked to the timeframe of pre- and post-natal development which include duration of the gestation period, independence of pups at birth, age of puberty, and animal lifespan, among others (see Workman et al., 2013 and the related site: http://www.translatingtime.net). It is therefore difficult to identify specific inferences, rather than a general trend. Our conclusions concerning the developmental stage of the neonatal dolphin brain are intended to stay within limits of a confirmation of previous findings, the main focus of this study being the neonatal dolphin forebrain in a perspective of future analyses of neurogenic processes. In this context, data obtained on dolphin cerebellar maturation fit well with the apparent lack of periventricular germinal layer in the neonatal forebrain. The absence of a subventricular zone-derived germinal layer in neonatal dolphins appears to set them strikingly apart from all terrestrial mammals studied so far, including rodents and humans (Tramontin et al., 2003; Peretto et al., 2005; Sanai et al., 2011; Figure 10B’). In humans, whose brain size and lifespan match those of most dolphins species, such periventricular layer is still detectable at birth (Del Bigio, 2011; Sanai et al., 2011), although almost exhausted by 18 months of postnatal life (Sanai et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2011). Its absence in neonatal dolphins, besides confirming the advanced developmental stage of their brain at birth, might open the possibility that persistent forebrain neurogenesis, which is known to supply new neurons to the olfactory bulb in all mammals studied so far (Lois and Alvarez-Buylla, 1994), might not persist in marine mammals devoid of olfaction/olfactory brain structures. Although dolphins and other toothed whales possess a terminal nerve and its relative ganglion (Oelschläger et al., 1987; Demski et al., 1990), they remain in the primitive meninx and their function is still uncertain (Buhl and Oelschläger, 1986). A recent study carried out in the cetacean hippocampus by doublecortin detection reported absence of this marker of structural plasticity in the dentate gyrus, thus suggesting that adult hippocampal neurogenesis could be absent in aquatic Cetartiodactyla (Patzke et al., 2015). Based on these observations, if a residual germinal layer does persist within the forebrain of dolphins, it might be organized differently with respect to terrestrial mammals. Taking into account the extremely large size of the cetacean brain, the search for neurogenic remnants in these species may represent quite a titanic endeavor. The results obtained in the present study suggest that detailed investigation of the occurrence/absence/type of neurogenesis in aquatic Cetartiodactyla at birth (Parolisi et al., in preparation) could provide interesting insights into this fascinating topic, and set a neuroanatomical background for the achievement of this task.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The Authors thank Fondazione CRT for financial support (Bando Ricerca e Istruzione 2014), the University of Turin (Ph.D. program in Veterinary Sciences); the MMMTB of the Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science of the University of Padova for supplying tissue samples of the dolphin brain; Dr. Sandro Mazzariol and Dr. Michele Povinelli for tissue sampling in the necropsy room during post-mortems; and Serena Martire for helpful advice on statistics.

References

Abraham, H., Tornoczky, T., Kosztolanyi, G., and Seress, L. (2001). Cell formation in the cortical layers of the developing human cerebellum. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 19, 53–62. doi: 10.1016/S0736-5748(00)00065-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altman, J. (1969). Autoradiographic and histological studies of postnatal neurogenesis. III. Dating the time of production and onset of differentiation of cerebellar microneurons in rats. J. Comp. Neurol. 137, 433–458.

Google Scholar

Ballarin, C., Papini, L., Bortolotto, A., Butti, C., Peruffo, A., Sassu, R., et al. (2005). An on-line bank for marine mammals of the Mediterranean Sea and adjacent waters. Hystrix Ital. J. Mammal. 16, 127–133. doi: 10.4404/hystrix-16.2-4350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bearzi, G., Bonizzoni, S., Agazzi, S., and Gonzalvo, J. (2011). Striped dolphins and short-beaked common dolphins in the Gulf of Corinth, Greece: abundance estimates from dorsal fin photographs. Mar. Mamm. Sci. 27, E165–E184. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2010.00448.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonfanti, L., and Peretto, P. (2011). Adult neurogenesis in mammals-a theme with many variations. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 930–950. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07832.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonfanti, L., and Ponti, G. (2008). Adult mammalian neurogenesis and the New Zealand white rabbit. Vet. J. 175, 310–331. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2007.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonner, J. T. (1980). The Evolution of Culture in Animals. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Buhl, E. H., and Oelschläger, H. A. (1986). Ontogenetic development of the nervus terminalis in toothed whales. Evidence for its non-olfactory nature. Anat. Embryol. (Berl.) 173, 285–294. doi: 10.1007/BF00318911

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butti, C., Janeway, C. M., Townshend, C., Wicinski, B. A., Reidenberg, J. S., Ridgway, S. H., et al. (2014). The neocortex of cetartiodactyls: I. A comparative Golgi analysis of neuronal morphology in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus), the minke whale (Balaenoptera acutorostrata), and the humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae). Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 3339–3368. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0860-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Cagnolaro, L., Cozzi, B., Notarbartolo di Sciara, G., and Podestà, M. (2015). Fauna d’Italia. XLIX – Mammalia IV. Cetacea, Calderini. Bologna: Edagricole – New Business Media, 1–376.

