Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neuroanat., 12 December 2017
Volume 11 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2017.00122

Anatomical Analysis of the Retinal Specializations to a Crypto-Benthic, Micro-Predatory Lifestyle in the Mediterranean Triplefin Blenny Tripterygion delaisi

  • 1Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Department of Biology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 2The Oceans Institute, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
  • 3School of Biological Sciences, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia

The environment and lifestyle of a species are known to exert selective pressure on the visual system, often demonstrating a tight link between visual morphology and ecology. Many studies have predicted the visual requirements of a species by examining the anatomical features of the eye. However, among the vast number of studies on visual specializations in aquatic animals, only a few have focused on small benthic fishes that occupy a heterogeneous and spatially complex visual environment. This study investigates the general retinal anatomy including the topography of both the photoreceptor and ganglion cell populations and estimates the spatial resolving power (SRP) of the eye of the Mediterranean triplefin Tripterygion delaisi. Retinal wholemounts were prepared to systematically and quantitatively analyze photoreceptor and retinal ganglion cell (RGC) densities using design-based stereology. To further examine the retinal structure, we also used magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and histological examination of retinal cross sections. Observations of the triplefin’s eyes revealed them to be highly mobile, allowing them to view the surroundings without body movements. A rostral aphakic gap and the elliptical shape of the eye extend its visual field rostrally and allow for a rostro-caudal accommodatory axis, enabling this species to focus on prey at close range. Single and twin cones dominate the retina and are consistently arranged in one of two regular patterns, which may enhance motion detection and color vision. The retina features a prominent, dorso-temporal, convexiclivate fovea with an average density of 104,400 double and 30,800 single cones per mm2, and 81,000 RGCs per mm2. Based on photoreceptor spacing, SRP was calculated to be between 6.7 and 9.0 cycles per degree. Location and resolving power of the fovea would benefit the detection and identification of small prey in the lower frontal region of the visual field.

Introduction

Vertebrate eyes typically show remarkable specializations to view their surroundings with their ocular anatomy providing a strong indication of each species’ ecology and evolutionary history. While all vertebrate eyes share the same general organization, many species have evolved individual specializations that enhance their visual perception of the environment. These visual specializations often reflect ecological niches, which is particularly true for fishes as they occupy a diverse range of microenvironments. As the largest group of vertebrates, they have evolved diverse lifestyles in a wide range of habitats and these are often reflected in a variety of visual adaptations. The resultant link between a species’ visual ecology and eye anatomy allows one to make meaningful predictions from one about the other (Hughes, 1977). Such investigations are especially informative when little is known about the ecology and behavior of a species, for instance when behavioral observations are difficult.

Adaptive modifications of the basic eye structure to match specific visual demands can be found at all structural levels, e.g., the tubular eyes of some deep sea teleosts (Collin et al., 1997; Warrant and Locket, 2004; Biagioni et al., 2016), the bipartite pupil and asymmetrical lens of the four-eyed fish (Nicol and Somiya, 1989), or multifocal lenses (Kroger et al., 1999). However, the ultimate level of visual perception is the retina, where a range of specializations have been described including variations in photoreceptor number, density, arrangement, type and spectral sensitivity (Collin and Shand, 2003), and the presence and location of a number of types of ocular reflectors (Fritsch et al., 2017). Depending on visual demands, specializations often involve a trade-off between sensitivity and resolution. For instance, high summation rates, grouped receptors, and large receptor diameters can all serve to increase sensitivity, but inevitably lower spatial resolving power (SRP) (Land and Nilsson, 2012). As a compromise, many species possess distinct retinal areas of high receptor density to mediate higher spatial resolution. Different types and combinations of such areas, varying in shape, size, and retinal position, have been described for many fish species (Collin, 2008).

Which type of specialization occurs in a species has been proposed to correlate with their behavioral ecology and visual environment (Hughes, 1977). A horizontal streak of high receptor density is generally found in species whose environments feature a prominent horizon. This requires a fixed (and often wide) region of high visual acuity across the retina, which enables the eye to scan a large panoramic visual field for predators without the need for pronounced eye movements (Collin and Collin, 1999). In contrast, species that search for cryptic prey in highly structured, heterogeneous environments would be expected to feature a pronounced area centralis, a concentric region of increased cell density (Collin and Shand, 2003). In many species with particularly high spatial resolution, the neuronal elements other than the photoreceptors are partially or completely displaced from the center of the area centralis. This creates an indentation in the retinal structure, i.e., a fovea, which is typical for birds (Tucker, 2000; Coimbra et al., 2006, 2009, 2014) and primates (Williams and Coletta, 1987; Wassle et al., 1990; Wikler and Rakic, 1990), but has also been found in many fish species (Collin and Collin, 1988c, 1999; Collin et al., 2000; Collin and Shand, 2003). Blennioid species, which usually live in complex 3D environments, have also been known to possess foveae for a long time (Kahmann, 1936). These early studies, however, constituted only macroscopic and qualitative assessments. Since then, only few studies of retinal morphology, visual specializations, and SRP have been carried out in small, crypto-benthic fish species. Examples include the blenniid Petroscirtes variabilis (referred to as Dasson variabilis in the original publication), the tripterygiid Forsterygion varium, the sandlance Limnichthyes fasciatus, and several syngnathid species (Collin and Collin, 1988c, 1999; Collin and Pettigrew, 1989; Pankhurst et al., 1993; Lee and O’Brien, 2011). Even these more recent studies, however, often lack a systematic analysis of the retinal topography, or only consider retinal ganglion cells (RGCs).

Here, we present a comprehensive study that includes the ocular anatomy, the quantitative, topographic distribution of both photoreceptor cells and RGCs, and an estimate of the SRP of the eyes of the Mediterranean subspecies of the yellow black-faced triplefin blenny Tripterygion delaisi (Tripterygiidae). T. delaisi reaches a length of 3–6 cm (SL), occurs on algae-encrusted, rocky substrates at depths of 3–40 m in the Mediterranean and Northeast Atlantic, and feeds as an opportunistic micro-predator on a variety of small (1–3 mm) invertebrates, especially crustaceans (Wirtz, 1978; Zander, 1982; Zander et al., 1986). Both sexes feature the same mottled, white and brown coloration with darker stripes and bright spots (Wirtz, 1978; Figure 1A). This matches their benthic habitat characterized by rocks overgrown with diverse species of algae both in shaded and exposed sites (La Mesa et al., 2005). T. delaisi’s behavior contributes to its camouflage, as triplefins spend much of their time perching motionlessly on the substrate, while scanning the environment for potential threats and prey with frequent and extensive eye movements (unpublished observations). Occasionally, T. delaisi changes its position with short, impulsive darts, just to freeze again in the new location. Exceptions to its generally cryptic coloration are the males’ black-and-yellow breeding coloration (Wirtz, 1978), and the red-fluorescing irides (Figure 1B) of both sexes whose brightness can be physiologically regulated via melanophores (Wucherer and Michiels, 2014). The general intensity of the fluorescence increases with the depth at which an individual lives (Meadows et al., 2014) in response to the reduced overall light intensity at greater depth (Harant et al., 2016). Furthermore, T. delaisi’s set of cone receptors is sensitive to the long-wavelength light produced by its fluorescent pigments and the intensity of the fluorescence is sufficient to create perceivable contrasts (Kalb et al., 2015; Bitton et al., 2017). The function of a fluorescent iris and other unusual external features of the eye of T. delaisi and their effect on vision are still unclear. Their potential role is discussed and investigated in detail elsewhere (Fritsch et al., 2017; Harant and Michiels, 2017).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Tripterygion delaisi habitus and eye preparation. (A) T. delaisi in its natural environment. (B) Close-up of the eye showing the slightly elliptical dimensions of eyeball and pupil. (C) After removing cornea, iris, and lens, the remaining eyecup was fixed until the retina took on the translucent appearance seen here. The arrowhead marks the macroscopically visible, slightly elongated foveal pit situated in the temporal retina. (D) Back of the isolated retina with retinal pigment epithelium (RPE) still attached. (E) Retina after bleaching and flattening, seen from the vitread side, ready to be mounted on a slide. Scale bars equal 1 cm in (A), and 1 mm in (B–E); arrows indicate dorsal, D; rostral, R; and temporal, T, directions.

The intricate patterns and changes in coloration, the unusual visual features of its eyes, as well as its frequent eye movements to scan its environment, suggest a rich visual ecology and complex visual system. In this study, we use retinal wholemounts combined with design-based stereology, retinal cross sections, and MRI to characterize ocular and retinal specializations for T. delaisi’s particular micro-predatory, cryptic lifestyle.

Materials and Methods

Specimens

We used individuals of T. delaisi (n = 10) to either prepare retinal wholemounts (4), paraffin-embedded thick histological sections (4), or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans (2). All specimens were adults of indeterminate sex, since this species is sexually monomorphic outside the breeding season. Individuals used for retinal wholemounts were wild-caught at STARESO research station (Pointe Revellata, BP33 20260 Calvi, Corsica, France) in July 2014. All others specimens were wild-caught at the Centro Marino Elba research station (Loc. Fetovaia 72, I-57034 Campo nell’Elba, Italy) in June 2013. All animals were euthanized according to German animal ethics legislation under notifications AZ. 13.06.2013 and AZ. 29.10.2014 issued to NKM by the animal welfare department of the district administration of Tübingen (“Regierungspräsidium Tübingen”). Specifically, the fish were immersed in seawater containing a lethal dose of 500–1000 mg/l tricaine methanesulfonate (MS-222), adjusted to pH = 8.2 with NaOH, until there was no discernable opercular movement for at least 1 min. Cutting the spinal cord with a scalpel ensured euthanasia. We then measured the standard length (SL) of each individual. The eyes were excised from the skull and the horizontal and vertical diameters of the eye and pupil of each fish were measured using digital calipers (to the nearest 0.1 mm).

Eye Mobility

Tripterygion delaisi’s pronounced eye movements have already been mentioned in the most comprehensive study on the species’ behavior (Wirtz, 1978) but no study ever quantified that mobility. We extracted this information from video footage from previous, unpublished work. In these videos T. delaisi is seen from above while subjected to an optokinetic reflex experiment, demonstrating its horizontal eye mobility. We scanned the videos of three individuals for the frames with the maximal deflection of the eyes in relation to the midline of the head, exported the frames as still images, and measured the maximal angular change in ImageJ v.1.48t. We are aware that this approach only yields an approximate and incomplete estimate of eye mobility, but these data are nevertheless a meaningful addition to our main results. The implementation of a more exhaustive assessment of eye movements goes beyond our scope to describe this species’ retinal anatomy.

Histological Serial Sections and Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI)

The four T. delaisi prepared for paraffin-embedded thick sections and the two individuals used for MRI are the same as in a previous study (Fritsch et al., 2017), which provides a comprehensive description of the procedures. Briefly, the four dissected heads used for histological sections were immersion fixed with 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.155 M Sorensen‘s phosphate buffer, two heads were left whole, while two were split medially, then the resulting six samples were decalcified, dehydrated and embedded in paraffin blocks. Blocks were sectioned at 10 μm thickness. The whole-head samples were sectioned in the transverse plane, while one half of the split-head samples was sectioned coronally, and the other sagittally. The sections were collected on standard microscope slides, stained with phosphotungstic acid haematoxylin (Mallory, 1900), and then mounted with Entellan®. Sections from the region of interest were digitized with a Leica DM5000 B microscope and DFC320 camera (Leica Microsystems, 35578 Wetzlar, Germany), and measurements were acquired in ImageJ 1.48t (Fiji distribution package).

For the MRI scans performed on two specimens, the fish were euthanized as above, and immersion fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffered saline (PBS), which also contained 0.25% of a 1 M contrasting agent (Gadovist®). Scans were run on a 16.4 T UltrashieldTM Plus 700 WB Avance nuclear magnetic resonance spectrometer with ParaVision® 5.1 software (Bruker BioSpin GmbH, 76287 Rheinstetten, Germany). The digital 3D segmentations of the MRI data were created in ITK-SNAP, version 2.4.0 (Yushkevich et al., 2006).

Retinal Wholemount Preparation

Immediately after euthanasia, the four specimens used for retinal wholemount preparation were transferred to modified Ringer’s solution (MRS) optimized for marine teleosts, which contained 125.3 mmol NaCl, 2.7 mmol KCl, 1.8 mmol CaCl2, 1.8 mmol MgCl2, 5.6 mmol D(+)-Glucose, and 5.0 mmol Tris-HCl per liter, buffered to pH = 7.2 (Wucherer and Michiels, 2012). Dissection of the eyes and preparation of the retinal wholemounts are only summarized here, as we largely followed procedures comprehensively described in Coimbra et al. (2006) and Ullmann et al. (2012). We excised each eye from the skull, transferred it to MRS, and removed the dermal cornea, scleral cornea, lens, iris, and vitreous body. Lens diameter was measured to the nearest 0.01 mm with digital calipers. The remaining eyecup was then fixed with 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M, pH = 7.3, Sorensen’s phosphate buffer (SPB) for 20–30 min at room temperature just until the previously clear retina started to become translucent (Figure 1C). Excess fixative was subsequently removed by washing the eyecup in the buffer three times for 1 min each. In all following steps, the tissue was constantly submerged in or moisturized with SPB. We then applied four to five radial cuts to the eyecup that enabled us to consecutively separate the other tissue layers [choroid and retinal pigment epithelium (RPE)] from the retina, allowing the isolated retina to be flattened onto a non-subbed glass slide (Figure 1D). Since we wanted to analyze both photoreceptor and RGC populations in the same retinae, and the RGC preparation involves irreversible procedures, the preparation, viewing and analysis of the topography of the photoreceptors was carried out first, as follows.

To obtain a clear view of the photoreceptor layer without damaging it, only loose RPE was removed mechanically, while any remaining pigment was bleached by immersing the retina in a mixture of 5 parts double-concentration PBS to 4 parts 30% hydrogen peroxide to 1 part distilled water, with a final addition of one drop of 25% ammonia per 10 ml of combined volume (Hemmi and Grunert, 1999; Coimbra et al., 2015a). Bleaching was carried out at room temperature and in the dark until the remaining RPE had turned light amber to pale hazel in color (Figure 1E), which usually took eight or more hours and could be carried out overnight. This slow bleaching method carried out at approximately neutral pH preserves the Nissl substance for the later staining of the RGCs. Thereafter, the retina was washed in 0.1 M SPB three times, 1 min each, to remove any residual bleach, then gently cleaned with soft, fine brushes, transferred onto a standard microscope slide, and mounted in a solution of 80% glycerol and 20% SPB with 0.1% sodium azide added (Coimbra et al., 2015b). Two small strips of filter paper placed next to the retina served as spacers between wholemount and the coverslip. Finally, the edges were sealed with nail varnish to prevent evaporation. It generally took several days to clear the tissue in the glycerol (Coimbra et al., 2015a,b).

Subsequent to the photoreceptor analysis, the retinal wholemounts were remounted and prepared for RGC analysis. The varnish seal around the coverslip was cut away and the retina carefully transferred to and washed in 0.1 M SPB three times, 1 min each. Then, the retina was flipped over such that the RGC layer faced upward, transferred to a Superfrost® Plus adhesive microscope slide, and gently flattened without distending it. The slide was then placed overnight in a glass staining dish containing a few milliliters of 40% formaldehyde solution to create an atmosphere saturated with formaldehyde vapor (Stone, 1981; Coimbra et al., 2006). This procedure ensured the retina was well fixed and adhered to the slide. Subsequently, the retinal wholemount was stained for Nissl substances according to the protocol described in (Coimbra et al., 2006). Finally, the retinae were mounted in Eukitt® mounting medium and were allowed to dry for at least 24 h before analysis. This entire process can lead to shrinkage and distortions, especially when the retinal wholemounts (partially) detach from the microscope slide during the staining process. The retinae analyzed in this study did not detach during processing and shrinkage was very limited and corrected for in the following analysis.

Stereological Analysis and Data Processing

The retinal wholemounts were analyzed using a Zeiss AxioPlan II fluorescence microscope with a motorized specimen stage and a stereology system running Stereo Investigator 7 software from MBF bioscience. We used the optical fractionator method (West et al., 1991) with modifications for retinal wholemounts (Coimbra et al., 2009, 2012). The first stereological analysis was carried out with the glycerol-mounted, unstained wholemounts and focused on assessing the distribution of different retinal cone types within the photoreceptor layer. The receptor cells were visualized using a 40× magnification objective with differential interference contrast and focusing at the level of the cone inner segments. Individual rods could not be distinguished reliably throughout the retina due to their small size, the limited magnification available and tissue quality. However, all cone types could be readily distinguished by the size and shape of their inner segments. To obtain representative, systematic estimates of the density of each cone type and the total population of cone photoreceptors, we traced the coordinates of the entire retina’s outline and an additional outline that included the foveal and triple cone area. Both of these special retinal regions could be recognized at low magnification and we outlined them with a margin of 200–300 μm. To achieve sufficient areal coverage and representative sampling, we had the program assign 200 counting sites of 50 μm × 50 μm to the entire retina, which resulted in a grid width of 250–300 μm between loci, depending on the size of the retina. The foveal area was assigned a higher resolution grid with 100–150 μm between sites and counting frames of 25 μm × 25 μm to compensate for the marked changes in receptor density over small distances.

We repeated the same procedure for the RGC population within the RGC layer, using bright field illumination. We counted all cells with relatively large, slightly polygonal, positively Nissl-stained somata as RGCs, and only excluded cells with somata that were either distinctly small and round (amacrine cells) or cigar-shaped (glia). This approach has been found to provide an accurate representation of the ganglion cell population within the ganglion cell layer. However, despite the fact that in specialized regions, the high densities of neurons makes differentiating amacrine cells and ganglion cells difficult, the proportions of amacrine cells in these regions were found to be low and inclusion of amacrine cells in the counts does not affect the general topographic pattern (Collin and Pettigrew, 1988a).