Cozzi, B., Podestà, M., Vaccaro, C., Poggi, R., Mazzariol, S., Huggenberger, S., et al. (2015). Precocious ossification of the tympanoperiotic bone in fetal and newborn dolphins: an evolutionary adaptation to the aquatic environment? Anat. Rec. (Hoboken) 298, 1294–1300. doi: 10.1002/ar.23120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Bigio, M. R. (2011). Cell proliferation in human ganglionic eminence and suppression after prematurity-associated haemorrhage. Brain 134, 1344–1361. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demski, L. S., Ridgway, S. H., and Schwanzel-Fukuda, M. (1990). The terminal nerve of dolphins: gross structure, histology and luteinizing-hormone-releasing hormone immunocytochemistry. Brain Behav. Evol. 36, 249–261. doi: 10.1159/000115311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fields, R. D. (2008). White matter in learning, cognition and psychiatric disorders. Trends Neurosci. 31, 361–370. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furutani, R. (2008). Laminar and cytoarchitectonic features of the cerebral cortex in the Risso’s dolphin (Grampus griseus), striped dolphin (Stenella coeruleoalba), and bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). J. Anat. 213, 241–248. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2008.00936.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallyas, F. (1979). Silver staining of myelin by means of physical development. Neurol. Res. 1, 203–209.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Garey, L. J., and Leuba, G. (1986). A quantitative study of neuronal and glial numerical density in the visual cortex of the bottlenose dolphin: evidence for a specialized subarea and changes with age. J. Comp. Neurol. 247, 491–496. doi: 10.1002/cne.902470408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hof, P. R., Chanis, R., and Marino, L. (2005). Cortical complexity in cetacean brains. Anat. Rec. A Discov. Mol. Cell. Evol. Biol. 287, 1142–1152. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.20258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, M. S., McFarland, W. L., and Morgane, P. J. (1979). The anatomy of the brain of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Rhinic lobe (Rhinencephalon): the archicortex. Brain Res. Bull. 4, 1–108.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jefferson, T. A., Webber, M. A., and Pitman, R. L. (2015). Marine Mammals of the World: A Comprehensive Guide to Their Identification, 2nd Edn. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 1–616.

Google Scholar

Kern, A., Siebert, U., Cozzi, B., Hof, P. R., and Oelschläger, H. H. A. (2011). Stereology of the neocortex in Odontocetes: qualitative, quantitative, and functional implications. Brain Behav. Evol. 77, 79–90. doi: 10.1159/000323674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesch, P. (1969). The development of the neutral lipids and fatty acids in the brain of man, whales and dolphins. Clin. Chim. Acta 25, 269–282. doi: 10.1016/0009-8981(69)90265-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liste, F., Palacio, J., Ribes, V., Alvarez-Clau, A., Domínguez, L. F., and Corpa, J. M. (2006). Anatomic and computed tomographic atlas of the head of the newborn bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Vet. Radiol. Ultrasound 47, 453–460. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-8261.2006.00167.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lois, C., and Alvarez-Buylla, A. (1994). Long-distance neuronal migration in the adult mammalian brain. Science 264, 1145–1148. doi: 10.1126/science.8178174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, L. (1997). The relationship between gestation length, encephalization, and body weight in odontocetes. Mar. Mamm. Sci. 13, 133–138. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.1997.tb00616.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, L., Sudheimer, K. D., Murphy, T. L., Davis, K. K., Pabst, D. A., McLellan, W. A., et al. (2001a). Anatomy and three-dimensional reconstructions of the brain of a Bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) from magnetic resonance images. Anat. Rec. 264, 397–414. doi: 10.1002/ar.10018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, L., Murphy, T. L., Gozal, L., and Johnson, J. I. (2001b). Magnetic resonance imaging and three-dimensional reconstructions of the brain of a fetal common dolphin, Delphinus delphis. Anat. Embryol. 203, 393–402. doi: 10.1007/s004290100167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, L., Sudheimer, K., Pabst, D. A., McLellan, W. A., Arshad, S., Naini, G., et al. (2004a). Anatomical description of an infant Bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) brain from magnetic resonance images. Aquat. Mamm. 30, 315–326. doi: 10.1578/AM.30.2.2004.315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, L., Sudheimer, K., McLellan, W. A., and Johnson, J. I. (2004b). Neuroanatomical structure of the spinner dolphin (Stenella longirostris orientalis) brain from magnetic resonance images. Anat. Rec. A Discov. Mol. Cell. Evol. Biol. 279, 601–610. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.20047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montie, E. W., Schneider, G. E., Ketten, D. R., Marino, L., Touhey, K. E., and Hahn, M. E. (2007). Neuroanatomy of the subadult and fetal brain of the atlantic white-sided Dolphin (Lagenorhynchus acutus) from in situ magnetic resonance images. Anat. Rec. (Hoboken) 290, 1459–1479. doi: 10.1002/ar.20612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montie, E. W., Schneider, G. E., Ketten, D. R., Marino, L., Touhey, K. E., and Hahn, M. E. (2008). Volumetric neuroimaging of the atlantic white-sided Dolphin (Lagenorhynchus acutus) brain from in situ magnetic resonance images. Anat. Rec. (Hoboken) 291, 263–282. doi: 10.1002/ar.20654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mortensen, H. S., Pakkenberg, B., Dam, M., Dietz, R., Sonne, C., Mikkelsen, B., et al. (2014). Quantitative relationship in delphinid neocortex. Front. Neuroanat. 8:132. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2014.00132