The Stereo Investigator software automatically produces a summary of the collected counting data, including area sampling fraction, total population estimates, and coefficients of error (CE). We chose Scheaffer’s CE to assess the accuracy of the obtained estimates, as it most closely approximates the true CE in a stereological sampling context (Glaser and Wilson, 1998). In biological contexts, sampling schemes that result in a CE < 0.1 are deemed sufficiently representative and accurate (Fileta et al., 2008). For further analysis, the raw data were exported from Stereo Investigator to JMP® v.11.1.1 (SAS Institute Inc.). Some counting sites (5–10 per retina) were randomly allocated to locally compromised parts of the wholemounts. This mostly affected single cone counts, while the larger and characteristically shaped double cones usually were still discernible. In such cases, the cone ratio of neighboring sites and the double cone counts from the ambiguous site were used to correct the corresponding single cone numbers. If neither cone type was discernible, the entire site was omitted from counting (1–3 per retina). To correct the RGC density values for shrinkage due to Nissl staining, we divided the retinal wholemounts digitally into distinct regions, measured their area before and after staining in ImageJ v.1.48t, and calculated the individual shrinkage factor for each region. The raw RGC densities from each counting site were then multiplied with their respective region’s shrinkage factor. The corrected data were converted into topographic maps by plotting the densities of the individual cone types and RGCs against the X- and Y-coordinates of the counting sites, using JMP’s contour plot tool. These contour plots were then exported to Microsoft® PowerPoint® (v.14.2.3 for macs) and further edited, e.g., re-matched with their respective retinal wholemount’s original outline. The approximate angular position of the fovea was estimated from the finished topographic maps in ImageJ v.1.48t, based on its relative horizontal and vertical distance from the center of the retina, and assuming the horizontal and vertical diameter of the retina correspond to 180°.

Calculating Spatial Resolving Power

We based our estimate of T. delaisi’s anatomical SRP, expressed as Nyquist frequency (fN) in cycles per degree (cpd), on the eyes’ posterior nodal distance (PND, in mm) and foveal photoreceptor cell density (D, in mm-2), according to the following equation, modified from Williams (1988):

fN=π*PND360°*2*D3                 (1)

Due to the lateral displacement of RGCs in foveae (Wassle et al., 1990), we assumed the photoreceptor cell density to be the most reliable estimator for the anatomical SRP. Our reasoning will be explained in more detail in the results section. To reflect the potential of this species, we used the highest individual density value. A previous study on paired cones suggested that each cone member could produce an individual signal in the context of color vision (Pignatelli et al., 2010). If this is confirmed, individual signals might also be used for greater resolution. We therefore calculated both a conservative estimate for the anatomical SRP, assuming each twin cone to signal as a unit, and a maximal estimate, assuming each twin cone member signals individually, to show the highest possible SRP for this species. The linear density is based on a hexagonal cell arrangement in the fovea, which accounts for the 2/www.frontiersin.org factor in Equation 1.

Results

General Eye Features

Tripterygion delaisi possesses relatively large and conspicuous eyes for its small body size (Figure 1A). While standard length averaged 42.7 ± 2.8 mm (mean ± SD, N = 10), the eyes had diameters of 3.48 ± 0.15 mm antero-posteriorly and 3.00 ± 0.15 mm dorso-ventrally, corresponding to 8.2 ± 0.5% and 7.0 ± 0.4% of the standard length. Eyecup and retina are therefore hemi-elliptical with the dorso-ventral axis measuring 86.3 ± 3.8% of the length of the antero-posterior axis. This causes the distance between the spherical lens and different regions of the retina to vary and has implications for image focusing that are discussed later. The pupil of T. delaisi also features an elliptical shape (Figure 1B), with the antero-posterior axis measuring 1.47 ± 0.09 mm and the dorso-ventral axis measuring 1.24 ± 0.08 mm. This creates an antero-ventral aphakic gap when the lens is in its relaxed position, i.e., its focus set to infinity. The lens diameter measured 1.40 ± 0.08 mm (N = 4) in the eyes dissected for the retinal wholemounts, closely matching overall average pupil size. A well-developed stratum argenteum lines the back of the eye as part of the choroid, between the RPE and the sclera. Apart from containing the defining silvery, and highly reflective stacks of guanine crystals, the stratum argenteum is also associated with yellow chromatophores that give it a golden sheen. The argenteum was found to be continuous with a layer of iridophores in the iris that contained similar stacks of crystals but lacked the yellow pigment granules.

Tripterygion delaisi’s retina covers an almost hemispherical visual field. The horizontal viewing angle approximately spans 160°, as estimated from horizontal eye sections. In addition, T. delaisi is able to rotate its eyes by 60–70° in the horizontal plane. Consequently, the triplefin can observe its entire surrounding by eye movement alone and has areas of binocular overlap in frontal and caudal direction (Figure 2). Due to our limited data on the subject, the extent of T. delaisi’s visual field and eye movements shown here may even be an underestimate and has to be validated in future studies.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Horizontal visual field in T. delaisi. Schematic top view of T. delaisi’s head showing the extreme frontal and temporal orientation of its eyes (solid and dashed lines) and the associated foveal positions (). The shaded areas indicate the resulting maximum total (medium gray), binocular (dark gray), and foveate (light gray) horizontal fields of view based on the extent of eye movement.

Photoreceptors

We found four morphologically distinguishable photoreceptor types: rods, single cones, paired cones, or more specifically twin cones due to their morphologically equal members, and triangularly arranged triple cones with morphologically identical members. They occurred in three distinct mosaic patterns (Figures 3A–C). Rods outnumbered cones in the periphery (Figure 3A) but were neither detected in the vicinity of the fovea (Figure 3B) nor in the triple cone area (Figure 3C). They were not explicitly counted because their small diameter did not allow for reliable identification within our sampling framework. Single and twin cone densities were generally higher in the dorsal and temporal retina than in the ventral and nasal retina. The continuous increase in density from the periphery to the dorso-temporal retina culminated in a pronounced area centralis with a distinct fovea (illustrated in Figures 4A–D, 5A–D; detailed data in Table 1). Single cone density averaged 10,200 cells per mm2 and peaked at 30,800 cells per mm2 (typical distribution shown in Figure 4A). Twin cones averaged 23,300 cells per mm2 and peaked at 104,400 cells per mm2 (typical distribution shown in Figure 4B). With a total retinal surface area of 16.09 ± 0.18 mm2 (Table 1), these densities resulted in average total population estimates of 165,700 single cones and 369,700 twin cones for the entire retina. Triple cones were only found in a limited retinal region between the optic disk and the fovea (Figure 4C) at an average density of 16,700 ± 2,500 cells per mm2 (Table 1). At their peak density of 30,400 ± 5,700 cells per mm2, they outnumbered both single and twin cones in the same area. Due to their restricted distribution, the total number of triple cones was estimated at only 5,350 ± 1,100 (Table 1). The sum of all cone types showed a continuous distribution and consistent increase in overall density toward the fovea (Figure 4D). More stereological data and Scheaffer’s coefficient of error (CE) for each cone type are listed in Table 1. The relatively high CE for the triple cone estimates can be attributed to their restricted distribution and correspondingly fewer counting squares.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Cone types and mosaic patterns in the retina of T. delaisi. (A) Square mosaic pattern with a 2:1 ratio of twin to single cones, as found across most of the retina. Rods fill in the gaps between cones. (B) Dense, “lucky-clover” pattern with 4:1 ratio of twin cones to single cones and without rods, as found around the fovea. The fovea itself features a modified version of this pattern (Figure 5A). (C) A distinct area between optic disk and fovea features triple cones and a random arrangement of all cone types. Markers in the photomicrographs indicate: open arrowheads – rods; solid single arrowheads – single cones; solid double arrowheads – twin cones; open circles – triple cones.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Retinal topography of single cones (A), twin cones (B), triple cones (C), and the summed density of all cones (D) in a representative individual. The maps show the left retina of one T. delaisi individual. The central hole and the gap extending nasoventrally are from the optic nerve head () and falciform process (>). Black dots in the map correspond to counting sites. Arrows indicate dorsal, D; and temporal, T, direction.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Foveal features in T. delaisi. (A) Top view of the cone mosaic pattern in the fovea. The inset demonstrates that it is a skewed version of the 4:1 pattern shown in Figure 3B. (B) Coronal section of paraffin-embedded retina showing a steep foveal pit and locally thickened retina. The gaps between the outer plexiform and inner nuclear layers, as well as the RPE and choroid, are artifacts of the tissue processing. (C) Virtual segmentation of an in situ, right-eye retina from MRI data. The foveal area forms an almost hemispherical bulge in the retina, confirming the histological findings. The pronounced semi-ellipsoid shape of the retina matches that of the external eye. (D) Magnified view of the inset in (B). Note the densely packed receptor inner and outer segments with a diameter of about 2 μm each. cho, choroid; ffp, falciform process; fop, foveal pit; gcl, ganglion cell layer; inl, inner nuclear layer; ipl, inner plexiform layer; nfl, nerve fiber layer; od, optic disk; onl, outer nuclear layer; opl, outer plexiform layer; ris, receptor inner segments; ros, receptor outer segments; rpe, retinal pigment epithelium; vit, vitreous body.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Summary of stereological parameters and data, presented as means ± SD based on N = 4 individuals.