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oelschläger, H. A., Buhl, E. H., and Dann, J. F. (1987). Development of the nervus terminalis in mammals including toothed whales and humans. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 519, 447–464. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1987.tb36316.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oelschläger, H. H., Haas-Rioth, M., Fung, C., Ridgway, S. H., and Knauth, M. (2008). Morphology and evolutionary biology of the dolphin (Delphinus sp.) brain–MR imaging and conventional histology. Brain Behav. Evol. 71, 68–86. doi: 10.1159/000110495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oelschläger, H. H. A., and Oelschläger, J. S. (2009). “Brain,” in Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals, 2nd Edn, eds W. F. Perrin, B. Würsin, and J. G. M. Thewissen (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 134–149.

Google Scholar

O’Rahilly, R. (1972). Guide to staging of human embryos. Anat. Anz. 130, 556–559.

Google Scholar

Patzke, N., Spocter, M. A., Karlsson, K.Æ, Bertelsen, M. F., Haagensen, M., Chawana, R., et al. (2015). In contrast to many other mammals, cetaceans have relatively small hippocampi that appear to lack adult neurogenesis. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 361–383. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0660-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peretto, P., Giachino, C., Aimar, P., Fasolo, A., and Bonfanti, L. (2005). Chain formation and glial tube assembly in the shift from neonatal to adult subventricular zone of the rodent forebrain. J. Comp. Neurol. 487, 407–427. doi: 10.1002/cne.20576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peruffo, A., and Cozzi, B. (2014). Bovine brain: an in vitro translational model in developmental neuroscience and neurodegenerative research. Front. Pediatr. 2:74. doi: 10.3389/fped.2014.00074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pilleri, G., and Gihr, M. (1970). The central nervous system of the Mysticete and Odontocete whales. Invest. Cet. 2, 89–128.

Google Scholar

Pilleri, G., Peixun, C., and Zuohua, S. (1980). Concise Macroscopical Atlas of the Brain of the Common Dolphin (Delphinus delphis Linnaeus, 1758). Waldau-Berne: Brain Anatomy Institute, University of Berne.

Google Scholar

Pirlot, P., and Kamiya, T. (1975). Comparison of ontogenetic brain growth in marine and coastal dolphins. Growth 39, 507–524.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pirlot, P., and Kamiya, T. (1982). Embryonic brain-growth in a dolphin. Anat. Embryol. 164, 43–50. doi: 10.1007/BF00301877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ponti, G., Aimar, P., and Bonfanti, L. (2006a). Cellular composition and cytoarchitecture of the rabbit subventricular zone (SVZ) and its extensions in the forebrain. J. Comp. Neurol. 498, 491–507. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ponti, G., Peretto, P., and Bonfanti, L. (2006b). A subpial, transitory germinal zone forms chains of neuronal precursors in the rabbit cerebellum. Dev. Biol. 294, 168–180. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2006.02.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ponti, G., Crociara, P., Armentano, M., and Bonfanti, L. (2010). Adult neurogenesis without germinal layers: the “atypical” cerebellum of rabbits. Arch. Ital. Biol. 148, 147–158.