In terms of areal coverage, the retina of T. delaisi was found to be cone-dominated throughout, with an estimated 75–80% of its area occupied by cones, even in the periphery (Figure 3A). Most regions of the retina featured a cone mosaic pattern with a ratio of two twin cones to one single cone. The twin cones were arranged in a regular grid-like pattern with the single cones in the corner positions of the squares [as defined by Lyall (1957); see also Figure 3A]. We refer to this as the 2:1 square mosaic. This cone pattern was interspersed with randomly arranged rods in the spaces between the cones. Near the fovea, the cones became slenderer and were arranged more densely, while rods gradually decreased in number. In the area immediately surrounding the fovea, rods were missing entirely. The prevailing pattern changed such that the single cones were now in the central position of the twin cone squares and none of the surrounding twin cones was shared with other single cones (Figure 3B). This resulted in a 4:1 ratio of twin to single cones and is thus referred to as the 4:1 square mosaic. The fovea featured a skewed version of the 4:1 mosaic, in which twin cones formed a diamond rather than a square pattern (described below, Figure 5A). The triple cone area featured all cone types in an irregular arrangement, and no detectable rods (Figure 3C).

Fovea

The fovea in T. delaisi was apparent even macroscopically during the dissection of the eyes and preparation of the retinal wholemounts and appears as a thickened area with a central dip in the temporal region of the retina. The three-dimensional shape of the fovea is distorted by flattening in retinal wholemounts. The histological sections (e.g., Figure 5B) revealed the thickened retinal layers in the foveal area, and the central dip. Possibly due to differential shrinkage, however, the retina separated from the choroid and sclera, probably compromising the foveal shape in the process. We consider the virtual segmentation images produced from MRI data the most reliable impression of the foveal shape, as these were obtained from whole eyes in situ, and they revealed the foveal area to form an almost hemispherical convexity (Figure 5C). The position of the fovea, as estimated from the retinal wholemounts and in relation to the center of the retina, was approximately 30° ± 5° (N = 4) dorsal and 50° ± 9° (N = 4) temporal. This foveal position, when projected onto the visual field, means that T. delaisi’s high-acuity vision, and thus visual focus, is directed about 40° sideways and 30° downward in relation to its body axis when the eye is in its resting position. High-amplitude eye movements, however, allow the fovea to be pointed toward a large part of the frontal visual field and to image any object therein with high resolution (Figure 2). Nevertheless, eye movement seems to create only approximately 40° of binocular overlap, which may not include the foveal visual field. The apparent dip in the thickened foveal area was confirmed to be the vitread opening of a deep foveal pit with steep, convexiclivate walls (Figure 5B). Some section series suggested that the foveal pit was actually formed like a trough between the fovea and the optic nerve head, possibly allowing the axons of foveal ganglion cells to run to the optic nerve head along the shortest possible path. This could not be ascertained in all sections. The highest individual receptor cell density that we found in any fovea was 160,000 cones per mm2, comprised of 132,000 twin cones per mm2 and 28,000 single cones per mm2. This density was achieved through a gradual, marked reduction in cone cell width from the periphery toward the fovea and the omission of rods in the foveal area (Figures 3A,B, 5A,D). The above density theoretically leaves an average area of 2.5 μm × 2.5 μm for each cone to occupy in the fovea, which was matched by the 2–3 μm diameters of the cone inner segments (Figures 5A,D).

Retinal Ganglion Cells

Demounting, flipping, Nissl-staining, and remounting the retinas were successfully performed without damaging the wholemounts, which allowed us to determine the RGC distribution in the same retinae for which we had already obtained the receptor cell densities (Figure 6A). The stained RGC nuclei could be distinguished from other cell nuclei in the same retinal layer by their characteristic, slightly polygonal shape (Figure 6B).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. Ganglion cell counts from Nissl-stained retinae. (A) Sample retinal wholemount after Nissl-staining. Note that this is the same left retina for which receptor cell densities were presented in Figure 4. Apart from limited shrinkage along the edges, its size and outline are well preserved. (B) The retinal ganglion cell layer at 630× magnification. Ganglion cells feature relatively large, slightly polygonal nuclei (white arrowheads). Smaller or elliptical nuclei (black arrowhead) were attributed to different cell types and not counted. (C) The density distribution of retinal ganglion cells matches the overall pattern found for the photoreceptors. Note that the highest densities are found in the perifovea, with a drop in density in the center of the fovea. Black dots correspond to counting sites. Arrows indicate dorsal, D, and temporal, T, direction, marks the optic nerve head, and > the falciform process.

The RGC showed an overall mean density of 32,000 ± 2,000 cells per mm2 (Table 1). Their distribution followed that of the photoreceptor cells in that it was generally higher in the dorsal than the ventral part of the retina and steadily increased toward the fovea (compare Figures 4D, 6C). Maximal RGC densities occurred in a ring surrounding the fovea and reached 81,000 ± 10,300 RGC per mm2 (Figure 6C). The center of the fovea featured high numbers of RGC as well, but their density decreased slightly compared to the perifovea, which is the result of the thickened retinal layers in this area and the shape of the foveal pit. Stereological parameters and coefficients of error can be found in Table 1.

Anatomical Spatial Resolving Power

The lateral displacement of RGCs in the foveal area makes it impossible to infer the actual connectivity directly from the apparent RGC density. The center of the primate fovea features few, if any, RGCs, and yet each foveal photoreceptor is connected to 3–4 displaced RGCs in the perifovea (Wassle et al., 1990). In T. delaisi, we found the peak RGC densities located in the perifovea and decreasing toward the center of fovea, which suggests a similar lateral displacement, albeit to a lesser extent. Additionally, the fovea is generally accepted to be a retinal specialization for high-resolution vision, and therefore it is reasonable to assume the circuitry in the fovea is adapted to allow maximal resolution. For these reasons, we assumed a connectivity ratio of at least 1:1 between foveal photoreceptors and RGCs in T. delaisi, and used its photoreceptor cell densities directly to calculate the anatomical SRP. In the more conservative approach we used the highest individual combined density of single and twin cones, counting the latter as one receptor, which yielded a density of 160,000 per mm2. For the maximal anatomical SRP estimate, the assumption that each twin cone member provides direct input (Pignatelli et al., 2010), lead to a maximal receptor cell density of 292,000 per mm2. We estimated the PND at 1.79 mm, based on T. delaisi’s lens radius of 0.70 ± 0.04 mm (N = 4) multiplied by 2.55, the average Matthiessen’s ratio for teleost eyes (Shand et al., 1999). The resulting anatomical SRP was 6.7 cpd under the conservative assumptions and 9.0 cpd under the maximal assumptions. These values translate to a minimal separable angle (MSA) of 0.15° and 0.11°, respectively. Such MSAs would allow T. delaisi to resolve a 1 mm sized object at a distance of 38–52 cm (see Figure 7 for an ecological example).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 7. Illustration of the minimal separable angle (MSA; α) in T. delaisi. With its estimated MSA of α = 0.11–0.15°, T. delaisi could resolve a 1 mm long copepod, a typical prey item, at a distance of 38–52 cm. The eye of the copepod could play a role in its detection or identification as prey (Harant and Michiels, 2017). Assuming a diameter of 30 μm in this example, it might be resolved at 1.2–1.6 cm. The required ability to focus at such short distances is facilitated by several features of T. delaisi’s eyes. Sizes, distances, and angles are not to scale.

Discussion

General Eye Anatomy

Like most small, benthic fishes, T. delaisi possesses relatively large, prominent eyes. The correlation between body size (measured as total length) and eye diameter in 61 teleosts reported by Caves et al. (2017), however, suggests that T. delaisi’s eyes are actually slightly smaller than expected for a fish of its size. Total length might not be an accurate predictor in species with long and slender bodies, though. Since T. delaisi’s eyes are almost touching medially, they have maximized eye size within the constraints of their head size, underlining the importance of vision to them. The eyes’ dorsal position in combination with T. delaisi’s behavior of rearing up in their resting position (Wirtz, 1978), creates a slightly raised viewing angle of their surroundings, which might help to avoid their view getting blocked by algae or other substratal structures.

Fish eyes that require a wide accommodative range can adapt in different ways. In most cases the eyes simply evolve to be longer along the axis of the accommodative lens movement, whose largest component is usually along the pupillary plane (Fernald, 1990). Accordingly, the reason for the large relative distance between lens and retina in sea horse eyes (Mosk, 2004) could be the animals’ need to bring micro-prey directly before their snouts into focus, since they are visual planktivores (Lee and O’Brien, 2011). The sandlance L. fasciatus also requires precise and wide-range accommodation, as it strikes after tiny planktonic organisms that drift by at different distances (Pettigrew et al., 2000). In contrast to sea horses, it evolved an optical system unique among teleosts, with an adjustable corneal lenticle and a flattened lens, which, in combination, have an exceptionally high accommodative power and range (Collin and Collin, 1988a,b; Pettigrew and Collin, 1995). In T. delaisi it is the longer naso-temporal axis of the eye that widens the range of accommodation and thereby improves their ability to focus close objects in the anterior visual field. This is crucial for identifying small and well-camouflaged prey in the complex surrounding substrate and enables precise strikes at short distances, which reduces T. delaisi’s body movements and thus helps maintaining their own camouflage. A consequence of the lens moving parallel to the pupil is the coupling of accommodative states of different retinal areas. When the temporal retina is focused on the foreground in the anterior field of view, the nasal part must be focused on the background of the posterior field of view, and vice versa, while the central part of the retina is restricted to a constant, intermediate accommodative state (Fernald, 1990). This could be an advantage, as it enables T. delaisi to simultaneously have a focused image of both prey directly in front, and potential predators in the distance behind.