Google Scholar

Ponti, G., Peretto, P., and Bonfanti, L. (2008). Genesis of neuronal and glial progenitors in the cerebellar cortex of peripuberal and adult rabbits. PLoS ONE 3:e2366. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauschmann, M. A., Huggenberger, S., Kossatz, L. S., and Oelschlager, H. H. A. (2006). Head morphology in perinatal dolphins: a window into phylogeny and ontogeny. J. Morphol. 267, 1295–1315. doi: 10.1002/jmor.10477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridgway, S. H. (1986). “Dolphin brain size,” in Research on Dolphins, eds M. M. Bryden and R. Harrison (Oxford: Oxford Science Publications), 59–70.

Google Scholar

Ridgway, S. H. (1990). “The central nervous system of the bottlenose dolphin,” in The Bottlenose Dolphin, eds S. Leatherwood and R. R. Reeves (Waltham, MA: Academic Press), 69–97.

Google Scholar

Ridgway, S. H., and Brownson, R. H. (1984). Relative brain sizes and cortical surface areas in odontocetes. Acta Zool. Fenn. 172, 149–152.

Google Scholar

Sanai, N., Nguyen, T., Ihrie, R. A., Mirzadeh, Z., Tsai, H. H., Wong, M., et al. (2011). Corridors of migrating neurons in the human brain and their decline during infancy. Nature 478, 382–386. doi: 10.1038/nature10487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanchez-Villagra, M. R., and Sultan, F. (2002). The cerebellum at birth in therian mammals, with special reference to rodents. Brain Behav. Evol. 59, 101–113. doi: 10.1159/000064158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snaidero, N., and Simons, M. (2014). Myelination at a glance. J. Cell Sci. 127, 2999–3004. doi: 10.1242/jcs.151043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Štěrba, O., Klima, M., and Schildger, B. (2000). Embryology of dolphins – staging and ageing of embryos and fetuses of some cetaceans. Adv. Anat. Embryol. Cell Biol. 157, 1–133.

Sternberger, N. H., Itoyama, Y., Kies, M. W., and Webster, H. D. (1978). Myelin basic protein demonstrated immunocytochemically in oligodendroglia prior to myelin sheath formation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 75, 2521–2524. doi: 10.1073/pnas.75.5.2521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

The Welker collection at the University of Wisconsin Madison (n.d.). Available at: http://brainmuseum.org/Specimens/cetacea/dolphin/sections/coronal-cell/thumbnail.html

Thewissen, J. G. M., and Heyning, J. (2007). “Embryogenesis and development in Stenella attenuata and other cetaceans,” in Reproductive Biology and Phylogeny of Cetacea. Whales, Dolphins and Porpoises, ed. D. L. Miller (Enfield, NH: Science Publishers), 307–330.

Google Scholar

The Yakovlev-Haleem collection at the National Museum of Health and Medicine (n.d.). Available at: https://www.msu.edu/$\sim$brains/brains/dolphin/intro.html

Tramontin, A. D., Garcìa-Verdugo, J. M., Lim, D. A., and Alvarez-Buylla, A. (2003). Postnatal development of radial glia and the ventricular zone (VZ): a continuum of the neural stem cell compartment. Cereb. Cortex 13, 580–587. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.6.580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vincze, A., Mazlo, M., Seress, L., Komoly, S., and Abraham, H. (2008). A correlative light and electron microscopic study of postnatal myelination in the murine corpus callosum. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 26, 575–584. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2008.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C., Liu, F., Liu, Y. Y., Zhao, C. H., You, Y., Wang, L., et al. (2011). Identification and characterization of neuroblasts in the subventricular zone and rostral migratory stream of the adult human brain. Cell Res. 21, 1534–1550. doi: 10.1038/cr.2011.83

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Workman, A. D., Charvet, C. J., Clancy, B., Darlington, R. B., and Finlay, B. L. (2013). Modeling transformations of neurodevelopmental sequences across mammalian species. J. Neurosci. 33, 7368–7383. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5746-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: bottlenose dolphin, striped dolphin, postnatal brain development, striatum, thalamus, cerebellum, lateral ventricle, germinative layers

Citation: Parolisi R, Peruffo A, Messina S, Panin M, Montelli S, Giurisato M, Cozzi B and Bonfanti L (2015) Forebrain neuroanatomy of the neonatal and juvenile dolphin (T. truncatus and S. coeruloalba). Front. Neuroanat. 9:140. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2015.00140

Received: 27 July 2015; Accepted: 16 October 2015;
Published: 06 November 2015.

Edited by:

James A. Bourne, Australian Regenerative Medicine Institute, Australia

Reviewed by:

Lutgarde Arckens, KU Leuven, Belgium
Mary Baldwin, University of California, Davis, USA

Copyright © 2015 Parolisi, Peruffo, Messina, Panin, Montelli, Giurisato, Cozzi and Bonfanti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Luca Bonfanti, luca.bonfanti@unito.it; Bruno Cozzi, bruno.cozzi@unipd.it

Download