Rostral aphakic spaces, like the one in T. delaisi, are common in diurnal fishes (Schmitz and Wainwright, 2011) and have been suggested to extend the anterior visual field and to allow greater binocular overlap (Walls, 1942), to increase retinal illumination (Munk, 1980; Warrant and Locket, 2004), or simply to provide the required room for the accommodative movement of the lens, which protrudes through the pupil in fishes (Sivak, 1978; Nicol and Somiya, 1989). Although T. delaisi inhabits micro-environments with reduced or fluctuating light levels within the photic zone (Wirtz, 1978), these are still within the range of photopic vision, making an increase of general retinal illumination unlikely to be advantageous during the day. The orientation of the aphakic gap in T. delaisi coincides with the direction of lens movement and the visual axis of highest acuity, as is commonly found in other fishes, too (Nicol and Somiya, 1989). This supports the function of allowing greater accommodative lens movements and means the retinal area with the highest spatial acuity also has the widest accommodative range.

Eye Mobility and Visual Field

The degree of independent eye movement varies considerably between fish species (Fritsches and Marshall, 2002). With the ability to move its eyes around all axes of rotation and largely independent of each other, T. delaisi falls between syngnathids, with somewhat independent eyes, and the sandlance, with completely independent eye movements. The lateral position and high mobility of T. delaisi’s eyes result in a horizontal visual field of about 220–230° per eye (Figure 2), which means they can monitor their entire surroundings by eye movements alone. This helps avoiding body movements and maintaining their camouflage. Elaborate and frequent eye movements have been recognized as a universal indicator of a localized zone of acute vision, in the retina of any species (Bartels, 1931; Kahmann, 1936), which also applies to T. delaisi. The dorso-temporal location of T. delaisi’s fovea puts the zone of highest acuity in the naso-ventral visual field, directed toward the substrate in front of the fish. This can be seen as an adaptation to the triplefin’s targeting benthic invertebrates, and is generally expected for predators of substratal, small prey (Nicol and Somiya, 1989). Based on the estimated extent of eye movements, 60–70° horizontally, foveate vision can cover a total of about 120–140° in the anterior visual field (Figure 2). The maximal binocular overlap, however, appears to be only about 40–50°, which is not enough to include foveate stereoscopic vision. T. delaisi’s independent eye movement and its habit to focus on prey with just one eye before striking sideways, suggest that binocular foveate vision may be irrelevant to them and further imply a monocular distance estimating mechanism. Similar findings were reported for pipefish, including a possible functional link between their steep foveal pit and monocular depth perception (Collin and Collin, 1999).

Photoreceptor Types and Mosaic

Tripterygion delaisi’s retina was found to be cone-dominated, containing single, twin, and triple cones, while the number of rods declined from the periphery toward the fovea. Such cone-dominated retinae are common for diurnal, coastal fish species (Bowmaker, 1990). The wavelengths of maximal absorption of T. delaisi’s different photoreceptor types are about 500 nm for rods, 470 nm for the single cones, and 516/530 nm for the two members of the twin cones (Bitton et al., 2017). Possessing two different visual pigments while being morphologically indistinguishable, T. delaisi’s paired cones can be categorized as non-identical twin cones (Nicol and Somiya, 1989). The maximal absorption values lie within the variation of visual pigments across species, but their range is narrower and shifted toward the green part of the visible spectrum, compared to the average sensitivities found in other species (Bowmaker, 1990; Losey et al., 2003). Individual double cones members have been shown to produce independent signals and thus enable trichromatic color vision in a species of triggerfish (Pignatelli et al., 2010). If this applied to T. delaisi as well, it might allow enhanced discrimination of green and yellow colors, which are predominant in the triplefin’s sublittoral environment of algae- and sponge-encrusted substrates. Assessing visual capabilities should not be reduced to the receptors’ absorption maxima alone, however, but the broad shape of the visual pigments’ absorption spectra, which extends T. delaisi’s color perception well into the red part of the visible spectrum (Bowmaker, 1990; Kalb et al., 2015; Bitton et al., 2017). The triple cone related findings of this study are discussed in a separate section below.

Most teleosts have duplex retinae that contain both rods and cones, responsible for scotopic and photopic vision, respectively (Nicol and Somiya, 1989). The decrease of rod density and cone size from the peripheral retina to the fovea in T. delaisi can be seen as a compromise between adaptation for sensitivity in the periphery and for acuity in the foveal area. The photoreceptors of most teleosts are arranged in regular patterns, whose many types vary with phylogeny, ontogeny, and location across the retina (Lyall, 1957; Engstrom, 1963; Wagner, 1972). The framework of this so-called mosaic is formed by the cones, while the rods, if present, are usually randomly arranged and inserted between the cones. The dominant cone mosaic found throughout most of T. delaisi’s retina is a square mosaic with each single cone being surrounded by four twin cones, which it shares with the neighboring single cones, thus resulting in a 2:1 ratio between twin and single cones (Figures 3A, 8). The mosaic is unusual in that all single cones throughout the retina are in the corner position (“additional cones”) and never in the central position, as defined by Lyall (1957) based on the orientation of the paired cones. This pattern variant has only been previously observed in Nannostomus eques and Blennius vulgaris (Wagner, 1972). To our knowledge, the lucky-clover-like pattern with each single cone surrounded by its own four twin cones (Figure 3B) within the area centralis has not been described for any other species. The role of a regular cone mosaic is still uncertain and has never been tested experimentally, but several functions have been suggested based on correlations between pattern types and the ecology of multiple species (Wagner, 1990). We discuss three non-exclusive functions that could apply to T. delaisi and its ecology. Since testing these functions is beyond the scope of this study, they remain speculative for the time being, but we hope they will be investigated in future studies.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 8. Generalized retinal map of T. delaisi. The illustration summarizes the relative position and covered area of cone density, mosaic patterns, and other features found in the retinae of four T. delaisi. D, dorsal; T, temporal; RGC, retinal ganglion cell.

Geometrically analyzing different possible patterns and especially paired cone shapes suggested that the most common mosaic patterns might serve maximizing receptor packing, which would increase photon catch and sensitivity at a given density (van der Meer, 1992). In T. delaisi, this seems to apply to the tightly packed 4:1 square mosaic surrounding the fovea. The obvious, large gaps between the cones in the 2:1 square mosaic are filled with rods, such that the areal coverage is also maximized. In the center of the fovea, however, maximal packing is not realized, as there seem to be actual gaps between the cone inner segments. These might result from the changed twin cone shape, i.e., the twin members are rounder and relatively further apart, combined with maintaining the same regular 4:1 arrangement. This might indicate that the preservation of the mosaic is either a physiological constraint, or that mosaic regularity is more important than achieving maximum density in T. delaisi.

Another suggested function of cone mosaics is the improvement of motion detection through a dense and symmetrical arrangement of photoreceptors (Lyall, 1957; van der Meer, 1992). Square mosaics occur more often in species moving, or observing movement, in all three dimensions of their habitat, while row mosaics are more common in schooling fishes with predominantly horizontal swimming movements (Wagner, 1990). The consistent regularity of T. delaisi’s cone mosaic throughout most of its retina could grant it a sensitive and accurate sense of motion perception in all directions and across its entire visual field, which would benefit its predation of small but sometimes fast-moving crustaceans in a complex micro-environment.

Alternatively, a regular cone mosaic could improve chromatic resolution by uniformly distributing cones with differing spectral sensitivities across the retina, to help in adapting the visual system to the spectral composition of the photic environment through the relative sizes and ratios of individual cone types (Fernald, 1981; van der Meer, 1992). Accordingly, T. delaisi’s consistent mosaic could grant it acute color vision throughout its visual field. Additionally, the higher ratio of long-wavelength-sensitive twin cones in the foveal area (Figure 8) increases sensitivity to the respective parts of the spectrum and suggests that light of longer wavelengths carries important information for T. delaisi, maybe helping to distinguish camouflaged prey, predators, or conspecifics from the background.

Triple Cones

Various types of triple cones have been found in many teleost species (Hess, 2009) but their distribution across the retina is poorly documented, e.g., there’s no entry in the Clinical and Experimental Optometry’s retinal topography maps database (Collin, 2008). Our systematical analysis of T. delaisi’s triple cone distribution revealed them to be exclusively located in an area adjacent to the optic nerve head in the dorso-temporal retina (Figure 8), which seems to be a common location for triple cones (Hess, 2009). Due to the localized occurrence, the total population was estimated to be only 5000 triple cones, or roughly one percent of all cones, which is similar to the proportion found in most other species (Hess, 2009). The exact function of triple cones is yet unclear. In some species they only occur after regeneration of previously mechanically damaged retina and are regarded as malformations (Cameron and Easter, 1995; Cameron and Powers, 2000). In other species, however, a role in vision seems evident, especially where triple cones dominate parts of or the entire retina (Hess, 2009). Intriguingly, Cameron and Easter’s (1995) description of the triple cones’ location, morphology, and arrangement after regeneration in the retina of the green sunfish matches the situation found in T. delaisi almost exactly. However, T. delaisi’s triple cones being the result of regenerative processes is unlikely, given the consistency of their occurrence and the naive condition of all study individuals. A visual function is more likely, because triple cones dominate the area where they occur, and the RGC density in that area is both relatively high and continuous with that of the surrounding retina. In some fishes, like the cutlips minnow and several anchovy species (Collin et al., 1996; Kondrashev et al., 2012), the triple cones consist of unequal members. In anchovies, it could further be shown that the lateral and central components exhibit different spectral sensitivities, which may suggest triplecones serve color discrimination in the respective species (Kondrashev et al., 2012). In T. delaisi, however, the triple cones consist of morphologically equal members and, unfortunately, data on their spectral sensitivity is currently not available, as they were missed by Bitton et al. (2017) in their MSP study. Therefore, their role in the vision of T. delaisi remains to be determined in a future study.

Foveal Structure

Many teleost retinae feature an area centralis, while foveae are less common (Collin and Pettigrew, 1988b,c; Nicol and Somiya, 1989; Collin, 2008). T. delaisi’s type of fovea with a steep, convexiclivate pit and incompletely displaced inner retinal layers is also found in syngnathids and the sandlance, who also share some aspects of their ecology with triplefins (Collin and Collin, 1988b, 1999). The curvature of the foveal pit combined with the higher refractive index of the retinal tissue compared to the vitreous create optical effects that may enhance multiple visual functions in the foveal area. One commonly suggested effect is the magnification of an image in the center of the fovea (Walls, 1937; Snyder and Miller, 1978). This effect is generated at the base of the foveal pit and requires it to have a smooth and symmetrical curvature. T. delaisi’s foveal pit, however, can vary from a concentric dimple to a slit-like trough, which makes a consistent and precise optical magnification effect unlikely. Other studies suggest that image distortion by the foveal slope could inform about the focal state of the eye, help distinguish small targets from the background, and improve motion detection (Pumphrey, 1948; Harkness and Bennet-Clark, 1978; Steenstrup and Munk, 1980). All of these potential functions would be plausible and advantageous in T. delaisi’s ecological context, but unfortunately they might have to remain speculations, since the crucial factor of foveal slope might be impossible to experimentally manipulate.

Spatial Resolving Power

The major factors determining the SRP of an eye are the number of neural elements in the retina and the quality of the optical system of the eye (Land and Nilsson, 2012). In terms of retinal elements, previous studies have often used either photoreceptor or RGC densities to estimate SRP, as reviewed by Caves et al. (2017). Using RGC density directly to estimate spatial resolution in species with a pronounced fovea can be misleading, however, because foveae are associated with a partial to complete displacement of RGCs. Since foveae are retinal specializations for high acuity, it is reasonable to assume that each receptor contributes an individual signal, i.e., the foveal summation rate is 1:1, at least in retinae adapted to photopic conditions. The displacement of RGC, however, can make the summation rate seem lower than it actually is and cause acuity to be underestimated. Our methodology did not allow us to estimate the actual summation rate in T. delaisi, but it is interesting to note that the RGC density stays relatively high throughout the retina, and that its average (32,000 cells mm-2) almost matches the average density of all cones (33,500 cells mm-2). Assuming that rods have high summation rates, as they serve scotopic vision, they would utilize only a fraction of the available RGCs. This would allow a relatively low summation rate for cones throughout the retina, and indicate that acuity is prioritized over sensitivity even in the peripheral retina.

Our photoreceptor-spacing-based estimates of T. delaisi’s anatomical SRP resulted in an acuity range of 6.7–9.0 cpd (9.0–6.7 arc minutes). This is supported by similar values reported for another triplefin species (Pankhurst et al., 1993) and also matches the overall average visual acuity of 8.4 ± 6.5 cpd found among 159 teleost species, as recalculated from the data in Caves et al. (2017). Based on Caves et al.’s (2017) methodology, T. delaisi’s anatomical SRP is well above average for its “relative eye investment,” a factor relating to the expected eye size for a given body size, stressing once more the importance of acute vision for the triplefin. A SRP of 6.7–9.0 cpd means T. delaisi could resolve a 1 mm target, like a copepod or small gammarid, at 38–52 cm (Figure 7). This further implies that it could monitor a large part of its ∼1–3 m2 territory (Goncalves and Almada, 1998) from a stationary position, which matches their typical behavior.

Although T. delaisi’s retinal organization suggests a high investment in visual acuity, the receptor cell coverage in the center of the fovea, and thus the potential spatial resolution, is not maximized (Figure 5A). The altered, elongated shape of the twin cones skews the lucky-clover mosaic pattern and creates gaps between them and the single cones. That this pattern is nevertheless maintained in the fovea either suggests a developmental constraint, or that the benefits of a regular arrangement outweigh those of a maximized coverage. Maybe the fovea realizes a compromise between motion sensitivity, or maybe acute color vision, and absolute SRP.

Conclusion

Tripterygion delaisi’s eye morphology, retinal organization and receptor composition show a host of adaptations that demonstrate the crucial role acute, color- and motion-sensitive vision plays in the life of a crypto-benthic micro-predator. The consistent and dominant presence of cones and the probably low summation ratio, as suggested by the high numbers of RGC even in the periphery, reveal a visual system adapted to a diurnal, photopic lifestyle in which acuity and color vision are prioritized over sensitivity. The elliptical eye shape and the antero-ventrally directed aphakic gap allow a greater range of lens movement and thus accommodation. The prominent area centralis with a convexiclivate fovea, and their dorso-temporal location in combination with the wide range of eye movements, allow high-resolution imaging of the substrate across a range of 120–130° in an antero-ventral direction. These complementary specializations are particularly advantageous for small benthic foragers, as they allow them to bring the substrate in their immediate vicinity into focus and scan it for potential prey at high resolution. The consistent presence and arrangement of single and twin cones possibly enhances color vision across the entire visual field. The change in their ratio in the foveal region suggests a sensitivity boost for longer wavelengths of light. All of this might improve T. delaisi’s ability to distinguish and identify predators, prey, and conspecifics in its visually complex environment. Alternatively, or additionally, the consistent cone mosaic could enhance motion detection, allowing T. delaisi to notice minute movements in its entire visual field and draw its attention to the source to inspect it with its fovea. Finally, the pronounced eye mobility, relatively high acuity throughout the retina, and probably high motion sensitivity allow T. delaisi to monitor and assess threats or targets of interest in its vicinity from a stationary position while maintaining its crypsis. All of these features make T. delaisi a prime example of how visual systems are adapted to their specific, respective lifestyles and needs, and how much anatomical traits can tell us about a species’ ecology. Despite a plethora of studies that have established the functions of most retinal features and their links to the respective species’ ecology, some remain unresolved or are limited to speculation, like the function of triple cones and their peculiar, localized distribution in the retina of T. delaisi.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with German animal ethics legislation under notifications AZ. 13.06.2013 and AZ. 29.10.2014 issued to NM by the animal welfare department of the district administration of Tübingen (“Regierungspräsidium Tübingen”). The protocol was approved by the animal ethics committee of the above authority.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: RF and NM. Funding acquisition: NM and SC. Resources: SC and NM. Methodology: RF, SC, and NM. Investigation: RF. Formal analysis: RF. Writing – original draft: RF. Writing – review and editing: RF, SC, and NM. Supervision: SC and NM. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

The work presented in this study was funded by the German Science Foundation (“Reinhart Koselleck Project” grant Mi 482/13-1 to NM), the Volkswagen Foundation (“Experiment!” grant Az. 89 148 and 91 816 to NM), and by grants from the Australian Research Council (ARC) and the West Australian Government to SC. The authors are further thankful for the German Science Foundation’s and the University of Tübingen Open Access Publishing Fund’s support in making this study openly available.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank João Paulo Coimbra for his invaluable methodological involvement and critical input in the writing of this paper. They further thank Michael Archer, Tom Stewart, Jeremy Ullmann, and Gary Cowen for their assistance and guidance in producing the histological and MRI sections of T. delaisi’s head. They are deeply grateful to Radhika Puttagunta and the Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Tübingen, for allowing them access to their stereology set-up. Finally, they thank all colleagues and referees, whose critical comments helped improving this paper.

References

Bartels, M. (1931). “Vergleichendes über Augenbewegungen,” in Receptionsorgane II: Photoreceptoren Zweiter Teil, eds M. Bartels, M. Baurmann, A. Bielschowsky, W. Dieter, H. Guillery, K. Koffka, et al. (Berlin: Springer), 1113–1165. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-91025-8_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biagioni, L. M., Hunt, D. M., and Collin, S. P. (2016). Morphological characterization and topographic analysis of multiple photoreceptor types in the retinae of mesopelagic hatchetfishes with tubular eyes. Front. Ecol. Evol. 4:25. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2016.00025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bitton, P. P., Harant, U. K., Fritsch, R., Champ, C. M., Temple, S. E., and Michiels, N. K. (2017). Red fluorescence of the triplefin Tripterygion delaisi is increasingly visible against background light with increasing depth. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4:161009. doi: 10.1098/rsos.161009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowmaker, J. K. (1990). “Visual pigments of fishes,” in The Visual System of Fish, eds R. Douglas and M. Djamgoz (Dordrecht: Springer), 81–107. doi: 10.1007/978-94-009-0411-8_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, D. A., and Easter, S. S. Jr. (1995). Cone photoreceptor regeneration in adult fish retina: phenotypic determination and mosaic pattern formation. J. Neurosci. 15(3 Pt 2), 2255–2271.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cameron, D. A., and Powers, M. K. (2000). Morphology and visual pigment content of photoreceptors from injured goldfish retina. Vis. Neurosci. 17, 623–630. doi: 10.1017/S0952523800174115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caves, E. M., Sutton, T. T., and Johnsen, S. (2017). Visual acuity in ray-finned fishes correlates with eye size and habitat. J. Exp. Biol. 220(Pt 9), 1586–1596. doi: 10.1242/jeb.151183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coimbra, J. P., Collin, S. P., and Hart, N. S. (2014). Topographic specializations in the retinal ganglion cell layer correlate with lateralized visual behavior, ecology, and evolution in cockatoos. J. Comp. Neurol. 522, 3363–3385. doi: 10.1002/cne.23637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coimbra, J. P., Collin, S. P., and Hart, N. S. (2015a). Variations in retinal photoreceptor topography and the organization of the rod-free zone reflect behavioral diversity in Australian passerines. J. Comp. Neurol. 523, 1073–1094. doi: 10.1002/cne.23718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coimbra, J. P., Kaswera-Kyamakya, C., Gilissen, E., Manger, P. R., and Collin, S. P. (2015b). The retina of ansorge’s cusimanse (Crossarchus ansorgei): number, topography and convergence of photoreceptors and ganglion cells in relation to ecology and behavior. Brain Behav. Evol. 86, 79–93. doi: 10.1159/000433514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coimbra, J. P., Marceliano, M. L., Andrade-da-Costa, B. L., and Yamada, E. S. (2006). The retina of tyrant flycatchers: topographic organization of neuronal density and size in the ganglion cell layer of the great kiskadee Pitangus sulphuratus and the rusty margined flycatcher Myiozetetes cayanensis (Aves: Tyrannidae). Brain Behav. Evol. 68, 15–25. doi: 10.1159/000092310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coimbra, J. P., Nolan, P. M., Collin, S. P., and Hart, N. S. (2012). Retinal ganglion cell topography and spatial resolving power in penguins. Brain Behav. Evol. 80, 254–268. doi: 10.1159/000341901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coimbra, J. P., Trevia, N., Marceliano, M. L., da Silveira Andrade-Da-Costa, B. L., Picanco-Diniz, C. W., and Yamada, E. S. (2009). Number and distribution of neurons in the retinal ganglion cell layer in relation to foraging behaviors of tyrant flycatchers. J. Comp. Neurol. 514, 66–73. doi: 10.1002/cne.21992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collin, H. B., and Collin, S. P. (1988a). The cornea of the sand lance, Limnichthyes fasciatus (Creeiidae). Cornea 7, 190–203.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P. (2008). A web-based archive for topographic maps of retinal cell distribution in vertebrates. Clin. Exp. Optom. 91, 85–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1444-0938.2007.00228.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P., and Collin, H. B. (1988b). The morphology of the retina and lens of the sandlance, Limnichthyes fasciatus (Creeiidae). Exp. Biol. 47, 209–218.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P., and Collin, H. B. (1988c). Topographic analysis of the retinal ganglion cell layer and optic nerve in the sandlance Limnichthyes fasciatus (Creeiidae, Perciformes). J. Comp. Neurol. 278, 226–241. doi: 10.1002/cne.902780206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P., and Collin, H. B. (1999). The foveal photoreceptor mosaic in the pipefish, Corythoichthyes paxtoni (Syngnathidae, Teleostei). Histol. Histopathol. 14, 369–382.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P., Collin, H. B., and Ali, M. A. (1996). Ultrastructure and organisation of the retina and pigment epithelium in the cutlips minnow, Exoglossum maxillingua (Cyprinidae, Teleostei). Histol. Histopathol. 11, 55–69.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P., Hoskins, R. V., and Partridge, J. C. (1997). Tubular eyes of deep-sea fishes: a comparative study of retinal topography. Brain Behav. Evol. 50, 335–357. doi: 10.1159/000113345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P., Lloyd, D. J., and Wagner, H. J. (2000). Foveate vision in deep-sea teleosts: a comparison of primary visual and olfactory inputs. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 355, 1315–1320. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2000.0691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P., and Pettigrew, J. D. (1988a). Retinal ganglion cell topography in teleosts: a comparison between Nissl-stained material and retrograde labelling from the optic nerve. J. Comp. Neurol. 276, 412–422. doi: 10.1002/cne.902760306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P., and Pettigrew, J. D. (1988b). Retinal topography in reef teleosts.1. Some species with well-developed areae but poorly-developed streaks. Brain Behav. Evol. 31, 269–282. doi: 10.1159/000116594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P., and Pettigrew, J. D. (1988c). Retinal topography in reef teleosts.2. Some species with prominent horizontal streaks and high-density areae. Brain Behav. Evol. 31, 283–295. doi: 10.1159/000116595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P., and Pettigrew, J. D. (1989). Quantitative comparison of the limits on visual spatial resolution set by the ganglion cell layer in twelve species of reef teleosts. Brain Behav. Evol 34, 184–192. doi: 10.1159/000116504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collin, S. P., and Shand, J. (2003). “Retinal sampling and the visual field in fishes,” in Sensory Processing in Aquatic Environments, eds S. P. Collin and N. J. Marshall (New York, NY: Springer), 139–169. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-22628-6_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engstrom, K. (1963). Cone types and cone arrangements in teleost retinae. Acta Zool. 44, 179–243. doi: 10.1111/j.1463-6395.1963.tb00408.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernald, R. D. (1981). Chromatic organization of a cichlid fish retina. Vis. Res. 21, 1749–1753. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(81)90207-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernald, R. D. (1990). “The optical system of fishes,” in The Visual System of Fish, eds R. Douglas and M. Djamgoz (Dordrecht: Springer), 45–61.

Google Scholar

Fileta, J. B., Huang, W., Kwon, G. P., Filippopoulos, T., Ben, Y., Dobberfuhl, A., et al. (2008). Efficient estimation of retinal ganglion cell number: a stereological approach. J. Neurosci. Methods 170, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritsch, R., Ullmann, J. F. P., Bitton, P. P., Collin, S. P., and Michiels, N. K. (2017). Optic-nerve-transmitted eyeshine, a new type of light emission from fish eyes. Front. Zool. 14:14. doi: 10.1186/s12983-017-0198-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritsches, K. A., and Marshall, N. J. (2002). Independent and conjugate eye movements during optokinesis in teleost fish. J. Exp. Biol. 205(Pt 9), 1241–1252.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Glaser, E. M., and Wilson, P. D. (1998). The coefficient of error of optical fractionator population size estimates: a computer simulation comparing three estimators. J. Microsc. 192(Pt 2), 163–171. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2818.1998.00417.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goncalves, E. J., and Almada, V. C. (1998). A comparative study of territoriality in intertidal and subtidal blennioids (Teleostei, Blennioidei). Environ. Biol. Fishes 51, 257–264. doi: 10.1023/A:1007482630052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harant, U. K., and Michiels, N. K. (2017). Fish with red fluorescent eyes forage more efficiently under dim, blue-green light conditions. BMC Ecol. 17:18. doi: 10.1186/s12898-017-0127-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harant, U. K., Michiels, N. K., Anthes, N., and Meadows, M. G. (2016). The consistent difference in red fluorescence in fishes across a 15 m depth gradient is triggered by ambient brightness, not by ambient spectrum. BMC Res. Notes 9:107. doi: 10.1186/s13104-016-1911-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harkness, L., and Bennet-Clark, H. C. (1978). The deep fovea as a focus indicator. Nature 272, 814–816. doi: 10.1038/272814a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hemmi, J. M., and Grunert, U. (1999). Distribution of photoreceptor types in the retina of a marsupial, the tammar wallaby (Macropus eugenii). Vis. Neurosci. 16, 291–302. doi: 10.1017/S0952523899162102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, M. (2009). Triple cones in the retinae of three anchovy species: Engraulis encrasicolus, Cetengraulis mysticetus and Anchovia macrolepidota (Engraulididae, Teleostei). Vis. Res. 49, 1569–1582. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, A. (1977). “The topography of vision in mammals of contrasting life style: comparative optics and retinal organisation,” in The Visual System in Vertebrates, ed. F. Crescitelli (Berlin: Springer), 613–756.

Google Scholar

Kahmann, H. (1936). Über das foveale Sehen der Wirbeltiere. Albrecht Von Graefes Arch. Ophthalmol. 135, 265–276. doi: 10.1007/bf01856858

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalb, N., Schneider, R. F., Sprenger, D., and Michiels, N. K. (2015). The red-fluorescing marine fish Tripterygion delaisi can perceive its own red fluorescent colour. Ethology 121, 566–576. doi: 10.1111/eth.12367

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kondrashev, S. L., Gnyubkina, V. P., and Zueva, L. V. (2012). Structure and spectral sensitivity of photoreceptors of two anchovy species: Engraulis japonicus and Engraulis encrasicolus. Vis. Res. 68, 19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroger, R. H., Campbell, M. C., Fernald, R. D., and Wagner, H. J. (1999). Multifocal lenses compensate for chromatic defocus in vertebrate eyes. J. Comp. Physiol. A 184, 361–369. doi: 10.1007/s003590050335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

La Mesa, G., Di Muccio, S., and Vacchi, M. (2005). Structure of a Mediterranean cryptobenthic fish community and its relationships with habitat characteristics. Mar. Biol. 149, 149–167. doi: 10.1007/s00227-005-0194-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Land, M. F., and Nilsson, D.-E. (2012). Animal Eyes. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199581139.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H. R., and O’Brien, K. M. (2011). Morphological and behavioral limit of visual resolution in temperate (Hippocampus abdominalis) and tropical (Hippocampus taeniopterus) seahorses. Vis. Neurosci. 28, 351–360. doi: 10.1017/S0952523811000149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Losey, G. S., McFarland, W. N., Loew, E. R., Zamzow, J. P., Nelson, P. A., and Marshall, N. J. (2003). Visual biology of Hawaiian coral reef fishes. I. Ocular transmission and visual pigments. Copeia 3, 433–454. doi: 10.1643/01-053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyall, A. H. (1957). Cone arrangements in teleost retinae. Q. J. Microsc. Sci. 98, 189–201.

Google Scholar

Mallory, F. B. (1900). A contribution to staining methods : I. A differential stain for connective-tissue fibrillae and reticulum. Ii. Chloride of Iron Haematoxylin for Nuclei and Fibrin. Iii. Phosphotungstic Acid Haematoxylin for Neuroglia Fibres. J. Exp. Med. 5, 15–20. doi: 10.1084/jem.5.1.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meadows, M. G., Anthes, N., Dangelmayer, S., Alwany, M. A., Gerlach, T., Schulte, G., et al. (2014). Red fluorescence increases with depth in reef fishes, supporting a visual function, not UV protection. Proc. Biol. Sci. 281:20141211. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mosk, V. J. (2004). The Visual System of Seahorses and Pipefish: A Study of Visual Pigments and Other Characteristics. M.Sc. thesis, University of Western Australia, Crawley WA.

Google Scholar

Munk, O. (1980). Hvirveldyrøjet: Bygning, Funktion og Tilpasning. Copenhagen: Berlingske Forlag.

Nicol, J. A. C., and Somiya, H. (1989). The Eyes of Fishes. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Google Scholar

Pankhurst, P. M., Pankhurst, N. W., and Montgomery, J. C. (1993). Comparison of behavioral and morphological measures of visual-acuity during ontogeny in a teleost fish, forsterygion-varium, tripterygiidae (Forster, 1801). Brain Behav. Evol. 42, 178–188. doi: 10.1159/000114151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pettigrew, J. D., and Collin, S. P. (1995). Terrestrial optics in an aquatic eye - the Sandlance, Limnichthytes Fasciatus (Creediidae, Teleostei). J. Comp. Physiol. A Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 177, 397–408. doi: 10.1007/BF00187476

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pettigrew, J. D., Collin, S. P., and Fritsches, K. (2000). Prey capture and accommodation in the sandlance, Limnichthyes fasciatus (Creediidae; Teleostei). J. Comp. Physiol. A Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 186, 247–260. doi: 10.1007/s003590050425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pignatelli, V., Champ, C., Marshall, J., and Vorobyev, M. (2010). Double cones are used for colour discrimination in the reef fish, Rhinecanthus aculeatus. Biol. Lett. 6, 537–539. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.1010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Pumphrey, R. J. (1948). The theory of the fovea. J. Exp. Ecol. 25, 299–312.

Google Scholar

Schmitz, L., and Wainwright, P. C. (2011). Nocturnality constrains morphological and functional diversity in the eyes of reef fishes. BMC Evol. Biol. 11:338. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-11-338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shand, J., Doving, K. B., and Collin, S. P. (1999). Optics of the developing fish eye: comparisons of Matthiessen’s ratio and the focal length of the lens in the black bream Acanthopagrus butcheri (Sparidae, Teleostei). Vis. Res. 39, 1071–1078. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00215-216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sivak, J. G. (1978). The functional significance of the aphakic space of the fish eye. Can. J. Zool. 56, 513–516. doi: 10.1139/Z78-072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, A. W., and Miller, W. H. (1978). Telephoto lens system of falconiform eyes. Nature 275, 127–129. doi: 10.1038/275127a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenstrup, S., and Munk, O. (1980). Optical function of the convexiclivate fovea with particular regard to notosudid deep-sea teleosts. Opt. Acta 27, 949–964. doi: 10.1080/713820342

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, J. (1981). The Whole Mount Handbook: A Guide to the Preparation and Analysis of Retinal Whole Mounts. Sydney: Maitland Publications.

Google Scholar

Tucker, V. A. (2000). The deep fovea, sideways vision and spiral flight paths in raptors. J. Exp. Biol. 203(Pt 24), 3745–3754.

Google Scholar

Ullmann, J. F., Moore, B. A., Temple, S. E., Fernandez-Juricic, E., and Collin, S. P. (2012). The retinal wholemount technique: a window to understanding the brain and behaviour. Brain Behav. Evol. 79, 26–44. doi: 10.1159/000332802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Meer, H. J. (1992). Constructional morphology of photoreceptor patterns in percomorph fish. Acta Biotheor. 40, 51–85. doi: 10.1007/BF00046551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, H. J. (1972). Vergleichende untersuchungen uber das muster der sehzellen und horizontalen in der teleostier-retina (Pisces). Z. Morphol. Tiere 72, 77–130. doi: 10.1007/BF00285614

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, H.-J. (1990). “Retinal structure of fishes,” in The Visual System of Fish, eds R. Douglas and M. Djamgoz (Dordrecht: Springer), 109–157.

Google Scholar

Walls, G. L. (1937). Significance of the foveal depression. Arch. Ophthalmol. 18, 912–919. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1937.00850120046005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walls, G. L. (1942). The Vertebrate Eye and Its Adaptive Radiation. Bloomfield Hills MI: Cranbrook Institute of Science. doi: 10.5962/bhl.title.7369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warrant, E. J., and Locket, N. A. (2004). Vision in the deep sea. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 79, 671–712. doi: 10.1017/S1464793103006420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wassle, H., Grunert, U., Rohrenbeck, J., and Boycott, B. B. (1990). Retinal ganglion cell density and cortical magnification factor in the primate. Vis. Res. 30, 1897–1911. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90166-I

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, M. J., Slomianka, L., and Gundersen, H. J. (1991). Unbiased stereological estimation of the total number of neurons in thesubdivisions of the rat hippocampus using the optical fractionator. Anat. Rec. 231, 482–497. doi: 10.1002/ar.1092310411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wikler, K. C., and Rakic, P. (1990). Distribution of photoreceptor subtypes in the retina of diurnal and nocturnal primates. J. Neurosci. 10, 3390–3401.

Google Scholar

Williams, D. R. (1988). Topography of the foveal cone mosaic in the living human eye. Vis. Res. 28, 433–454. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(88)90185-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, D. R., and Coletta, N. J. (1987). Cone spacing and the visual resolution limit. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 4, 1514–1523. doi: 10.1364/Josaa.4.001514

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wirtz, P. (1978). The behaviour of the Mediterranean Tripterygion species (Pisces, Blennioidei). Z. Fuer Tierpsychol. 48, 142–174. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wucherer, M. F., and Michiels, N. K. (2012). A fluorescent chromatophore changes the level of fluorescence in a reef fish. PLOS ONE 7:e37913. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wucherer, M. F., and Michiels, N. K. (2014). Regulation of red fluorescent light emission in a cryptic marine fish. Front. Zool. 11:1. doi: 10.1186/1742-9994-11-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yushkevich, P. A., Piven, J., Hazlett, H. C., Smith, R. G., Ho, S., Gee, J. C., et al. (2006). User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of anatomical structures: significantly improved efficiency and reliability. Neuroimage 31, 1116–1128. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zander, C. D. (1982). Feeding ecology of littoral gobiid and blennioid fish of the Banyuls area (Mediterranean Sea). I. Main food and trophic dimension of niche and ecotope. Vie Milieu 32, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Zander, C. D., Whitehead, P. J. P., Bauchot, M. L., Hureau, J. C., Nielsen, J., and Tortonese, E. (1986). Tripterygiidae. Paris: Unesco.

Google Scholar

Keywords: Tripterygion delaisi, visual ecology, retinal wholemount technique, retinal specializations, retinal topography, visual acuity, cone mosaic, triple cones

Citation: Fritsch R, Collin SP and Michiels NK (2017) Anatomical Analysis of the Retinal Specializations to a Crypto-Benthic, Micro-Predatory Lifestyle in the Mediterranean Triplefin Blenny Tripterygion delaisi. Front. Neuroanat. 11:122. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2017.00122

Received: 19 October 2017; Accepted: 28 November 2017;
Published: 12 December 2017.

Edited by:

Nicolás Cuenca, University of Alicante, Spain

Reviewed by:

Antón Barreiro-Iglesias, Universidade de Santiago de Compostela, Spain
Preston E. Garraghty, Indiana University Bloomington, United States

Copyright © 2017 Fritsch, Collin and Michiels. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Roland Fritsch, roland.fritsch@uni-tuebingen.de; roland.fritsch@uqconnect.edu.au

Download